Abstract
A robust literature links childhood sexual abuse (CSA) to later substance use and sexual risk behavior; yet, relatively little empirical attention has been devoted to identifying the mechanisms linking CSA to risky behavior among youth, with even less work examining such processes in boys. With the aim of addressing this gap in the literature, the current study examined the indirect effect of childhood sexual abuse (CSA; from age 2 to 12) trajectory group on risky behavior at age 14 (alcohol use & sexual intercourse) via the intervening role of caregiver-reported internalizing and externalizing problems at age 12. Analyses were conducted with a subsample of youth (n = 657 sexual intercourse; n = 667 alcohol use) from the Longitudinal Studies of Child Abuse and Neglect (LONGSCAN), a multisite prospective study of youth at risk for maltreatment. For boys and girls, there was an indirect effect from CSA to sexual intercourse through externalizing problems. The same pattern emerged for alcohol use, but only for girls. Findings did not support an indirect path through internalizing problems for either boys or girls for either outcome. Findings suggest more focal targets for prevention efforts aimed at maintaining the health and safety of maltreated boys and girls during the adolescent transition.
Keywords: Childhood sexual abuse, adolescence, risk behavior, internalizing problems, externalizing problems, gender
By some estimates, childhood sexual abuse (CSA) is reported almost 80,000 times a year in the United States (American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 2004); yet, these numbers clearly underestimate the actual occurrence given children’s disclosure fear, as well as difficulties in the legal procedure inherent in validating an episode. Individuals with CSA histories are at an increased risk for a range of adverse outcomes, including increased engagement in risky behaviors (e.g., Brown et al., 2010; Senn et al., 2008; Senn & Carey, 2010).
CSA may co-occur with other types of maltreatment; however, the link between other maltreatment types and risky behavior has received far less attention in both the theoretical and empirical literature and the findings are more mixed (see Senn et al., 2008 for a review). In contrast, a robust literature suggests that individuals with CSA histories initiate sexual behavior earlier, are more likely to engage in risky sexual behaviors once they initiate sexual activity (e.g., lack of/inconsistent condom use, sex with multiple partners), and to continue to engage in risky behavior into adulthood (see Beitchman et al., 1992; Kendall-Tackett, Williams, & Finkelhor, 1993; Senn et al., 2008; Trickett & Putnam, 1998, for reviews). In turn, those with CSA histories are more likely to experience the consequences of risky sexual behavior, including unplanned pregnancy, HIV/AIDS, and other sexually transmitted diseases (e.g., Clum et al., 2009; Mugavero, et al., 2007; also see Senn et al., 2008 for a review).
Importantly, some work suggests that those with CSA histories may be uniquely vulnerable to sexual risk taking behavior in particular, relative to those with other types of maltreatment histories and/or relative to other risk behavior outcomes (e.g., Kendall-Tackett et al., 1993; Senn et al., 2008; Senn & Carey, 2010). Other work, however, suggests that individuals with CSA histories are, in fact, also more likely to use substances in adolescence and into adulthood, a pattern of comorbid behavior that is thought to increase their vulnerability for sexual and physical revictimization (e.g., Barnes, Noll, Putnam, & Trickett, 2009; also see Classen, Palesh, & Aggarwal, 2005 for a review). Accordingly, programs for adults have shifted to focus on the overrepresentation of individuals with CSA histories among both the substance abusing and the HIV positive with the aim of ameliorating the psychosocial vulnerabilities linking CSA history to ongoing risk behavior (Clum et al., 2011; Hien et al., 2010; Sikkema et al., 2007; 2008; Wilson & Widom, 2011). Given that approximately one-third (34%) of all CSA victims are under the age of 12 (Bureau of Justice Statistics, 2000), the development of similar programs for youth is critical (Danielson et al., 2010); yet, such targeted programming depends on a better understanding of the mechanisms linking CSA to risk behavior, as well as whether risk is unique to sexual behavior or more generalized to substance use as well.
Although much theoretical attention has been devoted to explaining the mechanisms linking CSA to risky behavior (e.g., Briere, 1994; Finkelhor and Browne, 1985; Quina et al., 2003), extant empirical work in this area is “sparse” (Senn et al., 2008, p. 730). Research examining such mechanisms among adults with CSA histories has focused in large part on post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (see Senn et al., 2008; Walker, Carey, Mohr, Stein, Seedat, 2004 for reviews); however, a narrow focus on PTSD alone with youth may not be optimal given the preliminary state of the literature for several reasons. First, some disagreement exists regarding the measurement and, in turn, the predictive power, of PTSD in youth (e.g., Pynoos et al., 2009; Ruggiero & McLeer, 2000; also see Kearney, Wechsler, Kaur, & Lemos-Miller, 2009 for a review). Moreover, PTSD is typically comorbid with other anxiety, mood, and disruptive behavior disorders (Kearney et al., 2010), suggesting that an exclusive focus on PTSD may underestimate the extent of youth distress or the extent to which such distress links CSA to early risk behavior among youth. In contrast, the merits of utilizing a dimensional approach to examine child adjustment problems have been discussed extensively, with a primary focus on the capacity of such an approach to better capture subthreshold symptomatology and, in turn, to increase predictive power (e.g., Achenbach, 2009; Haapasalo et al., 2000; Lahey et al., 2004).
Capitalizing on advances in research design and quantitative methods (e.g., Jones et al., 2010; Noll, Trickett, & Putnam, 2003; Wilson & Widom, 2011), this study utilizes group-based trajectory modeling to examine internalizing and externalizing problems (age 12) as mechanisms linking CSA trajectories (ages 2 to 12 years old) to early risk behavior at age 14, a particularly vulnerable period for alcohol use and sex (e.g., NIAAA, 2003; US DHHS, 2007; Zimmer-Gembeck & Helfand, 2008). At the most basic level, early risk behavior may be conceptualized in at least two ways: 1). Consistent with Problem Behavior Theory, early risk behavior may reflect a broader constellation of acting out behaviors, fueled by the anger associated with CSA and its effects, in youth with externalizing problems; and/or 2). Early risk behavior may function as a way to numb negative feelings about the self and/or a strategy to induce feelings that have been numbed by the trauma among youth with internalizing problems (e.g., Donovan & Jessor, 1985; Jessor et al., 1995; Schofield et al., 2008). As such, it is predicted that CSA history will be linked to early risk behavior via increases in internalizing and externalizing problems.
In addition, whether the mechanisms linking CSA to risky behavior differed for girls and boys was also examined. Research on CSA and risky behavior has focused primarily on females and in particular women, with fewer prospective accounts examining girls and far less attention to boys or men (see Kearney, Wechsler, Kaur, & Lemos-Miller, 2010; Senn et al., 2008; Walker et al., 2004 for reviews). The dearth of attention to gender remains despite growing attention to gender as a moderator of developmental pathways in the field more broadly (e.g., Mash & Barkley, 2003; Bell-Dolan, Foster, & Mash, 2005; also see Zimmer-Gembeck & Helfand, 2008 for a review). Among youth with CSA histories, research on the link between gender and internalizing and externalizing problems is mixed (e.g., Coohey, 2010; Maikovich-Fong et al., 2010; Rind et al., 1998). Moreover, prior work on CSA focuses primarily on the main effects of gender on internalizing and externalizing problems; however, prevention and intervention programming depends on establishing a better understanding of the extent to which the intervening role of internalizing or externalizing problems operates differently (or similarly) by gender (e.g., Bagley et al., 1995; Coohey, 2010; Garnefski & Kiekstra, 1990).
As noted above, findings are mixed regarding whether CSA is uniquely a vulnerability for sexual risk taking or whether the vulnerability generalizes to other risk behaviors, including alcohol (e.g., Kendall-Tackett et al., 1993; Senn et al., 2008; Senn & Carey, 2010). Thus, although prior work with the same sample combined sexual intercourse and alcohol use into a single risk behavior index and demonstrated a significant link with CSA history (Jones et al., 2010), this study examined sexual intercourse and alcohol use separately. Moreover, alcohol use in particular was the focus of the current study given its relatively wide availability to youth in this age range (e.g., Johnston, O’Malley, & Bachman, 2003; SAMSHA, 2004).
Ethnic minority youth are overrepresented in statistics on sexual risk behavior in particular (CDC, 2009) and its consequences, including HIV, other sexually transmitted diseases, and teen pregnancy (CDC, 2011a; 2011b; 2011c). However, we are not aware of any theoretical or empirical literature suggesting that the link between CSA and sexual or other risk behavior via internalizing or externalizing problems would operate differently by race/ethnicity. Accordingly, we controlled for race, rather than examine racial differences, in the proposed model (Baumeister, 1988; Cole & Stewart, 2001; Hare, Mustin, & Marecek, 1994; Phinney, 1996).
Method
Overview
The sample used in the current analyses was taken from the Longitudinal Studies of Child Abuse and Neglect (LONGSCAN), a consortium of studies using common instruments and interview protocols, located at five different sites across the United States (see Runyan et al., 1998 for more detail on study design and procedures). Primary caregivers whose primary language was English or Spanish were included. One site included children who were at high risk for maltreatment (based on attendance at pediatric clinics serving high risk populations), two sites included children who had been reported for maltreatment, and two sites included both children who had been reported as maltreated and children who were identified as at risk, based on demographic risk factors. In turn, maltreatment status varied at recruitment: 65.3% history of maltreatment; 17.8% at-risk for maltreatment; and 16.9% without risk factors for or history of maltreatment. All sites used common protocols approved by their Institutional Review Boards. The baseline sample included 1,354 youth, with data collected at ages 6, 8, 12, 14, 16, and 18.
Participants
Participants included in the current analyses were youth who had maltreatment data from at least three time points (2 to 12 years old) (n = 832), as well as complete data for the intervening variables (age 12) and risk behavior outcomes (age 14), yielding an analysis sample of 657 for sexual intercourse and 667 for alcohol use. The subsamples were evenly split by gender and girls and boys evidenced similar levels of internalizing and externalizing problems and were equally likely to report alcohol use (see Table 1); however, girls were signfiicantly less likely to endorse sexual intercourse. Approximately half of the youth in each subsample were African American (55% in alcohol use subsample; 54% in sexual intercourse subsample). African American and Caucasian youth were more likely to report alcohol use than youth classified as “Other” due to relatively small sample sizes. African American youth were also more likely to endorse sexual intercourse than Caucasian or Other youth. Accordingly, we statistically controlled for race in the primary analyses, as noted earlier. Youth excluded from the final subsamples did not differ from youth in the larger sample on CSA status, risky behavior, gender, or race.
Table 1.
Full | Boys | Girls | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
% | M (SD) | % | M (SD) | % | M (SD) | |
Sexual Intercourse (n = 657) | ||||||
Sex (% yes)a | 20.7 | 26.0 | 15.8 | |||
Externalizing Problemsb | 54.3 (11.9) | 54.5 (11.1) | 54.7 (11.0) | |||
Internalizing Problemsb | 51.1 (11.1) | 51.9 (11.1) | 49.9 (11.1) | |||
Alcohol Use (n = 667) | ||||||
Externalizing Problemsb | 54.3 (11.9) | 54.6 (11.4) | 54.9 (11.0) | |||
Internalizing Problemsb | 51.1 (11.1) | 52.0 (11.1) | 50.0 (11.0) | |||
Alcohol Use (% yes)b | 25.9 | 24.5 | 27.0 |
Girls were significantly less likely to endorse sexual intercourse than boys, χ2 = 11.8, p < .001
No significant gender difference
Procedure
All interviews were conducted by project-trained interviewers after consent was obtained from the primary caregivers and youth assented to be interviewed. The interviews were approximately 2 hours in duration, after which caregivers and children were compensated for their time. Children were included in data collection as early as the age 4 interviews; with increasing inclusion of self-report measures as the children aged. Beginning at age 12, children and caregivers were interviewed using an Audio-Computer Assisted Self-Interview (A-CASI), a program which both shows and reads the self-report questions aloud and allows participants to select the response on the computer keyboard that corresponds to their answer, maximizing confidentiality and reducing the potential for biased responses. Maltreatment data were collected from lifetime reviews of CPS records, which were collected and coded on regular intervals at each site (for a more complete description of LONGSCAN, see Runyan et al., 1998).
Measures
In order to decrease the likelihood of a common reporter inflating associations, the current study utilized different reporter/methods for each of the study variables: CSA (CPS records), internalizing and externalizing problems (caregiver-report), and risky behavior (youth-report).
CSA
Official CPS records, including both allegations and substantiations, of child sexual abuse were coded using a LONGSCAN modified version of Barnett et al.’s (1993) Maltreatment Classification System (MMCS; English & the LONGSCAN Investigators, 1997; as modified from Barnett, Manly & Cicchetti, 1993). Narrative reports made to CPS for maltreatment from birth to age 12 were reviewed, abstracted, and coded. The MMCS has been used extensively in coding maltreatment data across studies and is accepted as a reliable classification of maltreatment experiences based on CPS records (e.g., Dubowitz, Pitts, Litrownik, Cox, Runyan, & Black, 2005; English, Graham, Litrownik, Everson, & Bangdiwala, 2005; Litrownik, Lau, English, Briggs, Newton, Romney, & Dubowitz, 2005). LONGSCAN coders across sites were trained to 90% agreement with a gold standard coder, and a subsequent reliability assessment utilizing a sample of reports from all the sites indicated good overall agreement on the coding of type (all kappas > .7). Prior work with LONGSCAN data (Hussey et al., 2005), as well as other reports (e.g., Kohl, Jonson-Reid, & Drake, 2009; also see Senn et al., 2008 for a review), suggest that allegations may be a better indicator of actual experience. Based on these findings, CSA allegations codes (0 = no; 1 = yes) for each 2-year interval (baseline to age 12) were utilized.
Internalizing and externalizing problems
To assess internalizing and externalizing problems, the proposed intervening variables, the youth’s primary caregiver completed the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 (CBCL; Achenbach, 1991) at the age 12 visit. The CBCL includes 113 problem items that range from not true = 0 to true = 2. The CBCL includes broadband scales for internalizing problems, composed of three syndromes (withdrawn, somatic complaints, anxious/depressed), and externalizing problems, compromised of attention problems, rule-breaking behavior, and aggressive behavior. Although raw scores have been suggested for research because the standardized scores are truncated at the low-end, this problem is less of an issue in high-risk and clinical samples, including youth with maltreatment histories, for which scores at the lower end of the continuum are less expected and of less interest (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1991; also see Heflinger, Simpkins, & Combs-Orme, 2000, for a review). Accordingly, T-scores were used in this investigation with higher T-scores reflecting more internalizing and externalizing problems. The CBCL has very good test-retest reliability, internal consistency, reliability, and construct validity (Achenbach, 1991).
Some authors have cautioned that caregivers frequently underreport symptoms of their children (e.g., Angold, 1988; Kamphaus & Frick, 1996; March, 1999). Such caution may be particularly notable for internalizing symptoms, given that there may be fewer observable symptoms. That said, prior research with families of youth with trauma histories suggests that caregiver-reports on the CBCL for both internalizing and externalizing symptoms are consistent with clinical interviews (Saigh, Yasik, Oberfield, Halamandaris, & McHugh, 2002)
Alcohol use and sexual intercourse
The age 14 A-CASI interview is the first assessment at which adolescents reported on their history of alcohol use and sexual intercourse. Of note, early alcohol use, defined elsewhere as ranging from 9 to 15 years of age (e.g., NIAAA, 2003; US DHHS, 2007), as well as use by middle school age adolescents (which includes 14 y.o.), is considered particularly problematic because it is associated with later use, problem use, and associated consequences (e.g., D’Amico & McCarthy, 2006; Johnston et al., 2005; Simoes et al., 2008). For the current study, the item on alcohol use was taken from the Alcohol Module of the Computerized Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (C-DISC; Shaffer et al., 2000). The question used in the current study to assess alcohol use was preceded by the instruction “The next questions are about your use of alcohol - beer, wine, wine coolers, or hard liquors like vodka, gin, or whiskey. Each can or bottle of beer, glass of wine or wine cooler, shots of liquor, or mixed drink with liquor in it counts as one drink”. Then, youth were asked “Not including sips from another person’s drink, have you ever, in your whole life, even once, had a drink?” Responses were coded as either “yes” or “no”. Assessment of lifetime use is typical in the literature, given the relatively low rates of use by most youth of this age, even in high-risk samples (e.g., Schofield et al., 2008; Mrug & Windle, 2009; Taylor & Kliewer, 2006).
The item assessing intercourse (“Have you ever had sex?”) was taken from the Adolescent Sexual Experiences Measure (LONGSCAN Investigators, 1998). This item was introduced by a statement defining sex as sexual intercourse. Qualitative anlayses in prior work suggest that youth interpret this question as indicating vaginal intercourse (Stanton et al., 1995). Although the specific cut-point at which the initiation of sexual intercourse is considered risky typically ranges from 13 to 15 y.o. (or younger), sexual intercourse by age 14 has been defined as “early initiation” and, therefore, by definition any sex in this age range has been considered risky (e.g., Schofield, Bierman, Heinrichs, Nix, & the Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group, 2008; Wilson & Widom, 2008; Zimmer-Gembeck & Helfand, 2008). Sexual intercourse was also coded as “yes” or “no”, due to insufficient variability on other items (e.g., condom use).
Results
Plan of Analyses
The group-based trajectory analyses describing the pattern of CSA over the course of childhood (2 to 12 years old) yielded the same pattern of findings regardless of gender or subsample (i.e., % belonging to each trajectory; posterior probability; and the shape of the model); therefore, trajectory analyses for boys and girls together are described first. Then, subsequent path analytic models, covarying race and examining the primary study hypotheses, are presented (i.e., CSA group-based trajectories as the independent variable, internalizing and externalizing problems as the proposed intervening variables, and, sexual intercourse or alcohol use as the outcome of interest). Finally, the moderating role of gender was examined.
CSA Trajectories
Our aim in utilizing group-based trajectory analyses to examine CSA history over the course of childhood was to afford an empirical means of identifying clusters of individuals following different courses of development. The developmental trajectories for CSA were estimated using group-based trajectory modeling, covarying for site (PROC TRAJ; Jones, Nagin, & Roeder, 2001; Nagin, 2005; see also Jones et al., 2010 in which trajectories for CSA in LONGSCAN were initially reported). Given the dichotomous nature of the allegations variable (i.e., presence or absence of CPS allegation of CSA at each time point), the logistic (LOGIT) model was used to model the conditional distribution of data given group membership. For each child, the model’s coefficient estimates were used to calculate the probability that the child belongs to each group. Following the recommendations of Nagin et al. (e.g., Jones, Nagin, & Roeder, 2001; Nagin, 2005), the model with the fewest groups and the largest Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) was selected.
In turn, a two-group model yielded the maximum BIC (i.e., least negative) (BIC = −378.44) (see Figure 1). The “no allegations” trajectory reflected youth for whom there was no allegation of CSA (85.4% of the sexual intercourse subsample, 85.6% of the alcohol use subsample). The remaining youth were best described by a single curvilinear “allegations” trajectory, with the most allegations occurring between 4 and 8 y.o., with fewer allegations at relatively younger and older ages (14.6% of sexual intercourse subsample; 14.4% of alcohol use subsample). Consistent with data suggesting that CSA cuts across racial and ethnic groups (Finkelhor, Hammer, Sedlak, 2004; Sedlack & Broadhurst, 1996), youth in the CSA trajectory group were racially diverse (e.g., 30% African American).
Path Model Linking CSA to Risky Behavior via Internalizing and Externalizing Problems
The CSA trajectories were then used in a series of path analyses, an extension of the regression model, using MPlus software (Muthen & Muthen, 2008), which allows for the simultaneous testing of multiple relationships. The guidelines set forth by Preacher, Rucker, and Hayes (2007) were utilized to test the proposed indirect effects (i.e., whether the CSA trajectory groups operated through internalizing and externalizing problems to influence risk behavior outcome), as well as the proposed conditional indirect effects (i.e., whether the indirect effects via internalizing &/or externalizing problems varied by gender), covarying child race (see Figure 2). Consistent with Model 5 in the discussion of moderated mediation by Preacher, Rucker, & Hayes (2007), we tested the moderated effect of gender on (a) the effect of CSA on internalizing and externalizing problems and on (b) the effects of internalizing and externalizing problems on risky behavior (also see Baron & Kenny, 1986; Muller et al., 2005).
As noted elsewhere, indirect effects no longer require a main or direct effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable (i.e., CSA trajectory group on risky behavior) (see Hayes, 2009 for a review). However, Hayes suggests referring to indirect effects that occur without direct effects as “intervening variables”, rather than “mediators”. Indirect effects are significant if the 95% Bias Corrected and accelerated confidence intervals do not include 0 (Preacher & Hayes, 2004; Preacher et al., 2007). Although prior work with this sample combined sexual intercourse and alcohol use into a single risk behavior index (Jones et al., 2010), as noted previously, the risk behavior outcomes were examined separately in this investigation.
After controlling for race (β = .27; 95% CI: 0.039 to 0.500), path analyses failed to reveal a significant direct effect of CSA trajectory group on youth-reported alcohol use at age 14 for boys (β = .33, n.s.) or girls (β = .31, n.s.) (path c in Figure 2). However, the link from CSA to alcohol use via caregiver-reported externalizing problems (i.e., the indirect effect; path w2) was significant for girls (95% CI: .005 to .335), but not for boys (95% CI: −.134 to .308). For girls, the CSA allegations trajectory group, but not the no allegations trajectory, operated through externalizing problems at age 12 to influence lifetime alcohol use reported at age 14. In contrast, the indirect path via internalizing problems was non-significant for boys (95% CI: −.112 to .293) and for girls (95% CI: −.250 to .002)
There was also not a direct effect of CSA trajectory on youth-reported sexual intercourse for boys (β = .20, n.s.) or girls (β = .47, n.s.) (path c in Figure 2), after controlling for race (β = −.55; 95% CI:− 0.846 to − 0.275). However, the link from CSA trajectory group to sexual intercourse via caregiver-reported externalizing problems (i.e., the indirect effect; path w2) was significant for both boys (95% CI: .006 to .494) and girls (95% CI: .054 to .613). The allegations trajectory, but not the no allegations trajectory, operated through externalizing problems to influence sexual intercourse. The indirect path via internalizing problems for sexual intercourse was non-significant for boys (95% CI: −.422 to .018) and girls (95% CI: −.169 to .106)
Discussion
A robust and consistent literature links CSA in particular, relative to other forms of maltreatment, to later risk behavior, with the bulk of this work to date examining adult women and much of it retrospective. Relatively less attention has been devoted to examining the mechanisms accounting for this link (Senn et al., 2008). With the aim of moving toward the identification of potential targets for prevention and intervention programming, this prospective study examined whether CSA trajectories operated through internalizing and/or externalizing problems to influence adolescent risk behavior at age 14, a vulnerable period of development for risk behavior onset (e.g., NIAAA, 2003; US DHHS, 2007; Zimmer-Gembeck & Helfand, 2008). Of note, findings varied by gender, intervening variable, and risk behavior outcome.
First, it was predicted that CSA allegation group would be linked to risky behavior in early adolescence via increases in internalizing and externalizing problems. Although CSA was not directly associated with either alcohol use or sexual intercourse in the current study, findings revealed an indirect effect via externalizing problems for boys (sexual intercourse) and girls (alcohol use & sexual intercourse). This pattern is consistent with Problem Behavior Theory, which asserts that delinquency and risky behaviors form a syndrome of problematic behavior, which at its core reflects a detachment from convention (e.g., Donovan & Jessor, 1985; Jessor et al., 1995; Schofield et al., 2008). Youth with CSA histories may be especially vulnerable to this sense of detachment given the potential betrayal and stigma associated with CSA, as well as caregiver responses to the CSA and its disclosure (e.g., Finkelhor &Browne, 1985; Spaccarelli’s, 1994; Quinn et al., 2003). Specific aspects of externalizing problems, particularly impulsivity and inattention, may also exacerbate the potential for risk behavior by inhibiting control, planning, and decision-making (e.g., Crockett et al., 2006; McLeod & Knight, 2010; Zimmer-Gembeck et al., 2004), especially during early adolescence when spending time with peers is normative (e.g., Cicchetti & Rizley, 1981; Cicchetti & Toth, 1995).
Yet, although CSA influenced both risk behaviors via externalizing problems for girls, among boys the intervening role of externalizing problems was specific to sexual intercourse. As noted earlier, this idea of the specificity of the link between CSA and sexual intercourse in particular has been raised elsewhere, although the findings are mixed (e.g., Kendall-Tackett et al., 1993; Senn et al., 2008; Senn & Carey, 2010). Given that our findings regarding specificity are unique to boys, it is possible that inconsistencies in the prior literature is a result of a predominant focus on women and girls in this literature, with fewer investigations of boys or men and even fewer studies examining vulnerabilities by gender within the same analyses. In addition, CSA may operate through externalizing problems to influence the escalation (e.g., getting drunk), rather than lifetime use, in boys. As noted earlier, the current study did not have enough variability in risk behavior outcomes to examine more nuanced risk behavior outcomes. Third, CSA may operate indirectly on the initiation and/or escalation alcohol use for boys through more specific diagnoses (e.g., conduct disorder) or more specific aspects of externalizing problems (e.g., problems with effortful control) more common among boys (see Else-Quest et al., 2006; Zimmer-Gembeck & Helfand, 2008 for reviews). Finally, it may be that other intervening variables link CSA to alcohol use in boys (e.g., deviant peer group). These potential explanations are merely hypotheses meriting future studies among youth with CSA histories.
In addition to externalizing problems, we predicted that CSA group would also operate through internalizing problems to influence early risk behavior. This hypothesis stems from the conceptualization of risk behavior as a strategy to numb negative feelings about the self and/or a strategy to induce feelings that have been numbed by the trauma. Prior research suggests that both youth and adults with CSA histories evidence a vulnerability for internalizing problems (e.g., Coohey, 2010; Garnefski & Arends, 1998; Senn et al., 2008). Findings linking internalizing problems to risk behavior, however, have been less consistent in the broader literature than findings for externalizing problems (e.g., Abrantes, Strong, Ramsey, Kazura, & Brown, 2006; Brown et al., 2010).
With regard to youth with CSA histories in the age range of focus in this investigation, several explanations may help to explain the null findings regarding the indirect effect via internalizing problems. First, internalizing problems typically lead to behavioral deactivation rather than activation, decreasing the number and range of activities of youth and opportunities for risky behavior involvement. In addition, depending on the severity of the internalizing symptoms (e.g., suicidal ideation), youth with internalizing symptoms may be more closely monitored by caretakers, precluding the opportunity to engage in risky behavior. Third, internalizing and externalizing problems were both reported by caregivers; however, sensitivity to internalizing symptoms in particular suggests a level of involvement and awareness on the part of caregivers that may be a marker of a positive parenting style that also includes knowledge of the youth’s activities, decreasing the opportunity for alcohol use or sexual intercourse. That said, a re-analysis of our model with youth-report, rather than caregiver-report, of internalizing symptoms yielded the same pattern of findings [i.e., CSA did not operate through youth-reported internalizing symptoms to influence sexual intercourse (girls CI: −.17 to .11; boys CI: −.42 to .02) or alcohol use (girls CI: −.25 to .01; boys CI: −.11 to .29). Fifth, youth whose internalizing problems stem from a sense of betrayal or stigmatization associated with the CSA experience may be less likely to risk the involvement and relationships with peers associated with increased opportunity for alcohol use and sexual intercourse (Finkelor & Browne, 1985). Sixth, some work suggests that internalizing problems may lead to escalation (e.g., heavy or problematic alcohol use), but not the initiation, of risk behavior, as well as other substance use (e.g., tobacco) (Lewis et al., 2011; also see Colder, Chassin, Lee, & Villalta, 2010 for a review). Finally, internalizing problems are more likely than externalizing problems to onset in adolescence and are more common in girls (see Zahn-Waxler, Shirtcliff, & Marceau, 2008). Thus, internalizing problems may play an intervening role later in adolescence, particularly for girls.
Before moving on, it must be mentioned that there may also be a methodological explanation for the inconsistent pattern of findings between internalizing and externalizing problems. Consistent with Problem Behavior Theory, the CBCL externalizing problems scale includes items that assess impulsivity, inattention, and aggression, but also includes items that assess rule-breaking, including alcohol and other substance use, as well as problems with and thoughts about sex. Accordingly, the indirect effect of CSA on risky behavior through externalizing problems could be a function of overlapping constructs inherent in the age 12 caregiver-report on the CBCL and the youth report of risky behavior at age 14. Although certainly worthy of future study (e.g., utilizing raw scores, rather than T-Scores, on CBCL and conducting analyses both with and without these items; utilizing another problem behavior scale that does not include risk behaviors), item overlap alone is unlikely to have accounted for the link between externalizing and risk behavior in the current study for two reasons: 1). The frequency of boys endorsing sex and alcohol was not statistically different (and T-Scores for externalizing problems was the same in both the sex and alcohol use subsamples), yet externalizing was associated with sex but not alcohol use for boys; 2). Boys and girls reported statistically similar rates of alcohol use, yet externalizing problems were associated with alcohol use for girls but not boys.
Building upon the methodological considerations noted above, several other potential limitations of the study should be noted. First, the sexual intercourse item did not explicitly ask youth to exclude penetration that occurred in the context of the CSA experience; however, a review of the CPS records suggest that only one of the youth whose CSA allegation included penetration endorsed sexual intercourse at age 14 and chi-square analyses revealed no differences on the sexual intercourse item between youth whose allegation(s) ever included penetration and those whose allegation(s) never included penetration. In addition, an examination of intervening variables would ideally occur at age 13 in this investigation (i.e., the midpoint between CSA trajectories from 2 to 14 y.o. & risk behavior at 14 y.o.); however, LONGSCAN did not conduct an age 13 assessment of internalizing and externalizing problems. That said, trajectory analyses revealed that the typical ages at which CSA experiences occurred fell before the age 12 assessment, suggesting that age 12 may be a reasonable assessment point at which to examine intervening variables. Similarly, it is also plausible that new CSA allegations at age 14, rather than the earlier allegations that were the focus of this investigation, would have a greater impact on risk behavior at age 14. However, only 4 youth in the CSA group had a new CSA allegation at age 14, decreasing the likelihood that the association is better accounted for by CSA at age 14. Fourth, this study examined one moderator, but other moderators should be considered in future work. For example, protective factors, such as history of therapy, as well as risk factors, such as deviant peer involvement, may interrupt or intensify, respectively, the cognitive, affective, and/or behavioral vulnerabilities linking CSA to externalizing problems to risk behavior. Fifth, with regard to substance use, the focus of the current study was on alcohol given its wider availability to 14 year olds than other substances (e.g., Johnston, O’Malley, & Bachman, 2003; SAMSHA, 2004); however, future work should examine the mechanisms that link CSA to other substances as well, particularly as opportunities for exposure increase.
Finally, as noted earlier, prior work with the LONGSCAN sample found a significant association between CSA and a combined alcohol and sexual intercourse index (Jones et al., 2010). It is plausible then that the resultant loss of variability in our outcomes due to our theoretical decision to examine sex and alcohol separately compromised our ability to detect significant direct effects. Although insufficient variability in sexual intercourse (e.g., sex with multiple partners, condom use) and alcohol use (e.g., frequency of use, getting drunk) existed in the age 14 data, it may be the case that as youth with CSA histories engage in more risk behaviors with age analyses reveal direct effects on these separate risk behaviors as well.
This study also has several strengths. Perhaps most notably, this prospective study represents an initial, but important, step toward examining the mechanisms linking CSA to early onset risky behavior in both boys and girls. Although mechanisms have been a primary focus of theoretical work in this area, far less empirical attention has followed (Senn et al., 2008). In addition, the current study is prospective, affording a previously unparalleled opportunity to follow youth forward across childhood and into adolescence to determine if CSA is associated with the onset of risk behavior at an age of vulnerability. In turn, the current study’s findings identify a potential target (externalizing problems) for preventive efforts aimed at ameliorating or delaying the onset of risk behavior among youth with CSA histories.
This study also included boys who have been relatively overlooked in this literature (see Kearney, Wechsler, Kaur, & Lemos-Miller, 2010; Senn et al., 2008; Walker et al., 2004 for reviews). Importantly, our prospective study of boys and girls and our examination of alcohol use and sexual intercourse separately afforded the opportunity to contribute to the growing literature on gender as a moderator of developmental pathways in the field more broadly (e.g., Mash & Barkley, 2003; Bell-Dolan, Foster, & Mash, 2005; also see Zimmer-Gembeck & Helfand, 2008 for a review).
Fourth, confidence in the current findings is strengthened by the use of different sources for each study variable, decreasing the likelihood that associations between maltreatment (CPS report of allegations), internalizing and externalizing problems (caregiver), and risky behavior (adolescent report) were due to a common reporter. Fifth, given the problems associated with the narrow reliance on substantiated cases alone (Hussey et al., 2005; Kohl, Jonson-Reid, & Drake, 2009; Senn et al., 2008), allegations of CSA were included in the current analyses. It is true that some data reveal that even a broader focus on allegations may still yield lower estimates of maltreatment relative to behaviorally specific and self-report measures (Everson et al., 2008; also see Senn et al., 2008 for a review); however, self-reports were not feasible for youth at the age of entry for LONGSCAN, making allegations a reasonable middle ground approach. Finally, more variability in the developmental trajectories among youth with CSA histories may have been expected (e.g., based on duration, frequency, or age of onset of CSA). However, the age of the child victim is also associated with the identity of the perpetrator, duration of the CSA, and use of force, making disentangling these correlates very difficult (see Putnam, 2003 for a review). As such, our group-based trajectory modeling approach afforded the capacity to identify qualitatively distinct developmental progressions among youth with CSA histories that may not have been readily identifiable using ad hoc classification rules (Nagin & Odgers, 2010).
In summary, our findings have implications both for future research and prevention. With regard to prevention, our findings highlight the role of externalizing problems in the association between CSA and early onset risk behavior and, in turn, a potential more proximal target for prevention. Approaches such as Trauma-Focused Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (TF-CBT; Cohen, Mannarino, & Deblinger, 2006) afford an evidence-based treatment approach for maltreated youth; however, TF-CBT appears to have benefits for reducing PTSD and depressive symptoms in particular, with less evidence to support reductions in externalizing problems or risk behavior (see Lang, Ford, & Fitzgerald, 2010 for a review). Thus, our findings highlight the need for risk reduction and prevention programs targeting the unique vulnerabilities for sexual intercourse among girls and boys with CSA histories, as well as alcohol use for girls; however, future work should replicate our gender findings before such distinctions should be pursued.
Finally, this study did not focus on race differences; however, as this work evolves theoretically-driven, culturally-specific models linking CSA to risk-behavior will be critical. For example, prior work with African American girls has highlighted vulnerabilities associated with marijuana use (e.g., Liau, 2002). Future work may consider the unique vulnerabilities associated with marijuana use among African American female CSA survivors in particular.
Acknowledgments
Funding for the current secondary analyses was provided by a Mentored Public Health Research Scientist Development Award awarded to the first author from the CDC (K01PS000795-01).
Funding for the Longitudinal Studies of Child Abuse and Neglect (LONGSCAN) is provided by grants from the National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect, the Children’s Bureau, Office of Child Abuse and Neglect Administration on Children, Youth, and Families, the National Institute for Child Health and Human Development, and National Institutes of Health. The findings and conclusions in this report are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the funding agencies.
We would like to acknowledge all the LONGSCAN principal investigators, coordinating center, and study staff. In addition, we would like to sincerely thank and recognize the families and participants of the LONGSCAN study for providing their time and valuable information.
Contributor Information
Deborah J. Jones, Email: djjones@email.unc.edu, Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill 919-962-3995
Terri Lewis, School of Public Health, University of Alabama.
Alan Litrownik, Department of Psychology, San Diego State University.
Richard Thompson, Juvenile Protective Association, Chicago, IL.
Laura J. Proctor, Judge Baker Children’s Center, Harvard Medical School
Trish Isbell, School of Public Health, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.
Howard Dubowitz, Division of Child Protection, Center for Families, & Pediatrics, University of Maryland.
Diana English, School of Social Work, University of Seattle.
Bobby Jones, Behavioral Genetics Research Program, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, University of Pittsburgh Medical Center.
Daniel Nagin, Public Policy and Statistics, Carnegie Mellon University.
Desmond Runyan, Kempe Center, School of Medicine, University of Colorado.
References
- Achenbach T. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/2–3 and 1992 Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach T. Some needed changes in the DSM-V: But what about children? Clinical Psychology: Science & Practice. 2009;16:50–53. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach T, Edelbrock C. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4 – 18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Abrantes AM, Strong D, Ramsey SE, Kazura AN, Brown R. HIV risk behaviors among psychiatrically hospitalized adolescents with and without comorbid SUD. Journal of Dual Diagnoses. 2006;2:85–100. doi: 10.1300/J374v02n03_08. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Child Sexual Abuse. Washington, D.C: American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry; 2004. Advocates for Youth, “Child Sexual Abuse: An Overview,” January 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Bagley C, Bolitho F, Bertrand L. Mental health profiles, suicidal behavior, and community sexual assault in 2112 Canadian adolescents. Crisis. 1995;16:126–131. doi: 10.1027/0227-5910.16.3.126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes JE, Noll JG, Putnam FW, Trickett PK. Sexual and physical revictimization among victims of severe childhood sexual abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2009;33:412–420. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2008.09.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnett D, Manly JT, Cicchetti D. Defining child maltreatment: the interface between policy and research. In: Cicchetti D, Toth SL, editors. Child abuse, child development, and social policy. Norwood, NH: Ablex; 1993. pp. 7–73. [Google Scholar]
- Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51:1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baumeister R. Should we stop studying sex differences all together? American Psychologist. 1988;43:1092–1095. [Google Scholar]
- Bell-Dolan DJ, Foster SL, Mash EJ. Handbook of behavioral and emotional problems in girls. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Beitchman JH, Zucker Hood JE, daCosta GA, Akman D, Cassavia E. A review of the long-term effects of child sexual abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect. 1992;16:101–118. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(92)90011-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briere J. Integrating HIV/AIDS prevention activities into psychotherapy for sexual abuse survivors. In: Koenig LJ, Doll LS, O’Leary A, Pequegnat W, editors. From child sexual abuse to adult sexual risk: Trauma, revictimization,and intervention. Washington, DC: APA; 2004. pp. 219–232. [Google Scholar]
- Brown LK, Hadley W, Stewart A, Lescano C, Whiteley L, Donenberg G, DiClemente R the Project Style Study Group. Psychiatric disorders and sexual risk among adolescents in mental health treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2010;78:590–597. doi: 10.1037/a0019632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Browning CR, Laumann EO. Sexual contact between adults and children: A life course perspective. American Sociological Review. 1997;62:540–560. [Google Scholar]
- Bureau of Justice Statistics, US Department of Justice. Sexual Assault of Young Children as Reported to Law Enforcement: Victim, Incident, and Offender Characteristics. Jul, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- CDC, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Youth Risk Behavior Survey (YRBS): Selected health risk behaviors and health outcomes by race/ethnicity. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- CDC, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. HIV among Youth. 2011a Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/hiv/youth/
- CDC, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Sexually transmitted disease surveillance 2010. 2011b Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/std/stats.
- CDC, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2011/04/08. Vol. 60. Centers for Disease Control; 2011c. Vital signs: teen pregnancy--United States, 1991--2009; pp. 414–420. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D, Rizley R. Developmental perspectives on the etiology, intergenerational transmission and sequelae of child maltreatment. New Directions for Child Development. 1981;11:31–55. [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D, Toth SL. A developmental psychopathology perspective on child abuse and neglect. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1995;34:541–565. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199505000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Classen CC, Palesh OG, Aggarwal R. Sexual revictimization: A review of the empirical literature. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse. 2005;6:103–129. doi: 10.1177/1524838005275087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clum G, Andrinopoulos K, Muessing K, Ellen JA the Adolescent Medicine Trials Network for HIV/AIDS Interventions. Child abuse in young, HIV-positive women: Linkages to risk. Qualitative Health Research. 2009;19:1755–1768. doi: 10.1177/1049732309353418. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clum GA, Chung SE, Ellen JM, Perez V, Murphy DA, Harper GW, Hamvas L. Victimization and Sexual Risk Behavior in Young, HIV Positive Women: Exploration of Mediators. AIDS and Behavior. 2011 doi: 10.1007/s10461-011-9931-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen JA, Mannarino AP, Deblinger E. Treating trauma and traumatic grief in children and adolescents. New York, NY: Guilford; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Colder CR, Chassin L, Lee MR, Villalta IK. Developmental perspectives: Affect and adolescent substance use. In: Kassel J, editor. Substance abuse and emotion. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2010. pp. 109–136. [Google Scholar]
- Cole ER, Stewart AJ. Invidious comparisons: Imagining a psychology of race and gender beyond differences. Political Psychology. 2001;22:293–308. [Google Scholar]
- Coohey C. Gender differences in internalizing problems among sexually abused early adolescents. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2010;34:856–862. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2010.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crockett LJ, Raffaelli M, Shen Y. Linking self regulation and risk proneness to risky sexual behavior: Pathways through peer pressure and early substance use. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2006;16:503–525. [Google Scholar]
- D’Amico EJ, McCarthy DM. Escalation and initiation of younger adolescents’ substance use: The impact of perceived use. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2006;39:481–487. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Danielson CK, McCart M, de Arellano MA, Macdonald A, Silcott L, Resnick H. Risk reduction for substance use and trauma-related psychopathology in adolescent sexual assault victims: Findings from an open trial. Child Maltreatment. 2010;15:261–268. doi: 10.1177/1077559510367939. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darves-Bornoz JM, Choquet M, Ledoux S, Gasquet I, Manfredi R. Gender differences in symptoms of adolescents reporting sexual assault. Social Psychiatry and Psychatric Epidemiology. 1998;33:111–117. doi: 10.1007/s001270050030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeBellis MD, Baum AS, Birmaher B, Keshaven MS, Eccard CH, Boring A. Developmental traumatology: Pt. I. Biological Stress Systems. Biological Psychiatry. 1999;45:1259–1270. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00044-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donovan JE, Jessor R. Structure of problem behavior in adolescence and young adulthood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1985;53:890–904. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.53.6.890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubowitz H, Pitts S, Litrownick A, Cox CE, Runyan D, Black M. Defining child neglect based on Child Protective Services data. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2005;29:493–511. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2003.09.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Else-Quest NM, Hyde JS, Goldsmith HH, Van Hulle CA. Gender Differences in Temperament: A Meta-Analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132:33–72. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- English DJ, Graham JC, Litrownik AJ, Everson M, Bangdiwala SI. Defining maltreatment chronicity: Are there differences in child outcomes? Child Abuse & Neglect. 2005;29:575–595. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2004.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everson MD, Smith JB, Hussey JM, English D, Litrownik AJ, Dubowitz H, Thompson R, Knight ED, Runyan D. Concordance between adolescent reports of childhood abuse and Child Protective Service determinations in an at-risk sample of young adolescents. Child Maltreatment. 2008;13(1):14–26. doi: 10.1177/1077559507307837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finkelhor D, Browne A. The traumatic impact of child sexual abuse: A conceptualization. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 1985;55:530–541. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1985.tb02703.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finkelhor D, Hammer H, Sedlak AJ. Sexually assaulted children: National estimates and characteristics, in OJJDP: Juvenile Justice Bulletin. U.S. Department of Justice; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Garnefski N, Arends E. Sexual abuse and adolescent maladjustment: Differences between male and female victims. Journal of Adolescence. 1998;21:99–107. doi: 10.1006/jado.1997.0132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garnefski N, Diekstra RF. Child sexual abuse and emotional and behavioral problems in adolescence: Gender differences. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry. 1990;36:323–329. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199703000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haapasalo J, Tremblay RE, Boulerice B, Vitaro F. Relative advantages of person and variable centered approaches to predicting problem behavior from kindergarten assessments. Journal of Quantitative Criminology. 2000;16:145–168. [Google Scholar]
- Hare-Mustin R, Marecek J. Asking the right questions: Feminist psychology and sex differences. Feminism and Psychology. 1994;4:531–537. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes AF. Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs. 2009;76:408–420. [Google Scholar]
- Heflinger CA, Simpkins CG, Combs-Orme T. Using the CBCL to determine the clinical status of children in state custody. Children and Youth Services Review. 2000;22:55–73. [Google Scholar]
- Hibbard RA, Ingersoll GM, Orr DP. Behavior risk, emotional risk, and child abuse among adolescents in a nonclinical setting. Pediatrics. 1990;86:896–901. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hien DA, Campbell ANC, Killeen T, Hu MC, Hansen C, Jiang H, Hatch-Maillette M, Miele GM, Cohen LR, Gan W, Resko SM, DiBono M, Wells EA, Nunes EV. The Impact of Trauma-Focused Group Therapy upon HIV Sexual Risk Behaviors in the NIDA Clinical Trials Network “Women and Trauma” Multi-Site Study. AIDS and Behavior. 2010;14:421–430. doi: 10.1007/s10461-009-9573-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hussey JM, Marshall JM, English DJ, Knight ED, Lau AS, Dubowitz H, Kotch JB. Defining Maltreatment According to Substantiation: Distinction without a Difference? Child Abuse and Neglect. 2005;29:479–492. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2003.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jessor R, van den Bos J, Vanderryn J, Costa FM, Turbin MS. Protective factors in adolescent problem behavior: Moderator effects and developmental change. Developmental Psychology. 1995;31:923–933. [Google Scholar]
- Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. NIH Publication No 09-7401. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse; 2009. Monitoring the Future national results on adolescent drug use: Overview of key findings, 2008; p. 73. [Google Scholar]
- Jones DJ, Runyan D, Lewis T, Litrownik A, Black M, Wiley T, English D, Proctor L, Jones BL, Nagin D. Violence Exposure Trajectories among High Risk Youth and their Association with Two Subsequent HIV/AIDS Risk Behaviors. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2010;39:667–680. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2010.501286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones B, Nagin DS, Roeder K. A SAS procedure based on mixture models for estimating developmental trajectories. Sociological Research and Methods. 2001;29:374–393. [Google Scholar]
- Kearney CA, Wechsler A, Kaur H, Lemos-Miller Posttraumatic stress disorder in maltreated youth: A review of contemporary research and thought. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 13:46–76. doi: 10.1007/s10567-009-0061-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendall-Tackett K, Williams L, Finkelhor D. Impact of sexual abuse on children: A review and synthesis of recent empirical articles. Psychological Bulletin. 1993;113:164–180. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.113.1.164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kenny D. [Accessed 11, March, 2009];Mediation. 2009 Nov; Retrieved from http://davidakenny.net/cm/mediate.htm.
- Kincaid C, Jones DJ, Cuellar J, Gonzalez M. Psychological Control Associated with Youth Adjustment and Risky Behavior in African American Single Mother Families. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2010;20:102–110. [Google Scholar]
- Kohl PL, Jonson-Reid M, Drake B. Time to Leave Substantiation Behind: Findings From A National Probability Study. Child Maltreatment. 2009;14:17–26. doi: 10.1177/1077559508326030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lahey BB, Applegate B, Waldman ID, Loft JD, Hankin BL, Rick J. The structure of child and adolescent psychopathology: Generating new hypotheses. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113:358–385. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.3.358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lang JM, Ford JD, Fitzgerald MM. An algorithm for determining the use of trauma-focused cognitive behavioral therapy. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research & Practice. 2010;47:554–569. doi: 10.1037/a0021184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis T, Kotch J, Litrownik A, English DJ, Thompson R, Zolotor AJ, Block S, Duobowitz H. Internalizing problems: A potential pathway from childhood maltreatment to adolescent smoking. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2011;48:247–252. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Litrownik AJ, Lau A, English D, Briggs E, Newton R, Romney S, Dubowitz H. Measuring the severity of child maltreatment. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2005;29:553–573. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2003.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liau A, DiClemente RJ, Wingood GM, Crosby RA. Associations between biologically confirmed marijuana use and sexually transmitted diseases among African American adolescent females. Sexually Transmitted Diseases. 2002;29:387–390. doi: 10.1097/00007435-200207000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mallinckrodt B, Abraham WT, Wei M, Russel DW. Advances in testing the statistical significance of mediation effects. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2006;53:372–378. [Google Scholar]
- Mash EJ, Barkley RA. Child psychopathology. New York: Guilford Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- McLeod JD, Knight S. The association of socioemotinal problems and early sexual initiation. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health. 2010;42:93–101. doi: 10.1363/4209310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mrug S, Windle M. Initiation of alcohol use in early adolescence: Links with exposure to community violence over time. Addictive Behaviors. 2009;34:779–781. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mugavero MJ, Pence BW, Whetten K, Leserman J, Swartz M, Stangl D, Thielman NM. Childhood abuse and initial presentation for HIV care: An opportunity for early intervention. AIDS Care. 2007;19:1083–1087. doi: 10.1080/09540120701351896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muller D, Judd CM, Yzerbyt VT. When moderation is mediation and mediation is moderated. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 2005;89:852–863. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthen LK, Muthen BO. MPlus User’s Guide. 5. Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Nadeau KG, Littman EB, Quinn PO. Understanding girls with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Silver Spring, MD: Advantage; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Nagin D. Group-based modeling of development. Harvard University; Cambridge, MA: 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Nagin D, Odgers CL. Group-based trajectory modeling in clinical research. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2010;6:109–138. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagin D, Tremblay RE. Trajectories of boys’ physical aggression, opposition, and hyperactivity on the path to physically violence and nonviolencet juvenile delinquency. Child Development. 1999;70:1181–1196. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- NIAAA. National academies report to Congress released. 2003 Available from www.alcoholfreechildren.org/en/news/articles.
- Noll JG, Trickett PK, Putnam FW. A prospective investigation of the impact of childhood sexual abuse on the development of sexuality. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology. 2003;71:575–586. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.71.3.575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phinney J. When we talk about American ethnic groups, what do we mean? American Psychologist. 1996;51:918–927. [Google Scholar]
- Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research, Methods, Instruments, and Computers. 2004;36:717–731. doi: 10.3758/bf03206553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preacher KJ, Rucker DD, Hayes AF. Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2007;42:185–227. doi: 10.1080/00273170701341316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Putnam FW. Ten year research update review: Child sexual abuse. Journal of the Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003:42. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200303000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pynoos RS, Steinberg AM, Layne CM, Briggs EC, Ostrowski SA, Fairbank JA. DSM-V PTSD diagnostic criteria for children and adolescents: A developmental perspective and recommendations. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2009;22:391–398. doi: 10.1002/jts.20450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quina K, Morokoff PJ, Harlow LL, Zurbriggen EL. Cognitive and Attitudinal Paths From Childhood Trauma to Adult HIV Risk. In: Koenig L, Doll L, O’Leary A, Pequegnat W, editors. From child sexual abuse to adult sexual risk: Trauma, revictimization, and intervention. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2003. pp. 117–134. [Google Scholar]
- Ramrakha S, Bell M, Paul C, Dickson N, Moffitt T, Caspi A. Childhood behavior problems linked to sexual risk taking in young adulthood: A birth cohort study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2007;46:1272–1279. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e3180f6340e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rind B, Tromovitch P, Bauserman R. A meta-analytic examination of assumed properties of child sexual abuse using college samples. Psychological Bulletin. 1998;124:22–53. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.1.22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruggiero KJ, McLeer SV. PTSD scale of the Child Behavior Checklist: Concurrent and discriminant validity with non-clinic referred sexually abused children. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2000;13:287–299. doi: 10.1023/A:1007710828777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Runyan DK, Curtis PA, Hunter WM, Black MM, Kotch JB, Bangdiwala S, Dubowitz H, English D, Everson MD, Landsverk J. LONGSCAN: A consortium for longitudinal studies of maltreatment and the life course of children. Aggression and Violent Behavior. 1998;3:275–285. [Google Scholar]
- Saigh PA, Yasik AE, Oberfield RA, Halamandaris PV, McHugh M. An analysis of internalizing and externalizing behaviors of traumatized urban youth with and without PTSD. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111:462–470. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.111.3.462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schofield HL, Bierman KL, Heinrichs B, Nix RL the Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. Predicting early sexual activity with behavior problems exhibited at school entry and in early adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36:1175–1188. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9252-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sedlak AJ, Broadhurst DD. Executive summary of the third national incidence study of child abuse and neglect. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, Administration on Children, Youth and Families, National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect; Washington, D.C: 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Shaffer D, Fisher P, Lucas C, Dulcan M, Schwab-Stone M. NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2000;39:28–38. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200001000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sikkema KJ, Hansen NB, Kochman A, Tarakeshwar N, Neufeld S, Meade CS, et al. Outcomes from a group intervention for coping with HIV/AIDS and childhood sexual abuse: Reductions in traumatic stress. AIDS and Behavior. 2007;11:49–60. doi: 10.1007/s10461-006-9149-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sikkema KJ, Wilson PA, Hansen NB, Kochman A, Neufeld S, Ghebremichael MS, et al. Effects of a coping intervention on transmission risk behavior among people living with HIV/AIDS and a history of childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes. 2008;47:506–513. doi: 10.1097/QAI.0b013e318160d727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simoes C, Batista-Foguet JM, Matos MG, Calmeiro L. Alcohol use and abuse in adolescence: Proposal of an alternative analysis. Child Care, Health, and Development. 2008;34:291–301. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2007.00808.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Senn TE, Carey MP, Vanable PA. Childhood and adolescent sexual abuse and subsequent sexual risk behavior: Evidence from controlled studies, methodological critique, and suggestions for research. Clinical Psychology Review. 2008;28:711–735. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spaccarelli S. Stress, appraisal, and coping in child sexual abuse: A theoretical and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;116:340–362. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor KW, Kliewer W. Violence exposure and early adolescent alcohol use: An exploratory study of family risk and protective factors. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2006;15:207–221. [Google Scholar]
- Trickett PK, Putnam FW. Developmental consequences of child sexual abuse. In: Trickett PK, Schellenbach C, editors. Violence against children in the family and community. Washington, DC: APA; 1998. pp. 39–56. [Google Scholar]
- Udry JR, Kovenock J, Morris NM, van den Berg BJ. Childhood precursors of age at first intercourse for females. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 1995;24:329–337. doi: 10.1007/BF01541603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. The Surgeon General’s Call to Action To Prevent and Reduce Underage Drinking: A Guide to Action for Educators. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Office of the Surgeon General; 2007. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Roode T, Dickson N, Herbison P, Paul C. Child sexual abuse and persistence of risky sexual behaviors and negative sexual outcomes over adulthood: Findings from a birth cohort. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2009;33:161–172. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2008.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walker JL, Carey PD, Mohr N, Stein DJ, Seedat Gender differences in the prevalence of childhood sexual abuse and in the development of pediatric PTSD. Archives of Women’s Health. 2004;7:111–121. doi: 10.1007/s00737-003-0039-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson HW, Widom CS. An examination of risky sexual behavior and HIV in victims of child abuse and neglect: A 30 year follow-up. Health Psychology. 2008;27:149–158. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.2.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson HW, Widom CS. Pathways from childhood abuse and neglect to HIV-risk sexual behavior in middle adulthood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2011 doi: 10.1037/a0022915. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfe DA, Sas L, Weklerle C. Factors associated with the development of posttraumatic stress disorder among child victims of sexual abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect. 1994;18:37–50. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(94)90094-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zahn-Waxler C, Shirtcliff EA, Marceau K. Disorders of Childhood and Adolescence: Gender and Psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical psychology. 2008;4:275–303. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmer-Gembeck MJ, Helfand M. Ten years of longitudinal research on U.S. adolescent sexual behavior: Developmental correlates of sexual intercourse and the importance of age, gender, and ethnic background. Developmental Review. 2008;28:153–224. [Google Scholar]