Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: Prog Transplant. 2012 Sep;22(3):280–293. doi: 10.7182/pit2012890

Prevention of Poor Psychosocial Outcomes in Living Organ Donors: From Description to Theory-Driven Intervention Development and Initial Feasibility Testing

Mary Amanda Dew 1,2,3,4, Allan Zuckoff 2,1, Andrea F DiMartini 1,5, Annette J DeVito Dabbs 7, Mary L McNulty 1, Kristen R Fox 1, Galen E Switzer 6,8, Abhinav Humar 5,9, Henkie Tan 5,9
PMCID: PMC3442950  NIHMSID: NIHMS374597  PMID: 22951506

Abstract

Context

Although some living donors experience psychological, somatic, and interpersonal difficulties postdonation, interventions to prevent such outcomes have not been developed or evaluated.

Objective

To (a) summarize empirical evidence on postdonation psychosocial outcomes, (b) describe a theoretical framework to guide development of an intervention to prevent poor outcomes and (c) describe development and initial evaluation of feasibility and acceptability of the intervention.

Methods

Based on a narrative literature review suggesting that individuals ambivalent about donation are at risk for poor postdonation psychosocial outcomes, the intervention targeted this risk factor. Intervention structure and content drew on motivational interviewing principles in order to assist prospective donors to resolve ambivalence. Data were collected on donor characteristics at our institution to determine whether they constituted a representative population in which to evaluate the intervention. Study participants were then recruited to assess intervention feasibility and acceptability. They were required to have scores > 0 on the Simmons Ambivalence Scale (indicating at least some ambivalence about donation).

Results

Our population was similar to the national living donor population on most demographic and donation-related characteristics. Eight individuals approved to donate either a kidney or liver segment were enrolled for intervention pilot testing. All successfully completed the 2-session telephone-based intervention before scheduled donation surgery. Participant ratings of acceptability and satisfaction were high. Open-ended comments indicated that the intervention addressed participants’ thoughts and concerns about the decision to donate.

Conclusions

The intervention is feasible, acceptable, and appears relevant to donor concerns. A clinical trial to evaluate intervention efficacy is warranted.

Keywords: living organ donors, intervention development, psychosocial outcomes

INTRODUCTION

Living donors constitute 45% of all organ transplant donors in the United States.1 The rate of living donation has almost tripled in the past 20 years; over 113,000 individuals have served as living donors since 1988.1,2 Most of these individuals donated either a kidney (96%) or a liver segment (4%).2

Because living donors undergo major surgery involving the removal of critical organ mass for no direct medical benefit to themselves, the protection of their well-being and the prevention of any negative consequences of donation are among the foremost priorities in transplantation. Surgical innovations and increased experience among surgical teams have together continued to drive down the risks of donor intraoperative and perioperative morbidity.36 However, there remain ongoing concerns that donors are at risk for negative psychosocial sequelae and that these outcomes deserve increased attention.612

To the best of our knowledge, no preventive interventions have been systematically developed and tested for their ability to avert poor psychosocial outcomes, despite evidence that certain factors consistently predict postdonation psychological, somatic, and interpersonal difficulties.13 In particular, residual ambivalence about the prospect of donation—i.e., lingering feelings of hesitation and uncertainty that remain after the prospective donor’s pre-donation medical workup and that coexist with the donor’s intention to donate—appears to be a critical predictor of poor postdonation psychosocial outcomes.1316 We therefore sought to develop an intervention to address residual ambivalence in living donor candidates. We hypothesized that an intervention focused on a key risk factor for poor postdonation psychosocial outcomes would have the capacity to prevent them. Preliminary data on feasibility and acceptability of the intervention, however, are needed before it is reasonable to design a clinical trial to test the intervention’s efficacy at prevention.

In the present paper, therefore, we first summarize the empirical evidence on psychosocial outcomes in living donors that supports the focus of our intervention. We then describe the theoretical framework underlying our intervention, followed by the formative work we undertook to develop the intervention and collect initial data on its feasibility and acceptability.

Psychosocial outcomes in living donors

No matter whether living kidney or liver donors are queried shortly postdonation or many years later, studies consistently show that only a small minority of donors (less than 5%) regret having donated.1721 The vast majority state that they would make the same decision to donate again2228 and report deep gratification at having been able to help another person16,2931 Moreover, as summarized in several recent systematic reviews, numerous studies utilizing generic, non-donation specific assessments of health-related quality of life (HRQOL; e.g., the SF-36) show that donors’ well-being, on average, meets or exceeds that reported in the general population once donors have recovered from the immediate effects of surgery.13,32,33 Nevertheless, a growing body of studies suggest that sizable proportions of living donors experience major difficulties in the years after donation. These difficulties appear to lie predominantly in three key psychosocial domains: psychological well-being, perceived physical functioning, and interpersonal relationships.

With respect to psychological well-being, a number of empirical reports have described clinically significant psychological distress in both kidney donor and liver donor cohorts, typically at a rate of about 1 in every 4 donors.16,3444 Many of these distressed individuals meet diagnostic criteria for mood and anxiety disorders.12,21,4550 These findings are striking because, consistent with guidelines and mandates in the transplant community, 7,10,11,5154 donors routinely undergo careful evaluation before donation designed to screen out individuals with significant psychiatric (as well as medical) morbidity. Indeed, pre-donation assessments of living donors show uniform and high levels of psychological well-being relative to normative or comparison group levels.21,24,28,5558 Therefore, one would expect that donors would otherwise be at very low risk to develop significant psychological distress and diagnosable disorder postdonation.

The literature also contains anecdotal reports of suicidal ideation and attempted and completed suicide in donors.15,5961 In the only large-scale investigation to date, the Adult to Adult Living Liver (A2ALL) study examined data from nine centers and identified one suicide attempt, one accidental death that may have been a suicide, and one suicide, leading to an estimated suicide rate of over 2 per 1000 donors.12 The A2ALL study group noted that their data were limited by their reliance on medical records reviews rather than prospective assessments.12 Therefore, it is likely that the rates of suicide attempts and related psychiatric disorders were underestimated,62 suggesting the development of serious psychopathology potentially attributable to the donation experience may be more common than previously realized.

Self-reported decrements in physical well-being and elevated levels of somatic distress related to the donation also appear to be relatively common among donors. Thus, in studies that have examined donors who are beyond the perioperative recovery period, up to one third of donors report that their health is fair to poor or markedly worse since donation.15,22,43,6368 Among the most prevalent, enduring symptoms is pain (e.g., around the surgical incision).15,27,31,35,63,65,6972 Recent studies have also begun to identify persistent fatigue37,43,56,63,73 and ongoing worries among donors about lasting effects of the donation on their health.29,30,35,72,7478 Finally, negative body image changes (often related to the surgical scarring) have been described by donors.37,69,70,74,7880 Interestingly, although very few studies have performed direct comparisons between kidney and liver donors,26,70 there is no clear trend across the literature suggesting that the degree and nature of donors’ somatic distress beyond the perioperative recovery period differ markedly as a function of type of donation.

A third psychosocial domain that appears to show adverse effects in some donors pertains to interpersonal relationship strain. Although the majority of donors report that their relationship with the transplant recipient is unchanged or improved postdonation,27,39,44,73,78,81,82 relationships with other family members, including the spouse, have been reported to worsen.18,31,67,76,81,8386 Studies often do not specify which types of relationships with (non-recipient) family members have worsened (e.g., spousal vs. other relationships). However, the spousal relationship may be at particular risk for strain, given anecdotal reports of postdonation separations and divorces.15,39,81,85

Surprisingly, among donors who are beyond the perioperative recovery period, time since donation has not been consistently found to be related to the likelihood of problems in the various psychosocial domains we have discussed. In addition, it is worrisome that the specific problems that we have identified are reported in the same literature—and sometimes within the same study—that also reports that generic HRQOL in donors meets or exceeds that of the general population.21,35,42,44,46,56,7173 This suggests that generic measures are insensitive when used in living donors and, at best, should be used only as adjuncts to more sensitive, specific evaluation of potential psychosocial problems in donors33,83,87

Which donors are at risk for these problems? This question has been difficult to answer, especially given the cross-sectional, often retrospective designs utilized in many studies in this literature.88 Such design-related and other methodologic limitations may account for mixed findings to date in this still-small literature. Thus, although many variables have been examined as potential risk factors, for most, there appear to be as many studies failing to find effects on psychosocial outcomes as studies reporting such effects. This pattern of mixed findings applies to: (a) demographic characteristics including age, gender, education and marital status,1416,24,25,29,3639,65,8992 (b) the donor’s pre-donation psychological distress,14,16,24,43,47,60,93 (c) whether the donor was related (either biologically or emotionally) or was unrelated to the recipient (e.g., an acquaintance or an anonymous donor),15,25,31,38,39,65,90,92 (d) whether the donor had poor pre-donation relationships with their family or with the recipient,15,16,43,60,93 (e) the type of surgical procedure for donation (laparoscopic vs. open vs. mini-incision nephrectomy; left vs. right liver lobe resection),21,23,25,56,64,7072,94 (f) whether the donor had perioperative medical complications,25,38,44,46,65,70 and (g) whether the transplant was successful or not (i.e., the recipient lost the graft or died).15,21,23,39,50,55,56,60,65,67,73,79,83,86 Other potential risk factors (e.g., coping strategies, motives for donation), have been examined in only one or two investigations and thus strong conclusions about impact cannot yet be reached.1416,44,82

In contrast to the mixed or scant evidence for most putative risk factors, there is a growing evidence base indicating that residual ambivalence about donating heightens donors’ risk for poor postdonation psychosocial outcomes. First characterized in Simmons’ work,16 this type of ambivalence refers to lingering feelings of uncertainty and hesitation about donation (e.g., feeling unsure about whether one would want to donate if someone else could do it, wishing a deceased organ would become available, being fearful about going through with donation) that remain after the donor has completed the medical workup and that coexist with the donor’s intention to donate. These residual feelings must be distinguished from what, in contrast, can be termed acute ambivalence, i.e., feelings of indecision so marked that it is determined in the psychosocial evaluation that the prospective donor is unable to give adequate informed consent to proceed.7,10,11,95 Residual ambivalence, instead, pertains to feelings that remain shortly before donation, despite the fact that the donor intends to proceed with donation. While acute ambivalence, resulting in a rule-out of the individual as a donor, is uncommon (<2% of rule-outs),9699 some degree of residual ambivalence before donation has been noted in up to 75% of donors.16,22,29,35,36,77,101103

Studies linking residual ambivalence with poor postdonation outcomes include Simmons and colleagues’ work with kidney donors16,93,104 and later with bone marrow donors105,106 showing that the greater the level of donors’ residual ambivalence before donation, the more likely donors were to experience both psychological distress and physical complaints both in the short-term postdonation (e.g., several months to 1 year) and in the longer term (5–9 years). Similar findings have been reported in other studies of both kidney donors15,107 and liver donors.14,36 Although effects of residual ambivalence on familial relationship strain have received little attention, one report suggests that ambivalence was also associated with this domain of outcomes.107

In sum, the evidence we have reviewed on psychosocial outcomes postdonation suggests that efforts to avert poor outcomes are warranted. Although the literature on psychosocial outcomes and their risk factors has methodologic limitations, and evidence base for risk factors remains relatively small, knowledge to date regarding the most potent risk factors for these outcomes points to the potential importance of residual ambivalence. We thus hypothesized that an intervention focused on reducing residual ambivalence might be a useful prevention strategy in this area.

Theoretical framework for a preventive intervention with living donors

The conceptual model underlying a given intervention may be based on a single theory or it may draw on concepts from multiple scientific perspectives.108 We drew on concepts from the field of prevention science109 to delineate the broad type of preventive strategy to be used. Then, given our focus on ambivalence, we drew on ambivalence-related components from the theoretical framework of motivational interviewing (MI)110,111 to specify the structure and content of our intervention.

What type of prevention strategy?

Preventive interventions occur before the onset of a disorder or health outcome.112,113 There are three classes of preventive intervention models.109,114 Selective interventions target individuals with risk factors for the outcomes of interest. In contrast, indicated interventions target individuals who are prodromal for the outcomes (e.g., they already have signs/symptoms of the outcomes) and universal preventions are offered to an entire population regardless of risk or prodromal status.

We judged that a selective approach, targeted at prospective living kidney or liver donors with residual ambivalence, would be most appropriate. An indicated prevention intervention would not be suitable because individuals approved as prospective donors have already undergone extensive screening in order to minimize the likelihood that they have signs and symptoms of poor psychological or physical health, and prospective donors themselves perceive their well-being to be high, as reviewed earlier. On the other hand, a universal approach would be inefficient because it would require that even individuals with no prodromal signs or risk factors (e.g., no residual ambivalence) receive the intervention.

Theoretical framework for intervention structure and content: MI

We derived our selective intervention from the principles of MI.110,111 Although using MI chiefly in order to resolve ambivalence is novel, this application fits squarely within MI’s theoretical framework. To explain this, we first describe the elements of MI that are best-known in the field of behavioral intervention, and then describe how we draw on features of MI relevant to our focus on residual ambivalence about living donation.

A goal emphasized in most previous clinical and research applications of MI is the enhancement of individuals’ motivation for behavior change.115117 The change may involve refraining from activities (e.g., abstinence from substance use or other harmful behaviors)115,118,119 or initiating new behavior (e.g., health promotion activities).120122 MI has been found efficacious across these many applications.115117 However, equally important in both the theory110,111 and practice123 of MI is its focus on exploring and resolving ambivalence—no matter what the individual’s ultimate choice is regarding any behavior they might undertake (or decision they might make). Indeed, the conceptual framework underlying MI—integrating Rogers’ client-centered counseling theory124 augmented by Janis’ decision counseling approach125—does not presume that a certain direction of client action is “preferred” or should be promoted over any other course. This sets MI apart from other “behavior change” approaches,126,127 where movement toward change or client action in a specific direction is the key indicator of success (e.g., the transtheoretical model128). Rather, the theoretical underpinnings of MI assert that the interventionist’s foremost goal is to facilitate the client’s ability to freely choose a course to take (i.e., select a behavior, or make a decision, including the decision to take no action) consistent with the client’s own values and goals.110

We focused on this neglected application of MI. Specifically, we drew on the concepts on which MI itself has drawn in exploring and resolving ambivalence, namely the notion that people become increasingly committed to what they hear themselves defend.110,111,115,129 This is especially so when they possess inconsistent or discrepant cognitions—e.g., intentions that conflict with their beliefs about what they should do or what they would prefer to do. The presence of these discrepancies creates discomfort, including ambivalence.129 In the section on intervention development below, we explain how this premise is translated into action as MI is carried out.

In sum, our goal of developing an MI-based intervention with prospective donors was neither to encourage nor discourage donation, but to enable them to resolve residual ambivalence in order to reach their own final choice as to whether to proceed with donation or not. Among individuals who do become donors, the resolution of ambivalence will, we hypothesize, prevent poor postdonation psychosocial outcomes.

METHODS

Assembling the intervention development team

We assembled an interdisciplinary team to ensure that the intervention met clinical standards for MI and was also relevant to prospective donor (PD) concerns, based on existing literature and clinical practice. The team included members representing clinical transplantation (individuals from nursing, consultation-liaison psychiatry, surgery), behavioral medicine and medical sociology (experts in design, development and evaluation of psychosocial interventions in transplant-related populations), and motivational interviewing. The team’s primary concerns were not only to develop the intervention, but to understand PDs’ impressions of it so that we could modify its content and/or procedures as needed.

Assessing individuals potentially eligible for the intervention

We gathered information about background characteristics for living donors at our center in order to determine whether they were similar to the donor population nationally (and thus would constitute a representative subpopulation in which to examine intervention feasibility and acceptability). We also reviewed results of recent surveys examining ambivalence conducted at our center to determine whether there were likely to be sufficient proportions of living kidney and liver donors at our center who experienced residual ambivalence in order to justify offering an intervention to these individuals.

Recruiting representative patients

With institutional review board approval, we recruited and obtained informed consent for participation in feasibility testing of the intervention from individuals who had completed their medical evaluation (including the psychosocial component) and been approved to serve as living donors in the University of Pittsburgh Living Donor Kidney and Liver Programs. We aimed to enroll 5 to 8 individuals. This sample size was based on our own and others’ intervention development work, which has suggested that studying a small number of participants (<10) will be sufficient to expose major problems with intervention structure, format and content design that should be corrected before proceeding to a larger-scale effectiveness study.130,131 We used purposive sampling in order to help ensure recruitment of PDs who varied in demographic characteristics, their relationship to the transplant patient (e.g., related vs. unrelated) and whether they were considering kidney or liver donation.

The key inclusion criterion for enrollment was that PDs have a score exceeding zero on the Simmons Ambivalence Scale,16 indicating the presence of at least some degree of residual ambivalence. The measure is comprised of 7 items focused on lingering hesitation and uncertainty about donation. Each item is rated on a 4-point scale indicating how strongly the respondent feels about the item (e.g., “I would really want to donate, even if someone else could do it, 0 = agree a lot; 3 = disagree a lot). The 7 items are summed and a score exceeding zero has been used to identify respondents with at least some ambivalence.16,101,105,106 Simmons16 demonstrated both the construct validity of the measure (based on assessments of convergent and discriminant validity), and she and others have found it to have predictive validity in prospective studies.16,94,106 Its internal consistency reliability has been found to range from .78 to .83 in kidney and liver donors.16,101 A recent editorial supports the choice of this scale for the empirical measurement of ambivalence in living donors.132 This screening measure was assessed via a brief telephone call at the time that informed consent was obtained. Study participants were also required to be aged 18 or older, English-speaking, and have access to a telephone.

Developing the intervention

Defining intervention structure, format and timing relative to donation

We structured intervention sessions to be consistent with fundamentals of MI: MI is, by design, (a) nonconfrontational and collaborative, and (b) brief.110,115 With regard to the former, we judged it important to adhere to the principle that the MI interventionist does not present an array of choices that a client might make but instead works to promote clients’ own reflections on the choices they face, in the context of their values and goals.110,115,123 The interventionist guides the client to explore his/her own arguments for and against a certain course of action. Thus, the interventionist seeks to evoke clients’ expressions of their desire, ability, reasons and need to make a behavioral or decisional choice in one direction vs. another.110,115 Clients hear themselves explain their own motivations, and hear those explanations reflected again by the interventionist. This approach is thus based on an assumption of client autonomy.110,115 As such, MI is congruent with the ethical principles and standards for living donor organ transplantation, namely that PDs reach an autonomous decision to proceed (or not) with donation without experiencing coercion or undue pressure in favor of one choice vs. another, and with appropriate reflection on their possible courses of action.7,51,53

We judged that our MI-based intervention would most appropriately be conducted after PDs’ medical evaluation was completed. This is consistent with recommendations that they be given the opportunity to “cool off” before the donation surgery occurred, i.e., a chance to reflect on and reach a final decision about whether or not to donate.7,51,133 Such a cooling off period is especially important for PDs who have some residual ambivalence regarding whether or not to donate (i.e., the PDs targeted for our intervention).

With regard to intervention duration, MI-based interventions are usually brief (often 1–2 sessions, totaling only 1–2 hours, and offered within a short timespan of days or a week), and they can be conducted by telephone.115 The latter feature is an important asset since many PDs do not live near their Living Donor Transplant Program. MI’s brevity is also a strength because more lengthy interventions (even those lasting just a few weeks) would not be feasible within the context of the donor evaluation process. Approval for donation and donation surgery may take place relatively quickly once donors have completed their medical evaluation; a brief intervention is ideal for this short timeline.

In sum, we designed a 2-session telephone-based intervention employing MI principles of nonconfrontational but guided discussion of PDs’ remaining concerns about donation. We aimed for each session to last approximately 45 minutes, be conducted 5–7 days apart, and scheduled at PDs’ convenience. For purposes of our developmental work, the interventionist was a team member with extensive MI experience as an interventionist and trainer.

Developing the content of intervention sessions

The content was designed to achieve our key goals: to assist PDs to resolve residual ambivalence about donation and to build their motivation for making their final decision about whether or not to proceed with donation. The content was developed through a series of meetings of team members in which each possible component of the sessions was discussed, with role playing of segments of the sessions in order to further develop interventionist guidelines. In general, the sessions incorporated standard MI techniques such as the use of open questions, reflective listening, affirmations of the PD’s strengths, and the provision of frequent summaries of the discussion in order to ensure that the PD feels “heard” and respected.110,123 Each session had specific components to be covered, and we prepared a manual outlining each component and the types of queries that the interventionist would use in semi-structured fashion to guide the discussion.

Session 1 begins with introductory comments designed to establish a collaborative relationship that is nonjudgmental and confidential, and to recognize the PD’s personal choice and control with regard to the donation decision. PDs are assured that the content of the phone calls would not be revealed to transplant candidates or the transplant team. (As specified in the informed consent for study participation, however, study subjects were aware that if they were found to be at risk for harm to themselves or others, e.g., they reported suicidal ideation, the research team would take immediate steps to ensure their safety.) Discussion in Session 1 then moves to understanding the PD’s “story,” i.e., how the PD came to consider living donation and their experience with the evaluation for it. Next, the nature of ambivalent feelings is explored, with an emphasis on clarifying whether the PD is leaning in one direction or another in terms of proceeding with the surgery and identifying factors that remain as concerns. This discussion incorporates a review of the PD’s responses to the Ambivalence Scale administered at the time of study enrollment. The session continues with consideration of whether there are additional actions that the PD might take in order to resolve residual ambivalence (e.g., obtaining more information from the Living Donor Program or other sources in order to address concerns; talking further with family members). Specifics of a plan that could be undertaken before the second session are discussed, and the PD’s commitment to carrying out the plan is elicited. The session closes with a summary of the key points covered during the session, including the plan for actions the PD will take before the second session. The second session is also scheduled.

Session 2 begins with reminders to the PD about the purpose of the sessions and their confidentiality, followed by a discussion of any steps the PD took since the last call in order to address factors linked to residual ambivalence, and whether those steps affected the PD’s views about donation. This discussion of steps taken by the PD provides an important rationale for the second session: to ensure that the PD would have an opportunity to consider results of enacting the plan and think about any other steps that might be important to take. Equally important for Session 2, the interventionist then introduces a standard MI task, the Values Card Sort,110 which is designed to assist the PD to recognize the values most important to him/her (e.g., family, compassion, achievement, belonging), how the PD is living out those values, and how proceeding with (or deciding not to proceed with) living donation is linked to those values. The session closes with a summary of the discussion across both sessions, affirmation of the PD’s efforts in considering donation options, and consideration of whether the PD might engage in any other steps or plans to address any other concerns regarding decisions about living donation.

Assessing intervention feasibility and acceptability

We examined whether PDs were willing to enroll and complete the sessions, whether both sessions were successfully conducted in the short timeline before any donor surgery, and how many days elapsed between sessions. We obtained PD ratings of satisfaction and acceptability of the sessions using the Client Satisfaction Questionnaire,134 a survey with 8 closed-ended ratings, as well as open-ended items regarding PDs’ impressions. The scale was administered during a telephone assessment conducted within 1–3 days after the second intervention session. The assessment was conducted by a team member who received no other information about the specific discussion that occurred during the intervention sessions.

RESULTS

Individuals potentially eligible for the intervention

As shown in Table 1, living kidney and liver donors at our center were similar to donors nationally on distributions of gender, age, and relationship to the transplant patient. They differed from national distributions on ethnicity. The ethnicity distribution at our center more closely mirrors the population in the region served by our center.135 The data in Table 1 suggest that donors from our center would generally constitute a representative cohort from which to sample in the initial phases of examining feasibility and acceptability of the intervention, but that we should pay attention to ethnicity and consider oversampling on this factor in an eventual study of intervention effectiveness.

Table 1.

Living organ donor characteristics at the University of Pittsburgh in comparison to the national donor population, 2006 – 2010.

Characteristic University of Pittsburgh U.S. Living Donor Populationa One-sample χ2 test (comparing center to national population)

Kidney donors, n 397 31,116
 Gender, % female 62.5 58.2 0.85
 Age, % < 50 72.8 74.6 0.65
 Ethnicity
  European American 90.9 69.7 84.3***
  African American 5.8 12.0
  Hispanic American 1.0 13.7
  Asian American or Other 2.3 4.6
 Relationship to recipient
  Biological relative, first degree 54.6 50.2 7.22
  Biological relative, other 9.6 7.6
  Spouse/partner 12.1 13.1
  Unrelated 23.7 28.8

Liver donors, n 138 1,304
 Gender, % female 56.5 51.6 1.42
 Age, % < 50 87.7 84.8 1.35
 Ethnicity
  European American 91.3 78.8 17.56***
  African American 2.9 5.1
  Hispanic American 2.2 11.7
  Asian American or Other 3.6 4.4
 Relationship to recipient
  Biological relative, first degree 65.2 58.7 2.62b
  Biological relative, other 10.9 11.9
  Spouse/partner 1.4 5.7
  Unrelated 22.5 23.7
a

from OPTN/UNOS1

b

comparing biological first degree relatives, other biological relatives, and all unrelated donors (spouse + other) due to low frequencies in some groups.

***

p < .001, comparing European American to all other groups due to low frequencies in some groups.

In order to determine whether there were likely to be sufficient proportions of living kidney and liver donors at our center who experienced residual ambivalence in order to justify an intervention, we reviewed two surveys of center patients. In a recent study of 77 prospective liver donors (representing 73% of eligible PDs during the enrollment period), we found that 74% endorsed at least some degree of ambivalence on the Simmons Ambivalence Scale.101 We also previously surveyed 85 kidney donors (82% of eligible donors) who had completed kidney donation surgery an average of 2.2 years (SD=1.0) earlier.136 They were asked to retrospectively rate their level of ambivalence before donation, using the Ambivalence Scale, and 80% endorsed at least some ambivalence (L. Myaskovsky, personal communication).

Characteristics of study participants enrolled to test intervention feasibility and acceptability

We initially approached 14 PDs. Of these, 4 showed no residual ambivalence about donation (Ambivalence Scale scores of zero). Of the remaining 10 individuals, one refused to enroll and 9 gave consent. However, one PD’s intended recipient received a deceased donor transplant 1 day after enrollment and thus the PD could not participate in the study. The final sample therefore included 8 PDs. As shown in Table 2, these PDs varied in terms of demographic and donation-related characteristics.

Table 2.

Characteristics of prospective donors enrolled in feasibility and acceptability evaluation of intervention.

Characteristic No.

Gender
 Female 5
 Male 3
Age, years
 20–29 3
 30–39 1
 40–49 2
 50–59 2
Ethnicity
 European American 7
 African American 1
Education
 High school 3
 College 3
 Post-graduate 2
Employment
 Full-time 7
 Part-time 0
 Unemployed 1
Marital status
 Married 5
 Unmarried 3
Type of prospective donor
 Kidney, adult to adult (laparoscopic) 4
 Liver, adult to adult (right lobe) 3
 Liver, adult to child (left segment) 1
Relationship to recipient
 First degree relative 4
 Biological relative, other family 3
 Non-spouse, unrelated 1

Feasibility and acceptability of the intervention

All PDs completed both intervention sessions before any donor surgery. (Six donors went on to donate; in the other two cases deceased donor transplants were performed before the PDs’ scheduled surgery.) Sessions averaged 47 minutes (SD = 15). An average of six days (SD = 4) elapsed between the first and second session; all PDs except one completed both sessions with one week (one PD had two weeks between sessions).

PD ratings of the acceptability of the sessions and their satisfaction with them, based on the CSQ, are shown in Table 3. Ratings fell in a very narrow range, with most ratings at the highest or next to highest point on the 4 point scale for each item. PDs thus viewed the sessions as good to excellent in terms of quality and relevance for prospective living donors, they felt satisfied with their participation, and they would recommend the sessions to other living donors. PDs’ comments concerning intervention features that they liked most and least are shown in Table 4, categorized according to whether the comments pertained to session content or to procedural or structural issues. In general, comments indicated that the content of the intervention was appropriate for PDs and sensitive to their concerns. In terms of structure, PDs liked the brief, telephone-based format. Overall, PD comments indicated that they appreciated the opportunity to discuss remaining concerns that they had about donation. Even so, one PD noted that it was difficult to schedule and complete both sessions given other preparations for possible donation. (This was not the PD who required two weeks to complete both sessions, however.) In addition, although confidentiality of the discussion was emphasized during each session, one PD suggested that it might need to be emphasized even more.

Table 3.

Ratings of acceptability and satisfaction with the intervention.

Client Satisfaction Questionnaire items M (SD)

Program quality (1=poor, 4=excellent) 3.5 (0.5)
Kind of program others would want (1=no, definitely not, 4=yes, definitely) 3.6 (0.5)
Program met needs (1=none of needs were met; 4 = almost all needs were met) 3.2 (0.8)
Recommend program to others (1=no, definitely not, 4=yes, definitely) 3.6 (0.5)
Satisfied with help received from program (1=quite dissatisfied, 4=very satisfied) 3.3 (0.8)
Program helped me deal with my concerns (1=no, seemed to make things worse; 4=yes, helped a great deal) 3.3 (0.5)
Overall satisfaction with program (1=quite dissatisfied, 4=very satisfied) 3.5 (0.8)
If had it to do again, would participate in program (1=no, definitely not, 4 = yes, definitely) 3.8 (0.5)

Table 4.

Study participants’ responses to open-ended questions about their opinions of the intervention.

Features that participant: Intervention Content Intervention Structure and Procedures
Liked most
  • Was thought-provoking

  • Questions that I was asked were very well thought- out

  • Helped me think about what was important to me

  • Helped me look into my own persona; helped me to explain myself

  • Nice that it led me off my usual path of thinking to consider other things

  • Surprised at how much I was able to say—the level of honesty I spoke with; I viewed this as a conversation with myself

  • Gave me a chance to verbalize my thoughts and confirmed my decision

  • A supportive and confidential forum to share and vent my feelings about donation

  • Useful and helpful

  • Very convenient to complete by phone

  • Convenient scheduling to accommodate my available time

  • Did not need to go anywhere; could be at home and complete it

  • Helpful that it required just 2 calls

  • Talking on the phone makes it easier to express true feelings than if face-to-face

  • Interventionist was very conscious of how people feel—seemed to be focused on understanding the donor’s feelings and was not too pushy

Liked least
  • Questions sometimes seemed redundant; like we had covered that ground already

  • Hard to take the time to do it given everything else going on before donation

  • Hard to sit for so long on the phone without having interruptions from others at home

Recommended to be changed
  • Emphasize a little more often that the conversation is confidential

  • Wish there had been more focus on things I could do to physically prepare for donation like exercise

  • Make sure participants know they should be in a quiet place so they can focus on the conversation without distractions

DISCUSSION

Our initial work to develop an intervention to address PDs’ residual ambivalence about the prospect of donation arose from a growing literature on postdonation psychosocial outcomes and their predictors. We designed the intervention within a selective prevention model, i.e., focused on PDs who had completed the medical evaluation and been approved for donation surgery yet who expressed residual ambivalence or hesitation about donating. We believed that the theoretical framework of MI would be particularly beneficial for guiding interventionist efforts to assist PDs to think about and weigh their concerns about donation in order to become fully “at peace” with their decision about donation (no matter whether they continued to proceed with the plan to donate or decided not to donate).

Our initial examination of the acceptability and feasibility of our newly developed intervention suggests that PDs are willing and able to participate in both sessions of a brief MI-based intervention. Although our group of study participants was, by design, small, we selected them to represent the diversity of living donors—in type of relationship to the transplant patient, whether they were donating a kidney or liver segment, and in demographic characteristics. Across these individuals, we found consensus that the intervention content was relevant to the concerns they faced and that the structure of the intervention (e.g., telephone-based, 2 sessions within a brief time period before any surgery) was viewed favorably. Ratings of acceptability and satisfaction were also high. With regard to intervention structure in particular, open-ended comments suggested that the telephone-based approach was a strength due to factors related to convenience as well as to preserving confidentiality. This is consistent with an extensive body of work showing that despite the loss of some nonverbal information, telephone-based intervention and data assessment strategies are no less equally effective and yield data of comparable quality to face-to-face strategies.137,138 Indeed, telephone-based approaches can be preferable for discussion of sensitive topics.139

PDs offered valuable comments for revisions to the intervention. For example, the comment that some questions posed by the interventionist seemed to be redundant or covering ground that had already been addressed suggested that our manual should be revised to further tighten up the outline for the components to be addressed in each session, and the possible queries to be used by the interventionist to direct the discussion. A comment by one PD concerning difficulties with interruptions during the telephone-based sessions and another comment about the need to be able to focus during the sessions suggested that we needed to bring up these issues when initially scheduling sessions to help ensure that the timing would be optimal for PDs. A PD comment also indicated that assurances of confidentiality should be even further emphasized.

A possible limitation of the present study is PDs were aware that we were developing and pilot testing a new intervention, and that we would be asking for their opinions of it. They may have felt that they could give only predominantly positive comments. We attempted to minimize this potential bias by having a team member separate from the interventionist call to obtain PDs’ evaluation of the sessions. We also believe that our decision to focus on the PDs’ ratings of acceptability and satisfaction, as well as their comments, helps to reduce the risk of developing an intervention based solely on what we considered important or useful to PDs rather than what PDs themselves thought would be helpful.

Following revisions of the interventionist manual, intervention content, and general intervention procedures, the most critical steps for the future are to formally test the efficacy of the brief intervention for the prevention of poor psychosocial outcomes in the domains we initially identified: psychological distress, somatic complaints, and interpersonal relationship strains postdonation. It will be equally important to consider whether any effects of the intervention on these postdonation outcomes are mediated, or accounted for, by the ability of the intervention to reduce levels of residual ambivalence before donation. It may also be the case that any intervention effects on residual ambivalence or psychosocial outcomes may be more pronounced in some donors than others (i.e., other donor predonation characteristics may moderate any intervention impact). However, to date, the literature provides little evidence for generating hypotheses in this regard. A critical issue for testing the intervention will be the choice of a comparison condition in order to control for “attention”—i.e., the fact that donors had a skilled individual talking with them over the course of two phone calls totaling about 90–95 minutes. Is it the MI-based intervention that might be beneficial or the sheer attention that the donor would receive regarding issues relevant for their health and well-being? Finally, there will be other important issues to consider as well in planning how an intervention—if effective—might be disseminated into routine clinical care. Those issues will pertain, for example, to costs and resources needed for offering the intervention, and to how to address potential tensions between the need for PDs to have a confidential forum for discussing their concerns vs. any need of Living Donor Programs to learn of new, previously unidentified concerns or issues voiced by PDs during their intervention sessions. The results of efficacy testing and consideration of issues of dissemination may yield important insights into practical, scalable strategies to enhance the safety and well-being of individuals who have generously come forward to help another individual in need.

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this article was supported in part by Grant R21 NR011149 from the National Institute of Nursing Research, Bethesda, MD.

Footnotes

Financial Disclosures: None

References

  • 1.U.S. Organ Procurement and Transplantation Network (OPTN) and the Scientific Registry of Transplant Recipients (SRTR) Data Tables. Department of Health and Human Services, Health Resources and Services Administration, Healthcare Systems Bureau, Division of Transplantation; Rockville, MD: United Network for Organ Sharing (UNOS); Richmond, VA: University Renal Research and Education Association; Ann Arbor, MI: 2011. [Accessed November 3, 2011]. http://www.optn.transplant.hrsa.gov. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.U.S. OPTN and the SRTR. 2009 Annual Report, Transplant Data 1999–2008. Department of Health and Human Services, Health Resources and Services Administration, Healthcare Systems Bureau, Division of Transplantation; Rockville, MD: UNOS; Richmond, VA: University Renal Research and Education Association; Ann Arbor, MI: 2009. [Accessed November 3, 2011]. http:www.ustransplant.org/annual_report/current/default.htm. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Davis CL. Living kidney donors: current state of affairs. Adv Chronic Kid Dis. 2009;16(4):242–249. doi: 10.1053/j.ackd.2009.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Jeon H, Lee SG. Living donor liver transplantation. Curr Opin Organ Transplant. 2010;15:283–287. doi: 10.1097/MOT.0b013e32833983ee. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lee SG. Living-donor liver transplantation in adults. Br Med Bull. 2010;94:33–48. doi: 10.1093/bmb/ldq003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Sener A, Cooper M. Live donor nephrectomy for kidney transplantation. Nat Clin Pract Urol. 2008;5(4):203–210. doi: 10.1038/ncpuro1047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Dew MA, Jacobs C, Jowsey SG, Hanto R, Miller C, Delmonico FL UNOS; American Society of Transplant Surgeons; American Society of Transplantation. Guidelines for the psychosocial evaluation of living unrelated kidney donors in the United States. Am J Transplant. 2007;7(5):1047–1054. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-6143.2007.01751.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Institute of Medicine (U.S.) Committee on Increasing Rates of Organ Donation, Board on Health Sciences Policy. In: Childress JF, Liverman CT, editors. Organ donation: Opportunities for action. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Jowsey SG, Schneekloth TD. Psychosocial factors in living organ donation: clinical and ethical challenges. Transplant Rev. 2008;22(3):192–195. doi: 10.1016/j.trre.2008.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Olbrisch ME, Benedict SM, Haller DL, Levenson JL. Psychosocial assessment of living organ donors: clinical and ethical considerations. Prog Transplant. 2001;11(1):40–49. doi: 10.1177/152692480101100107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Schroder NM, McDonald LA, Etringer G, et al. Consideration of psychosocial factors in the evaluation of living donors. Prog Transplant. 2008;18(1):41–48. doi: 10.1177/152692480801800109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Trotter JF, Hill-Callahan MM, Gillespie BW, et al. Severe psychiatric problems in right hepatic lobe donors for living donor liver transplantation. Transplantation. 2007;83(11):1506–1508. doi: 10.1097/01.tp.0000263343.21714.3b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Dew MA, Switzer GE, DiMartini AF, Myaskovsky L, Crowley-Matoka M. Psychosocial aspects of living organ donation. In: Tan HP, Marcos A, Shapiro R, editors. Living Donor Organ Transplantation. New York, NY: Taylor and Francis; 2007. pp. 7–26. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.DuBay DA, Holtzman S, Adcock L, et al. Adult right-lobe living liver donors: quality of life, attitudes and predictors of donor outcomes. Am J Transplant. 2009;9(5):1169–1178. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-6143.2009.02614.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Schover LR, Streem SB, Boparai N, Duriak K, Novick AC. The psychosocial impact of donating a kidney: long-term followup from a urology based center. J Urol. 1997;157(5):1596–1601. doi: 10.1016/s0022-5347(01)64803-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Simmons RG, Klein SD, Simmons RL. Gift of Life: The Social and Psychological Impact of Organ Transplantation. Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Books; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Fehrman-Ekholm I, Brink B, Ericsson C, Elinder CG, Dunér F, Lundgren G. Kidney donors don’t regret: follow-up of 370 donors in Stockholm since 1964. Transplantation. 2000;69(10):2067–2071. doi: 10.1097/00007890-200005270-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Karliova M, Malagó M, Valentin-Gamazo V, et al. Living-related liver transplantation from the view of the donor: a 1- year follow-up survey. Transplantation. 2002;73(11):1799–1804. doi: 10.1097/00007890-200206150-00017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Reichman TW, Fox A, Adcock L, et al. Anonymous living liver donation: donor profiles and outcomes. Am J Transplant. 2010;10(9):2099–2104. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-6143.2010.03244.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Sharp J, McRae A, McNeill Y. Decision making and psychosocial outcomes among living kidney donors: a pilot study. Prog Transplant. 2010;20(1):53–57. doi: 10.1177/152692481002000109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Smith GC, Trauer T, Kerr PG, Chadban SJ. Prospective psychosocial monitoring of living kidney donors using the Short Form-36 Health Survey: results at 12 months. Transplantation. 2004;78(9):1384–1389. doi: 10.1097/01.tp.0000140967.34029.f1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Kusakabe T, Irie S, Ito N, Kazuma K. Feelings of living donors about adult-to-adult living donor liver transplantation. Gastroenterol Nurs. 2008;31(4):263–272. doi: 10.1097/01.SGA.0000334032.48629.c0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Clemens K, Boudville N, Dew MA, Geddes C, Gill JS, Jassal V, et al. Donor Nephrectomy Outcomes Research (DONOR) Network. The long-term quality of life of living kidney donors: a multicenter cohort study. Am J Transplant. 2011;11(3):463–469. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-6143.2010.03424.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Massey EK, Kranenburg LW, Zuidema WC, et al. Encouraging psychological outcomes after altruistic donation to a stranger. Am J Transplant. 2010;10(6):1445–1452. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-6143.2010.03115.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Mjøen G, Stavem K, Westlie L, et al. Quality of life in kidney donors. Am J Transplant. 2011;11(6):1315–1319. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-6143.2011.03517.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Rudow DL, Charlton M, Sanchez C, Chang S, Serur D, Brown RS., Jr Kidney and liver living donors: a comparision of experiences. Prog Transplant. 2005;15(2):185–191. doi: 10.1177/152692480501500213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Verbesey JE, Simpson MA, Pomposelli JJ, et al. Living donor adult liver transplantation: a longitudinal study of the donor’s quality of life. Am J Transplant. 2005;5(11):2770–2777. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-6143.2005.01092.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Walton-Moss B, Boulware LE, Cooper M, Taylor L, Dane K, Nolan MT. Prospective pilot study of living kidney donor decision-making and outcomes. Clin Transplant. 2007;21(1):86–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-0012.2006.00588.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Corley MC, Keswick RK, Sergeant CC, Scott S. Attitude, self-image, and quality of life of living kidney donors. Nephrol Nurs J. 2000;27(1):43–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lunsford SL, Shilling LM, Chavin KD, et al. Racial differences in the living kidney donation experience and implications for education. Prog Transplant. 2007;17(3):234–240. doi: 10.1177/152692480701700312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Rodrigue JR, Schutzer ME, Paek M, Morrissey P. Altruistic kidney donation to a stranger: psychosocial and functional outcomes at two US transplant centers. Transplantation. 2011;91(7):772–778. doi: 10.1097/TP.0b013e31820dd2bd. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Clemens KK, Thiessen-Philbrook H, Parikh CR, et al. Psychosocial health of living kidney donors: a systematic review. Am J Transplant. 2006;6(12):2965–2977. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-6143.2006.01567.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Parikh ND, Ladner D, Abecassis M, Butt Z. Quality of life for donors after living donor liver transplantation: a review of the literature. Liver Transpl. 2010;16(12):1352–1358. doi: 10.1002/lt.22181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Beavers KL, Sandler RS, Fair JH, Johnson MW, Shrestha R. The living donor experience: donor health assessment and outcomes after living donor liver transplantation. Liver Transpl. 2001;7(11):943–947. doi: 10.1053/jlts.2001.28443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Frade IC, Fonseca I, Dias L, et al. Impact assessment in living kidney donation: psychosocial aspects in the donor. Transplant Proc. 2008;40(3):677–681. doi: 10.1016/j.transproceed.2008.02.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hayashi A, Noma S, Uehara M, et al. Relevant factors to psychological status of donors before living-related liver transplantation. Transplantation. 2007;84(10):1255–1261. doi: 10.1097/01.tp.0000287455.70815.9e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Hsu HT, Hwang SL, Lee PH, Chen SC. Impact of liver donation on quality of life and physical and psychological distress. Transplant Proc. 2006;38(7):2102–2105. doi: 10.1016/j.transproceed.2006.07.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Jacobs C, Johnson E, Anderson K, Gillingham K, Matas A. Kidney transplants from living donors: how donation affects family dynamics. Adv Ren Replace Ther. 1998;5(2):89–97. doi: 10.1016/s1073-4449(98)70002-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Jordan J, Sann U, Janton A, et al. Living kidney donors’ long-term psychological status and health behavior after nephrectomy–a retrospective study. J Nephrol. 2004;17(5):728–735. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Lopes A, Frade IC, Teixeira L, et al. Depression and anxiety in living kidney donation: evaluation of donors and recipients. Transplant Proc. 2011;43(1):131–136. doi: 10.1016/j.transproceed.2010.12.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Taghavi R, Mahdavi R, Toufani H. The psychological effects of kidney donation on living kidney donors (related and unrelated) Transplant Proc. 2001;33(5):2636–2637. doi: 10.1016/s0041-1345(01)02123-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Virzi A, Signorelli MS, Veroux M, et al. Depression and quality of life in living related renal transplantation. Transplant Proc. 2007;39(6):1791–1793. doi: 10.1016/j.transproceed.2007.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Walter M, Bronner E, Pascher A, et al. Psychosocial outcome of living donors after living donor liver transplantation: a pilot study. Clin Transplant. 2002;16(5):339–344. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-0012.2002.02002.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Wiedebusch S, Reiermann S, Steinke C, et al. Quality of life, coping, and mental health status after living kidney donation. Transplant Proc. 2009;41(5):1483–1488. doi: 10.1016/j.transproceed.2009.02.102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Azar SA, Nakhjavani MR, Tarzamni MK, Faragi A, Bahloli A, Badroghli N. Is living kidney donation really safe? Transplant Proc. 2007;39(4):822–823. doi: 10.1016/j.transproceed.2007.04.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Erim Y, Beckmann M, Valentin-Gamazo C, et al. Quality of life and psychiatric complications after adult living donor liver transplantation. Liver Transpl. 2006;12(12):1782–1790. doi: 10.1002/lt.20907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Fukunishi I, Sugawara Y, Takayama T, Makuuchi M, Kawarasaki H, Surman O. Association between pretransplant psychological assessments and posttransplant psychiatric disorders in living-related transplantation. Psychosomatics. 2002;43(1):49–54. doi: 10.1176/appi.psy.43.1.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Heck G, Schweitzer J, Seidel-Wiesel M. Psychological effects of living related kidney transplantation–risks and chances. Clin Transplant. 2004;18(6):716–721. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-0012.2004.00285.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Hirvas J, Enckell M, Kuhlbäck B, Pasternack A. Psychological and social problems encountered in active treatment of chronic uraemia, II: the living donor. Acta Med Scand. 1976;200(1–2):17–20. doi: 10.1111/j.0954-6820.1976.tb08189.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Sharma VK, Enoch MD. Psychological sequelae of kidney donation: a 5–10 year follow up study. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1987;75(4):264–267. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1987.tb02787.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Abecassis M, Adams M, Adams P, et al. Live Organ Donor Consensus Group. Consensus statement on the live organ donor. JAMA. 2000;284(22):2919–2926. doi: 10.1001/jama.284.22.2919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Delmonico F. Council of the Transplantation Society. A report of the Amsterdam forum on the care of the live kidney donor: data and medical guidelines. Transplantation. 2005;79(6 suppl):S53–S66. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Pruett TL, Tibell A, Alabdulkareem A, et al. The ethics statement of the Vancouver Forum on the live lung, liver, pancreas, and intestine donor. Transplantation. 2006;81(10):1386–1387. doi: 10.1097/01.tp.0000214976.36526.e3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.OPTN, UNOS and Health Resources and Services Administration. [Accessed October 17, 2011];OPTN Bylaws, Attachment I to Appendix B: Criteria for Transplant Program Designation. http://optn.transplant.hrsa.gov/PoliciesandBylaws2/bylaws/OPTNByLaws/pdfs/bylaw_162.pdf.
  • 55.Erim Y, Beckmann M, Kroencke S, et al. Sense of coherence and social support predict living liver donors’ emotional stress prior to living-donor liver transplantation. Clin Transplant. 2008;22(3):273–280. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-0012.2007.00782.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Kok NF, Lind MY, Hansson BM, et al. Comparison of laparoscopic and mini incision open donor nephrectomy: single blind, randomised controlled clinical trial. BMJ. 2006;333(7561):221–226. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38886.618947.7C. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Schulz KH, Kroencke S, Beckmann M, et al. Mental and physical quality of life in actual living liver donors versus potential living liver donors: a prospective, controlled, multicenter study. Liver Transpl. 2009;15(12):1676–1687. doi: 10.1002/lt.21917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Walter M, Bronner E, Steinmuller T, Klapp BF, Danzer G. Psychosocial data of potential living donors before living donor liver transplantation. Clin Transplant. 2002;16(1):55–59. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-0012.2002.00057.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Albertsmeyer S, Renner FC, Yildiz S, et al. One hundred six live kidney donors in a single German transplantation center: renal, physical, and psychological follow-up. Transplant Proc. 2010;42(10):3992–3993. doi: 10.1016/j.transproceed.2010.09.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Morris P, St George B, Waring T, Nanra R. Psychosocial complications in living related kidney donors: an Australian experience. Transplant Proc. 1987;19(2):2840–2844. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Weizer N, Weizman A, Shapira Z, Yussim A, Munitz H. Suicide by related kidney donors following the recipients’ death. Psychother Psychosom. 1989;51(4):216–219. doi: 10.1159/000288159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Cepoiu M, McCusker J, Cole MG, Sewitch M, Belzile E, Ciampi A. Recognition of depression by non-psychiatric physicians—a systematic literature review and meta-analysis. J Gen Intern Med. 2008;2(1):3, 25–36. doi: 10.1007/s11606-007-0428-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Azoulay D, Bhangui P, Andreani P, et al. Short- and long-term donor morbidity in right lobe living donor liver transplantation: 91 consecutive cases in a European Center. Am J Transplant. 2011;11(1):101–110. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-6143.2010.03284.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Buell JF, Lee L, Martin JE, et al. Laparoscopic donor nephrectomy vs. open live donor nephrectomy: a quality of life and functional study. Clin Transplant. 2005;19(1):102–109. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-0012.2004.00308.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Giessing M, Reuter S, Schöberger B, et al. Quality of life of living kidney donors in Germany: a survey with the validated Short Form-36 and Giessen Subjective Complaints List-24 Questionnaires. Transplantation. 2004;78(6):864–872. doi: 10.1097/01.tp.0000133307.00604.86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Isotani S, Fujisawa M, Ichikara Y, et al. Quality of life of living kidney donors: the Short-Form 36-Item Health Questionnaire survey. Urology. 2002;60(4):588–592. doi: 10.1016/s0090-4295(02)01865-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Kim-Schluger L, Florman SS, Schiano T, et al. Quality of life after lobectomy for adult liver transplantation. Transplantation. 2002;73(10):1593–1597. doi: 10.1097/00007890-200205270-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Minz M, Udgiri N, Sharma A, et al. Prospective psychosocial evaluation of related kidney donors: Indian perspective. Transplant Proc. 2005;37(5):2001–2003. doi: 10.1016/j.transproceed.2005.03.110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Chan SC, Liu CL, Lo CM, Lam BK, Lee EW, Fan ST. Donor quality of life before and after adult-to-adult right liver live donor liver transplantation. Liver Transpl. 2006;12(10):1529–1536. doi: 10.1002/lt.20897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Feltrin A, Pegoraro R, Rago C, et al. Experience of donation and quality of life in living kidney and liver donors. Transpl Int. 2008;21(5):466–472. doi: 10.1111/j.1432-2277.2007.00632.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Perry KT, Freedland SJ, Hu JC, et al. Quality of life, pain and return to normal activities following laparoscopic donor nephrectomy versus open mini-incision donor nephrectomy. J Urol. 2003;169(6):2018–2021. doi: 10.1097/01.ju.0000067975.59772.b6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Rodrigue JR, Cross NJ, Newman RC, et al. Patient-reported outcomes for open versus laparoscopic living donor nephrectomy. Prog Transplant. 2006;16(2):162–169. doi: 10.1177/152692480601600213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Dols LFC, Ijzermans JNM, Wentink N, et al. Long-term follow-up of a randomized trial comparing laparoscopic and mini-incision open live donor nephrectomy. Am J Transplant. 2010;10(11):2481–2487. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-6143.2010.03281.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Gouge F, Moore J, Jr, Bremer BA, McCauly CR, Johnson JP. The quality of life of donors, potential donors, and recipients of living-related donor renal transplantation. Transplant Proc. 1990;22(5):2409–2413. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Heck G, Schweitzer J, Seidel-Wiesel M. Psychological effects of living related kidney transplantation risks–and chances. Clin Transplant. 2004;18(6):716–721. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-0012.2004.00285.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Neuhaus TJ, Wartmann M, Weber M, Landolt MA, Laube GF, Kemper MJ. Psychosocial impact of living-related kidney transplantation on donors and partners. Pediatr Nephrol. 2005;20(2):205–209. doi: 10.1007/s00467-004-1749-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Stothers L, Gourlay WA, Liu L. Attitudes and predictive factors for live kidney donation: a comparison of live kidney donors versus nondonors. Kidney Int. 2005;67(3):1105–1111. doi: 10.1111/j.1523-1755.2005.00176.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Trotter JF, Talamantes M, McClure M, et al. Right hepatic lobe donation for living donor liver transplantation: impact on donor quality of life. Liver Transpl. 2001;7(6):485–493. doi: 10.1053/jlts.2001.24646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Diaz GC, Renz JF, Mudge C, et al. Donor health assessment after living-donor liver transplantation. Ann Surg. 2002;236(1):120–126. doi: 10.1097/00000658-200207000-00018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.DuBay DA, Holtzman S, Adcock L, et al. Cosmesis and body image after adult right lobe living liver donation. Transplantation. 2010;89(10):1270–1275. doi: 10.1097/TP.0b013e3181d58654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Gökçe S, Durmaz O, Peykerli GG, et al. Assessment of living donors with respect to pre- and posttransplant psychosocial properties and posttransplant family functioning in pediatric liver transplantation. Turk J Gastroenterol. 2011;22(1):36–41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Holtzman S, Adcock L, Dubay DA, et al. Financial, vocational, and interpersonal impact of living liver donation. Liver Transpl. 2009;15(11):1435–1442. doi: 10.1002/lt.21852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Crowley-Matoka M, Siegler M, Cronin DC., II Long-term quality of life issues among adult-to-pediatric living liver donors: a qualitative exploration. Am J Transplant. 2004;4:744–750. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-6143.2004.00377.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Kärrfelt HM, Berg UB, Lindblad FI, Tydén GE. To be or not to be a living donor: questionnaire to parents of children who have undergone renal transplantation. Transplantation. 1998;65(7):915–918. doi: 10.1097/00007890-199804150-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Reimer J, Rensing A, Haasen C, Philipp T, Pietruck F, Franke GH. The impact of living-related kidney transplantation on the donor’s life. Transplantation. 2006;81(9):1268–1273. doi: 10.1097/01.tp.0000210009.96816.db. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Sterneck MR, Fischer L, Nischwitz U, et al. Selection of the living liver donor. Transplantation. 1995;60(7):667–671. doi: 10.1097/00007890-199510150-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Jay CL, Butt Z, Ladner DP, Skaro AI, Abecassis MM. A review of quality of life instruments used in liver transplantation. J Hepatol. 2009;51(5):949–959. doi: 10.1016/j.jhep.2009.07.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Dew MA, Jacobs CL. Psychosocial and socioeconomic issues facing the living kidney donor. Advances Chronic Kidney Dis. doi: 10.1053/j.ackd.2012.04.006. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Achille M, Soos J, Fortin MC, Pâquet M, Hébert MJ. Differences in psychosocial profiles between men and women living kidney donors. Clin Transplant. 2007;21(3):314–320. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-0012.2007.00641.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.de Graaf Olson W, Bogetti-Dumlao A. Living donors’ perception of their quality of health after donation. Prog Transplant. 2001;11(2):108–115. doi: 10.1177/152692480101100206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Minnee RC, Bemelman WA, Polle SW, et al. Older living kidney donors: surgical outcome and quality of life. Transplantation. 2008;86(2):251–256. doi: 10.1097/TP.0b013e31817789dd. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Shrestha A, Shrestha A, Vallance C, McKane WS, Shrestha BM, Raftery AT. Quality of life of living kidney donors: a single-center experience. Transplant Proc. 2008;40(5):1375–1377. doi: 10.1016/j.transproceed.2008.03.132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Simmons RG, Anderson CR. Related donors and recipients: five to nine years post-transplant. Transplant Proc. 1982;14(1):9–12. [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Dahm F, Weber M, Müller B, et al. Open and laparoscopic living donor nephrectomy in Switzerland: a retrospective assessment of clinical outcomes and the motivation to donate. Nephrol Dial Transplant. 2006;21(9):2563–2568. doi: 10.1093/ndt/gfl207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Erim Y, Malagó M, Valentin-Gamazo C, Senf W, Broelsch CE. Guidelines for the psychosomatic evaluation of living liver donors: analysis of donor exclusion. Transplant Proc. 2003;35(3):909–910. doi: 10.1016/s0041-1345(03)00159-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Erim Y, Beckmann M, Valentin-Gamazo C, et al. Selection of donors for adult living-donor liver donation: results of the assessment of the first 205 donor candidates. Psychosomatics. 2008;49(2):143–151. doi: 10.1176/appi.psy.49.2.143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Jendrisak MD, Hong B, Shenoy S, et al. Altruistic living donors: evaluation for nondirected kidney or liver donation. Am J Transplant. 2006;6(1):115–120. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-6143.2005.01148.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Schweitzer J, Seidel-Wiesel M, Verres R, Wiesel M. Psychological consultation before living kidney donation: finding out and handling problem cases. Transplantation. 2003;76(10):1464–1470. doi: 10.1097/01.TP.0000084320.57817.32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Valentin-Gamazo C, Malago M, Karliova M, et al. Experience after the evaluation of 700 potential donors for living donor liver transplantation in a single center. Liver Transpl. 2004;10(9):1087–1096. doi: 10.1002/lt.20223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Chen SC, Hsu HT, Hwang SL, Lee PH. Attitude toward living donor liver transplantation in Taiwan. Transplant Proc. 2006;38(7):2108–2110. doi: 10.1016/j.transproceed.2006.06.076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.DiMartini A, Cruz R, Dew MA, et al. Motives and decision making of potential living liver donors: comparisons between gender, relationships and ambivalence. Am J Transplant. 2012;12:136–151. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-6143.2011.03805.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Lee SH, Jeong JS, Ha HS, et al. Decision-related factors and attitudes toward donation in living related liver transplantation: ten-year experience. Transplant Proc. 2005;37(2):1081–1084. doi: 10.1016/j.transproceed.2005.01.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Simpson MA, Kendrick J, Verbesey JE, et al. Ambivalence in living liver donors. Liver Transpl. 2011;17(10):1226–1233. doi: 10.1002/lt.22342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Kamstra-Hennen L, Beebe J, Stumm S, Simmons RG. Ethical evaluation of related donation: the donor after five years. Transplant Proc. 1981;13(1 pt 1):60–61. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Switzer GE, Simmons RG, Dew MA. Helping unrelated strangers: physical and psychosocial reactions to the bone marrow donation process among anonymous donors. J Appl Soc Psychol. 1996;26(6):469–490. [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Switzer GE, Dew MA, Simmons RG. Donor ambivalence and postdonation outcomes: implications for living donation. Transplant Proc. 1997;29(1–2):1476. doi: 10.1016/s0041-1345(96)00590-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Smith MD, Kappell DF, Province MA, et al. Living-related kidney donors: a multicenter study of donor education, socioeconomic adjustment, and rehabilitation. Am J Kidney Dis. 1986;8(4):223–233. doi: 10.1016/s0272-6386(86)80030-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Glanz K, Rimer BK, Viswanath K, editors. Health Behavior and Health Education: Theory, Research, and Practice. 4. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass (Wiley); 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Kellam SG, Langevin DJ. A framework for understanding “evidence” in prevention research and programs. Prev Sci. 2003;4(3):137–153. doi: 10.1023/a:1024693321963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Miller WR, Rollnick S. Motivational Interviewing: Preparing People for Change. 2. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Miller WR, Rose GS. Toward a theory of motivational interviewing. Am Psychologist. 2009;64(6):527–537. doi: 10.1037/a0016830. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Flay BR, Biglan A, Boruch RF, et al. Standards of evidence: criteria for efficacy, effectiveness and dissemination. Prev Sci. 2005;6(3):151–175. doi: 10.1007/s11121-005-5553-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Institute of Medicine (U.S.) Crossing the Quality Chasm: A New Health System for the 21st Century. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2001. Committee on Quality Health Care in America. [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Institute of Medicine (U.S.) Committee on Prevention of Mental Disorders, Division of Biobehavorial Sciences and Mental Disorders. In: Mrazek PJ, Haggerty RJ, editors. Reducing Risks for Mental Disorders: Frontiers for Preventive Intervention Research. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1994. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Hettema J, Steele J, Miller WR. Motivational interviewing. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2005;1:91–111. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Martins RK, McNeil DW. Review of Motivational Interviewing in promoting health behaviors. Clin Psychol Rev. 2009;29(4):283–293. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Rubak S, Sandbaek A, Lauritzen T, Christensen B. Motivational interviewing: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Gen Prac. 2005;55(513):305–312. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Hodgins DC, Currie SR, el-Guebaly N. Motivational enhancement and self-help treatments for problem gambling. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2001;69(1):50–57. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.69.1.50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Monti PM, Barnett NP, Colby SM, et al. Motivational interviewing versus feedback only in emergency care for young adult problem drinking. Addiction. 2007;102(8):1234–1243. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.01878.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Bennett JA, Lyons KS, Winters-Stone K, Nail LM, Scherer J. Motivational interviewing to increase physical activity in long-term cancer survivors: a randomized controlled trial. Nurs Res. 2007;56(1):18–27. doi: 10.1097/00006199-200701000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Butterworth S, Linden A, McClay W, Leo MC. Effect of motivational interviewing-based health coaching on employees’ physical and mental health status. J Occup Health Psychol. 2006;11(4):358–365. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.11.4.358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Thompson DR, Chair SY, Chan SW, Astin F, Davidson PM, Ski CF. Motivational interviewing: a useful approach to improving cardiovascular health? J Clin Nurs. 2011;20(9–10):1236–1244. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2702.2010.03558.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Rollnick S, Miller WR, Butler CC. Motivational Interviewing in Health Care: Helping Patients Change Behavior. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Rogers CR. A theory of therapy, personality, and interpersonal relationships as developed in the client-centered framework. In: Koch S, editor. Psychology: The study of a science. Vol. 3. Formulations of the person and the social context. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1959. pp. 184–256. [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Janis IL, editor. Counseling on Personal Decisions: Theory and Research on Short-Term Helping Relationships. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Miller WR, Rollnick S. Talking oneself into change: motivational interviewing, stages of change, and therapeutic process. J Cogn Psychother. 2004;18(4):299–308. [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Prochaska JO, Butterworth S, Redding CA, et al. Initial efficacy of MI, TTM tailoring and HRI’s with multiple behaviors for employee health promotion. Prev Med. 2008;46(3):226–231. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2007.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Prochaska JO, Redding CA, Evers KE. The Transtheoretical Model and stages of change. In: Glanz K, Rimer BK, Viswanath K, editors. Health Behavior and Health Education: Theory, Research, and Practice. 4. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass (Wiley); 2008. pp. 97–122. [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Bem DJ. Self perception theory. In: Berkowitz L, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. Vol. 6. NY: Academic Press; 1972. pp. 1–62. [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Dabbs AJD, Myers BA, McCurry KR, et al. User-centered design and interactive health technologies for patients. Comput Inform Nurs. 2009;27(3):175–183. doi: 10.1097/NCN.0b013e31819f7c7c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Hulley SB, Cummings SR, Browner WS, Grady DG, Newman TB. Designing clinical research. 3. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Simpson MA, Pomfret EA. Searching for the optimal living liver donor psychosocial evaluation. Am J Transplant. 2012;12:7–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-6143.2011.03806.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.U.S. OPTN/UNOS Living Donor Committee. Guidance for the informed consent of living donors. OPTN/UNOS; Richmond, VA: Department of Health and Human Services; Rockville, MD: 2007. [Accessed October 22, 2011]. http://optn.transplant.hrsa.gov/ContentDocuments/Guidance_InformedConsentLiving_Donors.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Atkisson CC, Greenfield TK. The UCSF Client Satisfaction Scales: I. The Client Satisfaction Questionnaire-8. In: Maruish ME, editor. Instruments for Adults. 3. Vol. 3. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2004. pp. 799–811. The Use of Psychological Testing for Treatment Planning and Outcomes Assessment. [Google Scholar]
  • 135.U.S. Census Bureau. [Accessed October 22, 2011];United States Census. 2010 Web site. http://2010.census.gov/2010census/index.php.
  • 136.Myaskovsky L, Dew MA, Crowley-Matoka M, et al. Is donating a kidney associated with changes in health habits? Prog Transplant. Accepted. [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Couper MP. The future of modes of data collection. Pub Opin Quarterly. 2011;75:889–908. [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Currell R, Urquhart C, Wainwright P, Lewis R. Telemedicine versus face to face patient care: Effects on professional practice and health care outcomes. [Last accessed January 30, 2012];Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2000 (2):Art. No.: CD002098. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD002098. www.thecochranelibrary.com. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 139.Aquilino WS. Interview mode effects in surveys of drug and alcohol use. Pub Opin Quarterly. 1994;58:210–240. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES