Abstract
Objective
To evaluate the hypothesis that children with craniosynostosis and their parents have differences in psychosocial outcomes, as compared with an unaffected control group.
Design
Two studies were conducted, both which followed children born with and without craniosynostosis. Study 1 ascertained affected children from clinics and Study 2 ascertained affected children from a population-based study of birth defects.
Participants
Study 1 included 22 children with single-suture craniosynostosis and 18 controls, ages 4–5 years. Study 2 included 24 children with non-syndromic craniosynostosis and 124 unaffected controls, ages 5–9 years.
Main Outcome Measures
Outcome measures included the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), Social Competence Scale (SCS), Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory (PedsQL™), and Parenting Stress Index (PSI).
Results
We observed lower scores on measures of health-related quality of life in cases versus controls, with adjusted effect sizes ranging from −0.72 to −0.44 (p<0.05) on summary measures. Small but statistically non-significant increases in behavioral problems were observed in cases versus controls, with no apparent differences in social competence or parenting stress.
Conclusions
Results provide preliminary evidence suggesting that children with non-syndromic craniosynostosis may have elevated risk for psychosocial difficulties, particularly health-related quality of life. Continued follow-up through pre-adolescence and adolescence is warranted.
Keywords: Craniosynostoses, Quality of Life, Psychosocial, National Birth Defects Prevention Study
Craniosynostosis refers to the premature fusion of the sagittal, metopic, coronal, or lambdoidal cranial sutures in the perinatal period. One or multiple sutures may be involved, and craniosynostosis may occur with other structural malformations or in isolation. Treatment includes surgical excision of the fused suture(s), which for most cases of isolated craniosynostosis is performed within the first year of life, with continued follow-up throughout childhood (Mccarthy et al., 2011). Surgical and non-surgical management of craniosynostosis ideally includes follow-up throughout childhood and adolescence by a multidisciplinary craniofacial team.
The importance of outcomes research for understanding long-term morbidity in children with craniosynostosis was recently highlighted by a report from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (Rasmussen et al., 2008), which noted the few studies comparing children with and without craniosynostosis and the need to clarify behavioral and psychosocial outcomes in this population. Previous studies have generally had methodological limitations such as small sample sizes (Kapp-Simon and Mcguire, 1997; Virtanen et al., 1999; Ozgur et al., 2006), and variable ages, including infants and adults (Sidoti et al., 1996; Boltshauser et al., 2003; Becker et al., 2005). Samples often included heterogeneous craniofacial anomalies, with relatively few craniosynostosis cases (Krueckeberg and Kapp-Simon, 1993; Speltz et al., 1993; Kapp-Simon and Mcguire, 1997), or did not include a control group (Sidoti et al., 1996; Bottero et al., 1998; Becker et al., 2005; Kelleher et al., 2006; Ozgur et al., 2006; Wong-Gibbons et al., 2009). In addition, standardized psychosocial measures were often not used (Bottero et al., 1998; Kelleher et al., 2006; Ozgur et al., 2006; Wong-Gibbons et al., 2009). Moreover, prior studies have mainly evaluated neurocognitive problems rather than psychosocial outcomes (Arnaud et al., 1995; Speltz et al., 1997b; Kapp-Simon, 1998; Panchal et al., 2001; Arnaud et al., 2002; Magge et al., 2002; Cohen et al., 2004; Speltz et al., 2004; Bellew et al., 2005; Starr et al., 2007; Van Der Meulen et al., 2008).
Parenting behaviors may be influenced by the experience of having an infant with a birth defect (e.g. stress due to diagnosis, surgery, possible infant mortality, and concerns regarding the child’s future). Furthermore, there is evidence of higher stress and care-giving difficulties in mothers of children with craniofacial anomalies than among parents of children with no craniofacial anomalies (Barden et al., 1989; Speltz et al., 1990; Speltz et al., 1994), factors which likely affect children’s psychosocial adaptation.
We present two studies in which we examined behavioral adjustment, health-related quality of life (HRQoL) and social competence in 4 to 9 year-old children with craniosynostosis, as compared with children without craniofacial anomalies. Reports of HRQoL were taken from both mothers and the children themselves. We also compared maternal stress in families of affected and unaffected children. Study 1 included participants recruited from an ongoing follow-up study of children with and without isolated single-suture craniosynostosis (Speltz et al., 2007; Starr et al., 2007). Study 2 included cases with non-syndromic craniosynostosis and a comparison group of unaffected children identified through the Massachusetts Center of the National Birth Defects Prevention Study (NBDPS) (Yoon et al., 2001).
METHODS
Sample
Study 1
The current cross-sectional analysis includes a sample of children who have been followed longitudinally since infancy as part of a multi-center study of neurodevelopment in children with and without single-suture craniosynostosis (Speltz et al., 2007; Starr et al., 2007). The two study groups included cases with isolated single-suture craniosynostosis (confirmed by computed tomography), and a comparison group without major congenital anomalies. Controls were initially frequency matched to cases by sex, race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status (SES) and study center. At the time of enrollment into the original study, cases had not yet undergone surgery. Because of the potential differences in psychosocial and neurobehavioral outcomes among children who do versus do not receive surgery, participation in the original study was limited to those planning to undergo surgery as part of their clinical care. The vast majority of affected children undergo surgery in infancy to improve cosmesis, and all cases in the current sample did have surgical treatment. Exclusion criteria for all participants included the presence of major malformations or ≥3 minor anomalies (Leppig et al., 1987); age >30 months at enrollment; age at delivery < 34 weeks; and major medical or neurological conditions (e.g., seizure disorders).
For the current cross-sectional analysis, participants at one of four study centers (Seattle Children’s Hospital) were invited for follow-up in 2007 and 2008, between the ages of 4 to 5 years. We attempted to contact 69 families from this cohort. Of these, 22 (61%) cases and 18 (55%) controls were enrolled and provided complete data. Non-participants included 4 active decliners, 17 passive non-responders, and 8 families who did not complete study questionnaires. Overall, non-participating families had lower SES based on the Hollingshead Index than did participating families.
Study 2
Mothers of subjects were originally identified and interviewed through the Massachusetts Center of the NBDPS, a population-based case-control study of selected structural anomalies that began in 1997 (Yoon et al., 2001). Controls were randomly selected from Massachusetts birth records during the same time interval, and included children born without major structural anomalies.
Cases selected for inclusion in the current study were children with craniosyostosis born between October 1997 and December 2002, after excluding those with known syndromes or chromosomal anomalies (as described in the NBDPS)(Rasmussen et al., 2008). Cases with both single and multiple suture fusions were included, as were children with other malformations. Follow-up data were collected for all subjects in 2007. Among eligible individuals, 24 of 37 (65%) cases and 124 of 225 (55%) controls participated. On average, non-participating mothers were younger, less-educated and less likely to be white as compared with participants.
Data Collection
Study 1
Families were contacted by phone when their child was between the ages of 4 to 5 years to determine study interest and to complete a brief phone interview. Interested families were mailed a consent form and study measures, which included the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (Achenbach and Rescorla, 2000; Achenbach and Rescorla, 2001) and Social Competence Scale (SCS) (Corrigan A, 2002). Sociodemographic data collected at baseline included each child’s birthdate, sex, and race/ethnicity. Data updated as part of the interview included maternal age and household SES (Hollingshead Four Factor Index) (Hollingshead, 1975).
Study 2
Study packets sent to mothers included an introductory letter, consent form, and measures of psychosocial functioning, which included the CBCL, SCS, Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory (PedsQL™), and Parenting Stress Index (PSI). Sociodemographic variables were available from the NBDPS, and included each child’s birthdate and maternal race/ethnicity, age and education level at delivery. Mothers were telephoned by study staff one week after the initial mailing. After four weeks, non-responders were recontacted by telephone, with a follow-up letter two weeks later.
Each study was reviewed and approved by an institutional review committee. Parents of participating children provided written informed consent.
Measures
The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) is a norm-referenced parent-report measure of behavior problems, with versions for ages 1½ to 5 years (CBCL/1½–5) (Achenbach and Rescorla, 2000) and 6 to 18 years (CBCL/6–18) (Achenbach and Rescorla, 2000; Achenbach and Rescorla, 2001). Parents rate the frequency of behavior problems on a 3-point Likert-type scale (0=’not true’ to 2=’very true or often true’). The CBCL/1½–5 includes seven scales: emotionally reactive, anxious/depressed, somatic complaints, withdrawn, sleep problems, attention problems, and aggressive behavior. The CBCL/6–18 includes the following sub-scales: anxious/depressed, withdrawn/depressed, somatic complaints, social problems, thought problems, attention problems, rule-breaking behavior, and aggressive behavior. Summary scores are derived for internalizing problems (e.g., anxiety, depression), externalizing problems (e.g., aggression, acting out), and an overall score. T-scores with a normative mean of 50 [standard deviation (sd) 10] are generated, with higher scores reflecting more severe behavior problems. Summary results for both versions have been combined and from this point forward will simply be referred to as ‘CBCL.’
The Social Competence Scale (SCS) (Corrigan, 2002) includes 12 items describing scenarios commonly encountered by young children in social situations, and has been validated in community-based samples and in children at high-risk for conduct problems (Corrigan, 2002; Gouley et al., 2008; CPPRG, 1995). The parent rates each item based on how well it describes the child, selecting from a five-point Likert scale ranging from ‘not at all’ to ‘very well.’ Scores summarize pro-social/communication skills, emotional regulation, and overall social competence. Low scores are suggestive of poor social competence.
Health-related quality of life (HRQoL) was assessed using both the parent and youth report scales of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory, Ages 5 to 7 years (PedsQL™ 4.0) (Varni et al., 1999; Varni et al., 2001). The PedsQL™ has been validated in samples of healthy and chronically ill children, and provides summary measures for different aspects of HRQoL, including physical, psychosocial and total health scores. Within the psychosocial composite, sub-scale scores can be derived for emotional, social, and school functioning. Mother and child each respond to items based on frequency of occurrence. Response options range from ‘never’ to ‘almost always’ on a three-point scale for children, and a five-point scale for parents. Low scores indicate poor HRQoL.
The Parenting Stress Index (PSI) (Abidin, 1983) was selected to evaluate the parent-child relationship and family functioning. Parents are presented with 89 statements for which they respond using a five-point Likert scale ranging from ‘strongly agree’ to ‘strongly disagree.’ Twelve additional multiple choice items with 4 or 5 options are given. The PSI Child Domain includes the following subscales: distractibility/hyperactivity, adaptability, demandingness, mood, and reinforces parent. The Parent Domain subscales include competence, social isolation, attachment to child, health, role restriction, acceptability, depression, and relationship with spouse. A Total Stress Domain combines the Child and Parent scores. Parents are also asked to identify stressful life events occurring in the 12 months prior to completing the questionnaire, selecting from a list of 19 events (e.g. divorce, promotion at work). Good reliability and predictive validity have been demonstrated previously, including in other craniofacial populations (Speltz et al., 1997a). High scores indicate elevated levels of stress.
Statistical Analyses
Sociodemographic characteristics of children with and without craniosynostosis were descriptively summarized. We additionally calculated the number and proportion of cases and controls scoring in the recommended ‘borderline’ and ‘clinical’ ranges on the CBCL (borderline >84th percentile; clinical >92nd percentile) and PSI (clinical >85th percentile).
Linear regression was used to compare mean scores in the case versus control groups. When comparing two groups (i.e. case versus control), results from linear regression are equivalent to those from standard t-tests of two independent samples. However, linear regression is more flexible, and allows for the estimation of case-control differences in the outcome variable (e.g. PSI Total Score) after adjusting for potential confounders. Adjusted models included sex and age (continuous) as covariates. SES [I(high)=55–66; II=40–54; III–V=8–39] (Hollingshead, 1975) was additionally included as a covariate for Study 1, whereas maternal education (<16; ≥16 years) was included for Study 2 analyses. Standardized effect sizes (Hedge’s g) were additionally calculated for each measure (Durlak, 2009); absolute values of 0.2 or less are said to reflect a “small” association, 0.3 to 0.8 reflect a “moderate” association, and those 0.8 or greater reflect a “large” association (Cohen, 1988). Case-control differences are presented with 95% confidence intervals (CI) to show their relative stability. There was no adjustment for multiple comparisons.
Sensitivity Analyses
We additionally conducted several restricted analyses: excluding cases noted to have mutations (Study 1); excluding cases with multiple suture involvement (Study 2); and excluding children with additional malformations (Study 2). Case-control differences were virtually unchanged for each sensitivity analysis, and are not reported.
RESULTS
Participant Characteristics
The mean age of Study 1 participants was 5.3 (sd 0.4) years in both study groups. Among cases and controls, 15 (68%) and 11 (61%) were male, respectively (Table 1). The sample was non-Hispanic white (>80%) or of mixed racial and ethnic backgrounds (<20%). Household SES was categorized as Hollingshead categories I or II for most cases (77%) and controls (94%). By design, all Study 1 cases had single-suture craniosynostosis. The largest subgroup included 10 (45%) sagittal cases (Table 2). One sagittal case had a novel genetic mutation and one unicoronal case had Meunke Syndrome (FGFR mutation).
TABLE 1.
Characteristic | STUDY 1 | STUDY 2 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
Cases (N = 22) N (%) |
Controls (N = 18) N (%) |
Cases (N = 24) N (%) |
Controls (N = 124) N (%) |
|
Sex | ||||
female | 7 (32) | 7 (39) | 9 (38) | 66 (53) |
male | 15 (68) | 11 (61) | 15 (63) | 58 (47) |
Age at Ascertainment, years | ||||
4–4.9 | 6 (27) | 6 (33) | -- | -- |
5–5.9 | 16 (73) | 12 (67) | 13 (54) | 40 (32) |
6–6.9 | -- | -- | 5 (21) | 33 (27) |
7–6.9 | -- | -- | 5 (21) | 29 (23) |
8–8.9 | -- | -- | 1 (4) | 21 (17) |
≥9 | -- | -- | 0 (0) | 1 (1) |
Race/Ethnicity | ||||
Hispanic Caucasian | -- | -- | 1 (4) | 4 (3) |
Non-Hispanic Caucasian | 18 (82) | 15 (83) | 23 (96) | 114 (92) |
Other or Mixed Race/Ethnicity | 4 (18) | 3 (17) | 0 (0) | 6 (5) |
Maternal Education (at time of Delivery) | ||||
≤12yrs | -- | -- | 5 (21) | 18 (15) |
13–15yrs | -- | -- | 2 (8) | 24 (19) |
16+yrs | -- | -- | 17 (71) | 82 (66) |
Socioeconomic Statusa | ||||
I (High) | 5 (23) | 4 (22) | -- | -- |
II | 12 (55) | 13 (72) | -- | -- |
III–IV (Low) | 5 (23) | 1 (6) | -- | -- |
Hollingshead 4-factor index of social status:(Hollingshead, 1975) I=55–66; II=40–54; III=30–39; IV=20–29; V=8–19
TABLE 2.
Suture Involvement | STUDY 1 | STUDY 2 |
---|---|---|
| ||
N (%) | N (%) | |
Sagittal | 10 (45) | 12 (50) |
Metopic | 4 (18) | 7 (29) |
Right Unicoronal | 3 (14) | 2 (8) |
Left unicoronal | 2 (9) | 0 (0) |
Lambdoidala | 3 (14) | 1 (4) |
Multipleb | 0 (0) | 2 (8) |
Lamboidal cases are single-suture
Includes bicoronal cases
In Study 2, cases were somewhat younger (mean 6.2 years, sd 0.8) than controls (mean 6.7 years, sd 1.1), and were more likely to be male (Table 1). The majority (>90%) of participants were non-Hispanic white. Maternal education level at the time of the study child’s birth was ≥16 years for approximately two-thirds of participants. Among cases, the most common diagnosis was single-suture sagittal synostosis (50%), with multiple suture involvement in 2 (8%) participants. (Table 2) Three metopic cases had additional malformations (diaphragmatic hernia, cardiac and limb defects, hypospadias).
Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)
In Study 1, mean CBCL T-scores ranged from 44.6 to 45.7 in cases, and 41.1 to 42.3 in controls, with adjusted differences of approximately 3.5 points for each comparison (aES 0.38 to 0.41; p>0.05) (Table 3). One case and one control scored above the 84th percentile on at least one CBCL summary measure. Of these two children, only the control scored above the 92nd percentile (not shown).
TABLE 3.
Measure | Study 1 | Study 2 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||
Case Mean (sd) | Control Mean (sd) | Adjusted Case-Control Difference (95% CI)a | Hedge’s G | Case | Control | Adjusted Case-Control Difference (95% CI) b | Hedge’s G | |
Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) | ||||||||
Internalizing | 45.7 (7.6) | 42.3 (10.2) | 3.5 (−2.1, 9.1) | 0.38 | 49.3 (13.1) | 47.7 (10.2) | 3.1 (−2.5, 8.6) | 0.29 |
Externalizing | 44.7 (7.4) | 41.2 (9.1) | 3.5 (−1.7, 8.8) | 0.40 | 48.3 (11.8) | 45.4 (10.4) | 4.5 (−0.5, 9.5) | 0.44 |
Total | 44.6 (7.6) | 41.1 (9.9) | 3.6 (−1.7, 9.0) | 0.41 | 49.1 (12.2) | 45.3 (10.6) | 5.4 (0.3, 10.4)* | 0.51 |
Social Competence Scale (SCS) | ||||||||
Total | 2.7 (0.5) | 2.6 (0.5) | 0.2 (−0.1, 0.5) | 0.32 | 2.3 (0.9) | 2.6 (0.7) | −0.3 (−0.7, 0.1) | −0.36 |
Likelihood Ratio Test p-value < 0.05
Adjusted for sex, age (continuous), and SES
Adjusted for sex, age (continuous), and maternal education (<16, ≥16 years)
In Study 2, mean CBCL T-scores were consistently higher in cases (range 48.3 to 49.3) than controls (range 45.3 to 47.7). Differences were accentuated after adjustment for age, sex and maternal education (aES 0.29 to 0.51), and were statistically significant for the CBCL total score (Table 3). Four (17%) cases and 8 (7%) controls scored above the clinical cutoff (>92nd percentile) on at least one CBCL summary measure, although comparisons were not statistically significant (not shown).
Social Competence Scale (SCS)
In Study 1, mean SCS total scores were 2.7 (sd 0.5) and 2.6 (sd 0.5) in cases and controls, respectively (aES 0.32; p>0.05) (Table 3). In Study 2, average SCS total scores were lower in the case (mean=2.3, sd=0.9) versus control groups (mean=2.6, sd=0.7) (aES −0.36), though differences were not statistically significant.
Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory (PedsQL™)
On average, Study 1 cases scored 6.3 to 6.5 points lower on PedsQL™ Child Report summary measures as compared with controls (aES −0.59 to −0.44, p< 0.05) (Table 4). Mean summary scores by parent report were 9.0 to 10.5 points lower in cases versus controls (aES −l0.72 to −0.64, p<0.05). Case-control differences were particularly notable for school functioning (child aES −0.79; parent aES −0.71; p<0.05), with statistically significant differences for parent-reported social functioning (aES −0.53, p<0.05).
TABLE 4.
Measure | Case | Control | Adjusted Case-Control Difference (95% CI) a | Hedge’s G |
---|---|---|---|---|
PedsQL™ Child Report | ||||
Emotional | 73.8 (22) | 74.5 (19.6) | −1.8 (−11.5, 7.9) | −0.09 |
Social | 75.5 (16) | 80.5 (16.6) | −5.2 (−12.2, 1.7) | −0.32 |
School | 71.3 (24.6) | 82.7 (15.5) | −13.6 (−23.5, −3.6)* | −0.79 |
Psychosocial Summary | 73.5 (18.0) | 79.1 (14.1) | −6.7 (−14.4, 1.0)* | −0.44 |
Physical Summary | 82.8 (14.4) | 88.6 (9.6) | −6.3 (−12.3, −0.4)* | −0.59 |
Total | 76.8 (15.2) | 82.4 (11.4) | −6.5 (−12.9, −0.1)* | −0.53 |
PedsQL™ Parent Report | ||||
Emotional | 68.3 (19.3) | 74.5 (15.6) | −6.0 (−14.1, 2.1) | −0.37 |
Social | 75.9 (22.4) | 84.7 (15.6) | −9.0 (−18.3, 0.2)* | −0.53 |
School | 71.9 (19.7) | 82.3 (16.2) | −11.9 (−19.9, −3.8)* | −0.71 |
Psychosocial Summary | 72.0 (17.1) | 80.5 (13.0) | −9.0 (−16.2, −1.8)* | −0.64 |
Physical Summary | 77.5 (21.0) | 87.7 (13.9) | −10.5 (−18.9, −2.0)* | −0.69 |
Total | 73.9 (16.5) | 83.0 (12.2) | −9.5 (−16.4, −2.7)* | −0.72 |
Parenting Stress Inventory (PSI)b | ||||
Child Domain | 96.3 (34.8) | 86.9 (21.2) | 10.5 (−4.0, 24.9) | 0.43 |
Parent Domain | 111.6 (31.1) | 111.0 (24.5) | 0.3 (−12.3, 12.9) | 0.01 |
Total | 208.0 (61.9) | 198.0 (41.4) | 10.6 (−14.3, 35.6) | 0.23 |
Likelihood Ratio Test p-value < 0.05
Adjusted for sex, age (continuous), and maternal education (<16, ≥16 years)
PSI Child Domain score missing for 1 control; PSI Parent Domain and PSI
Total missing for 3 controls
Parenting Stress Index (PSI)
On average, PSI total stress scores were somewhat elevated in cases (208) versus controls (198) (aES 0.23). This appeared to be driven by increased child domain scores (96 versus 87 in cases and controls, respectively; aES 0.43), with negligible difference on the parent domain (Table 4). However, none of the comparisons were statistically significant. Seven (29%) case-parents scored above the clinical cutoff on at least one PSI domain, as compared with 28 (23%) control-parents (p>0.05). Measures of life stress did not differ according to group status (not shown).
DISCUSSION
Among the various psychosocial constructs assessed in this research, the measure of HRQoL used in Study 2 provided the clearest discrimination of cases and controls. On average, the mothers of children with craniosynostosis reported lower HRQoL than did the mothers of unaffected controls, and children’s reports paralleled this difference. Differences were particularly evident in school functioning, potentially reflecting previously reported neurodevelopmental differences and elevated risk for learning problems (Becker DB et al., 2005; Bellew M, et al., 2005; Cohen SR et al., 2004; Kapp-Simon KA, 1998; Kelleher MO et al., 2006; Magge SN et al., 2002; Starr JR et al., 2007). Furthermore, the magnitude of the observed differences suggests that they are likely to be clinically significant (i.e., absolute effect sizes greater than 0.5), with implications for clinical screening and monitoring of patients with craniosynostosis. However, as with any parent report measure, it is possible that these differences reflect in part mothers’ concerns about the quality of their children’s lives as well as their observations of how their children function. Children’s reports of their own HRQoL cannot be considered fully independent of their mothers’ reports, as parental instructions and help were probably required for children’s completion of the PedsQL™ at home. Confirmation of these HRQoL findings is therefore needed, ideally using teacher reports of school and social problems as is suggested by the PedsQL™.
With regards to reported behavior problems among Study 2 participants, group differences were statistically significant only for the total behavior score. Although children with non-syndromic craniosynostosis had consistently higher behavior problem scores than controls, it is unlikely that such differences are clinically significant. Overall, the majority of children with and without craniosynostosis scored within the normative range on behavior measures, and on average did not show meaningful differences in social competence.
Study 1 cases also had higher average scores on the CBCL broad domains than controls, but very few cases or controls in Study 1 had behavioral scores which would be considered clinically significant. The magnitude of group differences was similar to those observed in Study 2. However, in Study 2, children with craniosynostosis were more than twice as likely as controls to score within the clinically significant range. As behavior problems tend to be inversely correlated with socio-demographic factors (Pike et al., 2006). This shift in the distribution of scores among Study 1 participants, as compared with participants in Study 2, likely reflects the relatively low social risk (e.g. high SES) in participating Study 1 families. Yet, even in this lower-risk sample, differences between cases and controls were observed.
Two recent studies of clinical samples containing older children with non-syndromic craniosynostosis reported elevated levels of emotional and behavioral problems in relation to CBCL norms (Becker et al., 2005; Snyder and Pope, 2010). However, we found little support for this impression of elevated psychosocial risk, at least among younger children in the present study, which is one of the first to include a demographically-matched control group. It is possible that the marginally elevated problem scores reported here among cases become more pronounced with age and academic advancement, a possibility that we will examine in a longitudinal follow-up of the Study 1 cohort at age 7.
Some investigators and clinicians have raised the possibility of association between autistic traits and craniofacial malformations (Tripi et al., 2008), including infants with isolated suture fusions (Ijichi and Ijichi, 2002). The magnitude of observed group differences in this study suggest that this is highly unlikely, although individual diagnostic evaluations would be required to rule this out with certainty.
In addition to a control group, this study extends previous work in other ways: it includes a broad assessment battery that evaluates behavior problems, both parent- and child-reported HRQoL, social competence, and parents’ stress and it replicates key outcomes in two independent samples. The use of a population-based sample in Study 2 makes this study unique in the literature, and helps to overcome some of the limitations of studies that had recruited participants through clinical programs (e.g., potential ascertainment bias, with more complicated or impaired cases more likely to require ongoing medical care). Our results are further strengthened by a series of sensitivity analyses, in which we excluded cases with characteristics that might be expected to affect psychosocial functioning, such as additional malformations, genetic mutations, and multiple suture involvement.
Both studies had limitations that could have affected results. Participation was higher for cases than controls, and varied according to demographic variables that may also affect psychosocial outcomes (e.g., SES). Individuals who were at highest risk for adverse psychosocial outcomes may have therefore been under-represented. Verification of our findings in future longitudinal studies with expanded effort to retain ‘high-risk’ families remains a priority.
CONCLUSION
Our report addresses research goals identified by a panel of experts on craniosynostosis research by specifically addressing scientific gaps with respect to psychosocial functioning in school-aged children. Observed differences in HRQoL persisted regardless of the presence of additional malformations, analytic restrictions based on sutural involvement, and across two studies with differing sampling frames. Healthcare practitioners should be aware that children with non-syndromic craniosynostosis may have elevated risk for psychosocial sequelae that affect quality of life. Research in the field is most likely to be advanced by longitudinal studies that track psychosocial development from pre-school to early adolescence and simultaneously seek to identify predictive and risk factors associated with poor psychosocial functioning among children with non-syndromic craniosynostosis.
Acknowledgments
Grant Funding
Study 1 was funded by the National Institute of Dental and Craniofacial Research (R01DE13813, PI: Speltz) and by a Steering Committee Grant at Seattle Children’s Hospital (“Behavioral and Social Functioning in Preschoolers with Single-Suture Craniosynostosis”, PI: Collett). Study 2 was funded by the Centers for Disease Control (U50/CCU 1132247, PI: Anderka)
This research would not be possible without participants in the Infant Learning Project (ILP) and the Psychosocial Outcomes in Early Elementary School-Aged Children with Craniofacial Anomalies Study (PSOCCA). We also wish to thank the following individuals for contributing to the success of these research studies: Eileen Mack Thorley, MPH; Jane Sheehan, RN, MSN; Alison Peluso, MPH; Diana Prise; Kristen Daniels; Sharman Conner, MA. The Quality of Life study described in this paper was carried out using the PedsQL™, developed by Dr. James W. Varni.
Contributor Information
Yona K Cloonan, Department of Epidemiology, University of Pittsburgh Graduate School of Public Health, Pittsburgh, PA.
Brent Collett, Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, WA. Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Medicine, Seattle Children’s Hospital, Seattle, WA.
Matthew L Speltz, Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, WA. Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Medicine, Seattle Children’s Hospital, Seattle, WA.
Marlene Anderka, Center for Birth Defects Research and Prevention, Massachusetts Department of Public Health, Bureau of Family Health and Nutrition, Boston, MA.
Martha M Werler, Department of Epidemiology, Slone Epidemiology Center at Boston University, Boston, MA.
References
- Abidin R. Parenting stress index manual. Charlottesville, VA: Pediatric Psychology Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for aseba preschool forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for aseba school-age forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Arnaud E, Meneses P, Lajeunie E, Thorne JA, Marchac D, Renier D. Postoperative mental and morphological outcome for nonsyndromic brachycephaly. Plast Reconstr Surg. 2002;110:6–12. doi: 10.1097/00006534-200207000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnaud E, Renier D, Marchac D. Prognosis for mental function in scaphocephaly. J Neurosurg. 1995;83:476–479. doi: 10.3171/jns.1995.83.3.0476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barden RC, Ford ME, Jensen AG, Rogers-Salyer M, Salyer KE. Effects of craniofacial deformity in infancy on the quality of mother-infant interactions. Child Dev. 1989;60:819–824. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1989.tb03513.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becker DB, Petersen JD, Kane AA, Cradock MM, Pilgram TK, Marsh JL. Speech, cognitive, and behavioral outcomes in nonsyndromic craniosynostosis. Plast Reconstr Surg. 2005;116:400–407. doi: 10.1097/01.prs.0000172763.71043.b8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bellew M, Chumas P, Mueller R, Liddington M, Russell J. Pre- and postoperative developmental attainment in sagittal synostosis. Arch Dis Child. 2005;90:346–350. doi: 10.1136/adc.2003.035824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boltshauser E, Ludwig S, Dietrich F, Landolt MA. Sagittal craniosynostosis: Cognitive development, behaviour, and quality of life in unoperated children. Neuropediatrics. 2003;34:293–300. doi: 10.1055/s-2003-44667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bottero L, Lajeunie E, Arnaud E, Marchac D, Renier D. Functional outcome after surgery for trigonocephaly.[see comment] Plast Reconstr Surg. 1998;102:952–958. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen SR, Cho DC, Nichols SL, Simms C, Cross KP, Burstein FD. American society of maxillofacial surgeons outcome study: Preoperative and postoperative neurodevelopmental findings in single-suture craniosynostosis. Plast Reconstr Surg. 2004;114:841–847. doi: 10.1097/01.prs.0000132854.14237.a8. discussion 848–849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corrigan A. [Accessed March 3, 2010];Social competence scale – parent version, grade 1/year 2 (fast track project technical report) Available at: http://www.fasttrackproject.org/techrept/s/scp/
- Conduct problems prevention research group. [Accessed March 3, 2010];Psychometric properties of the social competence scale - teacher and parent ratings. Available at: http://www.fasttrackproject.org/technical-reports.php#scp.
- Durlak JA. How to select, calculate, and interpret effect sizes. J Pediatr Psychol. 2009;34:917–928. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsp004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gouley KK, Brotman LM, Huang KY, Shrout PE. Construct validation of the social competence scale in preschool-age children. Soc Dev. 2008;17:380–398. [Google Scholar]
- Hollingshead A. Four factor index of social status. New Haven, CT: Yale University; 1975. PhD. [Google Scholar]
- Ijichi S, Ijichi N. Minor form of trigonocephaly is an autistic skull shape? A suggestion based on homeobox gene variants and mecp2 mutations. Med Hypotheses. 2002;58:337–339. doi: 10.1054/mehy.2001.1528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kapp-Simon KA. Mental development and learning disorders in children with single suture craniosynostosis. Cleft Palate Craniofac J. 1998;35:197–203. doi: 10.1597/1545-1569_1998_035_0197_mdaldi_2.3.co_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kapp-Simon KA, McGuire DE. Observed social interaction patterns in adolescents with and without craniofacial conditions. Cleft Palate Craniofac J. 1997;34:380–384. doi: 10.1597/1545-1569_1997_034_0380_osipia_2.3.co_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelleher MO, Murray DJ, McGillivary A, Kamel MH, Allcutt D, Earley MJ. Behavioral, developmental, and educational problems in children with nonsyndromic trigonocephaly. J Neurosurg. 2006;105:382–384. doi: 10.3171/ped.2006.105.5.382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueckeberg SM, Kapp-Simon KA. Effect of parental factors on social skills of preschool children with craniofacial anomalies. Cleft Palate Craniofac J. 1993;30:490–496. doi: 10.1597/1545-1569_1993_030_0490_eopfos_2.3.co_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leppig KA, Werler MM, Cann CI, Cook CA, Holmes LB. Predictive value of minor anomalies. I. Association with major malformations. J Pediatr. 1987;110:531–537. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(87)80543-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magge SN, Westerveld M, Pruzinsky T, Persing JA. Long-term neuropsychological effects of sagittal craniosynostosis on child development. J Craniofac Surg. 2002;13:99–104. doi: 10.1097/00001665-200201000-00023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy JG, Warren SM, Bernstein JM, Burnett W, Cunningham ML, Edmond JC, Figueroa AA, Kapp-Simon KA, Labow B, Peterson-Falzone S, et al. Parameters of care for craniosynostosis. Cleft Palate Craniofac J. 2011 doi: 10.1597/11-138. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ozgur BM, Aryan HE, Ibrahim D, Soliman MA, Meltzer HS, Cohen SR, Levy ML. Emotional and psychological impact of delayed craniosynostosis repair. Child Nerv Syst. 2006;22:1619–1623. doi: 10.1007/s00381-006-0148-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panchal J, Amirsheybani H, Gurwitch R, Cook V, Francel P, Neas B, Levine N. Neurodevelopment in children with single-suture craniosynostosis and plagiocephaly without synostosis. Plast Reconstr Surg. 2001;108:1492–1498. doi: 10.1097/00006534-200111000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pike A, Iervolino AC, Eley TC, Price TS, Plomin R. Environmental risk and young children’s cognitive and behavioral development. Int J Behav Dev. 2006;30:55–66. [Google Scholar]
- Rasmussen SA, Yazdy MM, Frias JL, Honein MA. Priorities for public health research on craniosynostosis: Summary and recommendations from a centers for disease control and prevention-sponsored meeting. Am J Med Genet A. 2008;146A:149–158. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.32106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sidoti EJ, Jr, Marsh JL, Marty-Grames L, Noetzel MJ. Long-term studies of metopic synostosis: Frequency of cognitive impairment and behavioral disturbances. Plast Reconstr Surg. 1996;97:276–281. doi: 10.1097/00006534-199602000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snyder H, Pope AW. Psychosocial adjustment in children and adolescents with a craniofacial anomaly: Diagnosis-specific patterns. Cleft Palate Craniofac J. 2010;47:264–272. doi: 10.1597/08-227.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Speltz ML, Armsden GC, Clarren SS. Effects of craniofacial birth defects on maternal functioning postinfancy. J Pediatr Psychol. 1990;15:177–196. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/15.2.177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Speltz ML, Endriga MC, Fisher PA, Mason CA. Early predictors of attachment in infants with cleft lip and/or palate. Child Dev. 1997a;68:12–25. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Speltz ML, Endriga MC, Mouradian WE. Presurgical and postsurgical mental and psychomotor development of infants with sagittal synostosis. Cleft Palate Craniofac J. 1997b;34:374–379. doi: 10.1597/1545-1569_1997_034_0374_papmap_2.3.co_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Speltz ML, Greenberg MT, Endriga MC, Galbreath H. Developmental approach to the psychology of craniofacial anomalies. Cleft Palate Craniofac J. 1994;31:61–67. doi: 10.1597/1545-1569_1994_031_0061_dattpo_2.3.co_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Speltz ML, Kapp-Simon K, Collett B, Keich Y, Gaither R, Cradock MM, Buono L, Cunningham ML. Neurodevelopment of infants with single-suture craniosynostosis: Presurgery comparisons with case-matched controls. Plast Reconstr Surg. 2007;119:1874–1881. doi: 10.1097/01.prs.0000259184.88265.3f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Speltz ML, Kapp-Simon KA, Cunningham M, Marsh J, Dawson G. Single-suture craniosynostosis: A review of neurobehavioral research and theory. J Pediatr Psychol. 2004;29:651–668. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsh068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Speltz ML, Morton K, Goodell EW, Clarren SK. Psychological functioning of children with craniofacial anomalies and their mothers: Follow-up from late infancy to school entry. Cleft Palate Craniofac J. 1993;30:482–489. doi: 10.1597/1545-1569_1993_030_0482_pfocwc_2.3.co_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Starr JR, Kapp-Simon KA, Cloonan YK, Collett BR, Cradock MM, Buono L, Cunningham ML, Speltz ML. Presurgical and postsurgical assessment of the neurodevelopment of infants with single-suture craniosynostosis: Comparison with controls. J Neurosurg. 2007;107:103–110. doi: 10.3171/PED-07/08/103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tripi G, Roux S, Canziani T, Bonnet Brilhault F, Barthelemy C, Canziani F. Minor physical anomalies in children with autism spectrum disorder. Early Hum Dev. 2008;84:217–223. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Meulen J, van der Vlugt J, Okkerse J, Hofman B. Early beaten-copper pattern: Its long-term effect on intelligence quotients in 95 children with craniosynostosis. J Neuros-Pediatr. 2008;1:25–30. doi: 10.3171/PED-08/01/025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varni JW, Seid M, Kurtin PS. Pedsql™: Reliability and validity of the pediatric quality of life inventory™ version 4.0 generic core scales in health and patient populations. Med Care. 2001;39:800–812. doi: 10.1097/00005650-200108000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varni JW, Seid M, Rode CA. The pedsql™: Measurement model for the pediatric quality of life inventory. Med Care. 1999;37:126–139. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199902000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Virtanen R, Korhonen T, Fagerholm J, Viljanto J. Neurocognitive sequelae of scaphocephaly. Pediatrics. 1999;103:791–795. doi: 10.1542/peds.103.4.791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong-Gibbons DL, Kancherla V, Romitti PA, Tyler MC, Damiano PC, Druschel CM, Robbins JM, Kizelnik-Freilich S, Burnett W. Maternal reports of satisfaction with care and outcomes for children with craniosynostosis. J Craniofac Surg. 2009;20:138–142. doi: 10.1097/SCS.0b013e318190e28c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoon PW, Rasmussen SA, Lynberg MC, Moore CA, Anderka M, Carmichael SL, Costa P, Druschel C, Hobbs CA, Romitti PA, et al. The national birth defects prevention study. Public Health Rep. 2001;116(Suppl 1):32–40. doi: 10.1093/phr/116.S1.32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]