Abstract
There is accumulating evidence that prior knowledge about expectations plays an important role in perception. The Bayesian framework is the standard computational approach to explain how prior knowledge about the distribution of expected stimuli is incorporated with noisy observations in order to improve performance. However, it is unclear what information about the prior distribution is acquired by the perceptual system over short periods of time and how this information is utilized in the process of perceptual decision making. Here we address this question using a simple two-tone discrimination task. We find that the “contraction bias”, in which small magnitudes are overestimated and large magnitudes are underestimated, dominates the pattern of responses of human participants. This contraction bias is consistent with the Bayesian hypothesis in which the true prior information is available to the decision-maker. However, a trial-by-trial analysis of the pattern of responses reveals that the contribution of most recent trials to performance is overweighted compared with the predictions of a standard Bayesian model. Moreover, we study participants' performance in a-typical distributions of stimuli and demonstrate substantial deviations from the ideal Bayesian detector, suggesting that the brain utilizes a heuristic approximation of the Bayesian inference. We propose a biologically plausible model, in which decision in the two-tone discrimination task is based on a comparison between the second tone and an exponentially-decaying average of the first tone and past tones. We show that this model accounts for both the contraction bias and the deviations from the ideal Bayesian detector hypothesis. These findings demonstrate the power of Bayesian-like heuristics in the brain, as well as their limitations in their failure to fully adapt to novel environments.
Author Summary
In this paper we study how history affects perception using an auditory delayed comparison task, in which human participants repeatedly compare the frequencies of two, temporally-separated pure tones. We demonstrate that the history of the experiment has a substantial effect on participants' performance: when both tones are high relative to past stimuli, people tend to report that the 2nd tone was higher, and when they are relatively low, they tend to report that the 1st tone was higher. Interestingly, only the most recent trials bias performance, which can be interpreted as if the participants assume that the statistics of stimuli in the experiment is highly volatile. Moreover, this bias persists even in settings, in which it is detrimental to performance. These results demonstrate the abilities, as well as limitations, of the cognitive system when incorporating expectations in perception.
Introduction
Perception is a complex cognitive process, in which noisy signals are extracted from the environment and interpreted. It is generally believed that perceptual resolution is limited by internal noise that constrains our ability to differentiate physically similar stimuli. The magnitude of this internal noise is typically estimated using the 2-alternative forced choice (2AFC) paradigm, which was introduced to eliminate participants' perceptual and response biases [1], [2]. In this paradigm, a participant is presented with two temporally-separated stimuli that differ along a physical dimension and is instructed to compare them. The common assumption is that the probability of a correct response is determined by the physical difference between the two stimuli, relative to the level of internal noise. Performance is typically characterized by the threshold of discrimination, referred to as the Just Noticeable Difference (JND). Thus, the JND is a measure of the level of internal noise such that the higher the JND, the higher the inferred internal noise.
However, the idea that there is a one-to-one correspondence between the JND and the internal noise is inconsistent with theoretical considerations which postulate that participants' performance can be improved by taking into account expectations about the stimuli in the process of perception or decision-making. If the internal representation of a stimulus was uncertain, the prior expectations should bias the participant against unlikely stimuli. The larger the uncertainty, the larger the contribution of these prior expectations should be. The Bayesian theory of inference describes how expectations regarding the probability distribution of stimuli should be combined with the noisy representations of these stimuli in order to optimize performance [3].
In fact, expectations, formalized as prior distribution of stimuli used in the experiment, have been shown to bias participants' responses in a way that is consistent with the Bayesian framework (reviewed in [4]). In particular, responses in the 2AFC paradigm have been shown to be biased by prior expectations: when the magnitudes of the two stimuli are small relative to the distribution of stimuli used in the experiment, participants tend to respond that the 1st stimulus was larger, whereas they tend to respond that the 2nd stimulus was larger when the magnitudes of the two stimuli are relatively large [5]–[7]. In a previous study we have shown that this bias, known as the “contraction bias”, can be understood in the Bayesian framework: following the presentation of the two stimuli, the participant combines her noisy representations of the two stimuli with the prior distribution of the stimuli to form two posterior distributions. Rather than comparing the two noisy representations of the stimuli, the participant is assumed to compare the two posteriors in order to maximize the probability of a correct response. The contribution of the prior distribution to the two posteriors is not equal. The larger the level of noise in the representation of the stimulus, the larger is the contribution of the prior distribution to the posterior. The level of noise in the representation of the magnitude of the 1st stimulus is larger than the level of noise in the representation of the magnitude of the 2nd stimulus because of the noise associated with the encoding and maintenance of the 1st stimulus in memory [8], [9]. As a result, the posterior distribution of the 1st stimulus is biased more by the prior distribution than the posterior distribution of the 2nd stimulus. If the prior distribution is unimodal, both posteriors are contracted towards the median of the prior distribution. Because the posterior of the 1st stimulus is contracted more than the posterior of the 2nd stimulus, participants' responses are biased towards overestimating the 1st stimulus when it is relatively small and underestimating it when it is relatively large [7].
One limitation of the Bayesian model is that it relies heavily on the assumption that the prior distribution of stimuli is known to the observer. While this assumption may be plausible in very long experiments comprising a large number of trials (e.g. thousands in [10]) or in experiments utilizing natural tasks (e.g., reading, [11]), it is unclear how Bayesian inference can take place if participants have less experience in the task.
In this paper we study participants' pattern of responses in a 2AFC tone discrimination task in relatively short experiments consisting of tens of trials. We report a substantial contraction bias that persists even when it hampers performance due to a-typical statistics. We show that participants' pattern of behavior is consistent with an “implicit memory” model, in which the representation of previous stimuli is a single scalar that continuously updates with examples. Thus, this model can be viewed as a simple implementation of the Bayesian model that provides a better account of participants' perceptual decision making.
Results
The contraction bias
We measured the performance of our participants in the random 2AFC paradigm (Materials and Methods, Fig. 1), in which subjects compared the frequencies of two sequentially presented tones drawn from a broad frequency range. Averaged across the population of participants, the JND was 13.6%±0.7% (SEM), which is higher than typically reported in the literature ([12], [13]). The relatively high value of the JND, which is likely to result from the lack of experience of the participants and the fact that no reference was used, is comparable with previous studies using the random frequency paradigm, with short stimuli and untrained participants [14], [15].
As predicted by the contraction bias, the JND did not capture the full pattern of participants' responses. This is depicted in Fig. 2A. The coordinates of each dot in Fig. 2A correspond to the frequencies of the 1st and 2nd tones in a trial, referred to as and . Blue and red dots denote trials, in which the participant's response was correct and incorrect, respectively. The closer the dots are to the diagonal, the smaller is the difference in the frequencies of the two tones. Therefore naively, one would expect that the probability of a trial to be incorrect (red dot) would be highest near the diagonal. Moreover, if the probability of a correct response as a function of is symmetrical around 0, as implicitly assumed when measuring the JND, then the pattern of red and blue dots is expected to be symmetrical around the diagonal. In contrast, we found that the pattern of incorrect responses is highly non-symmetrical. Participants tended to err more when both frequencies were high and and when both frequencies were low and . To quantify this asymmetry, we considered separately two regions: the Bias+ region corresponds to trials in two sections of this plane (yellow in Fig. 2A): in the first section are trials in which the frequencies of both stimuli are above the median (1000 Hz) and the frequency of the 1st tone is lower than that of the 2nd tone. In the second section are trials in which the frequencies of both stimuli are below the median frequency and the frequency of the 1st tone is higher than that of the 2nd tone. Similarly, The Bias− region (gray in Fig. 2A) corresponded to trials in which the frequencies of both stimuli are above the median (1000 Hz) and the frequency of the 1st tone is higher than that of the 2nd tone and trials in which the frequencies of both stimuli are below the median frequency and the frequency of the 1st tone is lower than that of the 2nd tone. Participants' rate of success differed greatly between the Bias+ and Bias− regions. Participants were typically successful when either the two tones were low (<1000 Hz) and the 2nd tone was lower (lower left yellow region, 88.2%±0.5% correct responses, mean ± SEM) or when the two tones were high (>1000 Hz) and the 2nd tone was higher (upper yellow region, 88.4%±0.6% correct responses). On the other hand, performance was relatively poor either when the two tones were low and the 1st tone was lower (lower left gray region, 63.2%±0.8% correct responses) or when the two tones were high and the 1st tone was higher (upper gray region, 61.8%±0.8% correct responses). These effects were highly significant in each of the two quadrants (p<10−6, Monte Carlo Permutation test). The differential level of proficiency in the yellow and gray regions indicates a substantial contraction bias, in line with that bias described in previous studies [6], [7]: when the frequency of the 1st tone was relatively low, participants tended to overestimate it (leading to successful performance when the 1st tone was higher). The opposite was true when the frequency of the 1st tone was relatively high (leading to successful performance when the 1st tone was lower). The differential level of proficiency in the yellow and gray regions is evident not only in the response pattern of the population of participants but also in the response pattern in individual blocks (Fig. S1A–C). Moreover, it was evident for all levels of proficiency in the task (Fig. S1D).
To further illustrate the contraction bias, we constructed a two-dimensional histogram of participants' performance by binning the space of Fig. 2A and computing the fraction of correct responses in each bin (Fig. 2B, grayscale). The non-symmetrical distribution of the shades of gray of the squares around the diagonal reflects the contraction bias. Note in particular the two squares denoted by arrows. Despite the fact that they were of equal ‘objective’ difficulty (the absolute difference in frequencies was the same), the performance in the bottom right square region was almost perfect (92.2% correct responses; n = 324), whereas it was almost at chance level in the top left square region (50.8% correct responses; n = 323; p<10−33, Fisher's exact test). It should be noted that the bias in participants' response cannot be accounted for by a general preference in favor of one of the alternative answers, because the bias is opposite in the low and high frequencies.
The non-symmetrical performance around the diagonal (Fig. 2) is not captured by a single performance measure, the JND. This has motivated us to consider a measure of performance that captures some of this asymmetry. To that goal, we computed two separate JNDs for each participant (see Materials and Methods): one for the trials in the regions in which the contraction bias augments behavior (Bias+, yellow) and the other for the regions in which the contraction bias impairs behavior (Bias−, gray). These JNDs differed by more than 6 fold (the medians of JNDs across the population were 4.1%, and 27.0% for the Bias+ and Bias− regions, respectively; p<10−5, Monte Carlo Permutation test). In fact, as depicted in Fig. S2 in the Supporting Information section, a participant's proficiency on a trial depended more on the contraction bias (i.e. Bias+ versus Bias− regions) than on the participant's overall proficiency (overall low versus high JND). These results demonstrate the substantial contribution of this bias to behavior.
Recency effect and the prior distribution
In a previous study we have shown that the contraction bias in a visual discrimination task is consistent with a model of an ideal detector that utilizes Bayes' rule to incorporate the prior distribution with the sensed stimuli in order to optimize performance [7]. In agreement with that study, such a Bayesian model, with 2 free parameters that correspond to the noise in the internal representation of each of the two stimuli, can qualitatively account for the observed contraction in the two-tone discrimination task (see Fig. S3 in the Supporting Information section).
However, it should be noted that the Bayesian model relies on the assumption that the prior distribution of stimuli is known to the observer, which seems unreasonable in our experiment, which consisted of merely tens of trials. Therefore, it is not clear how the history of trials experienced by the participants in the experiment contributes to the bias. To address this question, we considered the contribution of individual trials to the bias. Because the statistics of stimuli in our experiment are stationary, all past trials are equally informative about the prior distribution. Therefore, normative considerations that incorporate an assumption of stationarity imply that the effect of past trials on participants' choices will be independent of the number of trials elapsed between these trials and the choice. By contrast, previous studies have reported that participants' responses are influenced to a greater degree by recent stimuli, which is known as the recency effect [16]–[21]. In addition, the activity of neurons in the primary auditory cortex has been shown to contain information about both current and previous stimuli [22]. To test for recency in our dataset, we fitted a linear non-linear model that relates the response in each trial to a linear combination of present and past stimuli according to the following equation:
(1) |
where is the probability that the model would report that the frequency of the 1st tone was higher than that of the 2nd tone in trial ; is the normal cumulative distribution function such that ; and are parameters, and are the frequencies of the 1st and 2nd tone, respectively, in trial and is the geometric mean of the frequencies of all stimuli in the experiment until trial .
To gain insights into the behavior of the model (Eq. (1)) we consider the simple case in which and . In this case, Eq. (1) becomes , which corresponds to a model participant that is indifferent to the history of the experiment and its choices depend solely on the ratio of the frequencies of the two tones and the internal noise. The value of denotes the level of internal noise of the model participant. If is very small, then independently of the frequencies of the stimuli, and , , and the model participant responds at random. In contrast, if is very large, then where is the Heaviside step function such that for and for . In other words, if is very large the model participant always answers correctly. The larger the value of , the smaller the JND of the model participant. The values of the parameters determine the contribution of past stimuli to perception, where the value of determines the contribution of the stimulus presented trials ago and the value of determines the contribution of the average frequency of past stimuli to perception. If all past stimuli contribute equally to perception, as expected from normative participants who assume that the distribution of stimuli is stationary then we expect and . In contrast, if the participant assumes that the statistics of the experiment is non-stationary then we expect the most recent trials to have a stronger effect on behavior, resulting in whose magnitude decreases as the value of increases.
Assuming that , we analyzed the sequence of frequencies and decisions of our participants. We found the values of the parameters (Fig. 3, green), (dark blue) and (black) that minimize the mean square error (MSE), the mean square distance of the vector of probabilities, from the vector of choices, such that if the participant responded that the frequency of the 1st tone was higher than the frequency of the 2nd tone in trial and otherwise. Note that the values of and in Fig. 3 are larger than the values of all other coefficients, . This result reflects the simple fact that the tones presented in a trial influence the decision in that trial more than tones presented in previous trials. The recency effect is manifested in the non-zero coefficients of (see Materials and Methods). As depicted in Fig. 3, the contribution of past trials to choice diminishes within several trials. This result is consistent with other findings of rapid perceptual learning [23], [24] (but see also [25]) and demonstrates that at least some aspects of the prior distribution are estimated using a small number of the most recent trials. It should also be noted that the contribution of past stimuli to decision is dominated by past values of and not past values of (Fig. 3. See also Materials and Methods).
The implicit-memory model
The recency effect described in the previous section is difficult to reconcile with a Bayesian inference model that takes into account the stationary statistics of the experiment. This finding has motivated us to consider the possibility that the contraction bias described in Fig. 2 emerges from simpler cognitive processes that do not require an explicit representation of the prior distribution. In this section we present a simple model that accounts for the contraction bias and the recency effect, which does not explicitly keep track of the prior distribution of stimuli presented in the experiment.
In our model, the memory trace of past stimuli is a single scalar (rather than the full prior distribution). In response to the presentation of , the participant updates the value of such that is a linear combination of the past value of with the present stimulus, corrupted by sensory and encoding noise. Formally, the value of in trial , is given by
(2) |
where is the weight given to the memory and is the noise associated with the encoding of . We assume that this noise is Gaussian with variance and is uncorrelated across trials: , where is the Kronecker delta function, if and if .
A decision in a trial in this model depends on the relative values of and . If , the model responds that “”. Otherwise it responds that “”. In this model we assume that the noise is restricted to the representation of . The reason for ignoring noise in the representation of is that noise in is mathematically equivalent to a larger magnitude noise in when considering decision in a given trial.
It is easy to show that in this model, is an exponentially weighted sum of the current and past stimuli and their respective encoding noises:
(3) |
Note that in this model past values of do not contribute to behavior. This reflects the dominance of past values of in Fig. 3 (see also Materials and Methods). It should also be noted that in this model, the contribution of past stimuli to decision (which plays the role of the prior distribution in the Bayesian model) is encoded using the same variable as the encoding of . Therefore, the model does not require any form of separate representation of the long term memory of past trials.
The implicit-memory model is characterized by two parameters that denote the level of noise, and the extent to which the history of the experiment affects perception, . Fig. 4 depicts the results of a simulation of a population of implicit memory models, each with the parameters and best fitting a single block in our dataset (see Materials and Methods). As shown in Figs. 4A and 4B, the model results in a contraction bias, which is comparable to the experimentally observed (compare Figs. 4A and 4B to Figs 2A and 2B, respectively). A quantitative analysis reveals that the goodness-of-fit of the Implicit memory model is comparable to that of the Bayesian model (Fig. S4). However, in contrast to be Bayesian model that assumes a constant prior, the contribution of very recent trials to performance (Eq. (1)) in the Implicit memory model is similar to that of our participants (compare Fig. 4C to Fig. 3).
The rigidity of the contraction bias
The contraction bias in Fig. 2 can be justified using optimality considerations, in which prior knowledge is incorporated with the observations in order to maximize performance (Fig. S3). Would contraction bias persist in an experiment in which it impairs performance due to the dependencies between the frequency distribution of the two tones?
In order to address this question, we conducted a second experiment (Experiment 2 in the Materials and Methods), in which we manipulated the correlations between the frequencies of the two tones such that in some blocks the contraction bias is beneficial to performance whereas in others it is detrimental. Contraction bias is beneficial in the Bias+ region (yellow in Figs. 2A and 4A) and is detrimental in the Bias− region (gray in Figs. 2A and 4A). Therefore, in this experiment we manipulated the fraction of trials in the Bias+ and Bias− regions in different blocks. In one condition, the two tones were chosen such that the 2nd tone was typically higher than the 1st when the two frequencies were relatively high, and the 2nd tone was typically lower than the 1st when the two frequencies were relatively low. We refer to this condition as the ‘Bias+ condition’, because there were many more trials in the Bias+ region than in the Bias− region (11,233 vs. 1172). In the other condition, the two tones were chosen such that the 1st tone was typically higher than the 2nd when the frequencies of the two tones were relatively high and the 1st tone was typically lower than the 2nd when the frequencies of the two tones were relatively low. This ‘Bias− condition‘ was comprised of substantially more trials in the Bias− region than in the Bias+ region (8111 vs. 952). Figs. 5A and 5B depict the distribution of trials and correct and incorrect responses in the Bias+ and Bias− conditions, respectively. Similar to the pattern of responses in the first experiment (Fig. 2A), participants were more likely to be correct in the Bias+ regions, compared to the Bias− regions. This was true both for the Bias+ condition (82.0%±0.4% correct responses vs. 44.5%±1.6% correct responses, p<10−126 Fisher exact test) and the Bias− condition (88.0%±1.2% correct responses vs. 72.6%±0.6% correct responses, p<10−21 Fisher exact test). The JNDs were significantly different in the two conditions: the mean JND in the Bias+ condition was only 4.3%±0.6%, compared to 14.1%±1.1% in Bias− condition (Fig. 5C, black, p<10−25, Wilcoxon rank sum test).
In the framework of the Bayesian model, the difference in proficiency between the two conditions is surprising because given the joint distribution, the detection problem in the two conditions is symmetric. However, our results indicate that our participants did not utilize these probabilities when making a decision about the relative frequencies in this task.
To test the ability of the implicit-memory model to account for the results of the second experiment, we fitted the parameters of the model ( and ) to the experimental data of the Bias+ condition. We then simulated each of the model participants in both the Bias+ and Bias− conditions. The resulting JNDs (mean ± SEM 3.7%±0.5% and 13.3%±0.9% for the Bias+ and Bias− conditions, respectively, purple in Fig. 5C) are not statistically different from to the experimentally measured JNDs (4.3%±0.6% and 14.1%±1.6%; p = 0.78 and p = 0.85, respectively, Wilcoxon rank sum test), suggesting that the participants did not utilize the differential statistics of the two tones in the two conditions. For example, they did not decrease the weights of recent trials even when their performance was consequently hampered. In fact, adapting to the Bias− condition simply by setting the weight of the history-dependence parameter to 0 (effectively eliminating the contribution of past stimuli to decision in the model) would have improved their performance. To demonstrate this, we simulated the model participants in the Bias− condition while assuming that . The resultant JND was only 9.1%±0.7%, lower than the JND of the model participants when assuming the history-dependence parameter measured in the Bias+ condition.
Discussion
In this work we showed that the contraction bias is a dominant determinant of participants' behavior in a 2AFC tone frequency discrimination task. Some aspects of this bias are consistent with the behavior of an ideal detector that utilizes the prior distribution to maximize performance. However, a substantial recency effect combined with a failure of the participants to utilize the joint distribution of the stimuli implies that this Bayesian-like computation is approximated using a much simpler algorithm, in which the prior distribution is not fully represented.
What information does the cognitive system store about the prior distribution? The full Bayesian model represents one extreme approach, in which it is assumed that the participant has full information about the joint distribution of the two stimuli. The standard way in which signal detection theory is applied to psychophysics represents the other extreme, in which the participant does not have (or does not utilize) any prior information about the identity of the stimuli (but only about the probability of each response being correct [1]). The contraction bias in Fig. 2 demonstrates that participants have some information about the marginal probabilities. However, the strong recency effect (Fig. 3) indicates that this marginal probability is constantly updated using a small number of most recent observations, even in stationary environments. In a normative framework, the recency effect, observed previously in various tasks [26], [27], implies that participants believe that the environment is highly volatile and as a result only the very recent history is informative about future stimuli.
The results of experiment 2 (Fig. 5) indicate that participants are either unable to compute the joint distribution or unable to utilize it, at least within a single experimental block of 80 trials. The implicit memory model can be viewed as a minimal modification of the standard approach of applying signal detection theory to perception in the direction of the full Bayesian model. Here, participants represent the prior distribution of the stimuli with a single scalar, which is an estimate of the mean of the marginal of the prior distribution. Nevertheless this implicit model captures many aspects of the behavioral results. Further studies are needed to determine whether, and to what extent other moments of the prior distributions are learned and utilized in the 2AFC discrimination task, especially under longer exposure to distribution statistics.
Several studies have shown that the magnitude of the contribution of the prior distribution to perception on a given trial depends on the level of internal noise [10], [28]. In particular in the framework of the 2AFC task, increasing the delay between the 1st and 2nd stimuli [29], [30] or introducing a distracting task between them [7] enhances the contraction bias. These results are consistent with the Bayesian approach. How can these results be accounted for in the framework of the implicit memory model? One possibility is to assume that the relative contribution of the prior in the simplified online rule of Eq. (2) is affected by perceptual noise. However, it should be noted that at least in one case, the level of noise was determined after the encoding of the 1st stimulus [7]. The dependence of on the level of noise can be accounted for in the framework of the implicit memory model if we assume that the computation of , which incorporates the prior knowledge with the response to the 1st stimulus, is carried out simultaneously by several neurons, or populations of neurons, which are characterized by different values of [22], [31], [32]. At the time of the decision, the magnitude of the noise determines which populations of neurons will be the most informative with respect to the 1st stimulus. If the level of noise is high, the populations of neurons that are more affected by past trials (for whom the value of is large) will dominate perception, resulting in a substantial contraction bias. Otherwise the populations that are less affected by past trials will dominate perception, resulting in a small contraction bias.
Almost 40 years ago, Tversky and Kahneman characterized irrational decision making and reasoning and concluded that “people rely on a limited number of heuristic principles which reduce the complex tasks … to simpler judgmental operations. In general, these heuristics are quite useful, but sometimes they lead to severe and systematic errors” [33]. Our study extends these results to the domain of implicit perceptual judgments.
Materials and Methods
Ethics Statement
The research was approved by the department ethics committee, and all participants signed consent forms.
Experiment 1
150 participants (mean age 24±3.1 years) engaged in a 2AFC high/low pure tone frequency discrimination task, after signing consent forms. 18 participants were excluded due to poor performance on a hearing test or because they did not complete the full schedule. Each participant performed 2 blocks of 80 trials. Each trial consisted of two 50 ms pure tones, with 10 ms linear rise time, and 10 ms linear fall time, separated by 950 ms. Immediately after the 2nd stimulus was played, the text ‘Which tone was higher?’ appeared on screen, and the participant responded by clicking one of two on-screen buttons using a computer mouse, with no time constraint. Visual feedback of a smiling face or a sad face was presented for 300 ms after correct and incorrect responses, respectively. After a pause of 700 ms the next trial began (Fig. 1). All stimuli were presented binaurally through Sennheiser HD-265 linear headphones using a TDT System III signal generator (Tucker Davis Technologies) controlled by in-house software in a sound attenuated room in the laboratory. Tone intensity was 65 dB SPL. Both the 1st and the 2nd frequencies in each trial were drawn from a wide distribution according to the following procedure: a frequency was drawn from a uniform distribution between 800 Hz and 1200 Hz. Another frequency, either or was drawn with a probability 0.5, where was controlled by an adaptive 3-down 1-up staircase, in which the initial difference between the stimuli in each block was 20% and was bounded from below by 0.1%. The step size decreased every four reversals, from 4.5% to 2% to 1% to 0.5% to 0.1%. One of the two frequencies was randomly selected as and the other frequency was selected as . This schedule is expected to converge to a for which the participant answers correctly in 79.4% of the trials ([34]; Fig. 2A, dots). Blocks that did not converge to at least 65% correct responses in the last 40 trials were excluded from the analysis (12 of 264 blocks). The JND was calculated as the average difference between the stimuli frequencies in the last 6 reversals. As a result of the adaptive staircase schedule, the ratios between the frequencies of the two stimuli tended to decrease in the first trials of the block. On average, after 15 trials this ratio stabilized and therefore the first 15 trials of each block were excluded from the analysis.
Estimating the JND in Bias+ and Bias− regions
To estimate the JND in a Bias+ or Bias− region of a block, we fitted a cumulative normal distribution function psychometric curve that relates the response in each trial to the difference in the logarithm of the 1st and 2nd frequencies: where is the normal cumulative distribution function, such that . The value of the parameter was chosen as to minimize the square difference between the vector predictions and the vector of choices such that on trials in which the participant responded “” and otherwise. Assuming that the cumulative normal distribution function reflects the probability of responding “”, the corresponding value of the JND is the difference in the natural logarithms of and such that the probability of a correct response is the asymptotic performance level in our staircase paradigm, 0.794. Therefore, .
Statistical methods
To test for differences in performance between different regions, we used a Monte Carlo permutation test in which the identities of and in a trial were randomly shuffled. We used 106 permutations, and in all cases the experimentally observed differences were larger than the differences observed in all permutations, resulting in p<10−6.
To test for differences in the JNDs between different regions, we used a Monte Carlo permutation test in which the identities of and in a trial were randomly shuffled. We estimated the JND of these simulated results using the same process as described for the data, and estimated the median JND+ and median JND- for the whole population. We used 105 permutations and the experimentally observed difference was larger than the difference observed in all permutations, resulting in p<10−5.
In order to verify the contribution of the parameters for to the linear-non-linear model (Eq. 1), we compared several models using a cross validation test: the parameters of the different models were estimated using all blocks but one, and these parameters were used in order to compute the MSE for that block. The MSE of the model was computed by repeating this procedure for all blocks in the experiment and averaging the resultant MSE.
We considered three models: (1) a naïve model with no history dependence: ; (2) a model with a global history term, ; (3) the full model with an explicit history dependence of three previous trials, and a global term, . The resultant MSEs are ; ; . We found that is significantly smaller than and ( and respectively, Wilcoxon signed rank test).
In order to verify that the contribution of past trials is dominated by values of , we compared two additional models, using the same analysis as above: (4) a model in which the recent history is represented by only: ; (5) a model in which the recent history is represented by only: . The resultant MSEs are and . While is not statistically different from (), is significantly higher () indicating that the model with only coefficients corresponding to the contribution of is as predictive as the full model.
Experiment 2
Experiment 2 was similar to experiment 1, except for the joint distribution of and : in each trial, a frequency was chosen such that the natural logarithm of , measured in Hz, was drawn from a normal distribution with mean 6.908 (corresponding to 1000 Hz), and standard deviation 0.115. In all trials, the mean of and (in the logarithmic domain) was . Another frequency, either or (in the logarithmic domain) was drawn with a probability 0.5, where was controlled by an adaptive 3-down 1-up staircase schedule. In contrast to Experiment 1, the order of frequencies was biased and depended on . In trials in which , was chosen to be larger than with a probability . In contrast, in trials in which , was chosen to be larger than with a probability . We studied two conditions: in one condition, which we refer to as “Bias+”, . In the second condition, referred to as “Bias−”, . 60 participants (mean age 23.8±3.3 years) that did not participate in experiment 1 performed 6 interleaved blocks of Bias+ and Bias− conditions, with the order counterbalanced across participants. Similar to experiment 1, each block consisted of 80 trials.
Fitting implicit memory model parameters
Rewriting Eq. (3), where is a “signal” term that depends on previous trials and is a “noise” term. The probability of responding “” response is thus given by , where is the normal cumulative distribution function, and is the standard deviation of . Because we excluded the first 15 trials from our analysis, we assumed that . We fitted the pair to the remaining 65 trials of each block to minimize the square error between the predictions of the model and the actual responses, .
Supporting Information
Funding Statement
This research was supported by grants from the National Institute for Psychobiology in Israel - founded by The Charles E. Smith Family, from the Israel Science Foundation (grants No. 868/08 and 616/11), and from the Gatsby Charitable Foundation. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Green DM, Swets JA (1966) Signal detection theory and psychophysics. Wiley. 455 p.
- 2.Macmillan NA, Creelman CD (2005) Detection theory: a user's guide. Cambridge University Press.
- 3.Knill DC, Richards W (1996) Perception as Bayesian Inference. In: Knill DC, Richards W, editors. Cambridge University Press.
- 4. Körding K (2007) Decision theory: what “should” the nervous system do? Science 318: 606–610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Berliner JE, Durlach NI, Braida LD (1977) Intensity perception. VII. Further data on roving-level discrimination and the resolution and bias edge effects. J Acoust Soc Am 61: 1577–1585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Preuschhof C, Schubert T, Villringer A, Heekeren HR (2010) Prior Information biases stimulus representations during vibrotactile decision making. J Cognitive Neurosci 22: 875–887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Ashourian P, Loewenstein Y (2011) Bayesian inference underlies the contraction bias in delayed comparison tasks. PLoS ONE 6: e19551. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Bull AR, Cuddy LL (1972) Recognition memory for pitch of fixed and roving stimulus tones. Atten Percept Psychophys 11: 105–109. [Google Scholar]
- 9. Wickelgren WA (1969) Associative strength theory of recognition memory for pitch. J Math Psychol 6: 13–61. [Google Scholar]
- 10. Körding KP, Wolpert DM (2004) Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning. Nature 427: 244–247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Norris D (2006) The Bayesian reader: explaining word recognition as an optimal Bayesian decision process. Psychol Rev 113: 327–357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Wier CC, Jesteadt W, Green DM (1977) Frequency discrimination as a function of frequency and sensation level. J Acoust Soc Am 61: 178–184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Dai H, Micheyl C (2011) Psychometric functions for pure-tone frequency discrimination. J Acoust Soc Am 130: 263–272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Ahissar M, Lubin Y, Putter-Katz H, Banai K (2006) Dyslexia and the failure to form a perceptual anchor. Nat Neurosci 9: 1558–1564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Nahum M, Daikhin L, Lubin Y, Cohen Y, Ahissar M (2010) From comparison to classification: a cortical tool for boosting perception. J Neurosci 30: 1128–1136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Holland MK, Lockhead GR (1968) Sequential effects in absolute judgments of loudness. Percept Psychophys 3: 409–414. [Google Scholar]
- 17. Jesteadt W, Luce RD, Green DM (1977) Sequential effects in judgments of loudness. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 3: 92–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Purks SR, Callahan DJ, Braida LD, Durlach NI (1980) Intensity perception. X. Effect of preceding stimulus on identification performance. J Acoust Soc Am 67: 634–637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Treisman M, Williams TC (1984) A theory of criterion setting with an application to sequential dependencies. Psychol Rev 91: 68–11. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Baird JC (1997) Sensation and judgment: complementarity theory of psychophysics. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
- 21. Stewart N, Brown GDA (2004) Sequence effects in the categorization of tones varying in frequency. J Exp Psychol Learn 30: 416–430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Ulanovsky N, Las L, Farkas D, Nelken I (2004) Multiple time scales of adaptation in auditory cortex neurons. J Neurosci 24: 10440–10453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Agus TR, Thorpe SJ, Pressnitzer D (2010) Rapid Formation of Robust Auditory Memories: Insights from Noise. Neuron 66: 610–618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Chalk M, Seitz AR, Seriès P (2010) Rapidly learned stimulus expectations alter perception of motion. J Vis 10: 1–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Chopin A, Mamassian P (2012) Predictive properties of visual adaptation. Curr Biol 22: 622–626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Neiman T, Loewenstein Y (2011) Reinforcement learning in professional basketball players. Nat Commun 2: 569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Summerfield C, Behrens TE, Koechlin E (2011) Perceptual Classification in a Rapidly Changing Environment. Neuron 71: 725–736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Trommershäuser J, Gepshtein S, Maloney LT, Landy MS, Banks MS (2005) Optimal compensation for changes in task-relevant movement variability. J Neurosci 25: 7169–7178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Berliner JE, Durlach NI (1973) Intensity perception. IV. Resolution in roving-level discrimination. J Acoust Soc Am 53: 1270–1287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Hanks TD, Mazurek ME, Kiani R, Hopp E, Shadlen MN (2011) Elapsed decision time affects the weighting of prior probability in a perceptual decision task. J Neurosci 31: 6339–6352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Jun JK, Miller P, Hernández A, Zainos A, Lemus L, et al. (2010) Heterogenous population coding of a short-term memory and decision task. J Neurosci 30: 916–929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Bernacchia A, Seo H, Lee D, Wang X-J (2011) A reservoir of time constants for memory traces in cortical neurons. Nat Neurosci 14: 366–372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Tversky A, Kahneman D (1974) Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Science 185: 1124–1131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Levitt H (1971) Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. J Acoust Soc Am 49: 467–477. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.