Abstract
Evidence suggests that the acquisition of recognition memory depends upon CREB-dependent long-lasting changes in synaptic plasticity in the perirhinal cortex. The CREB-responsive microRNA miR-132 has been shown to regulate synaptic transmission and we set out to investigate a role for this microRNA in recognition memory and its underlying plasticity mechanisms. To this end we mediated the specific overexpression of miR-132 selectively in the rat perirhinal cortex and demonstrated impairment in short-term recognition memory. This functional deficit was associated with a reduction in both long-term depression and long-term potentiation. These results confirm that microRNAs are key coordinators of the intracellular pathways that mediate experience-dependent changes in the brain. In addition, these results demonstrate a role for miR-132 in the neuronal mechanisms underlying the formation of short-term recognition memory.
Keywords: LTD, memory, miR-132, perirhinal cortex, Rattus norvegicus
Introduction
There is strong evidence that the acquisition and consolidation of the familiarity discrimination component of visual recognition memory is mediated by synaptic plasticity in the perirhinal cortex (PRh). Lesions of PRh produce impairments in the ability of rats and monkeys to distinguish between novel and familiar objects (Zola-Morgan et al., 1989; Meunier et al., 1993; Mumby & Pinel, 1994; Ennaceur et al., 1996). In vivo electrophysiological recordings have identified a subset of neurons in the PRh which undergo a long-term decrement of neuronal firing upon presentation of a novel image (Fahy et al., 1993). Further studies have investigated the mechanisms of PRh synaptic plasticity and recognition memory, and have identified many common features revealing that long-term depression (LTD) of synaptic transmission in this region may represent the cellular correlate of visual recognition memory (Massey et al., 2001, 2008; Warburton et al., 2003; Griffiths et al., 2008; Tinsley et al., 2009, 2011a,b). In addition we have revealed the presence of two independent parallel pathways that are involved in the acquisition of recognition memory. Inhibition of kainate receptors or muscarinic acetylcholine receptors (mAChRs) blocks the formation of short-term memories but spares long-term memory, whilst inhibition of NMDA receptors or α7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors selectively impairs long-term memory (Barker et al., 2006b; Tinsley et al., 2011a,b).
MicroRNAs (miRNAs) are short (22-nucleotide) non-coding RNAs that bind the 3′ untranslated region of target mRNAs to mediate post-transcriptional gene silencing. miRNAs can be activated in response to neuronal activity and therefore offer a highly effective means of controlling the expression of proteins involved in memory formation. One such miRNA is miR-132, the expression of which is regulated by neuronal activity due to the presence of CRE elements in its promoter region (Vo et al., 2005; Nudelman et al., 2010; Wibrand et al., 2010). Several miRNAs, including miR-132, have been shown to regulate the size and number of dendritic spines and alter synaptic activity (Edbauer et al., 2010; Impey et al., 2010; Lambert et al., 2010). miR-132 has been shown to target p250GAP, which regulates Rac1-PAK-mediated dendrospinogenesis (Vo et al., 2005; Wayman et al., 2008; Impey et al., 2010), and methyl CpG-binding protein 2 (MeCP2), a protein involved in dendritic development and synaptogenesis and whose modulation can alter synaptic plasticity (Collins et al., 2004; Asaka et al., 2006; Moretti et al., 2006; Klein et al., 2007). Repression of these target proteins has been shown to account for many of the observed physiological effects of miR-132 (Vo et al., 2005; Klein et al., 2007; Wayman et al., 2008; Impey et al., 2010).
It has recently been reported that a transgenic mouse overexpressing miR-132 throughout the forebrain showed impairments in recognition memory (Hansen et al., 2010). However, no studies have investigated the effect of miR-132 on measures of synaptic plasticity associated with memory formation. To address this question we produced a lentiviral vector to overexpress miR-132 and used this to selectively transduce neurons within the PRh. We then performed both behavioural and electrophysiological analyses to investigate the function of miR-132.
Materials and methods
Lentiviral vectors
MiR-132 and its flanking sequence was cloned from rat genomic DNA using the following primers: forward 5′-CTAGCCCCGCAGACACTAGC, reverse 5′-CCCCGCCTCCTCTTGCTCTGTA. The gene was then cloned into a lentiviral plasmid under the control of the CMV promoter and upstream of the enhanced green-fluorescent protein (EGFP) cDNA. miR-3120 was used as a control (Scott et al., 2012). Vesicular stomatitis virus glycoprotein pseudotyped lentivirus was produced by cotransfection of HEK293T cells as described previously (Dull et al., 1998; Wong et al., 2005). Viral titre was calculated by fluorescence activated cell sorting analysis of transduced HEK293T cells.
Culture of HeLa cells
HeLa cells were maintained in Dulbecco’s modified Eagle’s medium supplemented with foetal bovine serum (10%), penicillin (50 U/mL), streptomycin (50 μg/mL) and l-glutamine (2 mm). Cells were plated in 24-well plates at a density of 40 000 per well and transduced with lentivirus the following day.
Culture of primary cortical neurons
Cortical neurons were cultured from embryonic day 18 Wistar rat brain as described previously (Howarth et al., 2009). Cortical neurons were transduced with lentivirus after 3 days in vitro (DIV).
Western blotting
Seven days post-transduction cultured neurons were lysed in RIPA buffer [Tris, 50 mm; NaCl, 150 mm; Triton X-100, 1%;sodium deoxycholate, 1%; SDS, 0.1%; and 1× protease inhibitor cocktail (Roche Applied Science), pH 8.0]. Ten micrograms of protein was mixed with an equal volume of sample buffer (Tris, 100 mm; glycerol, 25%; SDS, 2%; bromophenol blue, 0.01%; and DTT, 100 mm; pH 6.8) and run on 8% polyacrylamide gels. Protein was transferred to a PVDF membrane, blocked with 5% milk powder in PBS for 1 hour at room temperature and incubated with primary antibody (1:1000; a kind gift from Tadashi Yamamoto, University of Tokyo) at 4 °C overnight. Membranes were washed in PBS–Tween then incubated with HRP-conjugated secondary antibody (1:10 000; GE Healthcare). Enhanced chemiluminescence was used to visualise the proteins. Membranes were re-probed for α-tubulin (1:2000; Sigma–Aldrich) as a loading control. Densitometry analysis of bands was performed using imagej (NIH) and all measurements normalised to the α-tubulin value. One-way anova tests and post hoc Bonferroni’s multiple comparison tests were used to compare expression between groups.
Transduction of perirhinal cortex
Experiments were performed on 41 adult male Dark Agouti rats (200–250 g; Bantin and Kingman, Hull, UK). Procedures had approval from the University of Bristol ethics committee and were performed in accordance with the United Kingdom Animals Scientific Procedures Act (1986). Transductions were performed under isoflurane-induced anaesthesia as described previously (Griffiths et al., 2008). Equal numbers of viral particles (typically 2 × 106 viral particles per injection) were injected bilaterally in behavioural experiments (36 animals). For electrophysiology experiments miR-312 virus was delivered to one hemisphere whilst EGFP virus was delivered to the contralateral hemisphere to be used as control (five animals). A period of at least 3 weeks was allowed between surgery and the commencement of behavioural testing or electrophysiology to allow recovery and expression of the viral cassette.
Novel object recognition task
Novel object recognition testing was performed as described previously (Griffiths et al., 2008). Rats were habituated to the arena for 3 days (10 min/day) prior to the start of the novel object recognition task. For the sample phase of the task two identical copies of an object were placed in the corners of the arena and the rat was allowed to freely explore the objects for a maximum of 40 s or until it had been in the arena for 4 min. Following the appropriate delay (20 min or 24 h) the rat was returned to the arena for the test phase (3 min) which involved the presentation of a novel object and a third copy of the object presented during the sample phase. The time spent exploring each of the objects was recorded then the discrimination ratio (D2) was calculated as the difference between the time spent exploring the novel and familiar objects divided by the total time exploring both objects. The use of each object as the novel or familiar object was counter balanced between rats. Each rat completed the task at both delays, with a minimum of 7 days interval. A different set of objects was used for each delay. One-sample t-tests were used to determine the significance of the D2 (compared with zero discrimination) for each group. Two-sample t-tests were used to determine the significance of the difference in D2 between groups. All tests were two-tailed and used a significance level of P < 0.05.
RNA extraction and RT-PCR
To obtain RNA from the lentiviral-transduced perirhinal cortex rats were killed according to schedule 1 of the United Kingdom Animals Scientific Procedures Act (1986) following the completion of behavioural testing and the PRh was rapidly dissected and transferred to RNA later solution for subsequent RNA extraction. RNA was extracted from PRh and cultured cortical neurons using the miRVANA kit (Ambion). Mature miR-132 levels were measured by quantitative RT-PCR and normalised to the small RNA U6B using miRNA TaqMan Assays (Applied Biosystems). The mean value of three technical repeats was used to calculate the relative expression of miR-132 by the ΔΔCt method for each biological replicate. The mean and SEM values for each condition were then calculated across biological replicates; n-numbers in the text refer to biological replicates. One-way anova tests and post hoc Bonferroni’s multiple comparison tests were used to determine the significance of viral overexpression in cultured neurons. Two-sample t-tests were used to determine the significance of the difference in miR-132 expression between behavioural groups. All tests were two-tailed and used a significance level of P < 0.05.
Northern blotting
Total RNA (2 μg) was mixed with an equal volume of gel loading buffer (Ambion). Samples were denatured for 2 min at 95 °C and separated on a denaturing 15% polyacrylamide (19:1 acrylamide:bis-acrylamide) gel. The gel was then bathed for 5 min in a 0.5 μg/mL solution of ethidium bromide in 1 × TBE. RNA was transferred to an uncharged nylon membrane (Hybond-N; GE Healthcare) by semi-dry technique in 0.5 × TBE at 400 mA. RNA was UV-fixed to membranes at 254–302 nm for 1 min (Geldoc-it; UVP). DNA oligonucleotides, complementary to mature miR-132 (Eurofins MWG) were radio-labelled with γ32P (PerkinElmer, USA). Membranes were prehybridised for 1 h at 65 °C. Hybridisation buffer comprised 4 × SSC, 10 × Denhardt’s solution and 0.2% SDS. Membranes were then hybridised with radio labelled probe (15 pm in hybridisation buffer) at 37 °C. After 24 h incubation, membranes were washed three times with constant agitation (4 × SSC with 0.2% SDS). Membranes were then exposed to x-ray film (Amersham Biosciences) for 24 h at room temperature before visualising. Blots were then stripped using 1 × SSC and 0.1% SDS for 30 min at 60 °C before membranes were re-probed for total RNA.
Histology
Following the completion of behavioural testing, rats were terminally anaesthetised with pentobarbitone and transcardially perfused with PFA (4% in PBS). Brains were post-fixed in PFA for at least 48 hours before cryoprotection (40% sucrose in PBS). 30 μm sections were cut using a cryostat and sections stored at −20 °C in cryoprotection solution (30% sucrose, 1% PVP-40 and 30% ethylene glycol in PBS). Sections were mounted onto slides and examined for EGFP fluorescence using a Leica DMRB microscope. Images were captured using a Leica DFC340FX camera.
Electrophysiology
Following killing with isoflurane, brains were rapidly removed and placed in ice-cold artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) oxygenated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2. ACSF comprised (in mm) NaCl, 124; KCl, 3; NaHCO3, 26; NaH2PO4, 1.25; CaCl2, 2; MgSO4, 1; and d-glucose, 10. Horizontal slices (400 μm) that included hippocampus, PRh, entorhinal and temporal cortices were cut using a vibroslice (WPI, UK). All slices were left to recover in oxygenated aCSF at room temperature for at least 1 h prior to recording. After recovery, a single slice was placed in a submerged recording chamber and superfused with oxygenated aCSF (temperature, 30–32 °C; flow rate, 2 mL/min).
Evoked field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSPs) were recorded from layers II and III of PRh. Stimulating electrodes were placed on both caudal and rostral sides 0.5 mm from the recording electrode. Stimuli (constant voltage) were delivered alternately to the two stimulating electrodes (each electrode 0.033 Hz). Input–output curves were produced by stimulating initially at 0 V and then at increasing intensities in 10 V steps until a maximal fEPSP was achieved. The minimum intensity to produce an fEPSP discernible from the noise across experiments ranged between 5 and 7V. fEPSPs were reduced to 50–60% of maximum amplitude to achieve a baseline of synaptic transmission before application of stimulating protocols. After a baseline of at least 30 min, high-frequency stimulation (HFS; four trains, each of 100 Hz, 1 s, every 15 s) was delivered on the caudal input to induce long-term potentiation (LTP). Subsequently, 1 hour post-tetanus, carbachol (CCh; 20 μm) was bath-applied for 10 min and the recording continued for a further hour. Bath application of CCh influenced both caudal and rostral input-evoked fEPSPs without significant differences between the two inputs within the same slice (i.e. the previous HFS delivered to the caudal input did not affect the induction of CCh-LTD). fEPSPs were recorded and analysed on-line with winltp software (Anderson & Collingridge, 2007). The peak amplitude of evoked fEPSPs was measured and expressed as percentage of the baseline. For CCh experiments fEPSPs were renormalized to the new baseline prior to addition of CCh. There was no difference between the effects of CCh on fEPSPs recorded from the two inputs and therefore they were averaged. Data pooled across slices are expressed as means ± SEM. LTP and CCh-LTD were measured at 60 and 50 min post-induction respectively. Comparisons of LTP and LTD between EGFP- and miR-132-transduced slices were made over the last data point (mean response over 2 min) for each experiment. The significance of plasticity changes (LTP and CCh-LTD) was established using either paired or unpaired two-tailed t-tests as appropriate. Only one slice was used from each transduced hemisphere.
Results
The miR-132 gene was cloned from rat genomic DNA and inserted upstream of the EGFP coding sequence in the lentiviral cassette (Fig. 1A). To validate the virus, HeLa cells were transduced and EGFP fluorescence was examined to confirm transduction. RNA was extracted and subjected to Northern blot analysis to confirm expression of the mature miRNA (Fig. 1B). Primary cortical neurons transduced with this vector demonstrated EGFP fluorescence along with increased levels of miR-132 compared to control neurons as measured by TaqMan assay (F2,9 = 4.392, P < 0.05; Fig. 1C).To further validate the virus we confirmed knockdown of the miR-132 target p250GAP. Primary cortical neurons were transduced with either the miR-132 virus, an EGFP-only virus or virus expressing a control miRNA, and protein lysates were collected 7 days post-transduction. Western blotting showed reduced levels of p250GAP in the miR-132-transduced neurons (F2,20 = 6.136, P < 0.05, n = 6; Fig. 1D and E) compared to either the EGFP or miRNA control, confirming that the miR-132 expressed from the viral cassette was processed to a functioning mature miRNA. As there was no effect of the control miRNA compared to EGFP the EGFP vector was used a control in the subsequent experiments.
To investigate function in vivo we injected lentiviral miR-132-expressing vectors bilaterally into the PRh and assessed recognition memory using the novel object recognition paradigm (Fig 2A). We observed that short-term memory (20-min delay) was significantly impaired in miR-132-transduced rats compared to EGFP controls (miR-132 D2, 0.123 ± 0.052, n = 18; EGFP D2, 0.262 ± 0.036, n = 18; P < 0.05; Fig. 2B). Long-term memory (24-h delay) was not affected by miR-132 overexpression (miR-132 D2, 0.187 ± 0.054, n = 18; EGFP D2, 0.214 ± 0.043, n = 18; P = 0.704; Fig 2C). There was no bias for any of the objects used as shown by the exploration times for each object during the sample phase (Table 1). Similarly there were no differences in total exploration time during each phase between groups (Table 2).
Table 1.
Exploration times (s; mean ± SEM) | |||
---|---|---|---|
Short-term memory (20 min delay) | Long-term memory (24 h delay) | ||
Object A | Object B | Object A | Object B |
10.824 ± 1.079 | 10.308 ± 0.857 | 7.115 ± 1.107 | 9.990 ± 1.161 |
Presented are the mean exploration times of each object during the sample phase at each delay. There were no significant differences between the exploration times.
Table 2.
Exploration times (s; mean ± SEM) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Short-term memory (20 min delay) | Long-term memory (24 h delay) | |||
Virus | Sample | Test | Sample | Test |
EGFP | 23.847 ± 1.627 | 18.037 ± 1.317 | 18.935 ± 1.068 | 19.042 ± 1.476 |
miR-132 | 23.463 ± 1.989 | 17.777 ± 1.320 | 17.469 ± 1.015 | 19.279 ± 0.970 |
Presented are the mean exploration times during the sample and test phases for each group of rats at each delay. There were no significant differences between the EGFP- and miR-132-transduced rats.
To confirm viral transduction of PRh a subset of rats were perfused 2 weeks after the behavioural testing and slices of the brain examined for EGFP fluorescence. We observed transduction of a large region of PRh particularly in the deeper layers. The transduced neurons were seen over an anterior–posterior range of > 1 mm within the region from bregma −4.8 to −6.3 mm (Fig. 3A–C). To quantify the expression of miR-132 in transduced PRh we extracted the RNA from this region of a second group of rats 2 weeks after behavioural testing and analysed using a miR-132-specific TaqMan assay. We observed a nearly twofold increase in miR-132 levels in RNA from miR-132-transduced PRh compared to EGFP-transduced control (miR-132, 1.84 ± 0.314, n = 8 hemispheres; EGFP, 1.00 ± 0.168, n = 8 hemispheres; P < 0.05; Fig. 3D).
We next investigated the cellular mechanisms underlying the miR-132-induced memory impairment. To achieve this, miR-132 was injected into the left PRh and EGFP injected into the right PRh of each animal. We then performed electrophysiological recordings in acute slices of PRh following these in vivo viral transductions (only one slice was used from each transduced hemisphere; Fig. 4A and B). Transduction of PRh with miR-132 lentivirus did not affect basal synaptic activity as shown by input–output analysis (Fig. 4C).
After the recording of a stable baseline (30 min) HFS was delivered to the caudal input whilst the rostral input served as a control (for clarity, data for the rostral input are not shown). HFS induced LTP of the fEPSP in slices from the EGFP-transduced hemisphere (60 min post-HFS, 116.0 ± 3.5% of baseline, n = 5 slices; P < 0.01; Fig. 4D and F). Slices from the miR-132-transduced hemisphere showed no significant LTP (60 min post-HFS, 105.0 ± 3.5% of baseline, n = 5 slices; P = 0.102; Fig. 4D and F). However, the fEPSP amplitude one hour following HFS did not significantly differ between EGFP- and miR-132-transduced slices (Fig 4F; P = 0.058).
Subsequently, we investigated whether overexpression of miR-132 was interfering with mAChR-dependent plasticity mechanisms, as we have shown previously (Warburton et al., 2003; Tinsley et al., 2011a,b) that blockade of mAChRs by scopolamine produces similar impairments in recognition memory as the overexpression of miR-132 observed in the current study (i.e. impairment at short delay but not at long delay). Application of CCh to acute PRh slices induces a long-lasting depression of synaptic transmission (Massey et al., 2001). In acute slices from the EGFP-transduced hemisphere, application of CCh (20 μm) induced a large acute depression of synaptic transmission measured immediately before CCh washout (40 min fEPSP, 33.6 ± 4.3% of baseline, n = 3 slices, P < 0.01; Fig. 4E and F). In slices from the miR-132-transduced hemisphere there was also an acute depression of synaptic transmission (40 min fEPSP, 59.7 ± 8.0% of baseline, n = 3 slices, P < 0.01; Fig. 4E and F) but this was significantly reduced compared with the depression in control EGFP-transduced slices (Fig 4F; P < 0.05). The acute depression was followed by long-lasting depression in slices from the EGFP-transduced hemisphere (50 min post-CCh, 79.7 ± 2.7% of baseline, n = 3 slices, P < 0.01; Fig. 4E and F). However, in slices from the miR-132-transduced hemisphere there was no long-term depression and the responses returned to baseline amplitude (50 min post-CCh, 94.9 ± 2.3% of baseline, n = 3 slices, P = 0.35; Fig. 4E and F).The fEPSP amplitude 50 min after the washout of CCh was significantly smaller in the EGFP-transduced slices than in the miR-132-transduced slices (Fig 4F; P < 0.05).
Discussion
There is increasing evidence that miRNAs play a pivotal role in regulating synaptic function. We have now shown that the specific expression of miR-132 within the PRh impairs short-term recognition memory, a process known to be dependent on the activity of the PRh. Furthermore we have shown, in miR-132-transduced PRh slices, deficits in synaptic plasticity that are probably substrates for the behavioural impairment.
Hansen et al. (2010) demonstrated impaired recognition memory in transgenic mice overexpressing miR-132 throughout the brain. Hence, the overexpression of miR-132 could potentially mediate its effects during neuronal development and by altering gene expression in multiple brain regions. Our results confirm and refine this previous study by showing that overexpression of miR-132 that is restricted to the adult PRh leads to impairment in recognition memory, and also highlight changes in synaptic plasticity that may underlie this impairment. It is interesting to note that only short-term memory mechanisms were affected by the overexpression of miR-132. We have previously demonstrated dissociation between the pathways that underlie short- and long-term recognition memory. Inhibition of either kainite receptors or mAChRs impairs only short-term memory leaving long-term memory intact (Warburton et al., 2003; Barker et al., 2006a,b; Tinsley et al., 2011a,b). In contrast, inhibition of NMDA receptors, group I and group II metabotropic glutamate receptors, nicotinic acetylcholine receptors or voltage-dependent calcium channels spares short-term memory but abolishes long-term memory (Barker et al., 2006a,b; Seoane et al., 2009; Tinsley et al., 2011a,b). These findings clearly demonstrate the presence of at least two independent pathways for the acquisition of recognition memory, with different temporal properties of induction and duration. Our data suggest that miR-132 downregulates proteins whose expression is required for the fast-onset but short-duration memory pathways. Interestingly, activation of many of the receptors and channels required for the slow-onset long-lasting mechanisms would result in an increase in intracellular calcium concentration, which could lead to the phosphorylation of CREB (which we have previously demonstrated to be required for long-term memory; Warburton et al., 2005) and the transcription of miR-132. Therefore a delayed, downstream, increase in miR-132 may actually be permissive to long-term memory.
miR-132 targets p250GAP, a Rac1 GTPase-activating protein (Vo et al., 2005). By repressing p250GAP levels miR-132 increases (via Rac1–PAK signalling) the activity of the actin depolymerising protein n-cofilin (Impey et al., 2010). This modulation of actin turnover in dendritic spines has been shown to regulate both spine size and spine density (Edbauer et al., 2010; Hansen et al., 2010; Impey et al., 2010). Inhibition of miR-132 in primary hippocampal cultures decreased spine density and size (Impey et al., 2010) whereas overexpression in vivo increased spine density (Hansen et al., 2010). These morphological effects are accompanied by changes in basal synaptic transmission. Although there is some variability between individual studies, miR-132 generally appears to have a positive effect on both the frequency and amplitude of mEPSCs (Edbauer et al., 2010; Impey et al., 2010; Lambert et al., 2010) and also modulates short-term synaptic plasticity (Lambert et al., 2010).
We now demonstrate that miR-132 can also regulate longer-term synaptic plasticity. Application of the mAChR agonist carbachol to slices of PRh results is a large acute depression of synaptic transmission (Massey et al., 2001). In slices of PRh that had been transduced in vivo with miR-132 this acute depression was significantly reduced. The time-course of the acute phase of CCh-LTD coincides with the period at which a behavioural deficit was observed in the miR-132-overexpressing rats and, furthermore, we have previously reported that activity of mAChRs is required for novel object recognition memory at this time delay (Warburton et al., 2003; Tinsley et al., 2011a,b). The reduced acute depression and impairment of novel object recognition reported in this paper suggests an impairment in mAChR signalling caused by the overexpression of miR-132. The targets of miR-132 responsible for this deficit are yet to be elucidated.
The effects of miR-132 overexpression of the late phase of CCh LTD and on LTP were more modest. Both these forms of plasticity were impaired in slices from miR-132-transduced slices compared to controls although the effects were small compared to the acute CCh-LTD deficit. We did not observe any effect of miR-132 overexpression on long-term memory tested at 24 h so one would not necessarily predict effects on the later phases of plasticity. The impairments in both LTD and LTP indicate that this miRNA has a general negative effect on synaptic plasticity mechanisms and does not shift the balance towards either depression or potentiation. The impairment in LTD would be predicted by the overexpression of miR-132 reducing the actin depolymerisation underlying LTD-associated spine shrinkage (Zhou et al., 2004). However, one would predict the opposite effect on LTP (Fortin et al., 2010). It is possible that the impairment we find in LTP is due to some homeostatic adjustment to the chronic overexpression of miR-132, but the exact mechanisms remain to be elucidated. Tognini & Pizzorusso (2012) have proposed a model whereby precise control of miR-132 levels is required for regulation of synaptic plasticity. They suggest that if levels are too high then dendritic spines will be excessively stable, impairing plasticity; likewise, if levels are too low then the spines will be very unstable, also hindering plasticity.
Whilst this manuscript was in preparation two groups have demonstrated that miR-132 regulates experience-dependent ocular dominance plasticity (Mellios et al., 2011; Tognini et al., 2011). Visual activity was shown to increase the expression of miR-132 in primary visual cortex. Expression of a sponge construct to inhibit the function of miR-132 blocked ocular dominance plasticity induced by monocular deprivation in control animals (Mellios et al., 2011; Tognini et al., 2011).
Our observations give empirical support to the hypothesis that miR-132 regulates experience-dependent synaptic function. We demonstrate that the impaired plasticity seen as a result of miR-132 overexpression underlies a functional deficit in short-term recognition memory. These results support a role for miRNAs in co-ordinating the complex changes in protein expression that underpin the synaptic mechanisms that control memory acquisition.
These data provide novel insight into the importance of miR-132 in regulating the neuronal function that underpins learning and memory.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to acknowledge the financial support from the Medical Research Council, the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council and the Wellcome Trust.
Glossary
- CCh
carbachol
- D2
discrimination ratio
- DIV
days in vitro
- EGFP
enhanced green-fluorescent protein
- fEPSP
field excitatory postsynaptic potential
- HFS
high-frequency stimulation
- LTD
long-term depression
- LTP
long-term potentiation
- mAChR
muscarinic acetylcholine receptor
- MeCP2
methyl CpG-binding protein 2
- miRNA
microRNA
- PRh
perirhinal cortex
References
- Anderson WW, Collingridge GL. Capabilities of the WinLTP data acquisition program extending beyond basic LTP experimental functions. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2007;162:346–356. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asaka Y, Jugloff DGM, Zhang L, Eubanks JH, Fitzsimonds RM. Hippocampal synaptic plasticity is impaired in the Mecp2-null mouse model of Rett syndrome. Neurobiol. Dis. 2006;21:217–227. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2005.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barker GRI, Bashir ZI, Brown MW, Warburton EC. A temporally distinct role for group I and group II metabotropic glutamate receptors in object recognition memory. Learn. Mem. 2006a;13:178–186. doi: 10.1101/lm.77806. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barker GRI, Warburton EC, Koder T, Dolman NP, More JCA, Aggleton JP, Bashir ZI, Auberson YP, Jane DE, Brown MW. The different effects on recognition memory of perirhinal kainate and NMDA glutamate receptor antagonism: implications for underlying plasticity mechanisms. J. Neurosci. 2006b;26:3561–3566. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3154-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins AL, Levenson JM, Vilaythong AP, Richman R, Armstrong D, Noebels JL, Sweatt JD, Zoghbi HY. Mild overexpression of MeCP2 causes a progressive neurological disorder in mice. Hum. Mol. Genet. 2004;13:2679–2689. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddh282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dull T, Zufferey R, Kelly M, Mandel RJ, Nguyen M, Trono D, Naldini L. A third-generation lentivirus vector with a conditional packaging system. J. Virol. 1998;72:8463–8471. doi: 10.1128/jvi.72.11.8463-8471.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edbauer D, Neilson JR, Foster KA, Wang C-F, Seeburg DP, Batterton MN, Tada T, Dolan BM, Sharp PA, Sheng M. Regulation of synaptic structure and function by FMRP-associated microRNAs miR-125b and miR-132. Neuron. 2010;65:373–384. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ennaceur A, Neave N, Aggleton JP. Neurotoxic lesions of the perirhinal cortex do not mimic the behavioural effects of fornix transection in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 1996;80:9–25. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(96)00006-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fahy FL, Riches IP, Brown MW. Neuronal activity related to visual recognition memory: long-term memory and the encoding of recency and familiarity information in the primate anterior and medial inferior temporal and rhinal cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 1993;96:457–472. doi: 10.1007/BF00234113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fortin DA, Davare MA, Srivastava T, Brady JD, Nygaard S, Derkach VA, Soderling TR. Long-term potentiation-dependent spine enlargement requires synaptic Ca2+-permeable AMPA receptors recruited by CaM-Kinase I. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:11565–11575. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1746-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths S, Scott H, Glover C, Bienemann A, Ghorbel MT, Uney J, Brown MW, Warburton EC, Bashir ZI. Expression of long-term depression underlies visual recognition memory. Neuron. 2008;58:186–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hansen KF, Sakamoto K, Wayman GA, Impey S, Obrietan K. Transgenic miR132 alters neuronal spine density and impairs novel object recognition memory. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e15497. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howarth JL, Glover CPJ, Uney JB. HSP70 interacting protein prevents the accumulation of inclusions in polyglutamine disease. J. Neurochem. 2009;108:7. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05847.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Impey S, Davare M, Lasiek A, Fortin D, Ando H, Varlamova O, Obrietan K, Soderling TR, Goodman RH, Wayman GA. An activity-induced microRNA controls dendritic spine formation by regulating Rac1-PAK signaling. Mol. Cell. Neurosci. 2010;43:146–156. doi: 10.1016/j.mcn.2009.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein ME, Lioy DT, Ma L, Impey S, Mandel G, Goodman RH. Homeostatic regulation of MeCP2 expression by a CREB-induced microRNA. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10:1513–1514. doi: 10.1038/nn2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lambert TJ, Storm DR, Sullivan JM. MicroRNA132 modulates short-term synaptic plasticity but not basal release probability in hippocampal neurons. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:8. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Massey PV, Bhabra G, Cho K, Brown MW, Bashir ZI. Activation of muscarinic receptors induces protein synthesis-dependent long-lasting depression in the perirhinal cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2001;14:145–152. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01631.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Massey PV, Phythian D, Narduzzo K, Warburton EC, Brown MW, Bashir ZI. Learning-specific changes in long-term depression in adult perirhinal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:7548–7554. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1935-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mellios N, Sugihara H, Castro J, Banerjee A, Le C, Kumar A, Crawford B, Strathmann J, Tropea D, Levine SS, Edbauer D, Sur M. miR-132, an experience-dependent microRNA, is essential for visual cortex plasticity. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:1240–1242. doi: 10.1038/nn.2909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meunier M, Bachevalier J, Mishkin M, Murray E. Effects on visual recognition of combined and separate ablations of the entorhinal and perirhinal cortex in rhesus monkeys. J. Neurosci. 1993;13:5418–5432. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-12-05418.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moretti P, Levenson JM, Battaglia F, Atkinson R, Teague R, Antalffy B, Armstrong D, Arancio O, Sweatt JD, Zoghbi HY. Learning and memory and synaptic plasticity are impaired in a mouse model of Rett syndrome. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:319–327. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2623-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mumby DG, Pinel JPJ. Rhinal cortex lesions and object recognition in rats. Behav. Neurosci. 1994;108:11–18. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.108.1.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nudelman AS, DiRocco DP, Lambert TJ, Garelick MG, Le J, Nathanson NM, Storm DR. Neuronal activity rapidly induces transcription of the CREB-regulated microRNA-132, in vivo. Hippocampus. 2010;20:492–498. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott H, Howarth J, Lee YB, Wong L-F, Bantounas I, Phylactou L, Verkade P, Uney JB. MiR-3120 Is a mirror microRNA that targets heat shock cognate protein 70 and auxilin messenger RNAs and regulates clathrin vesicle uncoating. J. Biol. Chem. 2012;287:14726–14733. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M111.326041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seoane A, Massey PV, Keen H, Bashir ZI, Brown MW. L-type voltage-dependent calcium channel antagonists impair perirhinal long-term recognition memory and plasticity processes. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:9534–9544. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5199-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tinsley CJ, Narduzzo KE, Ho JW, Barker GRI, Brown MW, Warburton EC. A role for calcium-calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II in the consolidation of visual object recognition memory. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2009;30:1128–1139. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06917.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tinsley CJ, Fontaine- Palmer NS, Vincent M, Endean EP, Aggleton JP, Brown MW, Warburton EC. Differing time dependencies of object recognition memory impairments produced by nicotinic and muscarinic cholinergic antagonism in perirhinal cortex. Learn. Mem. 2011a;18:484–492. doi: 10.1101/lm.2274911. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tinsley CJ, Narduzzo KE, Brown MW, Warburton EC. A role for the CAMKK pathway in visual object recognition memory. Hippocampus. 2011b;22:466–476. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tognini P, Pizzorusso T. MicroRNA212/132 family: molecular transducer of neuronal function and plasticity. Int. J. Biochem. Cell Biol. 2012;44:6–10. doi: 10.1016/j.biocel.2011.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tognini P, Putignano E, Coatti A, Pizzorusso T. Experience-dependent expression of miR-132 regulates ocular dominance plasticity. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:1237–1239. doi: 10.1038/nn.2920. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vo N, Klein ME, Varlamova O, Keller DM, Yamamoto T, Goodman RH, Impey S. A cAMP-response element binding protein-induced microRNA regulates neuronal morphogenesis. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA. 2005;102:16426–16431. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0508448102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warburton EC, Koder T, Cho K, Massey PV, Duguid G, Barker GRI, Aggleton JP, Bashir ZI, Brown MW. Cholinergic neurotransmission is essential for perirhinal cortical plasticity and recognition memory. Neuron. 2003;38:987–996. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00358-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warburton EC, Glover CPJ, Massey PV, Wan H, Johnson B, Bienemann A, Deuschle U, Kew JNC, Aggleton JP, Bashir ZI, Uney J, Brown MW. cAMP responsive element-binding protein phosphorylation is necessary for perirhinal long-term potentiation and recognition memory. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:6296–6303. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0506-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wayman GA, Davare M, Ando H, Fortin D, Varlamova O, Cheng H-YM, Marks D, Obrietan K, Soderling TR, Goodman RH, Impey S. An activity-regulated microRNA controls dendritic plasticity by down-regulating p250GAP. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 2008;105:9093–9098. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0803072105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wibrand K, Panja D, Tiron A, Ofte ML, Skaftnesmo K-O, Lee CS, Pena JTG, Tuschl T, Bramham CR. Differential regulation of mature and precursor microRNA expression by NMDA and metabotropic glutamate receptor activation during LTP in the adult dentate gyrus in vivo. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2010;31:636–645. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07112.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong L-F, Scott Ralph G, Walmsley LE, Bienemann AS, Parham S, Kingsman SM, Uney JB, Mazarakis ND. Lentiviral-mediated delivery of Bcl-2 or GDNF protects against excitotoxicity in the Rat hippocampus. Mol. Ther. 2005;11:89–95. doi: 10.1016/j.ymthe.2004.08.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou Q, Homma KJ, Poo M. Shrinkage of dendritic spines associated with long-term depression of hippocampal synapses. Neuron. 2004;44:749–757. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zola-Morgan S, Squire L, Amaral D, Suzuki W. Lesions of perirhinal and parahippocampal cortex that spare the amygdala and hippocampal formation produce severe memory impairment. J. Neurosci. 1989;9:4355–4370. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-12-04355.1989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]