Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2012 Oct 18;7(10):e47428. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047428

Chemical Alarm Cues Are Conserved within the Coral Reef Fish Family Pomacentridae

Matthew D Mitchell 1,*, Peter F Cowman 1, Mark I McCormick 1
Editor: Timothy Ravasi2
PMCID: PMC3475700  PMID: 23094047

Abstract

Fishes are known to use chemical alarm cues from both conspecifics and heterospecifics to assess local predation risks and enhance predator detection. Yet it is unknown how recognition of heterospecific cues arises for coral reef fishes. Here, we test if naïve juvenile fish have an innate recognition of heterospecific alarm cues. We also examine if there is a relationship between the intensity of the antipredator response to these cues and the degree to which species are related to each other. Naïve juvenile anemone fish, Amphiprion percula, were tested to see if they displayed antipredator responses to chemical alarm cues from four closely related heterospecific species (family Pomacentridae), a distantly related sympatric species (Asterropteryx semipunctatus) and a saltwater (control). Juveniles displayed significant reductions in foraging rate when exposed to all four confamilial heterospecific species but they did not respond to the distantly related sympatric species or the saltwater control. There was also a strong relationship between the intensity of the antipredator response and the extent to which species were related, with responses weakening as species became more distantly related. These findings demonstrate that chemical alarm cues are conserved within the pomacentrid family, providing juveniles with an innate recognition of heterospecific alarm cues as predicted by the phylogenetic relatedness hypothesis.

Introduction

Accurate assessment of predation risk is vital to the success of any individual, as early detection of a predator enhances the chances of prey survival [1], [2]. However, to be successful antipredator defences must be balanced with other fitness enhancing behaviours (e.g. feeding and reproduction) [1]. This leads to a selective pressure on individuals to acquire information about current predation risks within their environment, in order to modify their antipredator behaviour to reflect their current level of risk. Such a strategy should optimise the trade-off between predator avoidance and other fitness enhancing behaviours [3], [4]. Individuals that are also able to detect and respond to alarm cues from heterospecific species that share a common predator will also have a fitness advantage [5], [6]. The use of heterospecific alarm signals in risk assessment is common across multiple taxa: birds [7], mammals [8], freshwater fishes [9], amphibians [10], [11], insects [12] and crustaceans [13]. Furthermore, information from heterospecific individuals may be more valuable than that from conspecifics, as heterospecific species may impose a lower competitive cost than a conspecific [14].

In aquatic systems, chemical cues along with visual cues are the primary sources of information for assessment of predation risk [15]. Released from specialised cells in the epidermis, following mechanical damage during a predation event, chemical alarm cues provide early warning of potential danger for other individuals within the local area [16], enhancing chances of survival [9], [17], [18]. The importance of chemical cues is highlighted by the simultaneous evolution of chemical alarm cues within most aquatic taxa found in both freshwater and marine environments (reviewed in [19]). They are of particular importance in complex or turbid habitats where visual cues are reduced [15], [20]. Unsurprisingly, prey also use chemical alarm cues derived from heterospecifics to gain information about local predation risks [9], [10], [21].

Responses to heterospecific alarm cues may arise from one of two non-exclusive mechanisms: 1) Individuals may possess an innate recognition of alarm cues common to closely related species (the “phylogenetic relatedness hypothesis”) [10], [11], [22]; or 2) Individuals may acquire recognition of relevant alarm cues through experience (the “ecological coexistence hypothesis”) [6], [22], [23]. The phylogenetic relatedness hypothesis proposes that alarm cues are conserved within taxonomic groups and thus individuals are able to generalise the recognition of their own alarm cue to those of closely related heterospecific species, as the composition of both alarm cues should be similar having been derived from a recent common ancestor [10], [11]. Individuals should therefore display a stronger antipredator response to closely related species and a weaker response to species that are more distantly related, irrespective of whether the species are allopatric or sympatric [11], [24], [25]. Strong evidence supporting the phylogenetic conservation of alarm cues is provided for grey tree frog tadpoles, Hyla versicolor [11] and Ostariophysan fishes [26].

In contrast, the ecological coexistence hypothesis suggests that responses to alarm cues from heterospecific species arise due to individuals co-existing with species that are part of the same prey guild [11], [23]. As both species share a common predator it is beneficial to respond to each other’s alarm cues as it will enhance early detection of a predator. Such responses may arise through learning as individuals gain experience with the predator-prey community in their local environment [21], [27], or they may be innately fixed through co-habitation with sympatric prey guild members over several generations [11]. Support for this hypothesis is often confounded by the use of wild caught individuals, as it is not possible to control for experience prior to collection. Consequently, it is not possible to make definitive conclusions about how observed responses to heterospecific alarm cues arose. Interestingly, two studies suggest that ecological coexistence may play an important role in modifying responses to phylogenetically conserved responses to heterospecific alarm cues [6], [23].

For fishes, how responses to heterospecific cues arise is still open to debate. The ability to acquire learnt recognition of heterospecific cues has been demonstrated across a wide range of fish taxa: minnows [28], sticklebacks [9], cichlids [29], gobies [30] and salmonids [24]. This suggests that ecological coexistence plays a significant role in acquiring recognition of heterospecifics at the individual level. However, support for the conservation of chemical alarm cues within taxonomic groups varies greatly. Of the taxonomic groups tested to date, alarm cues appear to be highly conserved within the superorder Ostariophysan (where the putative chemical alarm cue hypoxanthine-3-N-oxide has been identified [26]) and the salmonid family [24]. Other studies on wild darters, genus Etheostoma [31][33] and two species of coral reef gobies Asterropteryx semipunctatus and Brachygobius sabanus [30] provide inconclusive support for either hypothesis. Indeed, a more rigorous empirical assessment is still needed to address the phylogenetic relatedness hypothesis among fishes and other vertebrates, and the extent to which phylogeny determines the magnitude of antipredator responses. The answers to these questions are particularly important in understanding antipredator behaviour in species-rich habitats such as coral reefs.

Recent studies have highlighted the importance of chemical alarm cues in predator-prey dynamics for coral reef fishes, particularly for newly settled recruits [18], [34]. Recruits are exposed to a period of extremely high predation following settlement [35] and must rapidly learn to recognise predators to survive. During this period, chemical alarm cues play a crucial role in predator recognition [34], [36] and survival [18]. Given that many species recruit to reefs around the new moon period and are likely exposed to a similar suit of predators, the ability to access information from heterospecifics will facilitate the rapid acquisition of predator identities and increase an individual’s chances of surviving, particularly if they have an innate recognition of alarm cues from heterospecific species that share a common predator. However, to date only a goby, Asterryoptryx semipunctatus has been shown to be able to display antipredator responses to heterospecific alarm cues.

The aim of this study was firstly, to see if a common coral reef fish had an innate knowledge of heterospecific alarm cues at the time of settlement and secondly, to assess whether there was a relationship between the intensity of response to heterospecific cues and the extent to which they were related to each other, indicative of a phylogenetically conserved alarm cue. To do this we tested naïve juvenile anemone fish, Amphiprion percula (family: Pomacentridae), for an innate antipredator response to a range of chemical alarm cues from four heterospecific species within the pomacentrid family. They were also tested for their response to an alarm cue from a distantly related prey guild member, Asterropteryx semipunctatus, and a saltwater control. We then compared the intensity of the response to the heterospecific alarm cues to the time of divergence from the nearest common ancestor shared between A. percula and each of the heterospecific species.

Methods

Ethics Statement

Research was carried out under approval of the James Cook University animal ethics committee (permit: A1067) and according to the University’s animal ethics guidelines. Fish collections around Orpheus Island, Great Barrier Reef were carried with permission of the Great Barrier Reef Parks Authority (permit: G10/33239.1) and Queensland Government Department of Primary Industry and Fisheries (permit: 103256).

Study Species

Amphiprion percula is a member of the highly diverse and abundant Pomacentridae family that inhabit coral reefs throughout the tropics. While it is found in the same general habitat to the heterospecific species in this study they display distinct micro-habitat difference due to its symbiotic relationship with certain anemone species [37][41]. Consequently the extent to which they are exposed to their alarm cues should be similar for all species. Additionally, all species in this study are targeted by similar range of predators [42][44]. The heterospecific species were chosen based on their phylogenetic relationship to A. percula; Amphiprion melanopus is a closely related congeneric species, Pomacentrus moluccensis and Acanthochromis polyacanthus are both from different genera with the Pomacentrinae sub-family, Chromis atripectoralis is from the Chrominae sub-family, one of the most basal groups with the Pomacentridae [45] and Asterropteryx semipunctatus are from the distantly related Gobiidae family. All species are known to possess chemical alarm cues ([20], [30], [36], Mitchell unpublished data).

Collection and Maintenance

A. percula juveniles were captive bred and reared to settlement at the James Cook University aquarium facility, following the methods outlined in Dixon et al. [46]. Juveniles were maintained in three 40-l flow-through aquaria and fed 2/4 NRD marine food pellets (Spectrum Aquaculture) until they reached ∼20 mm in length, at which point they were large enough to use in the experiments. Captive breeding ensured that the fish would be completely naïve to the alarm cues of other species.

The five donor species were either taken from captive breed stocks or collected from the wild. A. melanopus and Ac. polyacanthus were captive bred at the university aquarium facility and reared to the same size as A. percula. All other species (P. moluccensis, C. atripectoralis and As. semipunctatus) were collected from coral reefs around Orpheus Island, Great Barrier Reef, Australia. Juveniles of each species were collected using hand nets and anaesthetic clove oil. All fishes were maintained in separate 40-l flow-through aquaria and fed ad libitum twice a day with 2/4 NRD marine food pellets (Spectrum Aquaculture).

Stimulus Preparation

Alarm cues were prepared fresh directly before being used in each trial. One individual per treatment was sacrificed by a quick blow to the head and placed in a disposable Petri dish. Using a clean scalpel blade, 15 superficial cuts were made along each flank of the fish. Fish were rinsed with 15-ml seawater and the solution was filtered through filter paper to remove any solid material.

Observation Tanks

Conditioning and recognition trials were conducted in 11-l flow-through aquaria (30×20×15 cm). Each tank had a 2-cm layer of gravel, a small terracotta pot (5-cm diameter) for shelter at one end and an air stone at the opposite end. An injection tube was attached to the air stone tube to allow food and odours to be introduced with minimal disturbance to the fish. A 3×6 grid (4×5 cm) was drawn onto the front of each tank. Each tank was surrounded on three sides with black plastic to visually isolate the fish and a black plastic curtain was hung in front of the tanks to create an observation blind.

Recognition Trials

Individual A. percula were placed into test aquaria and left to acclimate for two days. On the morning of testing fish were fed 30-ml Artemia solution (containing ∼200 individuals per ml) and left for at least 1 h before testing began. Trials were conducted between 0800 h and 1600 h each day. Prior to the start of trials the flow-through system was turned off and 10-ml of seawater were withdrawn and discarded from the tube, to remove any stagnant water, and a further 20 ml were withdrawn and retained for flushing. Trials consisted of an initial 2-min feeding period, a 5-min pre-stimulus observation period and a 5 min post-stimulus period. At the start of the 2-min feeding period 30-ml of Artemia were injected into the tank followed by 10-ml seawater to flush the tubing to allow feeding rates to stabilise. Once feeding rates had stabilised the 5-min pre-stimulus observation commenced. At the end of the observation period 15-ml of stimulus odour were injected followed by 10-ml of seawater for flushing and the post-stimulus observation period began 1 min later. The stimuli consisted of one of the five skin extracts or a saltwater control. Stimuli were assigned randomly to the tanks. Individuals were tested for their response to one skin extract only. A total of 150 fish were tested (18–20 individuals per treatment).

The behaviour of the focal fish was observed during the pre- and post-stimulus observation periods. We quantified two response variables: foraging rate and distance from shelter. Decreased foraging rate and distance from shelter are well known antipredator responses displayed by a number of prey species, including coral reef fishes [19], [36], [47]. Foraging rate included all feeding strikes irrespective of whether they were successful at capturing prey. For distance from shelter, the horizontal and vertical locations of the fish in the tank were recorded every 15s, using the grid drawn on the side of the tank. The position of the fish in the tank was then converted into a linear distance from shelter using the dimensions of the grid squares and Pythagoras’s theorem.

Identification of Phylogenetic Relatedness

To assess if the magnitude of an antipredator response to a heterospecific alarm cue is regulated by the phylogenetic relatedness of the focal species to the heterospecific species, we used the ‘time of divergence’ of our focal species (A. percula) and the most recent common ancestor (MRCA) to the heterospecific lineage in question. We made use of a recently published chronogram (time-calibrated phylogeny) of the family Pomacentridae [45] to find the divergence time of the MRCA of A. percula and the heterospecific alarm cue donors (Table 1). The pomacentrid chronogram was reconstructed using Bayesian age estimation methods and fossil calibration techniques (see methods [45]). It includes all of the pomacentrid taxa used in this study and all major lineages of the family Pomacentridae. The timing of divergence (TD) of each pomacentrid heterospecific from A. percula was taken as the age of the MRCA of both lineages (TMRCA), minus the age of the node representing the origin of A. percula (TAp; Fig. 1a; Table 1). This correction for the age of the A. percula lineage standardises the MRCA age to a metric that is specific to an ancestor node of A. percula.

Table 1. Divergence times of heterospecific lineages from A. percula.

Species MRCA age (TMRCA) MRCA distance (TDP)
(MY) (MY)
A. percula 2.7 (TAP) 0
A. melanopus 15.3 12.6
P. moluccensis 33.4 30.7
Ac. polyacanthus 34.7 32.0
C. atripectoralis 51.1 48.4

Ages are in millions of years (MY) before present and are taken from [45].

Figure 1. The phylogeny of Pomacentridae study species and antipredator response to heterospecific alarm cues.

Figure 1

The phylogenetic relationship and antipredator response of Amphiprion percula, to heterospecific family members (Amphiprion melanopus ¸ Pomacentrus moluccensis, Acanthochromis polyacanthus and Chromis atripectoralis), a distantly related sympatric prey guild member (Asterropteryx semipunctatus) or a saltwater control. a) A chronogram (modified from [45]) displaying the divergence times of the MRCA of the focal species, A. percula to each of the heterospecific donor lineages within the family Pomacentridae. Ages are calibrated to millions of years before present. b) The mean change in foraging rate (±SE) of juvenile A. percula exposed to the chemical alarm cues of five heterospecific species and a saltwater control. Fishes are ordered with respect to their relatedness to A. percula. Letters below bars indicate Tukey’s groupings.

Statistical Analysis

The proportional difference between pre- and post-stimulus behavioural observations were calculated and used as the raw data. The effects of test odour (the six fish alarm cues and saltwater) on foraging rate and distance from shelter of A. percula were analysed using individual 1-factor ANOVA’s. To account for ANOVA’s being run on two variables that were potential interrelated a Bonferroni adjustment was employed (adjusted α = 0.025). The ANOVAs revealed that only foraging rate was affected by test odour. The subsequent analyses were done on the foraging variable only. Tukey’s HSD post-hoc analysis was used to identify significant differences between responses to the test odours.

The relationship between the foraging response of individuals to the different pomacentrid chemical alarm cues and the divergence time between the different pomacentrid species and A. percula (TD) was investigated using a linear regression. Divergence time was used as the predictive variable and mean change in foraging rate was used as the response variable. For both analyses, the data was checked for outliers and residual analyses revealed that all data met the assumptions of homogeneity of variance and normality.

Results

Test odour had a significant influence on A. percula foraging rate (F 6,111 = 18.78, p<0.0001). Post-hoc tests showed that individuals displayed a significant reduction in foraging rate when exposed to alarm cues from conspecific A. percula and the heterospecifics A. melanopus, P. moluccensis and Ac. polyacanthus compared to the saltwater control and As. semipunctatus control (Fig. 1b). Individuals also showed a significant reduction foraging rate when exposed to C. atripectoralis compared to the saltwater control but not to the As. semipunctatus outgroup control. There was no difference in foraging rate between the saltwater control and As. semipunctatus, with feeding rate remaining constant throughout the trials (see Fig. S1 for mean pre- and post-exposure foraging rates). The 1-factor ANOVA on distance from shelter revealed there was no significant effect of test odour on A. percula (F 6,111 = 1.38, p = 0.23).

There was a significant relationship between the response to pomacentrid chemical alarm cues and the timing of divergence of the MRCA of the donor species and A. percula, which accounted for 66% of the intensity in antipredator response (r 2 = 0.66, F 1,88 = 16.72, p<0.001; Fig. 2). The greatest reduction in foraging rate was displayed by individuals exposed to alarm cues from conspecifics and A. melanopus, the intensity of response then decreased as the donor species became more distantly related (Fig. 2).

Figure 2. Relationship between antipredator responses and divergence times.

Figure 2

The relationship between divergence time from the most recent common ancestor and the intensity of antipredator response of juvenile Amphiprion percula exposed to chemical alarm cues from various heterospecific species within the family Pomacentridae. Circles represent in the mean change in foraging rate (±SE) of A. percula to chemical alarm cues of each heterospecific species.

Discussion

Our results demonstrate that juvenile reef fish are able to detect and respond to heterospecific chemical alarm cues and that chemical alarm cues are conserved within the Pomacentridae family. Naïve juvenile A. percula displayed a significant reduction in foraging rate, when exposed to alarm cues from conspecific and heterospecific family members but not to alarm cues from the distantly related sympatric As. semipunctatus or the saltwater control. Additionally, the intensity of antipredator responses to heterospecific alarm cues diminished as the timing of divergence between the heterospecific cue and A. percula increased. These results support the findings of similar studies on salmonids [24] and invertebrates [10], [11], [23]. However, this is the first to demonstrate a strong relationship between phylogenetic relatedness and response intensity to heterospecific chemical alarm cues for a vertebrate species. This strong relationship suggests that the innate recognition of heterospecific cues by A. percula resulted from phylogenetic conservation of alarm cues as predicted by the ‘phylogenetic relatedness hypothesis’.

The ability to recognise and respond to heterospecific alarm cues will confer a significant survival advantage for reef fish throughout their lives but particularly during critical ontogenetic life history changes. Following an initial pelagic stage, larval reef fish recruit to reefs in pulses around the new moons throughout summer [48]. During this transition to the reef they enter an environment rich in generalist, opportunistic predators [49] and are subject to extremely high mortality (up to 60% in first 2 days [35]). Several studies have shown that coral reef fishes lack an innate antipredator responses to predator odours with regards to short term changes in risk perception [36], [47], [50], although Vail and McCormick [51] and Dixon et al. [46] suggest there maybe some level of innate recognition of certain predators. In the absence of innate predator recognition, there will be a strong selection pressure to rapidly gain information about potential predators, risky habitats or time periods in respect to predation. Consequently, individuals that are able to detect and respond to heterospecific alarm cues will increase their chances of detecting an active predator in their local vicinity and enhance their chances of surviving any subsequent attack.

The finding that A. percula responded to all the heterospecific alarm cues but not to As. semipunctatus (a prey guild member) demonstrates that alarm cues are conserved within the pomacentrid family. There was a strong relationship between the intensity of the antipredator response and the time since each heterospecific species diverged from its common ancestor with A. percula. These results support the predictions of the phylogenetic relatedness hypothesis, matching the findings of a number of previous studies on salmonids [24] and invertebrates [10], [11], [23]. In contrast, other studies investigating antipredator responses to heterospecific alarm cues found that responses to heterospecific cues were highly variable and there appeared to be no support for the phylogenetic relatedness hypothesis and only tentative support for the ecological coexistence hypothesis [13], [30], [52]. For example, while As. semipunctatus responded to both conspecific cues and heterospecific cues from Gnatholepis anjerensis, G. anjerensis responded to only conspecific cues [30]. Similarly, studies on freshwater darters [33] and sea urchins [52] found inconsistent patterns in responses to both conspecific and heterospecific cues. However, the previously mentioned studies were confounded by the fact that they used wild caught individuals rather than naïve individuals. Consequently, any innate responses to phylogenetically conserved alarm cues (if present) may have been modified through experience with coexisting prey guild members, masking any response patterns indicative of phylogenetically conserved cues.

While there is the potential that ecological coexistence could have influenced the innate patterns of response observed here, we would suggest it is unlikely that it caused the responses observed. The heterospecific species in this study were selected based on the consistency of overlap in habitat preference and exposure to common predators between A. percula and the heterospecific donor species. Given this, if ecological coexistence was causing the innate response to heterospecific alarm cues we would have expected the responses heterospecific cues to be uniform irrespective of the time of divergence from their common ancestor with A. percula. Additionally, we would have expected individuals to respond to As. semipunctatus as well. However, as we were unable to include any allopatric pomacentrid species there is the possibility that ecological coexistence might have influenced the responses observed. Dalesman et al. [23] and Dalesman and Rundle [6] demonstrated that ecological coexistence with heterospecific species can modify responses to phylogenetically conserved cues in snails, both at the population level, through coexistence over several generations, and at the individual level, through short term changes in prey guild community structure. Ecological coexistence may therefore play a secondary role in determining responses to phylogenetically conserved cues.

The capacity of any species to use heterospecific cues may depend on a number of intrinsic (e.g. the ability to detect heterospecific alarm cues) and extrinsic factors (e.g. how the individual interprets the relevance of the information once detected). The ability to detect heterospecific cues is dependent on them being sufficiently similar to the focal species’ own cues for recognition to occur. As demonstrated here, the intensity of response to heterospecific cues is directly related to the time of divergence from the most recent common ancestor. Species may not recognise heterospecific cues simply because the time since the two species diverged from their common ancestor was sufficient for the cues to become unrecognisable. Similarly, the rate at which such changes to the chemical cues occur will determine recognition patterns. For example, it is thought that chemical alarm cues play a significant role in immune system function for fishes [53], [54]. The composition of the alarm cues may therefore be affected in part by the need to maintain immune system functioning. Consequently, changes in the composition of alarm cues may be driven by changes in the environment (and immune challenges) to which the individual species is exposed. Such drivers may cause a rapid change in the chemical alarm cue of species that have moved into a markedly different ecological niche.

Extrinsic factors, such as the prey species’ ecology and life history, or the composition and foraging strategies of the predator community to which they are exposed may also influence how they respond to heterospecific alarm cues. The diversity of predatory species and their preferred foraging mode will likely influence responses of prey to heterospecific cues. Prey exposed to generalist predators (abundant on coral reefs), which target a broad range of species within a preys’ guild, will benefit from responding to heterospecific cues. Conversely, prey individuals exposed to specialist predators that target discrete types of species (or ontogenetic stages) within the prey guild may not gain benefits of responding to heterospecific cues, especially if the focal prey is rarely targeted by that predator [23]. Furthermore, life history strategies have the potential to strongly influence responses to heterospecific cues. Hazlett and McLay [13] suggested that the extent to which various crayfish responded to heterospecific cues did not depend on phylogenetic relatedness, but rather on whether they evolved in specious regions and had the ability to disperse widely. The dispersive pelagic larval phase of reef fish may help to maintain a prey fish’s responsiveness to heterospecifics, through the necessity for conservative risk assessment when settling to an environment that is highly patchy and unpredictable.

This study demonstrates that juvenile A. percula have an innate ability to recognise and respond to chemical alarm cues from closely related heterospecifics. The patterns of response strongly suggests that responses to heterospecific alarm cues result from a conserved chemical alarm cue within the Pomacentridae family as predicted by the phylogenetic relatedness hypothesis. Given the similarities between early life histories within reef fish, such baseline knowledge will enhance their capacity to detect risky situations and learn about the predators present in their new environment during a critical period in their life history. However, these innate patterns of response may not be permanently fixed. Previous studies have shown that responses to alarm cues can change throughout development, particularly in regards to how individuals perceive heterospecific cues [55]. As prey grow not only does perception of risk change with experience [56] but they will move into new prey guilds composed of different prey species and are exposed to different predators. Consequently, the patterns of responses to heterospecific cues will change throughout their lives to suit their current situation, incorporating new prey guild members and modifying innate responses as the perceived value of the information changes. To further understand the complexities of the predator-prey interactions that affect community composition and diversity on coral reefs, future studies need to look at how perception of risk alters with development and experience.

Supporting Information

Figure S1

The mean foraging rates (± S.E.) of juvenile Amphiprion percula before (shaded bars) and after (open bars) being exposed to the chemical alarm cues from conspecifics and five heterospecific species and a saltwater control. A one-factor ANOVA revealed there was no significant difference in foraging rate between treatments foraging rates before being exposed to one of the odours (F 7, 140 = 1.77, p = 0.097).

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thanks the staff and the MARFU aquarium facility for logistic support.

Funding Statement

Funding was provided by the Australian Research Council (MIM) and the ARC Centre of Excellence for Coral Reef Studies (Grant No. CE0561432). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1. Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation- a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68: 619–640. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Mirza RS, Chivers DP (2001a) Do chemical alarm signals enhance survival in aquatic vertebrates? An analysis of the current research paradigm. In: Marchlewska-Koj A, Lepri JJ, Müller-Schwarze D, editors. Chemical Signals in Vertebrates, Vol. 9. Plenum Press, New York.
  • 3. Helfman GA (1989) Threat-Sensitive Predator Avoidance in Damselfish-Trumpetfish Interactions. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 24: 47–58. [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Lima SL, Bednekoff PA (1999) Temporal variation in danger drives antipredator behaviour: The predation risk hypothesis. Am Nat 153: 649–659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Chivers DP, Mirza RS, Johnston J (2002) Learned recognition of heterospecific alarm cues enhances survival during encounters with predators. Behaviour 139: 929–938. [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Dalesman S, Rundle SD (2010) Cohabitation enhances the avoidance response to heterospecific alarm cues in a freshwater snail. Anim Behav 79: 173–177. [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Fallow PM, Gardner JL, Magrath RD (2011) Sound Familiar? Acoustic similarity provokes responses to unfamiliar heterospecific alarm calls. Behav Ecol 22: 401–410. [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Shriner WM (1998) Yellow-bellied marmot and golden-mantled ground squirrel responses to heterospecific alarm calls. Anim Behav 55: 529–536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Mathis A, Smith RJF (1993) Chemical alarm signals increase the survival time of fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas) during encounters with northern pike (Esox lucius). Behav Ecol 4: 260–265. [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Schoeppner NM, Relyea RA (2005) Damage, digestion, and defence: the roles of alarm cues and kairomones for inducing prey defences. Ecol Let 8: 505–512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Schoeppner NM, Relyea RA (2009) When should prey respond to consumed heterospecifics? Testing hypotheses of perceived risk. Copeia 1: 190–194. [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Huryn AD, Chivers DP (1999) Contrasting behavioural responses by detritivorous and predatory mayflies to chemicals released by injured conspecifics and their predators. J Chem Ecol 25: 2729–2740. [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Hazlett BA, McLay C (2005) Responses of the crab Heterozius rotundifrons to heterospecific chemical alarm cues: phylogeny vs. ecological overlap. J Chem Ecol 31: 671–677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Seppänen J-T, Forsman JT, Mönkkönen M, Thomson RL (2007) Social information use is a process across time, space and ecology, reaching heterospecifics. Ecology 88: 1622–1633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Hartman EJ, Abrahams MV (2000) Sensory compensation and the detection of predators: the interaction between chemical and visual information. Proc R Soc Lond B 267: 571–575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Brown GE, Chivers DP (2005) Learning as an adaptive response to predation. In: Barbosa P, Castellanos I, editors. Ecology of Predator-Prey Interactions. Oxford University Press, Oxford. p. 34–54.
  • 17. Mirza RS, Chivers DP (2001b) Chemical alarm signals enhance survival of brook charr (Salvelinus fontinalis) during encounters with chain pickerel (Esox niger). Ethology 107: 989–1005. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lönnstedt OM, McCormick MI, Meekan MG, Ferrari MCO, Chivers DP (2012) Learn and live: predator experience and feeding history determines prey behaviour and survival. Proc R Soc B doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.2516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 19. Ferrari MCO, Wisenden BD, Chivers DP (2010) Chemical ecology of predator-prey interactions in aquatic ecosystems: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 88: 698–724. [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Leahy SM, Mitchell MD, McCormick MI, Ferrari MCO (2011) To fear or to feed: the effects of turbidity on perception of risk by a marine fish. Biol Lett 7: 811–813. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Mirza RS, Chivers DP (2003) Fathead minnows learn to recognise heterospecific alarm cues they detect in the diet of a known predator. Behaviour 140: 1359–1369. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Chivers DP, Mirza RS (2001) Predator diet cues and the assessment of predation risk by aquatic vertebrates: a review and prospectus In: Marchlewska-Koj A, Lepri JJ, Müller-Schwarze D, editors. Chemical Signals in Vertebrates, Vol. 9. Plenum Press, New York. p. 277–284.
  • 23. Dalesman S, Rundle SD, Bilton DT, Cotton PA (2007) Phylogenetic relatedness and ecological interactions determine anti-predator behaviour. Ecology 88: 2462–2467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Mirza RS, Chivers DP (2001c) Are chemical alarm cues conserved within salmonid fishes? J Chem Ecol 27: 1641–1655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Sullivan AM, Madison DM, Rohr JR (2003) Behavioural responses by red-backed salamanders to conspecific and heterospecific cues. Behaviour 140: 553–564. [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Brown GE, Adrian JC Jr, Smyth E, Leet H, Scott Brennan (2000) Ostariophysan alarm pheromones: laboratory and field tests of the functional significance of nitrogen-oxides. J Chem Ecol 26: 139–154. [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Chivers DP, Smith RJF (1994) Intra- and interspecific avoidance of areas marked with skin extract from brook sticklebacks (Culaea inconstans) in a natural habitat. J. Chem. Ecol. 20: 1517–1524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Brown GE, Chivers DP, Smith RJF (1995) Fathead minnows avoid conspecific and heterospecific alarm pheromones in the faeces of northern pike. J Fish Biol 47: 387–393. [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Golub JL, Vermette V, Brown GE (2005) The response of pumpkinseed sunfish to conspecific and heterospecific chemical alarm cues under natural conditions- the effects of stimulus type, habitat and ontogeny. J Fish Biol 66: 1073–1081. [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Smith RJF, Lawrence BJ, Smith MJ (1991) Cross-reaction to skin extract between two gobies, Asterropteryx semipunctatus and Gnatholepis anjerensis . J Chem Ecol 17: 2253–2259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Commens AM, Mathis A (1999) Alarm pheromones of rainbow darters- responses to skin extracts of conspecifics and congeners. J Fish Biol 55: 1359–1362. [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Haney DC, Vokoun JC, Noltie DB (2001) Alarm pheromone recognition in a Missouri darter assemblage. J Fish Biol 59: 810–817. [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Commens-Carson AM, Mathis A (2007) Responses of three darter species (Genus Etheostoma) to chemical alarm cues from conspecifics and congeners. Copeia 4: 838–843. [Google Scholar]
  • 34. McCormick MI, Holmes TH (2006) Prey experience of predation influences mortality rates at settlement in a coral reef fish, Pomacentrus ambionensis . J Fish Biol 68: 969–974. [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Almany G, Webster M (2006) The predation gauntlet: early post-settlement mortality in reef fishes. Coral Reefs 25: 19–22. [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Mitchell MD, McCormick MI, Ferrari MCO, Chivers DP (2011) Coral reef fish rapidly learn to identify multiple unknown predators upon recruitment to the reef. PLoS One 6: e15764. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Thresher RE (1985) Distribution, abundance, and reproductive success in the coral reef fish Acanthochromis polyacanthus . Ecology 66: 1139–1150. [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Elliott JK, Elliott JM, Mariscal RN (1995) Host selection, location, and association behaviors of anemonefishes in field settlement experiments. Mar Biol 122: 377–389. [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Elliott JK, Mariscal RN (2001) Coexistence of nine anemonefish species: differential host and habitat utilization, size and recruitment. Mar Biol 138: 23–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Depczynski M, Bellwood DR (2004) Microhabitat utilisation patterns in cryptobenthic coral reef communities. Mar Biol 145: 455–463. [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Wilson SK, Burgess SC, Cheal AJ, Emslie M, Fisher R, et al. (2008) Habitat utilization by coral reef fish: implications for specialists vs. generalists in a changing environment. J Anim Ecol 77: 220–228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Mariscal RN (1970) A field and laboratory study of the symbiotic behaviour of fishes and sea anemones from the tropical Indo-Pacific. Univ California Publ Zool 91: 1–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.St John J (1995) Feeding ecology of the coral trout, Plectropomus leopardus Serranidae on the Great Barrier Reef, Australia. Ph.D. thesis, James Cook University of North Queensland, Townsville, Australia: 277 pp.
  • 44. Graham NAJ, Evans RD, Russ GR (2003) The effects of marine reserve protection on the trophic relationships of reef fishes on the Great Barrier Reef. Eviron Conserv 30: 200–208. [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Cowman PF, Bellwood DR (2011) Coral reefs as drivers of cladogenesis: expanding coral reefs, cryptic extinction events, and the development of biodiversity hotspots. J Evol Biol 24: 2543–2562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Dixon DL, Munday PL, Jones GP (2010) Ocean acidification disrupts the innate ability of fish to detect predator olfactory cues. Ecol Lett 13: 68–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Mitchell MD, McCormick MI, Ferrari MCO, Chivers DP (2011) Friend or foe? The role of latent inhibition in predator and non-predator labelling by coral reef fishes. Anim Cog 14: 707–714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Milicich MJ, Doherty PJ (1994) Larval supply of coral reef fish populations: magnitude and synchrony of replenishment to Lizard island, Great Barrier Reef. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 110: 121–134. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Goldman B, Talbot FH (1976) Aspects of the ecology of coral reef fishes. In: Jones OA, Endean R, editors. The biology and geology of coral reefs. New York, NY: Academic Press. 125–154.
  • 50. Karplus I, Katzenstein R, Goren M (2006) Predator recognition and social facilitation of predator avoidance in coral reef fish Dascyllus marginatus juveniles. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 319: 215–223. [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Vail AL, McCormick MI (2011) Metamorphosing reef fishes avoid predator scent when choosing a home. Biol Lett 7: 921–924. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Vadas RL, Elner RW (2003) Responses to predation cues and food in tow species of sympatric, tropical sea urchins. Mar Ecol 24: 100–121. [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Chivers DP, Wisenden BD, Hindman CJ, Michalak TA, Kush RC, et al. (2007) Epidermal ‘alarm substance’ cells of fishes maintained by non-alarm functions: possible defence against pathogens, parasites and UVB radiation. Proc R Soc B 274: 2611–2619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Halbgewachs CF, Marchant TA, Kusch RC, Chivers DP (2009) Epidermal club cells and the innate immune system of minnows. Biol J Linn Soc 98: 891–897. [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Golub JL, Brown GE (2003) Are all signals the same? Ontogenetic change in the response to conspecific and heterospecific chemical alarm signals by juvenile green sunfish (Lepomis cyanellus). Behav Ecol Socio 54: 113–118. [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Berejikian BA, Tezak EP, LaRae AL (2003) Innate and enhanced predator recognition in hatchery-reared Chinook salmon. Environ Biol Fish 67: 21–251. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Figure S1

The mean foraging rates (± S.E.) of juvenile Amphiprion percula before (shaded bars) and after (open bars) being exposed to the chemical alarm cues from conspecifics and five heterospecific species and a saltwater control. A one-factor ANOVA revealed there was no significant difference in foraging rate between treatments foraging rates before being exposed to one of the odours (F 7, 140 = 1.77, p = 0.097).

(PDF)


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES