Abstract
OBJECTIVES
This study tested two hypotheses: (1) chemotherapy increases the rate of cognitive decline in breast and colorectal cancer patients beyond what is typical of normal aging and (2) chemotherapy results in systematic cognitive declines when compared with breast and colorectal cancer patients who did not receive chemotherapy.
SUBJECTS
Data came from personal interviews with a prospective cohort of patients with breast (n = 141) or colorectal cancer (n = 224) with incident disease drawn from the nationally representative Health and Retirement Study (1998–2006) with linked Medicare claims.
MEASURES
The 27-point modified Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status was used to assess cognitive functioning, focusing on memory and attention. We defined the smallest clinically significant change as 0.4 points per year.
RESULTS
We used Bayesian hierarchical linear models to test the hypotheses, adjusting for multiple possible confounders. Eighty-eight patients were treated with chemotherapy; 277 were not. Mean age at diagnosis was 75.5. Patients were followed for a median of 3.1 years after diagnosis, with a range of 0 to 8.3 years. We found no differences in the rates of cognitive decline before and after diagnosis for patients who received chemotherapy in adjusted models (p=.86, one-sided 95% PI lower bound: 0.09 worse after chemotherapy), where patients served as their own controls. Moreover, the rate of cognitive decline after diagnosis did not differ between patients who had chemotherapy and those who did not (p=0.84, one-sided 95% PI lower bound: 0.11 worse for chemotherapy group in adjusted model).
CONCLUSION
There was no evidence of cognitive decline associated with chemotherapy in this sample of older adults with breast and colorectal cancer.
Keywords: breast cancer, cancer care, cancer survivors, colorectal cancer, cognition, chemotherapy
INTRODUCTION
In the United States alone, there are currently more than 11 million cancer survivors.1,2 This makes the long-term consequences of cancer treatments an area of substantial public health concern.3,6–7 Several studies have shown declines in self-reported cognitive functioning following chemotherapy;4,5 However, research on the effect of chemotherapy on neuropsychological tests has been mixed.8,9 Some prominent studies have shown small-to-moderate negative effects of chemotherapy on measures of memory and executive functioning,10–18 while others have shown no chemotherapy-based cognitive declines.19–24 However, these studies have primarily included younger women.
Older adults are at an increased risk of dementia,25 and having cancer is a risk factor for long-term cognitive deficits.26–27 Thus, chemotherapy could have a greater effect on cognitive function in older adults. Yet, few studies to date have examined older adults, and the results of these studies have been mixed.28–29 Given that older adults with cognitive impairment require greater care30 and have higher mortality rates,31 there is a great need to establish whether chemotherapy has enduring cognitive side effects in older adults.
To address this need, we took advantage of a unique, prospectively collected assessment of cognitive function in the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) linked to Medicare claims data to test two hypotheses about the long-term effect of chemotherapy on cognitive functioning. First, we tested the hypothesis that for patients who receive chemotherapy the rate of cognitive decline after treatment would be greater than their rate of cognitive decline before the receipt of chemotherapy. Second, we hypothesized that the rate of cognitive decline after diagnosis would be greater for patients who receive chemotherapy than for cancer patients who did not have chemotherapy while simultaneously controlling for other risk factors for cognitive decline.
METHOD
Settings and Participants
The Health and Retirement Study (HRS) is a longitudinal panel study that surveys a nationally representative sample of Americans over the age of 50 every two years about a wide array of topics, including detailed questions about their cognitive function.32 On turning 65, most of the cohort consented to link their Medicare administrative data with the HRS interview data.
We examined all respondents who had cognitive testing in the HRS-Medicare cohort between 1998 and 2004. Within this group, we identified patients with incident breast or colorectal cancer using Medicare claims from 1998 through 2006. We chose to focus on these two types of cancer because of their high incidence and high survivability. Incident breast cancer cases were identified via the validated Nattinger algorithm.33–34 Incident colorectal cancer cases were defined by the method of Yabroff,35 where we excluded prevalent cases by requiring no previous colorectal cancer claims in the prior 1095 days.36
Chemotherapy was defined by the presence of claims for chemotherapy infusion, using a validated method.37 Outpatient chemotherapy was defined for both types of cancer by the occurrence of HCPCS codes in an outpatient or carrier file or the inpatient file; the relevant codes are included in Supplementary Digital Content 1. These codes were identified by independent review of all Medicare-covered infusion drugs by a medical oncologist (JJG) and an internist (TJI).
We required at least one cognitive status exam prior to diagnosis and continuous enrollment in fee-for-service Medicare, in order to ensure full claims data were present. Date of diagnosis was inferred as the earliest inpatient or outpatient claim date associated with cancer. The time between diagnosis and the last cognitive status exam prior to diagnosis varied by respondent; median length was 360 days, with a range of 2 days to 3,394 days. All patients were followed through death or the 2006 HRS survey. These inclusion criteria resulted in a minimum of one observation (before cancer diagnosis) and a maximum of six observations per respondent. Observations were collected every two years resulting in a maximum follow-up time of 9 years (e.g. patient diagnosed in 1998 immediately after 1998 observation with follow up data from 2000–2006 collected every two years). The timing of diagnosis varied between patients ranging from five observations before diagnosis and one after to one observation before diagnosis to five after.
Outcomes
Cognitive function was measured using a modified version of the Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS-m), which was developed from two well-validated scales: 1) the original TICS measure and 2) the Mini-Mental State Examination. The TICS-m, validated on large nationally representative samples, has been shown to have satisfactory psychometric properties and to correlate with important sociodemographic and health characteristics in predictable ways38–39 and has been successfully used to document the effects of other diseases on long-term cognitive function.40–41 More information on the HRS cognitive scale is available at the HRS website.42 HRS respondents represented by a proxy are not administered the cognitive scale. Therefore, data from proxies were excluded from our primary analysis, resulting in the loss of 8% of all observations. Using data for the full HRS sample we calculated that among 65 year-olds, TICS-m scores declined an average of 0.55 points over 3 years, 0.92 points over 5 years, and 1.84 points over 10 years, or approximately 0.18 points per year. Using this pattern of cognitive decline for “normal aging,” we defined a change of 0.4 points per year as the minimum clinically significant change in TICS-m scores, with lower scores indicating declines in cognitive function. This magnitude of change would indicate that 1 year of cognitive decline due to chemotherapy approximates 2 years of “normal” cognitive decline.
Analyses
To examine the impact of chemotherapy on cognitive functioning, we studied the psychometric properties of the TICS-m for our specific population of interest (individuals in the HRS with breast or colorectal cancer). We then estimated several hierarchical longitudinal models of patients’ TICS –m scores, which allow these scores to have one pattern of change before diagnosis and a second pattern of change after diagnosis. These models used TICS-m score as the outcome and time, diagnosis (before/after), and chemotherapy receipt (receive/did not receive) as predictors. To develop the unadjusted model, we tested several linear and nonlinear hierarchical longitudinal models that included these predictor variables and their interactions. Models were estimated via restricted maximum likelihood using the lme4 package in R43 and via Markov chain Monte Carlo using the rjags package in R.44 The best model, as judged by AIC, BIC, and deviance, is described and expanded upon below; our results were not sensitive to model choice. Following the selection of the best fitting unadjusted model, we estimated the same model while adjusting for effects of age, body mass index (BMI), Charlson score, activities of daily living (ADL), instrumental ADL (IADL), education, income, tobacco and alcohol use, and census region. The covariates were chosen because of their known relationships with cognitive functioning and/or administration of chemotherapy.38,45–46 In the adjusted model, the covariates were treated as fixed-effects, and effects were estimated separately on the slope and on the intercept of the growth curve as well as on the interaction between chemotherapy receipt and time. For all models, we employed one-tailed tests of the hypothesis that chemotherapy is associated with declines in cognitive function; therefore we report only the lower bound of all posterior intervals.
In addition to these primary analyses, we replicated four versions of this model to determine whether our conclusions were robust to a variety of confounders. We tested 1) whether the exclusion of proxies influenced our results by setting cognition score to zero when a proxy was used, 2) whether the chemotherapy group was substantively different from the control group before treatment using a propensity matching approach, 3) whether only short-term effects of chemotherapy existed by analyzing only the observation immediately after diagnosis, and 4) whether a continuous or categorical interpretation of the TICS-m affected the results. We also used data simulation to estimate the probability of obtaining our results, assuming existence of a minimum clinically significant change on the TICS-m defined above. That is, we estimated the probability that we would obtain the reported results if chemotherapy truly decreased cognitive function by 0.4 points per year. This is essentially a posthoc power calculation under the hypothesis of a minimum clinically significant change. Details of these models are presented in the Appendix.
In the frequentist analyses, a p-value of 0.05 was considered significant. Because we also estimated Bayesian models, we report 95% Posterior Intervals (PI) rather than Confidence Intervals (CI), although they are similar in that both provide ranges of plausible values for the parameter tested.
This work was approved by the University of Michigan Institutional Review Board. Participants provided informed consent on enrollment in the HRS and again for linkage to Medicare claims.
RESULTS
Of the 16,772 respondents in the HRS-Medicare data, we identified 141 breast and 224 colorectal cancer patients with incident disease between 1998 and 2006, 24% (n=88) of whom received chemotherapy. Of the 365 patients, only 255 had cognitive status exams after diagnosis (61 of chemotherapy patients had cognitive exams). Those patients were followed for a median of 3.1 years after diagnosis and up to 8.3 years afterwards. There were few significant differences between the chemotherapy and non-chemotherapy groups before diagnosis; see Table 1. Figure 1 depicts the mean TICS-m score at each observation for the chemotherapy and no chemotherapy groups. Negative numbers represent observations before diagnosis, and positive numbers represent observations after diagnosis. Very few patients have more than three observations before or after diagnosis. Therefore, Figure 1 was limited to three observations before and after diagnosis. Both groups exhibited declines in cognitive functioning at a rate of 0.27 points per year before diagnosis (p<.01, 95% PI: −0.29,−0.14) with no significant difference in rate of change between patients who eventually received chemotherapy and those who did not (p =.28, 95% PI: −0.13, 0.33).
Table 1.
Sample characteristics at observation prior to breast or colorectal cancer diagnosis
Patient characteristics | Chemotherapy (88) | No Chemotherapy (277) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
Breast (24) | Colorectal (64) | Breast (117) | Colorectal (160) | |
Age1 | ||||
Mean (SD) | 70.5 (5.3) | 72.4 (7.9) | 75.8 (7.5) | 77.3 (9) |
Median | 69.3 | 72 | 75.7 | 79.6 |
| ||||
Charlson Score2 | ||||
Mean (SD) | 0.8 (1.4) | 1.2 (1.7) | 1.2 (1.4) | 2.2 (2.6) |
Median | 0 | 1 | 1 | 2 |
| ||||
TICS-m score3 | ||||
Mean (SD) | 15.8 (4.1) | 14.8 (4.6) | 15.3 (4.3) | 13.4 (4.1) |
Median | 17 | 15 | 16 | 13 |
| ||||
Activities of Daily Living (ADL) n (%) | ||||
No limitations prior to Cancer | 21 (88%) | 50 (78%) | 91 (78%) | 107 (67%) |
Limitations prior to Cancer | 3 (12%) | 14 (22%) | 26 (22%) | 53 (33%) |
| ||||
Instrumental ADL n (%) | ||||
No limitations prior to Cancer | 22 (92%) | 54 (84%) | 102 (87%) | 120 (75%) |
Limitations prior to Cancer | 2 (8%) | 10 (16%) | 15 (13%) | 40 (25%) |
| ||||
Education n (%)4 | ||||
High School or less | 8 (33%) | 23 (36%) | 24 (21%) | 51 (32%) |
Some College | 5 (21%) | 17 (27%) | 53 (45%) | 70 (44%) |
College Graduate | 11 (46%) | 24 (38%) | 40 (34%) | 39 (24%) |
| ||||
Tobacco user n (%) | ||||
Never | 13 (54%) | 25 (39%) | 60 (51%) | 71 (44%) |
Former | 7 (29%) | 30 (47%) | 40 (34%) | 69 (43%) |
Current | 4 (17%) | 9 (14%) | 16 (14%) | 18 (11%) |
Missing | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 1 (1%) | 2 (1%) |
| ||||
Alcohol n (%) | ||||
0 Days/week | 18 (75%) | 49 (77%) | 79 (68%) | 125 (78%) |
<1 Day/week | 0 (0%) | 3 (5%) | 10 (9%) | 9 (6%) |
1–2 Days/week | 2 (8%) | 3 (5%) | 3 (3%) | 3 (2%) |
> 2 Days/week | 4 (17%) | 9 (14%) | 25 (21%) | 23 (14%) |
| ||||
Census region n (%) | ||||
1 | 2 (8%) | 11 (17%) | 23 (20%) | 26 (16%) |
2 | 6 (25%) | 24 (38%) | 29 (25%) | 53 (33%) |
3 | 13 (54%) | 25 (39%) | 46 (39%) | 62 (39%) |
4 | 3 (12%) | 4 (6%) | 19 (16%) | 19 (12%) |
| ||||
Net worth | ||||
Mean | 907 | 303 | 443 | 276 |
Median | 213 | 162 | 184 | 96 |
Chemotherapy and no chemotherapy groups differed significantly in:
Age, t (363) = 4.83, p < .001
Charlson score, t (363) = 2.62, p = .01
Lower scores on the TICS indicate declines in cognitive function
Education, χ2 (2) = 10.6, p = .005
Figure 1.
Mean cognition score by survey1–3
1Error bars indicated 95% confidence intervals for the standard error of the mean.
2These unadjusted results show no evidence of greater decline in the group of patients who received chemotherapy than those who did not.
3The numbers on each bar represents the number of patients contributing data the observation.
As applied to the current population, we studied the psychometric properties of the TICS-m by fitting a one-factor model to the four items making up the scale. In this analysis, we used all data observed at the wave immediately prior to individuals’ cancer diagnosis. In fitting the one-factor model to the data, we rejected the hypothesis of exact fit (χ2 (2) = 10.5, p < .05). The hypothesis of exact fit is often rejected at larger sample sizes, so we also examined the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). The 90% confidence interval for the RMSEA was (.053, .184), which implies a range from “reasonable fit” to “poor fit”.47 The proportion of explained common variance was .65, Cronbach’s alpha was .62, and the corrected item-total correlations ranged from .33 to .86. The misfit was mainly due to two tasks (serial 7 and backwards counting) that accounted for only seven points on the 27-point scale, so we elected to use the full scale in the analyses below. However, the substantive results described below hold for each of the four individual items making up the TICS-m.
We used two strategies to test for the presence of chemotherapy-associated cognitive declines. First, using a prospective approach, we compared rates of decline in cognitive function before and after diagnosis for patients receiving chemotherapy and found no significant differences in either the unadjusted (p=.86, one-sided 95% PI lower bound: 0.12 worse for chemotherapy group) or adjusted models (p=.86, one-sided 95% PI lower bound: 0.09 worse for chemotherapy group). Second, using a cohort approach, we compared rates of decline in cognitive function after diagnosis between those who received chemotherapy and those who did not. We found no significant differences in either the unadjusted (p=.85, one-sided 95% PI lower bound: 0.12 worse for chemotherapy group) or the adjusted models (p=0.84, one-sided 95% PI lower bound: 0.11 worse for chemotherapy group), see Table 2. We also estimated the adjusted model with the entire HRS-cohort and found that cognitive function declined at a rate of 0.28 points per year (z = −8.8, p < .05, SE = 0.03); this value was very similar to the rate of change for cancer patients.
Table 2.
Parameter estimates for the adjusted hierarchical longitudinal model of TICS-m scores
Parameter | β | Standard Error | p |
---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 5.05 | 3.25 | 0.12 |
Time | −0.12 | 0.04 | 0.00 |
Chemotherapy receipt | 0.52 | 0.45 | 0.25 |
Body mass index | 1.75 | 0.96 | 0.07 |
Charlson score | −0.42 | 0.35 | 0.23 |
Activities of Daily Living (ADL) | −0.16 | 0.27 | 0.56 |
Instrumental ADL | −0.61 | 0.31 | 0.05 |
| |||
Age | −0.15 | 0.02 | 0.00 |
| |||
Education | |||
High School or less | <reference> | ||
Some College | 2.42 | 0.44 | 0.00 |
College Graduate | 3.54 | 0.46 | 0.00 |
| |||
Tobacco user | |||
Never | <reference> | ||
Former | −0.22 | 0.37 | 0.54 |
Current | 0.64 | 0.53 | 0.22 |
| |||
Alcohol | |||
0 Days/week | <reference> | ||
<1 Day/week | 0.46 | 0.38 | 0.22 |
1–2 Days/week | −0.23 | 0.52 | 0.66 |
> 2 Days/week | 0.18 | 0.34 | 0.60 |
| |||
Census region | |||
1 | <reference> | ||
2 | −0.29 | 0.48 | 0.56 |
3 | −0.40 | 0.47 | 0.39 |
4 | 0.04 | 0.59 | 0.94 |
| |||
Net worth | 0.17 | 0.05 | 0.00 |
| |||
Interactions | |||
Time × chemotherapy receipt1 | 0.11 | 0.10 | 0.86 |
Time × chemotherapy receipt × diagnosis2 | 0.17 | 0.17 | 0.84 |
Term tests whether the change in TICS-m score over time is a function of chemotherapy receipt
Term tests whether the differences in the slope of TICS-m scores before or after cancer diagnosis is a function of chemotherapy receipt. TICS-m scores are allowed to have different slopes before and after diagnosis.
Our results were robust to a variety of confounders. First, instead of excluding proxies in the analyses, we included proxies in the sample and set their cognitive function scores to zero, thereby allowing us to test whether excluding proxies gave an unfair advantage to the null hypothesis. Even in such an extreme case, the model still ruled out all chemotherapy-based cognitive declines (p=0.95, one-sided 95% PI lower bound: 0.08 better for chemotherapy group). In fact, non-chemotherapy patients utilized proxy respondents more often, so if proxy use was informative about poor cognitive functioning, it would provide yet stronger evidence against a chemotherapy-associated decline. Additionally we considered the possibility that 1) the chemotherapy group was substantively different from the control group, 2) chemotherapy affected cognitive function only at the first observation after diagnosis, and 3) categorical measures of cognitive function would yield different results than our continuous measure. We used three additional modeling approaches to test these alternative explanations, see Supplementary Digital Content 2–5. The same general pattern emerged across all models: With 95% certainty, we can exclude declines in cognitive function associated with chemotherapy that are greater than one year of “normal aging”.
Finally, using the data simulation method described above, we calculated the probability that we would obtain these results if chemotherapy truly decreased cognitive function by 0.4 points per year (the minimum clinically significant change). In doing so, we were able to reject the hypothesis that chemotherapy induces cognitive declines of that at least that magnitude with a certainty of p=.05.
DISCUSSION
In this study, which used a prospectively collected assessment of cognitive function in the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) linked with Medicare claims data, we did not find support for either of our hypotheses. The rate of cognitive decline after diagnosis did not differ from the rate of cognitive decline before diagnosis for older Americans with breast or colorectal cancer. Moreover, the rate of cognitive decline after cancer diagnosis did not differ between patients who had chemotherapy and those who did not. These results were robust in several sensitivity analyses, and the likelihood of obtaining these results if clinically significant chemotherapy-based cognitive declines truly exist was 5%.
Our findings are in contrast to a number of cross-sectional studies, which have reported small-to-moderate effects of chemotherapy on cognitive functioning.10–18,48–51 However, these results are in line with other prospective studies that have shown no long-term impact of chemotherapy on cognitive function. 19–24,48–51 In addition to extending the findings of other prospective studies to a new measure of cognitive function, the HRS-Medicare data address the collective weaknesses of prior prospective studies: a lack of data on cognitive function prior to diagnosis, limited data on cognitive function after treatment, short follow-up periods, and statistical models that do not include longitudinal components or individual differences.
There are some limitations of using the TICS-m to measure cognitive function: 1) The TICS-m is a cognitive screening measure not a full cognitive assessment; as such, it is less sensitive to small changes in cognitive function. Yet it has been successfully used to document the effects of other diseases on long-term cognitive function;40–41 2) Given the somewhat less than optimal reliability estimates in combination with relatively small sample sizes, the possibility of lower detectable effect sizes cannot be ruled out; 3) The TICS-m largely measures memory and attention;52 additional research is needed to determine whether long-term declines exist in other areas of domains of cognitive function. There are also several limitations specific to the use of Medicare administrative data: 1) Medicare data was only available for consenting participants, which could bias our results; 2) While we used a validated algorithms to detect cancer cases in Medicare, this is not the same as clinical assessment; 3) We were unable to assess second primaries or recurrences in the chemotherapy-treated group; 4) Stage of disease is not available in the claims data. More generally, this sample only includes patients with breast and colorectal cancer; therefore, generalizations of these findings to other types of cancer treated with other types of chemotherapy may be limited. We were also unable to examine type or duration of chemotherapy with this sample size. Although there may be subpopulations at greater risk for adverse cognitive effects of chemotherapy,16,17 our analysis speaks to the lack of an association at the population-level. However, we believe the merits of a longitudinal design with cognitive data collected prior to cancer diagnosis outweigh the limitations.
In addition to patient clinical factors and cancer characteristics factors, chemotherapy decisions also include a consideration of potential treatment toxicity and patient willingness to experience that toxicity for sometimes uncertain benefit.53 Given the importance that patients place on preservation of cognitive function when weighing treatment decisions,54 the information garnered from this study may be useful to patients and physicians alike. Using a prospectively collected assessment of cognitive function, we found no evidence of clinically significant declines in memory and attention due to chemotherapy. Therefore, concerns about chemotherapy commonly reported in the media may not be justified for most older breast and colorectal cancer patients.6,7
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
Funding This work was supported by NIH grants K08 HL091249, R01 AG027010 and R01 AG030155, the Society of Critical Care Medicine’s 2010 Vision Grant, and by pilot support from the Michigan Institute for Clinical and Health Research (MICHR), UL1RR024986. The National Institute on Aging provides funding for the Health and Retirement Study (U01 AG09740), which is performed at the Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan.
APPENDIX A
We replicated several versions of this model to determine whether our conclusions were robust to a variety of confounders. First, we considered the fact that excluding proxies from our sample may have given an unfair advantage to the null hypothesis if there were a proportionally greater number of proxy respondents in the chemotherapy group. To address this, we replicated the adjusted model while including proxies in the sample and setting their cognition scores to zero every time a proxy was used. Second, we considered the possibility that the chemotherapy group was substantively different from the control group before treatment. To account for this, we created propensity scores for obtaining chemotherapy for each individual using all covariates from the observation immediately prior to diagnosis. Minimum distance matched sampling55 was then carried out, yielding a matched control patient for each chemotherapy patient. We then tested for chemotherapy-based cognitive declines by comparing cognition scores from patients that received chemotherapy to the newly created matched cohort. Third, we considered the possibility that chemotherapy-based cognitive declines only arise immediately following diagnosis (i.e., the patient takes an initial “hit” and then recovers). To test for short-term chemotherapy related declines in cognition scores, we estimated the original adjusted model including only the first observation after diagnosis for each chemotherapy patient. Fourth, we examined whether using a continuous or categorical measure of cognition score affected interpretations of the findings. The original, adjusted model used a continuous measure of cognition. Therefore we fit an additional hierarchical ordinal logistic regression (i.e., proportional odds) model to a trichotomous cognition measure (normal cognitive functioning, cognitive impairment without dementia, or dementia). The cut points used to create these categories were based on prior studies with the HRS data.56–58 Normal was defined as 8–27; cognitive impairment without dementia was 6–7; dementia was 0–5. The covariates in the model were exactly the same as the original, adjusted model.
We also used data simulation to estimate our ability to detect a chemotherapy-associated decrease in cognition scores of 0.4 points per year, which was defined as the minimum clinically significant change on this measure of cognitive functioning. To do so, we treated all estimates from our adjusted model (i.e., the modeling including covariates) as parameters, with the exception of the estimated effect of chemotherapy, which we set to the minimum clinically significant value of a decline in cognition score of 0.4 points per year. We then generated 5,000 datasets from this model. All generated datasets matched our observed data on number of patients, time at which each patient was observed, and number of patients receiving chemotherapy. We then fit the hierarchical model described above to each of our 5,000 generated datasets, recording the size of the estimated effect of chemotherapy on cognitive functioning for each dataset. We then determined the proportion of times we observed an estimate of chemotherapy-based cognitive decline as extreme or greater than that observed in our adjusted model. In doing so, this analysis provides the probability that we would obtain the reported results if chemotherapy truly decreased cognitive function by 0.4 points per year (the minimum clinically significant change).
References
- 1.Howlader N, Noone AM, Krapcho M, et al., editors. SEER Cancer Statistics Review, 1975–2008. National Cancer Institute; Bethesda, MD: 2011. http://seer.cancer.gov/csr/1975_2008/, based on November 2010 SEER data submission, posted to the SEER web site. [Google Scholar]
- 2.J Natl Cancer Inst. 2008 Feb 20;100(4):236. doi: 10.1093/jnci/djn018. Epub 2008 Feb 12. Stat bite: Cancer survivors in the United States. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hewitt M, Greenfield S, Stovall E, editors. From cancer patient to cancer survivor: Lost in translation. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Boykoff N, Moieni M, Subramanian SK. Confronting chemobrain: An in-depth look at survivors’ reports of work, social networks, and health care response. J Cancer Surviv. 2009;3:223–32. doi: 10.1007/s11764-009-0098-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Mehnert A, Scherwath A, Schirmer L, et al. The association between neuropsychological impairment, self-perceived cognitive deficits, fatigue and health related quality of life in breast cancer survivors following standard adjuvant versus high-dose chemotherapy. Patient Educ Couns. 2007;66:108–18. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2006.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gross J. Chemotherapy fog is no longer ignored as an illusion. [Accessed November 17, 2010.];New York Times. 2007 Apr 29; Available at: http://nytimes.com/2007/04/29/health/29chemo.html.
- 7.Brody JE. The fog that follows chemotherapy. [Accessed December 13, 2010.];New York Times. 2009 Aug 3; Available at: http://www.nytimes.com/2009/08/04/health/04brod.html?_r=1.
- 8.Soussain C, Ricard D, Fike JR, et al. CNS complications of radiotherapy and chemotherapy. Lancet. 2009;374:1639–51. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61299-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Correa DD, Ahles TA. Neurocognitive changes in cancer survivors. Cancer J. 2008;14(6):396–400. doi: 10.1097/PPO.0b013e31818d8769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ahles TA, Saykin A. Cognitive effects of standard-dose chemotherapy in patients with cancer. Cancer Invest. 2001;19(8):812–20. doi: 10.1081/cnv-100107743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Ahles TA, Saykin AJ, Furstenburg CT, et al. Neuropsychological impact of standard-dose systemic chemotherapy in long-term survivors of breast cancer and lymphoma. J Clin Oncol. 2002;20:485–93. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2002.20.2.485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Wefel JS, Lenzi R, Theriault RL, et al. The cognitive sequelae of standard-dose adjuvant chemotherapy in women with breast carcinoma. Cancer. 2004;100:2292–9. doi: 10.1002/cncr.20272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Quesnel C, Savard J, Ivers H. Cognitive impairments associated with breast cancer treatments: Results from a longitudinal study. Breast Cancer Res Treat. 2009;116:113–23. doi: 10.1007/s10549-008-0114-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Weis J, Poppelreuter M, Bartsch HH. Cognitive deficits as long-term side effects of adjuvant therapy in breast cancer patients: ‘Subjective’ complaints and ‘objective’ neuropsychological test results. Psychooncology. 2009:18775–82. doi: 10.1002/pon.1472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Wefel JS, Saleeba AK, Buzdar AU, et al. Acute and late onset cognitive dysfunction associated with chemotherapy in women with breast cancer. Cancer. 2010;116(14):3348–56. doi: 10.1002/cncr.25098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Ahles TA, Saykin AJ, McDonald BC, et al. Longitudinal assessment of cognitive changes associated with adjuvant treatment for breast cancer: Impact of age and cognitive reserve. J Clin Oncol. 2010;28(29):4434–40. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2009.27.0827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Janelsins MC, Kohli S, Mohile SG, et al. An update on cancer- and chemotherapy-related cognitive dysfunction: Current status. Semin Oncol. 2011;38(3):431–8. doi: 10.1053/j.seminoncol.2011.03.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Phillips KM, Jim HS, Small BJ, Laronga C, et al. Cognitive functioning after cancer treatment: A 3-year longitudinal comparison of breast cancer survivors treated with chemotherapy or radiation and noncancer controls. Cancer. 2011 doi: 10.1002/cncr.26432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Donovan KA, Small BJ, Andrykowski MA, et al. Cognitive functioning after adjuvant chemotherapy and/or radiation for early-stage breast cancer. Cancer. 2005;104(11):2499–507. doi: 10.1002/cncr.21482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Jenkins V, Shilling V, Deutsch G, et al. A 3-year prospective study of the effects of adjuvant treatments on cognition in women with early stage breast cancer. Br J Cancer. 2006;94:828–34. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjc.6603029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Jim HSL, Donovan KA, Small BJ, et al. Cognitive functioning in breast cancer survivors: A controlled comparison. Cancer. 2009;115(8):1776–83. doi: 10.1002/cncr.24192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mehlsen M, Pedersen AD, Jensen AB, et al. No indications of cognitive side-effects in a prospective study of breast cancer patients receiving adjuvant chemotherapy. Psychooncology. 2009 Mar;18(3):248–57. doi: 10.1002/pon.1398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Debess J, Riis JØ, Engebjerg MC, et al. Cognitive function after adjuvant treatment for early breast cancer: a population-based longitudinal study. Breast Cancer Res Treat. 2010 May;121(1):91–100. doi: 10.1007/s10549-010-0756-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Tager FA, McKinley PS, Schnabel FR, et al. The cognitive effects of chemotherapy in post-menopausal breast cancer patients: a controlled longitudinal study. Breast Cancer Res Treat. 2010 Aug;123(1):25–34. doi: 10.1007/s10549-009-0606-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Plassman B, Langa K, Fisher G, et al. Prevalence of cognitive impairment without dementia in the United States. Ann of Internal Med. 2008;148(6):427–434. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-148-6-200803180-00005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Helfin LH, Meyerowitz BE, Hall P, et al. Cancer as a risk factor for long-term cognitive deficits and dementia. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2005;97:854–6. doi: 10.1093/jnci/dji137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Khan NF, Mant D, Carpenter L, et al. Long-term health outcomes in a British cohort of breast, colorectal and prostate cancer survivors: A database study. Br J Cancer. 2011;105:S29–37. doi: 10.1038/bjc.2011.420. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Baxter NN, Durham SB, Phillips KA, et al. Risk of dementia in older breast cancer survivors: A population-based cohort study of the association with Adjuvant chemotherapy. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2009;57:403–11. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.02130.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Heck JE, Albert SM, Franco R, et al. Patterns of dementia diagnosis in surveillance, epidemiology, and end results breast cancer survivors who use chemotherapy. J Am Geriat Soc. 2008;56:1687–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01848.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Graham JE, Rockwood K, Beattie BL, et al. Prevalence and severity of cognitive impairment with and without dementia in an elderly population. Lancet. 1997;349:1793–96. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)01007-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Dewey ME, Saz P. Dementia, cognitive impairment and mortality in persons aged 65 and over living in the community: A systematic review of the literature. Int J Geriar Psychiatry. 2001;16:751–61. doi: 10.1002/gps.397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. [Accessed December 8, 2011.];Health and Retirement Study. http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/
- 33.Nattinger AB, Laud PW, Bajorunaite R, et al. An algorithm for the use of Medicare claims data to identify women with incident breast cancer. Health Serv Res. 2004 Dec;39(6 Pt 1):1733–1749. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6773.2004.00315.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Gold HT, Do HT. Evaluation of three algorithms to identify incident breast cancer in Medicare claims data. Health Serv Res. 2007 Oct;42(5):2056–2069. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6773.2007.00705.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Yabroff KR, Warren JL, Banthin J, et al. Comparison of approaches for estimating prevalence costs of care for cancer patients: what is the impact of data source? Med Care. 2009 Jul;47(7 Suppl 1):S64–69. doi: 10.1097/MLR.0b013e3181a23e25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.McBean AM, Warren JL, Babish JD. Measuring the Incidence of Cancer in Elderly Americans Using Medicare Claims Data. Cancer. 1994;73(9):2417–2425. doi: 10.1002/1097-0142(19940501)73:9<2417::aid-cncr2820730927>3.0.co;2-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Lamont EB, Herndon JE, 2nd, Weeks JC, et al. Criterion validity of Medicare chemotherapy claims in Cancer and Leukemia Group B breast and lung cancer trial participants. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2005 Jul 20;97(14):1080–1083. doi: 10.1093/jnci/dji189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Herzog AR, Wallace RB. Measures of cognitive functioning in the AHEAD Study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1997;52(spec No):37–48. 20. doi: 10.1093/geronb/52b.special_issue.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Welsh KA, Breitner JCS, Magruder-Habib KM. Detection of dementia in the elderly using telephone screening of cognitive status. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 1993;6:103–110. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Iwashyna TJ, Ely EW, Smith DM, et al. Long term cognitive impairment and functional disability among survivors of severe sepsis. JAMA. 2010;304(16):1787–94. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.1553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Khatri M, Nickolas T, Moon YP, et al. CKD associates with cognitive decline. J Am Soc Nephrol. 2009;20:2427–32. doi: 10.1681/ASN.2008101090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Ofstedal MB, Fisher GG, Herzog AR. Documentation of Cognitive Functioning Measures in the Health and Retirement Study. [Accessed November 17, 2010.];2005 http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/sitedocs/userg/dr-006.pdf.
- 43.Bates D, Maechler M. lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999375-34. 2010 http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.
- 44.Plummer M. rjags: Bayesian graphical models using MCMC. R package version 2.1.0-5. 2010 http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=rjags.
- 45.Griggs JJ, Sorbero MES, Lyman GH. Undertreatment of obese women receiving breast cancer chemotherapy. Arch Intern Med. 2005;165:1267–73. doi: 10.1001/archinte.165.11.1267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Griggs JJ, Culakova E, Sorbero MES, et al. Effect of patient socioeconomic status and body mass index on the quality of breast cancer Adjuvant chemotherapy. J Clin Oncol. 2007;25:277–84. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2006.08.3063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen KA, Long JS, editors. Testing structural equation models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 2005. pp. 136–62. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Anderson-Hanley C, Sherman ML, Riggs R, et al. Neuropsychological effects of treatments for adults with cancer: A meta-analysis and review of the literature. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2003;9:967–82. doi: 10.1017/S1355617703970019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Faletti MG, Sanfilippo A, Maruff P, et al. The nature and severity of cognitive impairment associated with adjuvant chemotherapy in women with breast cancer: A meta-analysis of the current literature. Brain Cogn. 2005;59:60–70. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Jansen CE, Miaskowski C, Dodd M, et al. A metaanalysis of studies of the effects of cancer and chemotherapy on various domains of cognitive function. Cancer. 2004;104:2222–33. doi: 10.1002/cncr.21469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Stewart A, Bielajew C, Collins B, et al. A meta-analysis of the neuropsychological effects of adjuvant chemotherapy treatment in women treated for breast cancer. Clin Neuropsychol. 2006;20:76–89. doi: 10.1080/138540491005875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Crooks VC, Petitti DB, Robins SB, et al. Cognitive domains associated with performance on the Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status-modified. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2006;21:45–53. doi: 10.1177/153331750602100104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Jansen SJ, Otten W, Stiggelbout AM. Review of determinants of patients’ preferences for adjuvant therapy in cancer. J Clin Oncol. 2004 Aug 1;22(15):3181–90. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2004.06.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Fried TR, Bradley EH, Towle VR, et al. Understanding the Treatment Preferences of Seriously Ill Patients. New England Journal of Medicine. 2002;346(14):1061–1066. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa012528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Rosenbaum PR, Ross RN, Silber JH. Minimum distance matched sampling with fine balance in an observational study of treatment for ovarian cancer. J Am Stat Assoc. 2007;102:75–83. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Langa KM, Chernew ME, Kabeto MU, et al. National estimates of the quantity and cost of informal caregiving for the elderly with dementia. J Gen Intern Med. 2001;16(11):770–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2001.10123.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Langa KM, Larson EB, Karlawish JH, et al. Trends in the prevalence and mortality of cognitive impairment in the United States: Is there evidence of a compression of cognitive morbidity? Alzheimers Dement. 2008;4(2):134–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2008.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Alzheimer’s Association. Early Onset Dementia: A National Challenge, A Future Crisis. 2006 http://www.alz.org/national/documents/report_earlyonset_full.pdf.
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.