Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Nov 5.
Published in final edited form as: Brain Res. 2012 Sep 14;1483:54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.09.015

Administration of Dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) Increases Serum Levels of Androgens and Estrogens But Does Not Enhance Short-term Memory in Post-Menopausal Women

Paul Merritt a, Bethany Stangl b, Elliot Hirshman c, Joseph Verbalis d
PMCID: PMC3488281  NIHMSID: NIHMS411490  PMID: 22985672

Abstract

The current study examines the effect of administering dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) on short-term memory. This experiment used a double-blind placebo-controlled cross-over design to explore the effects of a four week regimen of 50 mg oral DHEA on performance on the digit span, verbal span, and Modified Sternberg (Oberauer) tasks. The results demonstrate that the current regimen of drug administration significantly increases serum levels of DHEA, DHEAS, testosterone and estrone and substantially alters the patterns of correlations among the serum levels of these hormones. Despite this substantial change in the hormonal milieu, DHEA administration produced no beneficial effects on cognitive performance in the digit span, verbal span, or modified Sternberg paradigm tasks. Ancillary analyses of the relation between hormone levels and cognitive performance demonstrated a strong positive correlation between DHEA levels and performance on digit span forward/backward and verbal span forward in the placebo drug condition, but not in the DHEA condition. We interpret the juxtaposition of the null results of DHEA administration and the correlation of DHEA levels and performance in the placebo condition to indicate that the referenced correlations arise because a third variable (i.e., age) is associated with both performance and DHEA levels. Additional analyses supported this hypothesis.

Keywords: Hormone Replacement Therapy, Aging, Short-term Memory, Cognition

1. Introduction

The relationship between sex steroids and cognitive performance is of critical importance in our understanding of cognition and aging. Our paper investigates the effect of dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) administration on short-term memory performance. Various studies have demonstrated that performance on short-term memory tasks declines with age (MacPherson et al., 2002; Wechsler, 1987), while at the same time circulating DHEA levels also decline (Sulcova et al., 1997). DHEA supplementation has been shown to elevate levels of DHEA, DHEA-S and other steroid metabolites, and has been shown to have beneficial effects on cognition in some populations (Stangl, Hirshman & Verbalis, 2011; Yamada et al., 2010). In this context, the current study examines the effect of administering DHEA on short-term memory in the aged population.

DHEA levels rise to their maximum in the mid 20s for females and 30s for males (Sulcova et al., 1997). After this early to mid-life peak, DHEA levels decline. DHEA levels in males steadily decline throughout older adulthood, while DHEA levels in women decline approximately 15 years before a second peak followed by a subsequent decline (Sulcova et al., 1997). Plasma levels of DHEA are typically only 30% of peak levels by age 70 (Labrie et al., 1997; Orentreich et al., 1992). Such dramatic decreases in DHEA over time are consistent with a hypothesized involvement in the aging process and consistent with demonstrated declines in short-term memory performance as a function of aging (Kemper & Summer, 2001; Mikels et al., 2010).

DHEA is primarily synthesized in the adrenal glands (Miller, 1998), however DHEA is also produced in the CNS (Baulieu & Robel, 1998) and DHEA levels are 6–8 times higher in the CNS than in general circulation in humans (Baulieu & Robel, 1998). As a neurosteroid, DHEA functions as a GABAA antagonist (Majewska et al., 1990; Majewska, 1992) and appears to potentiate NMDA receptors (Mellon & Griffin, 2002). DHEA and in particular, DHEA-S have been shown to alter 5-HT neuronal activity via modulation of GABAA receptors (Gartside et al., 2010). Animal research shows this effect in serotonin projections from the dorsal raphe nucleus to the forebrain (Gartside et al., 2010), pathways related to cognition (Terry et al., 2008). DHEA has also been shown to increase glutamate release in rat forebrain regions associated with learning and memory (Zheng, 2009). Finally, DHEA-S has been shown to promote spontaneous glutamate release in the hippocampus through activation of the sigma-1 receptor (Zheng, 2009), and has been demonstrated to substantially increase release of acetylcholine in the hippocampus (Rhodes et al., 1996) suggesting a mechanism by which DHEA administration can influence memory and cognition. Critically, exogenous administration of DHEA increases brain levels of DHEA in both humans (Labrie et al., 2007) and animals (Nicolas & Fry, 2007).

There are specific reasons why administration of DHEA might enhance short-term memory processes in the aged. Kimonides et al. (1999) have demonstrated that DHEA has both neuro-protective and neurotrophic effects. DHEA may have an effect in the anterior cingulated cortex (ACC) where it may be involved in pre-hippocampal memory processing via neuronal recruitment (Alhaj et al., 2006). Studies of the effects of DHEA in rodents demonstrate cellular effects of DHEA which are related to short-term memory (Gartside et al., 2010) and DHEA has been shown to exert an anti-amnestic effect in rats (Flood et al., 1988). Administration of DHEA can also influences levels of androgens and estrogens. DHEA is an adrenal steroid that is metabolized into its sulfated version, DHEA-S, androgens such as testosterone, and it becomes aromatized into estrogens such as estradiol and estrone (Barru et al., 1997). These sex steroids have been shown to produce effects on cognitive processes via receptors in brain regions known to influence cognition (e.g., androgen receptors in the neo-cortex; Puy et al., 1995; Shaywitz et al., 1999), as well as produce effects on the levels of neurotransmitters known to affect cognition (e.g., effects of estrogens on dopamine (Becker, 1990) and acetylcholine (Gibbs, 2000)). Similarly, sex steroids promote neuronal survival and growth in brain areas known to mediate cognition such as estrogens in the hippocampus (Woolley et al., 1997) and androgens in the pre-frontal cortex (Kolb & Stewart, 1995). More recently, it has been suggested that DHEA is involved in the division, as well as the maintenance of human neural stem cells (Suzuki et al., 2004).

Observational studies regarding the effects of DHEA and other sex steroids on short-term memory have produced a mixed and complex picture. Consistent with the view that DHEA administration enhances short-term memory, Carlson et al. (1999) demonstrated that Alzheimer’s disease patients with higher DHEAS levels had higher forward and backward digit span scores (cf., Yaffe et al., 1998). Similarly, Yamada et al (2010) have shown 6 months of DHEA supplementation improved verbal fluency in women with mild cognitive impairment. In contrast, Parsons et al. (2006) demonstrated levels of DHEA were negatively associated with backward digit span scores. Results from several observational studies on the effects of estrogens and androgens on short-term memory are broadly consistent with the possibility that DHEA administration may enhance short-term memory (Carlson & Sherwin, 1998; 2000; Duff & Hampson, 2000; Miller et al., 2002).

Prior studies of DHEA administration demonstrate mixed results. Hirshman et al. (2003) administered 50 mg of oral DHEA to post-menopausal women and found that performance on forward and backward digit span was approximately equal in the placebo and DHEA conditions. Similarly, using the same drug regimen and tasks Hirshman et al. (2004) found no difference in performance across DHEA and placebo conditions. Moreover, regression analyses showed no relation between serum levels of sex steroids and performance on the digit span tasks in either the DHEA or placebo conditions. However, Wolf et al. (1997) provided evidence that administration of 50 mg of oral DHEA does enhance picture recall. Given that picture recall may be influenced by short-term visual-spatial memory, this result raises the possibility that DHEA may enhance other forms of short-term memory, even if it does not enhance digit span performance. Stangl, Hirshman & Verbalis (2011) demonstrated that administration of 50 mg of DHEA had a substantial, beneficial effect on performance in a variety of visual-spatial tasks, and Yamada et al. (2010) have found significant cognitive improvements on verbal fluency following 6 months of DHEA supplementation in women with mild cognitive impairment. The possibility that short-term memory processes mediate these positive effects motivates further exploration of the effects of DHEA supplementation on tasks measuring short-term memory.

Given prior results from our laboratory and conflicting observational studies and animal studies of the effects of DHEA on short-term memory, our study sought to clarify if DHEA administration enhanced short-term memory in post-menopausal women. We focus on this group in order to assess the effects of DHEA in a sample with relatively low endogenous DHEA levels. We used a digit span task to ensure comparability to our prior studies (Hirshman et al., 2004b; Merritt et al., 2010). Given prior results demonstrating effects on verbal tasks (e.g., Phillips & Sherwin, 1992; Yamada et al., 2010) we also included a verbal span measure. Our third task was the modified Sternberg paradigm (Oberauer, 2001). While there are no studies that have explored the effects on sex steroids on this task, we include this task because it uses verbal material and, analogous to many of the tasks used above (e.g., use of unfamiliar material in Krug et al. (2006)), requires more complex short-term/working memory processing than the verbal span task. Our study is unique in that we are simultaneously testing a variety of short-term memory processes to thoroughly examine the potential for DHEA to influence short-term memory processes. The use of these three tasks will allow us to determine whether DHEA administration produces effects on any of a number of short-term memory processes.

Finally, given that potential effects of DHEA might arise from the effects of its metabolites, the time of cognitive testing is critical. Our prior work (Hirshman et al., 2004a) demonstrated that serum levels of DHEA and its metabolites peaked at approximately 10am and maintained this level for approximately 2 hours when 50 mg of DHEA was administered orally at approximately 8am. We selected this particular dose of DHEA given that at this dosage DHEA levels and hormone metabolites are significantly increased (Hirshman et al., 2004a) and has been shown to influence cognition (Stangl, Hirshman & Verbalis, 2012) while at the same time is a safe dose for our participant sample. Given this finding, all cognitive testing was conducted between 10am and noon in the current study to ensure that we were testing at the maximal levels of DHEA and its metabolites.

2. Results

3.1 Demographic Data

Participants were aged (M= 63.5, SD = 6.85), performed well on the Mini-Mental Status Exam (M = 27.77, SD =2.42) and did not have extreme Body Mass Indices (M = 27.46, SD = 4.36). As indicated in Table 1, baseline hormone levels at the pre-enrollment evaluation were in the normal range for post-menopausal women.

Table 1.

Circulating levels of DHEA, DHEAS, Testosterone, Estrone and Cortisol (means with standard deviations in parentheses)

Baseline DHEA Placebo
DHEA (ng/dL) 103.63 (74.23) 434.00 (660.31) 91.23 (72.82)
DHEAS (ug/dL) 52.15 (34.50) 269.19 (135.68) 44.39 (30.83)
Testosterone (ng/dL) 18.53 (13.26) 51.89 (52.28) 15.79 (10.56)
Estrone (pg/dL) 20.83 (14.64) 31.59 (16.96) 18.70 (12.56)
Cortisol (ug/dL) 10.18 (3.67) 10.46 (3.66) 10.35 (3.36)
*

There was 1 participant whose DHEA levels were very high. Because results did not significantly change when the participant was excluded, the participant was left in the analyses

3.2 Circulating Levels of Steroid Hormones

Table 1 represents circulating levels of DHEA, DHEAS, testosterone, estrone and cortisol as a function of Type of Drug (DHEA vs. Placebo), baseline values are also included for comparison. A paired samples t-test was conducted to determine that DHEA administration was effective as indicated by the substantially higher levels of circulating DHEA; t(47) = 3.53, p < .001. The results with DHEAS, testosterone and estrone provide support that DHEA was metabolized into these related hormones (t(47) = 11.07, p < .001, t(47) = 5.41, p < .001, and t(47) = 5.06, p < .001, respectively). Levels of cortisol did not differ significantly across the conditions (p > .05).

Table 2 represents the correlations (Pearson r’s) between hormones in the DHEA and placebo conditions. In the placebo condition, there were generally positive correlations among all the hormones, with many of these correlations attaining the traditional criterion for significance. The strongest of these positive correlations is between DHEA and DHEAS.

Table 2.

Correlations (Pearson r’s) Between Serum Levels of Hormones as a Function of Type of Drug (DHEA vs. Placebo)

DHEA DHEAS Testosterone Estrone Cortisol
 DHEA .40** .76** .56** −.06
 DHEAS 1 .53** .27 −.07
 Testosterone 1 .60** −.01
 Estrone 1 −.30*
Placebo DHEAS Testosterone Estrone Cortisol
 DHEA .75** .20 .37** .42**
 DHEAS 1 .24 .55** .25
 Testosterone 1 .31* .28
 Estrone 1 −.06
*

p < .05

**

p < .01

In contrast, within the DHEA condition, the correlation between DHEA and DHEA-S is more moderate and there are substantially larger correlations between testosterone and DHEA/DHEA-S, between estrone and DHEA and between estrone and testosterone. The changes in correlations in the DHEA condition reflect the concurrent increase in DHEA and its direct metabolites (estrone, testosterone) due to DHEA administration (see Table 1). In the case of cortisol, the observed correlation between cortisol and the other hormones in the placebo condition may arise from general health factors (e.g., the correlation of DHEA and cortisol may be due to differences in general levels of adrenal functioning across participants). In these circumstances, the substantial increases in DHEA, testosterone and estrone in the DHEA condition are not accompanied by similar increases in cortisol in the DHEA condition (see Table 1).

3.3 Affective Measures

A paired samples t-test revealed that the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) scores were not significantly different [t(47) = 1.31, p = 0.19, d = 0.19] between the DHEA condition (M = 3.58, SD = 4.19) and the placebo condition (M = 4.21, SD = 5.26). The Symptoms Checklist 90 (SCL-90) also did not differ between the DHEA condition and the placebo condition (p’s > .10) on all nine symptoms (Somatization, Obsessive Compulsive, Interpersonal Sensitivity, Anxiety, Depression, Hostility, Phobic Anxiety, Paranoid Ideation, and Psychoticism).

3.4 Digit Span Performance

Mean performance on the digit span task (proportion correct) as a function of Type of Drug (DHEA vs. placebo) and Type of Span (forward vs. backward) is presented in Table 3. A 2 × 2 (Drug × Span) repeated measures ANOVA was conducted in which the central finding is DHEA administration had no detectible effect [F(1,47) = 2.32, p = 0.13, η2partial = 0.05] on performance. However, there was an effect of the type of span [F(1,47) = 160.17, p < .001, η2partial = 0.77], with forward digit span having a higher proportion correct than backward digit span. There was also no interaction between Type of Drug and Type of Span (F < 1).

Table 3.

Digit Span Proportion and Verbal Span Proportion Correct for the Forward and Backward Tasks as well as Truncated Versions Correct for the Forward and Backward Verbal Span Tasks as a Function of Type of Drug (DHEA vs. Placebo) (means with standard deviations in parentheses)

DHEA Placebo
DS Forward .45 (.13) .47 (.15)
DS Backward .30 (.13) .31 (.17)
VS Forward .09 (.09) .09 (.09)
VS Forward truncated .21 (.20) .20 (.19)
VS Backward .09 (.09) .10 (.10)
VS Backward truncated .21 (.19) .22 (.22)

3.5 Verbal Span Performance

Table 3 also presents mean performance on the verbal span task (proportion correct) as a function of Type of Drug (DHEA vs. Placebo) and Type of Span (forward vs. backward). Given relatively low performance, we also used a secondary measure, proportion correct for lists of 5 or fewer words, to enhance our opportunity to detect effects of DHEA administration. We conducted separate 2 × 2 (Drug × Span) repeated measures ANOVAs for both verbal span measures. On both measures DHEA administration had no detectible effect on proportion correct in either the forward or the backward tasks using either measure (F’s < 1). There were also no significant effects of Type of Span or interactions between Type of Drug and Type of Span (F’s < 1). for both the regular and truncated measures. The null effects of DHEA administration were further confirmed using non-parametric analysis (p’s > 0.50).

3.6 Modified Sternberg Paradigm

Table 4 presents proportion correct and mean RT in the modified Sternberg paradigm as a function of length of word list (1 word vs. 3 words), stimulus type (positive, negative, intrusion) and drug condition (DHEA vs. Placebo). Proportion correct scores and mean RTs were submitted to separate 2 × 2 × 3 (Drug × List Length × Stimulus Type) repeated measures ANOVAs. DHEA administration had no detectable effect on proportion correct or mean reaction time in the modified Sternberg paradigm (F’s < 1) in these analyses. There was a significant effect of stimulus type [F(2,94) = 522.92, p < .001, η2partial = 0.92] on proportion correct. Consistent with past results (Oberauer, 2001), pairwise comparisons revealed accuracy was approximately equal for positive and negative stimuli, but was significantly lower for intrusion stimuli.

Table 4.

Modified Sternberg Paradigm Accuracy Means and Standard Deviations in Parentheses for Three Types of Stimuli (Positive, Negative, Intrusion)

Measure DHEA Placebo
1 Word Stimulus
Positive (Accuracy) .97 (.017) .96 (.016)
Negative .95 (.022) .95 (.018)
Intrusion .92 (.019) .91 (.016)
Positive (RT) 1065.96 (69.43) 1051.44 (68.40)
Negative 1130.92 (79.33) 1114.50 (66.03)
Intrusion 1387.40 (107.96) 1390.13 (123.48)
3 Words Stimulus
Positive (Accuracy) .96 (.017) .95 (.016)
Negative .96 (.018) .95 (.019)
Intrusion .91 (.022) .90 (.019)
Positive (RT) 1224.48 (82.58) 1215.26 (78.04)
Negative 1303.40 (86.77) 1320.34 (85.09)
Intrusion 1606.64 (81.83) 1627.51 (99.33)

A significant effect of list length was found for RTs [F(1,47) = 350.65, p < .001, η2partial = 0.88] with items from the 1 word list judged faster than items from the 3 word list. There was also an effect of stimulus type on reaction time [F(2,94) = 671.42, p < .001, η2partial = 0.93] in which participants responded more slowly to intrusion stimuli than to negative stimuli which were, in turn, responded to more slowly than positive stimuli. Finally, there was an interaction between length of word list and stimulus type [F(2,94) = 15.99, p < .001, η2partial = 0.25] in which the difference in reaction time between intrusion stimuli and the positive/negative stimuli was larger in the 3 word lists than in the 1 word lists..

3. Discussion

The main purpose of this study was to examine how DHEA and its metabolites influence short term memory performance. DHEA administration was effective in manipulating hormone levels (Table 1, Table 10). This supports past research showing that DHEA is an effective way to enhance circulating sex steroid levels (Morales et al, 1994, 1998; Wolf et al, 1997). Because DHEA is metabolized first into androgens, followed by aromatization of estrogens (Barru et al., 1997), the relative effects of DHEA administration on androgens and estrogens merit comment. Examining the circulating levels of each sex steroid, we notice substantial increases in DHEA, DHEA-S and testosterone. Estrone, although significantly different between drug conditions, did not increase as dramatically (see Table 1).

4.1 Prior Results on Short-term Memory Tasks

The results from our short-term memory tasks replicate multiple findings from the prior literature. Backward digit span was more difficult for our participants than forward digit span (see Table 3). Similarly, performance in the modified Sternberg paradigm demonstrated that, as in Oberauer’s (2001) study, accuracy and latency were poorer for intrusion stimuli. Along the same lines, accuracy and latency were poorer on three word lists than on one word lists. In contrast to previous findings, performance on our verbal span forward task and verbal span backward task was approximately equal (see Table 3). This raises an important caution in considering the results on this task and we return to this issue shortly.

4.2 Results of DHEA Administration

There were no significant differences between drug conditions on any of our short-term memory tests. Given the above evidence for the efficacy and metabolism of DHEA administration and the replication of traditional findings in digit span and the modified Sternberg paradigm, these results raise significant questions regarding whether DHEA administration enhances short term memory processes. We have cautions regarding this conclusion on the verbal span task. As mentioned above, the failure to replicate traditional findings on this task raises important questions. For example, given the relatively low level of performance on the verbal span task, this may have impaired our ability to detect effects of DHEA administration on this task.

Further, given the differences in androgenic and estrogenic metabolism cited above, the current results do not necessarily imply that estrogens do not play an important role in verbal short-term memory. Rather, it is possible that, due to the secondary aromatization of estrogens from androgens, the current manipulation of estrogens may not have been strong enough to influence cognitive performance. The prior literature highlighted a relationship between estrogen and short term verbal memory in women, and, based on these findings, it was hypothesized that DHEA administration might benefit our verbal memory measure. The null effect reported here may, however, reflect the limited estrogenic effects of clinically safe doses of DHEA, rather than the absence of estrogenic effects on verbal short-term memory tasks.

Importantly, our correlational findings in digit span and verbal span demonstrated that DHEA levels can be correlated with cognitive performance in a placebo condition (see Tables 5 & 6), even though administration of DHEA does not enhance cognitive performance in the experimental condition. It is possible that maintaining higher levels of DHEA throughout the aging process may be tied to cognition. While this result has been demonstrated across observational and experimental studies (Yanase et al., 2006; Carlson, Sherwin, & Chertkow, 1999), this is, as far as we are aware, the first demonstration of this complex pattern in a single study.

Table 5.

Correlations Between Hormone Levels and Span Tasks

Type of Drug Hormone DHEA DHEAS Testosterone Estrone Cortisol
DHEA DS Forward −.16 −.17 −.19 −.02 .12
DS Backward −.15 −.16 −.13 −.19 .17
VS Forward −.02 .12 .13 .15 .13
VS Backward −.08 .04 .09 .09 .11
VS Forward (truncated) −.03 .12 .12 .15 .13
VS Backward (truncated) −.08 .04 .09 .09 .11
Placebo DS Forward .42** .44** .06 .28 .03
DS Backward .42** .33* .11 .14 .17
VS Forward .38** .24 .08 .09 .15
VS Backward .28 .22 .13 −.03 .06
VS Forward (truncated) .38** .24 .08 .09 .15
VS Backward (truncated) .28 .22 .13 −.03 .06
*

p < .05

**

p < .01

Table 6.

Correlations (Pearson r’s) Between Serum Levels of Hormones and Reaction Time for Three Types of Stimuli (Positive, Negative, Intrusion) (averaged across 1 and 3 word lists) for the Modified Sternberg Paradigm

Measure DHEA DHEA-S Testosterone Estrone Cortisol
DHEA
Positive −.24 −.25 −.21 −.12 −.08
Negative −.08 −.12 −.14 −.22 −.05
Intrusion .02 .07 −.06 −.23 −.09
Placebo
Positive .15 −.01 −.12 −.01 .17
Negative .19 .04 −.07 −.03 .15
Intrusion −.08 −.11 −.06 −.14 .07

Thus, despite substantially increasing levels of a range of sex steroids, and observing correlations between hormones and cognitive performance in the placebo condition, we were not able to find evidence that DHEA administration enhances performance on digit span, verbal span or the modified Sternberg task. These results raise significant questions regarding whether DHEA administration enhances short-term memory processes in post-menopausal women.

4. Experimental Procedure

4.1 Participants

We examined 48 volunteer women 55–80 years old (M = 63.50, SD = 6.85) recruited by newspaper advertisement who were paid $300.00 for their participation. Participants met the World Health Organization’s criterion for post-menopausal status of one year’s absence of menses or bilateral ovariectomy that preceded the study by at least one year. No participant was using any form of hormone replacement therapy.

Participants were excluded if a pre-enrollment medical evaluation revealed contraindications to DHEA, estrogen or androgen treatment (i.e., personal history of, or active, breast cancer or other estrogen-dependent neoplasms, acute liver disease, undiagnosed vaginal bleeding, uncontrolled hypertension, deep venous thrombosis, pulmonary embolus, history of clotting disorders, history of psychiatric or cognitive disorders). Women whose pre-enrollment assays of DHEAS, estradiol or testosterone were above the normal post-menopausal women’s range or whose body mass index (BMI) exceeded 35 were excluded. Our final sample included women with BMIs from 19 to 35 (M = 27.46, SD = 4.36). Use of substances that influence cognition (e.g., amphetamines, benzodiazepines, narcotics, nicotine, steroid hormones, and steroid receptor antagonists) was also grounds for exclusion, as was a serious physical illness within the last year. All women enrolled had a normal mammogram within the prior year and a normal Pap smear within the prior three years.

4.2 Apparatus & Design

Tests were presented on a Dell Latitude D820 laptop computer (2006). All tasks were created using E Prime (2004). Type of Drug (DHEA vs. Placebo) was manipulated using a within subject crossover design. Participants received DHEA for one 4-week period and placebo for the other 4-week period with a 1-week “washout” period between treatments. Order of treatments was counterbalanced across participants.

We administered 50 mg of oral DHEA daily. DHEA was compounded by Belmar Pharmaceutical (Lakewood, CO), the source of DHEA for numerous clinical trials. Placebos consisted of lactose and were identical in appearance to DHEA capsules. The number of pills dispensed was greater than the number needed for purposes of retrospective verification of compliance.

4.3 Measures of Sex Steroids

Serum levels of DHEA, DHEAS, estrone, testosterone and cortisol were measured at each cognitive testing time. Each appointment required 0.25 mL of serum. Samples were collected in red top tubes without a serum separator. Blood was allowed to clot for 20 minutes. Afterward the blood samples were placed in a centrifuge at 4000 rpm for 5 minutes. The serum was then separated and frozen at −80C. Assays were all conducted on site at the General Clinical Research Center of Georgetown University Medical Center by Dr. Soldin’s laboratory. Researchers at Georgetown University (Guo et al., 2004) had recently developed a variant of liquid chromatography tandem mass spectrometry relying on stable isotope dilution to measure steroid hormone levels. These methods have the significant advantage of providing rapid simultaneous quantitation of multiple steroids in a single blood sample.

4.4 Procedure

Participants who responded to newspaper advertisements underwent a 5-minute telephone interview. Individuals who met the entry criteria were set up for a pre-enrollment evaluation at the General Clinical Research Center at Georgetown University Hospital. The evaluation began with informed consent. If the participant agreed to participate in the study a history, including review of illnesses, medications, and contraindications to steroid hormone administration was taken. A physical exam was performed and blood was drawn for a complete blood count (CBC), CHEM 7 (including electrolytes, blood urea nitrogen and creatinine), as well as baseline sex steroid assays (DHEA, DHEAS, testosterone, estrone and cortisol). The Symptom Checklist 90-R (SCL 90-R) (Derogatis & Savitz, 2000) and the Mini-Mental Status Exam (Turvey et al., 2000) were administered.

Participants were assigned to receive DHEA or placebo in a block-randomization scheme. All investigators and participants were blind to treatment status. Upon receipt of the appropriate pillbox, participants were instructed to take one pill each morning with breakfast and to write any significant adverse events on a provided diary sheet.

Cognitive testing occurred in 28 days and participants were instructed to fast after midnight and to not consume any caffeine. At 7:30am the participant was expected to arrive and given breakfast. At 8:00am the participant ingested either 50 mg of DHEA or placebo. A blood draw was performed at 9:50 am. Cognitive testing occurred between 10am and noon on each testing day. We chose this specific time because steroid levels are maximized during this period in the DHEA condition (Hirshman et al 2004a). Each cognitive task was presented once during each testing session. At 12:30pm, the Beck Depression Inventory and the SCL-90 tests were administered.

At the end of the first test day, participants were given appropriate pills, an appointment for the second testing session, and new diary sheets. Participants were instructed to begin taking the pills in one week and a reminder call was made prior to the second testing day. Testing procedures were identical on the second test day, except participants were debriefed at the end of this day.

4.5 Cognitive Measures

We tested participants on cognitive tasks designed to measure the elementary processes influencing short term memory. Order of task presentation was randomized across participants.

4.5.1 Digit Span

Participants were presented with a list of digits ranging from 2 to 10 in a random sequence. After presentation, participants were asked to repeat the digits in the order presented or “forwards” condition. There was also a “backwards” condition in which participants were asked to repeat the digits backwards from the original sequence. The order of these 2 conditions was randomized and there were a total of 45 trials in each condition. The proportion of trials out of 45 trials on which the participant correctly recalled the presented sequence was the participant’s score.

4.5.2 Verbal Span

On each trial, participants were presented with a series of words in a random sequence ranging from 2 to 10 words. A counting task then occurred in which the participant must count backwards from whatever number is presented for a period of 15 seconds. Following this, the participant must write down the words they were presented in the same order, or “forwards” condition. Participants were also asked to write down the words in a “backwards” condition from the original sequence. The order of these conditions was randomized and there were a total of 45 trials in each condition. The proportion of trials out of 45 trials on which the participant correctly recalled the presented sequence was the participant’s score.

4.5.3 Modified Sternberg Paradigm (Oberauer Serial Scanning)

Two lists of items were presented on the computer monitor to the participant (lists contained 1 and 3 words). The word list at the top of the screen was presented in blue ink and the list at the bottom was in red ink. The presentation time for the lists was 1.3 s times the total # of words in the 2 lists. The participant was cued to which list was the target list by a rectangular frame in the middle of the screen in the color of the list the participant should remember. After a 600 ms cue-stimulus interval, the test item was presented. This item was presented in the color of the target list. The participant’s task was to judge if the test item was from the target list or not. Participants were presented with 80 trials. Fifty percent of the tested items were from the target list, 25% of the tested items were from the other presented list (intrusion), and 25% of the tested items had not been presented on either list (negative). The proportion of correctly identified test words and the latency of response were measured.

4.6 Affective Measures

4.6.1 Modified Mini-Mental Status Exam

During the pre-enrollment period, participants were given the Mini-Mental Status Exam (Turvey, Schultz, Arndt, Wallace & Herzog, 2000). This consisted of 11 questions with a total possible score out of 30.

4.6.2 Beck Depression Inventory

The Beck Depression Inventory (Steer, Cavalieri, Leonard & Beck, 1999) was used to measure whether DHEA affected depression. This self-report inventory consists of 21 questions examining a participant’s hedonic state and physiological functioning. Scores less than 10 do not generally motivate clinical inquiries.

4.6.3 Symptom Checklist 90-R

The Symptom Checklist 90-R (SCL 90-R) (Derogatis & Savitz, 2000) was administered during the pre-enrollment period, Test Day 1 and Test Day 2. It consisted of 90 questions about physical and mental ailments that may have bothered them in the past week including that day. The test was then scored in 9 different scales for somatization, obsessive compulsive, interpersonal sensitivity, anxiety, depression, hostility, phobic anxiety, paranoid ideation, and psychoticism.

4.7 Hormone Variables

Serum levels of DHEA, DHEAS, testosterone, estrone and cortisol were also measured.

Highlights.

  • DHEA significantly increased DHEA, DHEAS, Testosterone and Estrone in post-menopausal women.

  • DHEA had no effect on short-term memory performance.

  • In the placebo condition, circulating DHEA was positively associated with short-term memory.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Alhaj H, Massey A, McAllister-Williams R. Effects of DHEA administration on episodic memory, cortisol and mood in healthy young men: a double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Psychopharmacology. 2006;188(4):541–551. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0136-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Barrou Z, Charru P, Liddy C. Dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and aging. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics. 1997;24:233–241. doi: 10.1016/s0167-4943(96)00761-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baulieu E, Robel P. Dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEAS) as neuroactive neurosteroids. Proceedings Of The National Academy Of Sciences Of The United States Of America. 1998;95(8):4089–4091. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.8.4089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Becker J. Estrogen rapidly potentiates amphetamine-induced striatal dopamine release and rotational behavior during micro-dialysis. Neuroscience Letters. 1990;118:169–171. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(90)90618-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Carlson LE, Sherwin B. Higher levels of plasma estradiol and testosterone in healthy elderly men compared with age-matched women may protect aspects of explicit memory. Menopause. 2000;7:168–177. doi: 10.1097/00042192-200007030-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Carlson L, Sherwin B. Relationships among cortisol (CRT), dehydroepiandrosterone-sulfate (DHEAS), and memory in a longitudinal study of healthy elderly men and women. Neurobiology of Aging. 1999;20(3):315–324. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(99)00052-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Carlson L, Sherwin B, Chertkow H. Relationships between dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEAS) and cortisol (crt)plasma levels and everyday memory in alzheimer’s disease patients compared to healthy controls. Hormones and Behavior. 1999;35(3):254–263. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1999.1518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Carlson LE, Sherwin B. Steroid hormones, memory and mood in a healthy elderly population. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1998;23:583–603. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(98)00025-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Derogatis LR, Savitz KL. The SCL-90-R and Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) in primary care. In: Maruish ME, editor. Handbook of psychological assessment in primary care settings. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Mahwah, NJ, US: 2000. pp. 297–334. [Google Scholar]
  10. Duff SJ, Hampson E. A beneficial effect of estrogen on working memory in postmenopausal women taking hormone replacement therapy. Hormones and Behavior. 2000;38:262–276. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2000.1625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gartside S, Griffith N, Kaura V, Ingram C. The neurosteroid dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and its metabolites alter 5-HT neuronal activity via modulation of GABAA receptors. Journal Of Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England) 2010;24(11):1717–1724. doi: 10.1177/0269881109105836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Elsabagh S, Hartley D, File S. Cognitive function in late versus early postmenopausal stage. Maturitas. 2007;56(1):84–93. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2006.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Flood J, Smith G, Roberts E. Dehydroepiandrosterone and its sulfate enhance memory retention in mice. Brain Research. 1988;447(2):269–278. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(88)91129-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gartside S, Griffith N, Kaura V, Ingram C. The neurosteroid dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and its metabolites alter 5-HT neuronal activity via modulation of GABAA receptors. Journal Of Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England) 2010;24(11):1717–1724. doi: 10.1177/0269881109105836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Gibbs R. Effects of gonadal hormone replacement on measures of basal forebrain cholinergic function. Neuroscience. 2000;101:931–938. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00433-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Guo T, Chan M, Soldin S. Steroid profiles using liquid chromatography tandem mass spectrometry with atmospheric pressure photoionization source. Archives of Pathology and Laboratory Medicine. 2004;128(4):469–475. doi: 10.5858/2004-128-469-SPULCM. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hirshman E, Merritt P, Wang CCL, Wierman M, Budescu DV, Kohrt W, Templin JL, Bhasin S. Evidence that androgenic and estrogenic metabolites contribute to the effects of dehydroepiandrosterone on cognition in postmenopausal women. Hormones & Behavior. 2004;45(2):144–155. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2003.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hirshman E, Rhodes D, Zinser M, Merritt P. The effect of smoking cessation on recognition memory, digit span recall and attentional vigilance. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2004b;12:76–83. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.12.1.76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hirshman E, Wells E, Wierman M, Anderson B, Butler A, Senholzi M, Fisher J. The effect of dehyroepiandrosterone (DHEA) on recognition memory decision processes and discrimination in postmenopausal women. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2003;10(1):125–134. doi: 10.3758/bf03196476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kemper S, Sumner The structure of verbal abilities in young and older adults. Psychology and Aging. 2001;16(2):312–322. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kimonides V, Spillantini M, Sofroniew M, Fawcett J, Herbert J. Dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and DHEA-sulfate (DHEAS) protect hippocampal neurons against excitatory amino acid-induced neurotoxicity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of American. 1999;95:1852–1857. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.4.1852. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kolb B, Stewart J. Changes in the neonatal gonadal hormonal environment prevent behavioral sparing and alter cortical morphogenesis after early frontal cortex lesions in male and female rats. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1995;109:285–294. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.109.2.285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Krug R, Born J, Rasch B. A 3-day estrogen treatment improves prefrontal cortex-dependent cognitive function in postmenopausal women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2006;31(8):965–975. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Labrie F, Bélanger A, Cusan L, Gomez JL, Candas B. Marked decline in serum concentrations of adrenal C19 sex steroid precursors and conjugated androgen metabolites during aging. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 1997;82:2396–2402. doi: 10.1210/jcem.82.8.4160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Luo L, Craik F. Aging and memory: A cognitive approach. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;53(6):346–353. doi: 10.1177/070674370805300603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Macpherson SE, Phillips LH, Sala SD. Age, executive function, and social decision making: A dorsolateral prefrontal theory of cognitive aging. Psychology and Aging. 2002;17:598–609. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Majewska M. Neurosteroids: endogenous bimodal modulators of the GABAA receptor. Mechanism of action and physiological significance. Progress In Neurobiology. 1992;38(4):379–395. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(92)90025-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Majewska M, Demirgören S, Spivak C, London E. The neurosteroid dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate is an allosteric antagonist of the GABAA receptor. Brain Research. 1990;526(1):143–146. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90261-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Mellon S, Griffin L. Neurosteroids: biochemistry and clinical significance. Trends In Endocrinology And Metabolism: TEM. 2002;13(1):35–43. doi: 10.1016/s1043-2760(01)00503-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Merritt P, Cobb A, Moissinac L, Hirshman E. Evidence that episodic memory impairment during tobacco abstinence is independent of attentional mechanisms. Journal of General Psychology. 2010;137:331–342. doi: 10.1080/00221309.2010.499395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Mikels J, Lockenhoff CE, Maglio SJ, Carstensen LL, Goldstein MK, Garber A. Following your heart or your head: Focusing on emotions versus information differentially influences the decisions of younger and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 2010;16(1):87–95. doi: 10.1037/a0018500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Miller WL. Early steps in androgen synthesis: from cholesterol to DHEA. Baillière’s Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 1998;12(1):67–81. doi: 10.1016/s0950-351x(98)80461-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Miller K, Conney J, Rasgon N, Fairbanks L, Small G. Mood symptoms and cognitive performance in women estrogen users and nonusers and men. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2002;50(11):1826–1830. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50511.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Morales AJ, Haubricht RH, Hwangt JY, Asakura H, Yen SSC. The effect of six months treatment with a 100 mg daily dose of dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) on circulating sex steroids, body composition and muscle strength in age-advanced men and women. Clinical Endocrinology. 1998;49:421–432. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2265.1998.00507.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Morales A, Nolan J, Nelson J, Yen S. Effects of replacement dose of dehydroepiandrosterone in men and women of advancing age. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 1994;78:1360–1367. doi: 10.1210/jcem.78.6.7515387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Nicolas L, Fry J. The steroid sulfatase inhibitor COUMATE attenuates rather than enhances access of dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate to the brain in the mouse. Brain Research. 2007;1174:92–96. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.07.078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Oberauer K. Removing irrelevant information from working memory. An age-comparative study with the modified Sternberg task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2001;27:948–957. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Orentreich N, Brind JL, Vogelman JH, Andres R, Baldwin H. Long-term longitudinal measurements of plasma dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate in normal men. Journal of Endocrinology and Metabolism. 1992;75:1002–1004. doi: 10.1210/jcem.75.4.1400863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Parsons T, Kratz K, Thompson E, Stanczyk F, Buckwalter J. DHEA supplementation and cognition in postmenopausal women. International Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;116(2):141–155. doi: 10.1080/00207450500341506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Phillips SM, Sherwin BB. Variations in memory function and sex steroid hormones across the menstrual cycle. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1992;17(5):497–506. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(92)90008-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Puy L, MacLusky N, Beker L, Karsan N, Trachtenberg J, Brown T. Immunocytochemical detection of androgen receptors in human temporal cortex. Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology. 1995;55:197–209. doi: 10.1016/0960-0760(95)00165-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Rhodes M, Li P, Flood J, Johnson D. Enhancement of hippocampal acetylcholine release by the neurosteroid dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate: an in vivo microdialysis study. Brain Research. 1996;733(2):284–286. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(96)00751-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Shaywitz S, Shaywitz B, Pugh K, Fulbright K, Skudlarski P, Mencl W, et al. Effect of estrogen on brain activation patterns in postmenopausal women during working memory tasks. JAMA. 1999;281:1197–1202. doi: 10.1001/jama.281.13.1197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Stangl B, Hirshman E, Verbalis J. Administration of dehydroepianrosterone (DHEA) enhances visual-spatial processing in post-menopausal women. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2011;125(5):742–752. doi: 10.1037/a0025151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Steer RA, Cavalieri TA, Leonard DM, Beck AT. Use of the Beck depression inventory for primary care to screen for major depression disorders. General Hospital Psychiatry. 1999;21(2):106–111. doi: 10.1016/s0163-8343(98)00070-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Sulcova J, Hill M, Hampl R, Starka L. Age and sex related differences in serum levels of unconjugated dehydroepiandrosterone and its sulfate in normal subjects. Journal of Endocrinology. 1997;154:57–62. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.1540057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Suzuki M, Wright L, Marwah P, Lardy H, Svendsen C. Mitotic and neurogenic effects of dyhydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) on human neural stem cell cultures derived from the fetal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2004;101(9):3202–3207. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0307325101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Terry A, Buccafusco J, Wilson C. Cognitive dysfunction in neuropsychiatric disorders: selected serotonin receptor subtypes as therapeutic targets. Behavioural Brain Research. 2008;195(1):30–38. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Turvey C, Schultz S, Arndt S, Wallace R, Herzog R. Memory complaints in a community sample aged 70 and older. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2000;48:1435–1441. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2000.tb02634.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  51. Wolf OT, Naumann O, Hellhammer D, Geiben AC, Strasburger CJ, Dressendorfer RA, Pirke KM, Kirschbaum C. Effects of a two-week physiological dehydroepiandrosterone substitution on cognitive performance and well being in healthy elderly women and men. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 1997;82(7):2363–2367. doi: 10.1210/jcem.82.7.4056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Woolley C, Weiland N, McEwen B, Shwarthroin P. Estradiol increases the sensitivity of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells to NMDA receptor-mediated synaptic input: Correlation with dendritic spine density. Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17:1848–1859. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-05-01848.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Yaffe K, Sawaya G, Lieberburg I, Grady D. Estrogen therapy in postmenopausal women: Effects on cognitive function and dementia. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1998;279:688–695. doi: 10.1001/jama.279.9.688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Yamada S, Akishita M, Fukai S, Ogawa S, Yamaguchi K, Matsuyama J, Ouchi Y. Effects of dehydroepiandrosterone supplementation on cognitive function and activities of daily living in older women with mild to moderate cognitive impairment. Geriatrics & Gerontology International. 2010;10(4):280–287. doi: 10.1111/j.1447-0594.2010.00625.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Yanase T, Muta K, Nawata H. Serum concentrations of dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEAS) in oldest japanese women correlate with cognitive activity than activities of daily life. Geriatrics and Gerontology International. 2006;6(3):194–198. [Google Scholar]
  56. Zheng P. Neuroactive steroid regulation of neurotransmitter release in the CNS: action, mechanism and possible significance. Progress In Neurobiology. 2009;89(2):134–152. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES