Skip to main content
PMC Canada Author Manuscripts logoLink to PMC Canada Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Sep 12.
Published in final edited form as: J Neurol Sci. 2011 Jun 17;310(1-2):202–207. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2011.06.006

Impulsivity and Parkinson’s disease: More than just disinhibition

Francesca Antonelli a,b, Nicola Ray a,b, Antonio P Strafella a,b,*
PMCID: PMC3440306  CAMSID: CAMS2370  PMID: 21683964

Abstract

In the last few years it has become clear that impulsivity is a complex behaviour composed of different domains and dependent on different neural networks. The proposed pathogenetic mechanisms for the emergence of impulsivity disorders in Parkinson’s Disease (PD) can be broadly separated into three potentially interacting processes: the contribution of premorbid susceptibility to impulsivity, the contribution of the disease itself to the behaviour and the potential contribution of therapeutic agents. Growing evidence suggests that dopamine and the subthalamic nucleus are playing a certain role in the pathophysiology of different aspects of impulsivity. In this review, we summarise the main concepts defining various components of impulsivity both in healthy subjects and patients affected by PD.

Keywords: Parkinson’s disease, Brain imaging, Dopamine, Subthalamic nucleus, Impulsivity, Impulse control disorders

1. Introduction

Impulsivity is a complex personality dimension, which might be defined in different ways. The Diagnostic and Statistical Mental disorders manual-IV RT6 defines impulsivity as “a failure to resist an impulse, drive or temptation to perform an act that is harmful to the person and others”. Recent studies have extended beyond the idea of impulsivity being based in poorly planned actions, and have yielded to a broader definition that includes more cognitive functions. In particular, according to Bechara’s model [1], two major processes linked to different neural networks and activated by different experimental paradigms have been defined: motor impulsivity and cognitive impulsivity. Motor impulsivity is described in terms of disinhibition of prepotent responses. Cognitive impulsivity [2] is a more complex process, and is the result of a suppression of previously activated cognitive contents [3]. This review has two goals: 1) to describe the two different dimensions of impulsivity, focusing on their neural pattern and their specific experimental paradigms 2) to review studies on impulsivity (taking into account the two dimensions) in patients with PD, exploring the role of dopamine replacement therapy (DRT) and the subthalamic nucleus (STN) in the pathophysiology of excessive impulsivity.

2. Cognitive impulsivity

Cognitive impulsivity is a complex psychological domain characterised by different facets.

Altered decision making is frequently associated with cognitive impulsivity [1,4,5]. It is the product of an imbalance between two separate, but interacting, neural systems: (1) an impulsive, amygdala-dependent system, which controls immediate and “somatic” behaviour related to a decision and (2) a reflective, prefrontal-dependent system [6], which evaluates the future prospects related to the decision [4]. The conditions that lead to this imbalance include a dysfunctional reflective system and a hyperactive impulsive system [4]. The paradigm most used to measure decision-making is the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), an ecologically valid decision task involving weighing of immediate rewards against long-term losses. Neuroimaging studies on healthy subjects (HS) showed that the activated regions during IGT were those underlying both somatic activation and future oriented decision-making [7]. However, recent reports showed the complexity of this task that might also be considered as a measure of risk taking [1,811]. In particular, poor performance during the first part of the task has been related to ambiguous risk taking [1215].

Risk taking is considered another facet of cognitive impulsivity [16]. Brand described two kinds of risk taking: risk taking under stable probabilistic contingencies (explicit risk-taking) and ambiguous risk taking, in which the subject is unaware of the probabilistic contingencies [1315]. Ambiguous risk taking is related to the functioning of the ventral frontostriatal loop [17,18]. In particular, in a recent fMRI study, the neural systems responding to degrees of uncertainty were related to the orbitofrontal cortex and the amygdala, activated in association with the “vigilance”/evaluation-system which responds rapidly to the degree of uncertainty, and to the dorsal striatum that was associated with reward-anticipation [19]. To explore ambiguous risk-taking, researchers have used the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) that involves actual risky behaviour for which riskiness is rewarded up until a point at which further riskiness results in poorer outcomes [20]. The task consists of different trials in which subjects inflate a virtual balloon that can either grow larger or explode [20]. Behavioural studies in HS revealed that poor performance on the BART correlates with high self-reported impulsivity in healthy subjects [20]. So far, only one imaging study has examined neural activation during the BART in HS. In a version of the task adapted for fMRI the authors confirmed the involvement of mesolimbic-frontal pathway during ambiguous active risk-taking [21]. The evaluation of explicit risk-taking may be done by using the Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT) in which the relevant information is presented to the subjects ‘up-front’ and there is no need to learn or retrieve information over consecutive trials. Because of that, the CGT is considered a measure of explicit risk and, compared to the IGT, a way to assess decision-making and risk-taking behaviour outside a learning context [22]. Some data are available on the neuroanatomical correlates of performance on the CGT in HS. A modified version of the CGT, the Risk Task [23], has been used in functional imaging studies, revealing significant activations in multiple regions within the inferior and orbital prefrontal cortex, which are involved in the representation of stimulus–reward relationships [24,25].

The perception of time and the duration between the choice and the reception of the reward are other crucial factors involved when individuals have to make decisions and consider the outcomes associated with their choices [26]. In this view, when we talk about impulsivity, it’s important also to consider the phenomenon of delay discounting, in which a delayed outcome of a choice reduces the subjective value of the reward [27,28]. A way to measure temporal discounting behaviour in human participants experimentally is the Delay Discounting Task (DDT). In delay discounting procedures, participants make choices between rewards that are smaller but sooner versus rewards that are larger but delayed. The temporal discounting pattern has repeatedly been described as following a hyperbolic function, meaning that it is related to a rapid fall of the subjective reward for small delay periods, whereas the decline is slower for longer delay periods [2931]. Impulsive individuals discount delayed rewards more strongly than do more self-controlled individuals [26]. Strong evidence of limbic and paralimbic cortical activation was found when HS were presented with choices between a smaller earlier reward and a greater but delayed reward [3234]. Lateral prefrontal and parietal cortices, associated with executive control and time processing itself, were activated when subjects delayed gratification [3234]. In addition, a study by Tanaka and colleagues [34] suggests that the striatum and the insula might be implicated in the evaluation of reward outcomes as a function of delay.

Reward and reversal learning (inability to reproduce behaviours that lead to positive outcomes and to extinguish behaviours that lead to negative outcomes) [35,36] are elementary cognitive processes of impulsivity [37]. Different experimental paradigms have been used to explore reward and reversal learning, for example the probabilistic reversal learning task proposed by O’Doherty [38], or the probabilistic selection task, proposed by Frank [36]. Behavioural and cognitive studies have identified two main neural systems that are involved in reinforcement and reversal learning in HS [39]. On the one hand, the OFC is implicated in the context of uncertain or changing contingencies [23,38]. In particular, the lateral OFC is activated following a punishing outcome, the medial OFC is activated following a rewarding outcome [38]. On the other hand, the basal ganglia and the neuromodulator dopamine are thought to participate in both action selection and reinforcement learning [4044], as confirmed by Pessiglione and colleagues in a behavioural and neuroimaging investigation. In that study, the authors demonstrated how dopamine might modulate, during instrumental learning, the magnitude of reward prediction error in the striatum [45].

3. Motor impulsivity

As mentioned above, motor impulsivity refers to the tendency to perform previously learned motor responses despite signals to the contrary. It is frequently measured in the laboratory within the framework of paradigms that infer that motor impulsivity can be quantified using a stop-signal reaction time task (SSRT) or a go/no-go task. As such, a premium is placed on the speed or accuracy with which we can inhibit an action that has, as a requisite of the task, become habitual. Thus, slow reaction times (to the ‘stop-signal’) and inaccurate responding indicate higher degrees of motor impulsivity, a trait that has been demonstrated in populations with an arguably poor ability to inhibit actions, for e.g., those with behavioural and chemical addictions [4648].

In recent years our understanding of the neural control of response inhibition has benefited from a large degree of attention from researchers using neuroimaging techniques. Using the SSRT and go/nogo tasks, research suggests that a distributed cortical and subcortical network controls our ability to inhibit unwanted actions, and that failures of inhibition can be traced back to altered activity within particular nodes. Areas that have been implicated in normally functioning response inhibition include, but aren’t limited to, the inferior frontal cortex (IFC), the SMA, the ACC and STN of the basal ganglia.

HS typically show activity within these areas when performing response inhibition tasks [49,50], and the normal variation of ability to inhibit responses correlates with their degree of engagement [49,51]. Thus, in one account of response inhibition stop-signals are processed in the IFC and are sent via a hyper direct pathway to the STN, which is then activated to inhibit activity within basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical loops related to the action to be inhibited [50]. Stop-signal and go/nogo tasks in which the participant simply attempts to inhibit all go-signals that are followed by the stop-signal, are thought to provide a measure of reactive inhibition, where the need to stop is ‘spur of the moment’. However, paradigms that employ conditional stop-signals, in which the stop-signal applies only to a selection of go-signals, may measure response inhibition that is more selective in nature [52], as well as being more ‘proactive’ in the case of tasks that inform the participant whether a stop response may be required for the upcoming trial. The latter tasks seem to engage the striatum [53] more than the former, suggesting that specificity of the action to inhibit combined with preparation to inhibit requires the indirect basal ganglia pathways to select inhibition of particular actions, whilst reactive inhibition tasks can be performed without striatal involvement and induce a more global stop response. As suggested in a recent review [52], proactive and selective inhibitions may be a more ecologically appropriate simulation for the control of real-life motivational urges such as gambling and shopping than the more global inhibition required for reactive inhibition tasks.

4. Impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease

To understand impulsivity in Parkinson’s has become important in and of itself since recent revelations regarding the development of impulsive behaviours (like pathological gambling) in PD treated with DRT [55]. The proposed pathogenetic mechanisms for the emergence of impulsivity disorders in PD can be broadly separated into three potentially interacting processes: the contribution of the disease itself to the behaviour, whether as a manifestation of a particular disease phenotype or genotype, or as a compensatory mechanism for the underlying disease process, the contribution of premorbid susceptibility to impulsivity, and the potential contribution of therapeutic agents and their potential interaction with either of above [54].

5. Cognitive impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease

The study of the role of the disease itself on cognitive impulsivity has shown controversial results, maybe due to the heterogeneity of the PD population. Regarding decision-making, only few studies have compared de-novo drug-naïve PD patients with HS. These revealed no differences in cognitive performance between PD patients and HS [56]. The majority of studies investigated decision-making in non-demented medicated PD patients, showing in some cases poorer IGT performance in PD patients compared to HS [57,58]. In others, similar IGT performance was reported [59,60]. However, they almost all agreed that there is no relationship between IGT performance and demographic and clinical features of the patients [61]. Looking at time processing, the same controversial results have been produced. In fact, some authors [62] did not find timing deficits in PD patients, whilst others suggested that time estimation, i.e. the ‘internal clock’, is abnormally slow in PD, [63] and that DRT [64] and deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus (STN DBS) [65] might reverse this condition.

Many authors have tried to evaluate cognitive impulsivity in different subgroups of PD patients. Compared to PD patients without impulse control disorder (ICDs), PD patients with pathological gambling (PG) showed impaired cognitive impulsivity, i.e. poorer performances on the IGT [66], and preference for immediate over future rewards [67]. Neuroimaging studies have underlined the idea of a susceptibility to impulsivity. Indeed in the subpopulation of PD patients with PG compared to PD non-gamblers, there is an abnormal activation of cortical [68] and subcortical [69] areas implicated in impulse control during the task. In particular Steeves et al. underlined the role of the striatum in a recent PET study [70]. The authors, using [11C] Raclopride to compare D2 receptor availability during a control and a gambling task in two groups of PD patients being treated with dopamine agonists (DAs), one with and one without PG, found that patients with PG had increased release of dopamine in the ventral striatum during the gambling task. The result may be due to the agonists themselves or depend on a sensitization of circuits [71] that is also seen in chemical addicts in response to their chosen drug of abuse [72,73], confirming the idea that DAs might also interact with an underlying susceptibility. Along similar lines, a more recent H2(15)O PET study showed that DAs increased the activity related to a gambling task in brain areas implicated in impulse control in PD patients without gambling. In contrast gamblers showed a DA-induced reduction of activity. Thus, by disrupting the inhibitory key functions of those brain areas in vulnerable patients with PD, DAs may foster the development of PG [74]. Similarly, PD gamblers recently have been shown to be more risk prone ON medication, compare to non-gamblers PD [75].

Dopaminergic medication could potentially modulate impulsivity itself at multiple levels and several hypotheses have been advanced to explain the phenomenon. Experiments using probabilistic selection and a transitive interference task in PD patients supported the hypothesis for which dopamine neurons encode positive and negative rewards in a phasic mode. In particular Frank et al., revealed how DRT (in this case a combination of Levodopa and DAs) can worsen learning from negative outcomes [35,36]. DAs, in contrast to Levodopa, tonically stimulate the dopamine receptors, and may therefore block a phasic dopamine dip that serves as a crucial component of the learning signal [35,36]. Those data were confirmed by an fMRI study, in which DAs, but not Levodopa, likely preventing pauses in dopamine transmission, impair the negative reinforcing effect of losing mediated by the orbitofrontal cortex [76]. An alternative, but not mutually exclusive mechanism of impaired learning induced by DAs was suggested by Voon [77]; using a reinforcement-learning model it was shown that DAs in susceptible individuals with PD provoke a distorted estimation of the gain cue. In particular, they augment the rate of learning from gain outcomes by increasing striatal prediction error activity. Moreover, some data revealed a specific role for dopamine in controlling the relationship between the timing of future rewards and their subjective value [78]. In this view, DRT seems to speed up the pacemaker during decisions [16]. Intriguing preclinical data recently showed that dopamine might have a controversial role in mediating risk-based decision making, with increased activation of D1 and D2 receptors biassing choice toward larger, probabilistic rewards, whereas D3 receptors appear to exert opposing effects on this form of decision making [79]. To our knowledge however no human data have been done to confirm those findings. Finally, some authors, using the IGT, suggested that therapy might have a specific role in ambiguous risk-taking, acting specifically on the ventral limbic loop [16,59]. Conversely, STN-DBS seems to play a major role in the second part of the task [77,78], which is more related to explicit risk-taking. That said, the literature on STN-DBS and cognitive impulsivity is controversial [80]. In fact, if some study revealed a specific effect on the dorsal executive loop [81,82], others revealed that explicit and implicit stimulus reward learning was unchanged ON and OFF stimulations [83]. Those controversies are possibly related to the different clinical features of the studied population, or to the position of the electrode in the STN, as elegantly demonstrated by Rodriguez Oroz [84], or finally to the experimental task. In fact, as Frank and colleagues [36] suggested, the STN seems to be involved, reducing premature responding, particularly when a response is executed between multiple competing others.

6. Motor impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease

The study of motor impulsivity in patients with PD may allow us to understand the effects of dopamine deficiency on our ability to inhibit motor responses. Further, recently developed surgical treatments for PD have allowed us to probe, and alter, an important node within the neural network responsible for response inhibition.

PD patients are found to perform poorly on measures of response inhibition [85], and this ability is altered during experimental manipulation of the network sub serving response inhibition using DBS. DBS of the STN has been shown to improve the motor symptoms of PD [86] and allows us to interrupt activity within the STN in order that we can investigate its function. However DBS has been shown to impair, improve and to make no change to patients’ ability to inhibit responses. [8791]. Such discrepancies in the literature may be explained by more general improvements on the task due to improved motor control [92], dissociable temporal effects of stimulation that result in increased impulsive responding as well as improvements in the engagement of inhibitory processes [93], or differences in the effect of STN DBS on inhibitory control depending on whether the ventral or dorsal STN is stimulated [94].

Specific neuroimaging tools have allowed us to visualise the brain as we apply DBS during response inhibition tasks. For example Campbell et al., used H2(15)O PET to measure STN DBS-induced variability in motor response inhibition [90]. They found that STN DBS caused blood flow changes in the ACC that correlated with a change in response inhibition. This suggests that stimulation of the STN may induce changes in the cortical (i.e. ACC) control of response inhibition, and the more it does so in individual patients, the greater the impairment in response inhibition. A later H2(15)O PET measured blood flow during a Go/NoGo and a control (Go) task to study response inhibition deficits associated with STN-DBS [91]. They found that STN DBS impaired response inhibition, measured as a greater number of errors during NoGo trials. The PET results revealed that changes on the task were accompanied by reduced activation in areas such as the left premotor cortex, pre-SMA, dorsal ACC and IFC. These areas are thought to sub-serve retroactive response inhibition in which a stimulus to stop must be processed and acted upon in order that inhibition is successful.

Thus, the work discussed in PD has confirmed the less direct evidence from neuroimaging in healthy controls that response inhibition depends on activity within cortico-basal ganglia loops. It is alteration of function within these connections that may be at the root of ICDs that develop in dopamine agonist treated Parkinson’s patients. As mentioned before, PD patients with gambling problems that develop after DAs are less able to learn from negative outcomes, perhaps due to the tonic occupation by DAs of striatal post-synaptic receptors, which may prevent the dips in dopamine transmission that would normally signal negative outcomes. Extrapolating to behavioural measures of response inhibition might imply that stop-signals are more difficult to respond to when patients are on dopamine agonists due to the more dominant go-signal mediated by post-synaptic occupation of dopamine receptors [36]. However, to the author’s knowledge, there have been no studies that have measured motor response inhibition whilst PD patients with dopamine agonist induced ICDs are on versus off medication.

As discussed above, activity in the striatum is modulated by stopping during response inhibition tasks [50], and is more active during periods of increased anticipation of stop-signals [53]. This suggests that the striatum may be involved in particular during proactive stopping, in which a person prepares to respond to stop-signals that are expected, which may be more closely related to the control of motivational urges, such as gambling, than reactive stopping [52]. Indeed, Steeves [70] found greater release of dopamine in the striatum during a gambling task in PD patients with DAs induced gambling behaviour than control PD patients also on agonists but without any gambling problems. Whilst this finding likely represents an inappropriate reward response during gambling for those patients, it may also suggest that abnormal striatal dopaminergic function could lead to impairments in proactive inhibition required for adequate control of motivational urges. Clearly however, more research on the role of dopamine during response inhibition tasks is required in order for us to understand the role dopamine may play in impulse control in PD.

7. Summary

The available evidence so far supports the idea of impulsivity as a complex concept, involving two major processes, motor impulsivity and cognitive impulsivity. On one hand motor impulsivity seems to have a more clear definition, maybe in relation to the efficacy of the paradigm to delineate a specific domain, related to the activity of specific neural network. That said, recent research has shown that proactive and selective inhibition may be a more ecologically appropriate simulation for the control of real-life motivational urges than the more global inhibition required for reactive inhibition tasks. On the other hand, cognitive impulsivity appears more difficult to define and to evaluate in all of its components. This might be in relation to specific experimental paradigms that do not clearly and totally delimit the cognitive impulsivity framework, and also to the lack of studies evaluating cognitive impulsivity in its entirety. For those reasons, future research in the field should be more extensive, beyond the idea of a one-dimensional subject of study, trying to link impulsivity to other cognitive processes. Considering the key role of dopamine in the impulsivity domain, the study of the PD model, the use of DAs acting on different kinds of dopamine receptors, and the study of the modulation of STN function will be very important to better understand the impulsivity concept itself, and to highlight the pathophysiology of those impulse control behaviours that are becoming more and more frequently diagnosed in the movement disorder field.

So far, the principal results have shown that increased impulsivity in PD patients may relate to the disease itself, to a susceptibility factor, and to the effect of PD treatment. These may modulate reward sensitivity by altering the fine balance between limbic/executive networks in favour of the limbic system. The outcomes are goal-oriented behaviours leading to greater risk or long term loss and decision making impairments, altered time processing and delay overestimation, distorted estimation of the gain and increasing striatal prediction error activity.

STN DBS may also be involved in cognitive impulsivity, in relation to the role of the STN in limbic circuitry and to its role in high-conflict decision-making processes and time processing. However, its role in decision-making and feedback based learning is still debated. More evident is the involvement of STN in motor impulsivity, even if its specific function is in part unknown, maybe in relation to the different functional sub-territories of the nucleus. New experimental studies evaluating impulsivity in all its components and exploring the effects of DRT and DBS should be carried out in order to clarify all these issues.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Canadian Institutes of Health Research [grant number MOP 64423] and from the Edmond J. Safra Philanthropic Foundation.

Footnotes

A.P.S. is supported by a grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (MOP 64423) and Edmond J. Safra Philanthropic Foundation.

References

  • 1.Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2000;3:295–307. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Harnishfeger K. The development of cognitive inhibition: theories, definition, and research evidence. In: Dempster F, Brainerd C, editors. Interference and inhibition in cognition. San Diego: 1995. pp. 175–204. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Aron AR. The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. Neuroscientist. 2007;3:214–28. doi: 10.1177/1073858407299288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bechara A. Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1458–63. doi: 10.1038/nn1584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Sweitzer MM, Allen PA, Kaut KP. Relation of individual differences in impulsivity to nonclinical emotional decision making. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2008;14:878–82. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708080934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hare TA, Camerer CF, Rangel A. Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science. 2009;324:646–8. doi: 10.1126/science.1168450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Verdejo-García A, Bechara A. A somatic marker theory o addiction. Neuropharmacology. 2009;56:48–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.07.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Krain AL, Wilson AM, Arbuckle R, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. Distinct neural mechanisms of risk and ambiguity: a meta-analysis of decision-making. Neuro-image. 2006;32:477–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Pagonabarraga J, Garcia-Sanchez C, Llebaria G, Pascual-Sedano B, Gironell A, Kulisevsky J. Controlled study of decsion-making and cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2007;22:1430–5. doi: 10.1002/mds.21457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Kobayakawa M, Koyama S, Mimura M, Kawamura M. Decision making in Parkinson’s disease: analysis of behavioral and physiological patterns in the Iowa gambling task. Mov Disord. 2008;23:547–52. doi: 10.1002/mds.21865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bechara A. Risky business: emotion, decision-making, and addiction. J Gambl Stud. 2003;19:23–51. doi: 10.1023/a:1021223113233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Franken IH, van Strien JW, Nijs I, Muris P. Impulsivity is associated with behavioral decision-making deficits. Psychiatry Res. 2008;158:155–63. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Brand M, Labudda K, Kalbe E, et al. Decision-making impairments in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Behav Neurol. 2004;15:77–85. doi: 10.1155/2004/578354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Brand M, Labudda K, Markowitsch HJ. Neuropsychological correlates of decision-making in ambiguous and risky situations. Neural Netw. 2006;19:1266–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2006.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Brand M, Recknor EC, Grabenhorst F, Bechara A. Decisions under ambiguity and decisions under risk: correlations with executive functions and comparisons of two different gambling tasks with implicit and explicit rules. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2007;29:86–99. doi: 10.1080/13803390500507196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Robert G, Drapier D, Verin M, Millet B, Azulay JP, Blin O. Cognitive impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease patients: assessment and pathophysiology. Mov Disord. 2009;24:2316–27. doi: 10.1002/mds.22836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lee TM, Chan CC, Han SH, Leung AW, Fox PT, Gao JH. An event-related fMRI study on risk taking by healthy individuals of high or low impulsiveness. Neurosci Lett. 2008;438:138–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.04.061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Brand M, Kalbe E, Labudda K, Fujiwara E, Kessler J, Markowitsch HJ. Decision-making impairments in patients with pathological gambling. Psychiatry Res. 2005;133:91–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hsu M, Bhatt M, Adolphs R, Tranel D, Camerer CF. Neural systems responding to degrees of uncertainty in human decision-making. Science. 2005;310:1680–3. doi: 10.1126/science.1115327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Lejuez CW, Read JP, Kahler CW, Richards JB, Ramsey SE, Stuart GL, et al. Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) J Exp Psychol Appl. 200;2:75–84. doi: 10.1037//1076-898x.8.2.75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rao H, Korczykowski M, Pluta J, Hoang A, Detre JA. Neural correlates of voluntary and involuntary risk taking in the human brain: an fMRI Study of the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART) Neuroimage. 2008;42:902–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Torralva T, Kipps CM, Hodges JR, Clark L, Bekinschtein T, Roca M, et al. The relationship between affective decision-making and theory of mind in the frontal variant of fronto-temporal dementia. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:342–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Rogers RD, Owen AM, Middleton HC, Williams EJ, Pickard JD, Sahakian BJ, et al. Choosing between small, likely rewards and large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and orbital prefrontal cortex. Neurosci. 1999;19:9029–38. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-20-09029.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Rolls ET. The orbitofrontal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1996;351:1433–43. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1996.0128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Dias R, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts. Nature. 1996;380:69–72. doi: 10.1038/380069a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Wittmann M, Paulus MP. Decision making, impulsivity and time perception. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12:7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kirby KN, Santiesteban M. Concave utility, transaction costs, and risk in measuring discounting of delayed rewards. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2003;29:66–79. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Laibson D. Golden eggs and hyperbolic discounting. Q J Econ. 1997;112:443–77. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Ainslie G. Specious reward — behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychol Bull. 1975;82:463–96. doi: 10.1037/h0076860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lane SD, Cherek DR, Pietras CJ, Tcheremissine OV. Measurement of delay discounting using trial by-trial consequences. Behav Processes. 2003;64:287–303. doi: 10.1016/s0376-6357(03)00143-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Madden GJ, Begotka AM, Raiff BR, Kastern LL. Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2003;11:139–45. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.11.2.139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.McClure SM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD. Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science. 2004;306:503–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1100907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.McClure SM, Ericson KM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD. Time discounting for primary rewards. J Neurosci. 2007;27:5796–804. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4246-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Tanaka SC, Doya K, Okada G, Ueda K, Okamoto Y, Yamawaki S. Prediction of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:887–93. doi: 10.1038/nn1279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Frank MJ, Seeberger LC, O’Reilly RC. By carrot or by stick: cognitive reinforcement learning in Parkinsonism. Science. 2004;306:1940–3. doi: 10.1126/science.1102941. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Frank MJ, Samanta J, Moustafa AA, Sherman SJ. Hold your horses: impulsivity, deep brain stimulation, and medication in Parkinsonism. Science. 2007;318:1309–12. doi: 10.1126/science.1146157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Fellows LK, Farah MJ. Different underlying impairments in decision-making following ventromedial and dorsolateral frontal lobe damage in humans. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15:58–63. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.O’Doherty J, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, Hornak J, Andrews C. Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:95–102. doi: 10.1038/82959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Frank MJ. Hold your horses: a dynamic computational role for the subthalamic nucleus in decision making. Neural Netw. 2006;19:1120–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2006.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Beiser DG, Houk JC. Model of cortical–basal ganglionic processing: encoding the serial order of sensory events. J Neurophysiol. 1998;79:3168–88. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.6.3168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Brown J, Bullock D, Grossberg S. How the basal ganglia use parallel excitatory and inhibitory learning pathways to selectively respond to unexpected rewarding cues. J Neurosci. 1999;19:10502–11. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-23-10502.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Brown JW, Bullock D, Grossberg S. How laminar frontal cortex and basal ganglia circuits interact to control planned and reactive saccades. Neural Netw. 2004 May;17:471–510. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2003.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Frank MJ, Loughry B, O’Reilly RC. Interactions between frontal cortex and basal ganglia in working memory: a computational model. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2001;1:137–60. doi: 10.3758/cabn.1.2.137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Frank MJ, O’Reilly RC. A mechanistic account of striatal dopamine function in human cognition: psychopharmacological studies with cabergoline and haloperidol. Behav Neurosci. 2006;120:497–517. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.3.497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Pessiglione M, Seymour B, Flandin G, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature. 2006;442:1042–5. doi: 10.1038/nature05051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Goudriaan AE, Oosterlaan J, De Beurs E, Van Den Brink W. The role of self-reported impulsivity and reward sensitivity versus neurocognitive measures of disinhibition and decision-making in the prediction of relapse in pathological gamblers. Psychol Med. 2008;38:41–50. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707000694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Lawrence AJ, Luty J, Bogdan NA, Sahakian BJ, Clark L. Impulsivity and response inhibition in alcohol dependence and problem gambling. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009;207:163–72. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1645-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Dannon PN, Shoenfeld N, Rosenberg O, Kertzman S, Kotler M. Pathological gambling: an impulse control disorder? Measurement of impulsivity using neurocognitive tests. Isr Med Assoc J. 2010;12:243–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Congdon E, Mumford JA, Cohen JR, Galvan A, Aron AR, Xue G, et al. Engagement of large-scale networks is related to individual differences in inhibitory control. Neuroimage. 2010;53:653–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Aron AR, Poldrack RA. Cortical and subcortical contributions to stop signal response inhibition: role of the subthalamic nucleus. J Neurosci. 2006;26:2424–33. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4682-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Li CS, Yan P, Sinha R, Lee TW. Subcortical processes of motor response inhibition during a stop signal task. Neuroimage. 2008;41:1352–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Aron AR. From reactive to proactive and selective control: developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biol Psychiatry. 2010 Oct 5; doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.024. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Vink M, Kahn RS, Raemaekers M, van den Heuvel M, Boersma M, Ramsey NF. Function of striatum beyond inhibition and execution of motor responses. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005 Jul;25:336–44. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Ray N, Strafella AP. Dopamine, reward, and frontostriatal circuitry in impulse control disorders in Parkinson’s disease: insights from functional imaging. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2010;41:87–93. doi: 10.1177/155005941004100208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Weintraub D, Koester J, Potenza MN, Siderowf AD, Stacy M, Voon V, et al. Impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease: a cross-sectional study of 3090 patients. Arch Neurol. 2010;67:589–95. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Poletti M, Frosini D, Lucetti C, Del Dotto P, Ceravolo R, Bonuccelli U. Decision making in de novo Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2010;25:1432–6. doi: 10.1002/mds.23098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Ibarretxe-Bilbao N, Junque C, Tolosa E, Marti MJ, Valldeoriola F, Bargallo N, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of impaired decision-making and facial emotion recognition in early Parkinson’s disease. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;30:1162–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06892.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Delazer M, Sinz H, Zamarian L, Stockner H, Seppi K, Wenning GK, et al. Decision making under risk and under ambiguity in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:1901–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Czernecki V, Pillon B, Houeto JL, Pochon JB, Levy R, Dubois B. Motivation, reward, and Parkinson’s disease: influence of dopatherapy. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:2257–67. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00108-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Euteneuer F, Schaefer F, Stuermer R, Boucsein W, Timmermann L, Barbe MT, et al. Dissociation of decision-making under ambiguity and decision-making under risk in patients with Parkinson’s disease: a neuropsychological and psychophysiological study. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2882–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Poletti M, Cavedini P, Bonuccelli U. Iowa gambling task in Parkinson’s disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2010;6:1–15. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2010.524150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Spencer RM, Ivry RB. Comparison of patients with Parkinson’s disease or cerebellar lesions in the production of periodic movements involving event-based or emergent timing. Brain Cogn. 2005;58:84–93. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Pastor MA, Jahanshahi M, Artieda J, Obeso JA. Performance of repetitive wrist movements in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 1992;115:875–91. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.3.875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Malapani C, Rakitin B, Levy R, Meck WH, Deweer B, Dubois B, et al. Coupled temporal memories in Parkinson’s disease: a dopamine-related dysfunction. J Cogn Neurosci. 1998;10:316–31. doi: 10.1162/089892998562762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Koch G, Brusa L, Caltagirone C, Oliveri M, Peppe A, Tiraboschi P, et al. Subthalamic deep brain stimulation improves time perception in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroreport. 2004;29(15):1071–3. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200404290-00028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Rossi M, Gerschcovich ER, de Achaval D, Perez-Loret S, Cerquetti D, Cammarota A, et al. Decision making in Parkinson’s disease patients with and without pathological gambling. Eur J Neurol. 2010;1(17):97–102. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2009.02792.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Housden CR, O’Sullivan SS, Joyce EM, Lees AJ, Roiser JP. Intact reward learning but elevated delay discounting in Parkinson’s disease patients with impulsive–compulsive spectrum behaviours. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(11):2155–64. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Frosini D, Pesaresi I, Cosottini M, Belmonte G, Rossi C, Dell’Osso L, et al. Parkinson’s disease and pathological gambling: results from a functional MRI study. Mov Disord. 2010;25:2449–53. doi: 10.1002/mds.23369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Rao H, Mamikonyan E, Detre JA, Siderowf A, Stern M, Potenza M, et al. Decreased ventral striatal activity with impulse control disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2011;25:1660–9. doi: 10.1002/mds.23147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Steeves TD, Miyasaki J, Zurowski M, Lang AE, Pellecchia G, Van Eimeren T, et al. Increased striatal dopamine release in Parkinsonian patients with pathological gambling: a [11C] raclopride PET study. Brain. 2009;132:1376–85. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Cilia R, Ko JH, Cho SS, van Eimeren T, Marotta G, Pellecchia G, et al. Reduced dopamine transporter density in the ventral striatum of patients with Parkinson’s disease and pathological gambling. Neurobiol Dis. 2010;39:98–104. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2010.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Childress AR, et al. Cocaine cues and dopamine in dorsal striatum: mechanism of craving in cocaine addiction. J Neurosci. 2006;26:6583–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1544-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Swanson JM, Telang F. Dopamine in drug abuse and addiction: results of imaging studies and treatment implications. Arch Neurol. 2007;64:1575–9. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.11.1575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.van Eimeren T, Pellecchia G, Cilia R, Ballanger B, Steeves TD, Houle S, et al. Drug-induced deactivation of inhibitory networks predicts pathological gambling in PD. Neurology. 2010;75:1711–6. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181fc27fa. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Djamshidian A, Jha A, O’Sullivan S, Silveira-Moriyama L, Jacobson C, Brown P, et al. Risk and learning in impulsive and non-impulsive patients with Parkinson’s Disease. Mov Disord. 2010;25:2203–10. doi: 10.1002/mds.23247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.van Eimeren T, Ballanger B, Pellecchia G, Miyasaki JM, Lang AE, Strafella AP. Dopamine agonists diminish value sensitivity of the orbitofrontal cortex: a trigger for pathological gambling in Parkinson’s disease? Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:2758–66. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.npp2009124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Voon V, Pessiglione M, Brezing C, Gallea C, Fernandez HH, Dolan RJ, et al. Mechanisms underlying dopamine-mediated reward bias in compulsive behaviors. Neuron. 2010;65:135–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Pine A, Shiner T, Seymour B, Dolan RJ. Dopamine, time, and impulsivity in humans. J Neurosci. 2010;30:8888–96. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6028-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.St Onge JR, Floresco SB. Dopaminergic modulation of risk-based decision making. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:681–97. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Antonelli F, Ray N, Strafella AP. Imaging cognitive and behavioural symptoms in Parkinson’s disease. Expert Rev Neurothe. 2010;10:1827–38. doi: 10.1586/ern.10.173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Smeding HM, Goudriaan AE, Foncke EM, Schuurman PR, Speelman JD, Schmand B. Pathological gambling after bilateral subthalamic nucleus stimulation in Parkinson disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2007;78:517–9. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.102061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Oyama G, Shimo Y, Natori S, Nakajima M, Ishii H, Arai H, et al. Acute effects of bilateral subthalamic stimulation on decision-making in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2011;17:189–93. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2010.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Czernecki V, Pillon B, Houeto JL, Welter ML, Mesnage V, Agid Y, et al. Does bilateral stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus aggravate apathy in Parkinson’s disease? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2005;76:775–9. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2003.033258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Rodriguez-Oroz MC, Lopez-Azcarte J, Garcia-Garcia D, Alegre M, Toledo J, Valencia M, et al. Involvement of the subthalamic nucleus in impulse control disorders associated with Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2011;134:34–49. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Gauggel S, Rieger M, Feghoff TA. Inhibition of ongoing responses in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2004;75:539–44. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2003.016469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Benabid AL, Chabardes S, Mitrofanis J, Pollak P. Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus for the treatment of Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2009;8:67–81. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70291-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Jahanshahi M, Ardouin CM, Brown RG, Rothwell JC, Obeso J, Albanese A, et al. The impact of deep brain stimulation on executive function in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2000;123:1142–54. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.6.1142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Hershey T, Revilla FJ, Wernle A, Gibson PS, Dowling JL, Perlmutter JS. Stimulation of STN impairs aspects of cognitive control in PD. Neurology. 2004;62:1110–4. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000118202.19098.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Witt K, Pulkowski U, Herzog J, Lorenz D, Hamel W, Deuschl G, et al. Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus improves cognitive flexibility but impairs response inhibition in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol. 2004;6:697–700. doi: 10.1001/archneur.61.5.697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Campbell MC, Karimi M, Weaver PM, Wu J, Perantie DC, Golchin NA, et al. Neural correlates of STN DBS-induced cognitive variability in Parkinson disease. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:3162–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Ballanger B, van Eimeren T, Moro E, Lozano AM, Hamani C, Boulinguez P, et al. Stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus and impulsivity: release your horses. Ann Neurol. 2009;66:817–24. doi: 10.1002/ana.21795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Ray NJ, Jenkinson N, Brittain J, Holland P, Joint C, Nandi D, et al. The role of the subthalamic nucleus in response inhibition: evidence from deep brain stimulation for Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2009 Nov;47:2828–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Wylie SA, Ridderinkhof KR, Elias WJ, Frysinger RC, Bashore TR, Downs KE, et al. Subthalamic nucleus stimulation influences expression and suppression of impulsive behaviour in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2010;133:3611–24. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Hershey T, Campbell MC, Videen TO, Lugar HM, Weaver PM, Hartlein J, et al. Mapping Go–No-Go performance within the subthalamic nucleus region. Brain. 2010;133:3625–34. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES