Skip to main content
Brain and Behavior logoLink to Brain and Behavior
. 2012 Aug 22;2(5):620–627. doi: 10.1002/brb3.88

Mild cognitive impairment: effect of education on the verbal and nonverbal tasks performance decline

Konstantinos Vadikolias 1, Anna Tsiakiri-Vatamidis 1, Grigorios Tripsianis 2, Georgios Tsivgoulis 1,5, Panagiotis Ioannidis 3, Aspasia Serdari 4, John Heliopoulos 1, Miltos Livaditis 4, Charitomeni Piperidou 1
PMCID: PMC3489814  PMID: 23139907

Abstract

We sought to longitudinally evaluate the potential association of educational level with performance on verbal and nonverbal tasks in individuals with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). We evaluated patients with MCI, age >50 years, no medication intake, absent vascular risk factors, and no lesions on brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Each patient underwent a clinical assessment packet and a series of neuropsychological tests of the language and constructional praxis subtests of Cambridge Cognitive Examination (CAMGOG) and the Boston naming test (BNT), at baseline, 6 months, and 12 months. Educational levels were defined taking into account the total years of education, the school level, and diplomas. MCI patients with low education level showed a stepwise reduction in scores of naming objects (NO; P = 0.009), definition (DF; P = 0.012), language (LT; P = 0.021), constructional praxis (CD; P = 0.022), confrontation naming skills (BXB; P = 0.033), phonemic help (BFB; P = 0.041), and BNT (P = 0.002). Analysis of covariance, controlling for baseline scores, showed that education was associated with NO score (P = 0.002), DF score (P = 0.005), LT (P = 0.008), CD score (P = 0.008), BXB score (44.36 ± 1.84, P = 0.0001), BFB (P = 0.022), and BNT (P = 0.004). Our findings indicate that education appeared to affect verbal and nonverbal task performance in MCI patients. Despite the fact that higher educated patients are more acquainted with the tasks, slower deterioration in consecutive follow-up examinations could be explained by the cognitive reserve theory. The potential association of this protective effect with delayed onset of symptoms deserves further investigation.

Keywords: Cognitive reserve, mild cognitive impairment, nonverbal, verbal

Introduction

Education is considered to provide a cognitive and neurological reserve through neuronal changes or increased efficacy of processing networks. The “reserve” hypothesis suggests that education should affect the clinical expression of Alzheimer's disease (AD). The concept of cognitive reserve has been proposed to account for the disjunction between the degree of brain damage or pathology and its clinical manifestations (Stern 2009). Twenty-five percent of elders whose neuropsychological testing is unimpaired prior to death meet full pathologic criteria for AD (Ince 2001), suggesting that this degree of pathology does not invariably result in clinical dementia. Educational and occupational exposure and leisure activities are considered that as related with a reduced risk of developing dementia (Stern 2009). Neuropathologic correlations support this theory showing that individuals with greater cognitive reserve, as reflected in years of education, are better able to cope with AD brain pathology without observable cognitive deficits (Roe et al. 2007). However, results from studies examining the relation of the education level with other than the clinical onset aspects, such as the rate of cognitive decline, were not consistent. In a study of AD patients with mild or moderate stage, higher educational attainment was associated with a slower rate of cognitive decline on the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) (Fritsch et al. 2001). Another study showed that higher educational attainment was associated with a slightly accelerated rate of cognitive deterioration (Wilson et al. 2009). Data analysis of a large cohort of participants in the Victoria Longitudinal Study showed that years of education were strongly related to cognitive level in all domains, particularly verbal fluency, but education was not related to rates of change over time for any cognitive domain (Wilson et al. 2004). In a prospective community survey in old subjects without an established clinical diagnosis of AD, education was robustly associated with level of cognitive function but not with the rate of cognitive decline (Zahodne et al. 2011). A meta-analysis of data of 34 previously published studies showed that education, hypertension, objective indices of health, cardiovascular disease, and apolipoprotein E (APOE) were associated with cognitive decline in old-age subjects (Anstey and Christensen 2000).

As mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is a clinically and pathologically heterogeneous state, showing a conversion rate into dementia of 11-33% within 2 years (Gauthier et al. 2006) or approximately 12% per year (Petersen et al. 1999; Anchisi et al. 2005), the question about the appliance of the cognitive reserve theory in MCI has probable conflicting answers. Recent investigations based on neuroimaging measurements (Solé-Padullésa et al. 2009), biochemical methods (Rolstad et al. 2010), and epidemiological studies (Afgin et al. 2012) were indicative that the cognitive reserve hypothesis may be applied also in MCI subjects.

In view of the former considerations, we sought to evaluate whether higher educated subjects with amnestic MCI (aMCI), without systemic diseases, cerebrovascular disease, hypertension, or other vascular risk factors achieve better performance on a series of verbal and nonverbal tasks than lower educated individuals and whether this effect of education persists in a series of repeated examinations over time, supporting the cognitive reserve theory.

Materials and Method

Subjects

We evaluated prospectively a cohort of consecutive individuals referred from the Dementia Outpatient Clinic fulfilling the following inclusion criteria: (1) diagnosis of aMCI (Petersen et al. 2001), (2) age 50 years or older, and (3) fluency in Greek language. We excluded subjects with score 13 or higher on the Hamilton Depression Scale (Hamilton 1967) and 12 or higher on the Neuro-Psychiatric Inventory (NPI; Cummings et al. 1994), presence of concomitant neurological or psychiatric disorders or systemic diseases, severe and uncorrected visual or auditory handicaps that would interfere with test performance or cognitive disorders, cognitive decline related to other causes (e.g., hypothyroidism), family history of dementia, clinical or neuroimaging evidence (e.g., silent infarcts or white-matter lesions on brain magnetic resonance imaging [MRI]) of vascular cognitive impairment, vascular risk factors (hypertension, diabetes mellitus, metabolic syndrome, heart disease, current smoking, and hyperlipidemia), and intake of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors (donepezil, rivastigmine, and galantamine), memantine, or other drugs with known direct CNS effects.

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of our institution. All participants and their caregivers were informed and gave informed consent for taking part in this study.

Clinical evaluation – neuropsychological tests

Each subject underwent the clinical assessment packet recommended by the Consortium to Establish a Registry for AD (CERAD) (Morris et al. 1989) and a hemi-structural interview. Neurological examination and psychiatric evaluation were performed by a team of experienced neurologists and psychiatrists. Cognitive tests were performed by a neuropsychologist (A.T.). All participants were examined at baseline, 6 months, and 12 months. All the measurements performed by the same examiner over time. Educational level was divided into two categories: (a) low: nonhigh school graduates or <6 years of education and high school graduates or maximum 15 years of education, (b) high: college/university or professional school graduates or >15 years of education.

As an overall measure for cognitive impairment, we used the MMSE (Folstein et al. 1975). We selected neuropsychological tests primary reflecting verbal and nonverbal functions. Verbal tests included the language subtest of Cambridge Cognitive Examination (CAMCOG) (Huppert et al. 1995, 1996). CAMCOG is designed to assess the range of cognitive functions required for a diagnosis of dementia, and to detect mild degrees of cognitive impairment which assesses naming objects (NO score: 0–14), comprehension (UN score: 0–7), definition (DF score: 0–6), repetition (RP score: 0–1), language (LT score: 0–28), and abstractive thought (AT score: 0–8). Boston naming test (BNT) (Kaplan et al. 1983) was also included in verbal assessment examining confrontation naming skills without help (BXB), semantic help (BSB), phonemic help (BFB), and time needed to complete the task (BT). Nonverbal tests comprised the constructional praxis subtest of CAMCOG examining copying and drawing (CD score: 0–6), spontaneous writing (SW score: 0–1), ideational praxis (IP score: 0–5), following commands (FC score: 0–4), and writing (WR score: 0–2) (score 0 indicates a poor performance).

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS), version 19.0 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL). The normality of continuous variables was tested with Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. Continuous variables were expressed as mean ± standard deviation (SD), and categorical variables were expressed as frequencies and percentages (%). The chi-square test and Student's t-test were used to evaluate differences in patients' characteristics between patients with low and high education level. Repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to examine the changes of the scores of cognitive function tests throughout the follow-up time; post hoc analysis was performed using Bonferroni's correction for multiple comparisons. The interaction between levels of education and the change of cognitive function tests over time was established by two-way analysis of variance. Linear regression analysis and analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) were performed to investigate the effect of education on the cognitive function tests on the 12th month, adjusting for baseline scores. Correlation calculations between education (in years) and the changes of the scores of cognitive function tests were performed by Pearson's correlation coefficient (r). All tests were two tailed, and statistical significance was considered for P-values less than 0.05.

Results

A total of 32 patients with aMCI (mean age 68.81 ± 8.40 years, 65.6% men) met the inclusion criteria. MMSE score was 27.88 ± 1.62. Years of education ranged from 0 to 16, with a median value of 12 years; patients were divided into following two educational levels: low level (n = 18) and high level (n = 14). The two educational groups did not differ in terms of gender (61.1% men vs. 71.4% men, P = 0.542), age (69.17 ± 9.10 years vs. 68.36 ± 8.50 years, P = 0.799), disease duration >2 years (33.3% vs. 42.9%, P = 0.581), and MMSE score (27.39 ± 1.61 vs. 28.53 ± 1.66, P = 0.060). Two subjects (low education level group) fulfilled the criteria of AD at the last 12-month assessment.

Scores of all cognitive function tests at baseline, 6 months, and 12 months in relation to the education level are shown in Tables 13. Within MCI patients with low education level, one-way repeated measures ANOVA showed a progressive reduction over time of the performance in the following tests: NO (P = 0.001), DF (P = 0.021), LT (P = 0.006), AT (P = 0.019), CD (P = 0.018), BXB (P = 0.011), and BNT (P = 0.001); a tendency toward lower scores over time were also observed in the BFB (P = 0.080) and ideational praxis score (P = 0.061). On the contrary, none of the tests changed significantly over time within MCI patients with high education level. Education influenced the performance over the follow-up time of seven of the above function tests, as the two-way mixed ANOVA showed that the interaction between the levels of education and the change over time was statistically significant for NO (P = 0.009), DF (P = 0.012), LT (P = 0.021), CD (P = 0.022), BXB (P = 0.033), BFB (P = 0.041), and BNT (P = 0.002) (Tables 13).

Table 1.

Verbal scores of subjects with MCI in relation to their educational level

Verbal scores (mean values ± SD)

Baseline 6th month 12th month P-value ΔScore0_12
Naming objects score 0.0092
 Low education level 10.33 ± 1.37 10.22 ± 1.52 9.28 ± 1.87 0.0011 −1.05 ± 1.26
 High education level 12.00 ± 1.46 12.22 ± 1.30 12.36 ± 1.08 0.7121 0.36 ± 0.683
Comprehension score 0.8222
 Low education level 6.00 ± 0.77 5.89 ± 0.67 5.66 ± 0.97 0.3501 −0.33 ± 1.24
 High education level 6.36 ± 0.73 6.43 ± 0.46 6.36 ± 0.70 0.6911 0.00 ± 0.00
Definition score 0.0122
 Low education level 4.83 ± 0.78 4.78 ± 0.80 4.33 ± 1.02 0.0211 −0.50 ± 0.86
 High education level 5.43 ± 0.82 5.36 ± 0.75 5.65 ± 0.52 0.2731 0.22 ± 0.463
Repetition score 0.4302
 Low education level 0.50 ± 0.51 0.67 ± 0.49 0.61 ± 0.50 0.1761 0.11 ± 0.47
 High education level 1.00 ± 0.00 1.00 ± 0.00 0.93 ± 0.31 0.5771 −0.07 ± 0.31
Language score 0.0212
 Low education level 21.66 ± 2.28 21.56 ± 2.15 19.89 ± 3.34 0.0061 −1.78 ± 2.88
 High education level 24.79 ± 1.91 25.00 ± 2.06 25.29 ± 1.75 0.6941 0.50 ± 0.903
Abstractive thought 0.7032
 Low education level 4.50 ± 1.62 4.88 ± 1.65 4.05 ± 1.69 0.0191 −0.44 ± 1.10
 High education level 6.07 ± 1.06 6.21 ± 1.12 6.00 ± 1.30 0.7881 −0.07 ± 0.66

ΔScore0_12, 12-month change in verbal scores; MCI, mild cognitive impairment.

Statistical significance: 1time effect within the same level of education group and 2interaction education level × time effect; 3statistically significant difference compared with low education level.

Table 3.

Boston Naming Test scores of subjects with MCI in relation to their educational level

Boston Naming Test scores (mean values ± SD)

Baseline 6th month 12th month P-value1 ΔScore0_12
Naming without help score 0.0333
 Low education level 33.50 ± 4.84 33.50 ± 5.15 30.55 ± 5.75 0.0111 −2.95 ± 5.10
 High education level 42.21 ± 4.93 43.00 ± 5.48 43.64 ± 4.48 0.5361 1.43 ± 4.033
Naming semantic help score 0.9653
 Low education level 4.39 ± 2.52 4.33 ± 2.52 4.33 ± 2.33 0.9951 −0.06 ± 3.17
 High education level 4.57 ± 1.86 4.22 ± 1.85 4.00 ± 1.66 0.6951 −0.57 ± 1.66
Naming phonemic help score 0.0413
 Low education level 5.11 ± 2.00 4.61 ± 2.45 4.00 ± 1.64 0.0801 −1.11 ± 1.95
 High education level 4.08 ± 1.30 4.00 ± 1.52 4.07 ± 1.11 0.8091 −0.01 ± 0.54
Boston (BNT) score 0.0023
 Low education level 43.00 ± 4.49 42.44 ± 4.18 38.94 ± 4.49 0.0011 −4.06 ± 5.74
 High education level 51.71 ± 4.18 51.22 ± 4.39 51.72 ± 3.70 0.9401 0.01 ± 3.723
Naming time score 0.8831
 Low education level 636.4 ± 113.1 658.1 ± 106.0 649.7 ± 136.6 0.5271 13.3 ± 102.9
 High education level 523.0 ± 108.7 533.6 ± 96.3 526.9 ± 112.8 0.8581 3.9 ± 75.1

ΔScore0_12, 12-month change in nonverbal scores; MCI, mild cognitive impairment.

Statistical significance: 1time effect within the same level of education group and 2interaction education level × time effect; 3statistically significant difference compared with low education level.

Table 2.

Nonverbal scores of subjects with MCI in relation to their educational level

Nonverbal scores (mean values ± SD)

Baseline 6th month 12th month P-value1 ΔScore0_12
Drawing score 0.0223
 Low education level 4.28 ± 0.75 4.44 ± 0.92 3.61 ± 0.85 0.0181 −0.67 ± 1.08
 High education level 4.78 ± 0.60 4.86 ± 0.61 4.79 ± 0.72 0.7631 0.01 ± 0.493
Spontaneous writing score 0.7473
 Low education level 1.06 ± 0.24 1.06 ± 0.42 0.94 ± 0.42 0.4621 −0.12 ± 0.47
 High education level 1.00 ± 0.00 1.00 ± 0.00 1.00 ± 0.00 1.0001 0.00 ± 0.00
Ideational praxis score 0.5113
 Low education level 4.55 ± 0.51 4.44 ± 0.51 4.11 ± 0.83 0.0611 −0.44 ± 0.86
 High education level 4.71 ± 0.52 4.71 ± 0.48 4.64 ± 0.59 0.8631 −0.07 ± 0.25
Following commands score 0.7153
 Low education level 3.94 ± 0.24 3.89 ± 0.32 3.61 ± 0.78 0.1261 −0.33 ± 0.84
 High education level 4.00 ± 0.00 3.86 ± 0.37 3.78 ± 0.43 0.2391 −0.22 ± 0.44
Writing score 0.9143
 Low education level 1.72 ± 0.46 1.78 ± 0.43 1.67 ± 0.59 0.4851 −0.05 ± 0.42
 High education level 1.78 ± 0.44 1.85 ± 0.40 1.78 ± 0.47 0.8391 0.00 ± 0.00

ΔScore0_12, 12-month change in nonverbal scores; MCI, mild cognitive impairment.

Statistical significance: 1time effect within the same level of education group and 2interaction education level × time effect; 3statistically significant difference compared with low education level.

During our sequential evaluations, we considered an outcome of interest, the cognitive performance at our last follow-up evaluation (12 months). Analysis of covariance, controlling for baseline scores, showed a statistically significant effect of education on the NO score (adjusted mean values ± SE, 9.88 ± 0.28 and 11.58 ± 0.44 in the low and high levels of education, respectively, P = 0.002), DF score (4.51 ± 0.16 and 5.41 ± 0.27, P = 0.005), LT (20.92 ± 0.60 and 23.96 ± 0.93, P = 0.008), CD score (3.70 ± 0.19 and 4.68 ± 0.31, P = 0.008), BXB score (33.12 ± 1.18 and 44.36 ± 1.84, P = 0.0001), BFB (3.62 ± 0.43 and 4.48 ± 0.32, P = 0.022), and BNT (41.19 ± 1.39 and 48.84 ± 2.17, P = 0.004), with lower scores being documented in the group of patients with low education level. Moreover, similar results were obtained when education was treated as a continuous variable (in years; range, 0–16 years; median value, 6 years); in the linear regression analysis (adjusting for demographic and clinical characteristics and baseline scores), the duration of education was independently and positively associated with the following function tests: NO (β = 0.457, SE = 0.087, P = 0.001, R2 = 27.7%), DF (β = 0.274, SE = 0.051, P = 0.002, R2=23.8%), LT (β = 0.980, SE = 0.141, P = 0.014, R2 = 15.1%), CD (β = 0.211, SE = 0.044, P = 0.023, R2 = 12.5%), BXB (β = 1.284, SE = 0.267, P = 0.017, R2 = 14.2%), BFB (β = 0.204, SE = 0.038, P = 0.031, R2 = 11.9%), and BNT (β = 2.085, SE = 0.310, P = 0.002, R2 = 25.3%).

The positive effect of higher education was reflected by comparing the mean change during the 12-month follow-up (ΔScore0_12; Tables 13) between the two levels of education; statistically significant differences were found on the following function tests: naming objects (NO) (P < 0.001), definition (DF) (P = 0.008), language (LT) (P = 0.008), drawing (CD) (P = 0.037), naming without help (BXB) (P = 0.013), naming with phonemic help (BFB) (P = 0.049), and Boston naming test (BNT) (P = 0.029). Finally, statistically significant positive correlations were also found between the duration of education (in years) and the 12-month change (ΔScore0_12) of the following function tests: NO (r = 0.588, P = 0.0004), DF (r = 0.487, P = 0.005), LT (r = 0.522, P = 0.002), CD (r = 0.408, P = 0.020), BXB (r = 0.441, P = 0.012), BFB (r = 0.380, P = 0.032), and BNT (r = 0.568, P = 0.0007).

Discussion

In this study, higher educational attainment in aMCI subjects was correlated with better performance in verbal and nonverbal tasks during repeated examinations over 1-year period. Subjects with low level of education performed worse than patients with high level of education who presented a more “stable” clinical course. These findings provide support for a cognitive reserve that could alter not only the onset of the symptoms but also the clinical rate slowing the cognitive decline during the predementia phase.

The neurobiologic mechanisms responsible for the association between education and cognitive functions are not known. One plausible explanation is that education impacts the rate at which plaques and tangles accumulate in the brain. Snowdon et al. (1996) found a relation between early life linguistic ability and density of neurofibrillary tangles. In contrast, Del Ser et al. (1999) did not reproduce the former correlation in their autopsy study evaluating patients with AD and Lewy body dementia. In fact, many studies agree that although the education level does not directly impact the accumulation of AD pathology, it can delay the clinical onset of the symptoms (Katzman et al. 1988; Stern et al. 1992a; Stern et al. 1995; Friedland et al. 2001). Alexander et al. (1997), using positron emission tomography, found that premorbid intellectual ability as it estimated by a demographics-based IQ and performance on a measure of word-reading task was inversely correlated with cerebral metabolism in prefrontal, premotor, parietal, and other cerebral regions among patients of similar dementia severity levels and concluded that higher intellectual ability altered the clinical expression of dementia. In other words, a better task performance that is related with higher education seems to mask the clinical expression of a higher degree of neurodegeneration (Bennett et al. 2003; Perneczky et al. 2006; Scarmeas et al. 2006; Stern et al. 1992b).

The potential association of this reserve mechanism with the course of disease in MCI individuals is intriguing and of potential clinical interest. AD pathology seems to progress independently from educational and occupational attainment, and when pathology becomes very severe, there is no longer a substrate for cognitive reserve to come into play (Stern 2002). The results about the rate of cognitive decline in AD patients are inconsistent, supporting a slower decline (Fritsch et al. 2001), no decline (Wilson et al. 2004), or accelerated decline (Teri et al. 1995; Wilson et al. 2000; Wilson et al. 2009; Zahodne et al. 2011) in higher educated subjects. Our results in MCI revealed a slower deterioration in performance of different tasks indicating delay in the cognitive decline in individuals with higher level of education.

Garibotto et al. (2008) showed a significant association between higher education/occupation and lower regional Cerebral Metabolic Rate of glucose consumption (rCMRglc) in posterior temporoparietal cortex and precuneus in AD and aMCI supporting the view that functional reserve is already at play in the MCI, but there are no specific data about the rate of decline in MCI. Karrasch and Laine (2003) showed that the tests of naming, verbal fluency, and verbal memory were affected by educational attainment. Lièvre et al. (2008), using a summary performance-based measure which reflected a range of cognitive abilities, including language and naming, concluded that development of cognitive impairment was highly affected by education. Years of education was also considered the best single predictor of overall cognitive performance (Kaplan et al. 2009) and patients with high education could gain an advantage by being more familiar with the kinds of tasks used in neuropsychological assessments (Kemppainen et al. 2008).

In our study, we found dissociation between verbal and nonverbal patterns. Among the latter, only changes in copying–drawing abilities were related to education. Other studies found no correlation in the nonverbal tasks in AD patients (Filley and Cullum 1997) or in normal elderly subjects (Meguro et al. 2001). In fact, cognitive reserve is not a unitary construct and do not affect all areas of cognitive functioning equally (Stern et al. 1999). In patients with mild AD, the abstract reasoning performance task score was correlated with the years of education (Vliet et al. 2003). Roe et al. (2008) suggest that cognitive reserve, as reflected in education, may have a stronger or earlier effect on specific cognitive processes such as the abstract reasoning, compared with other cognitive processes. An inverse correlation was found in the study by Le Carret et al. (2005).

Indeed, MCI is a clinically heterogeneous state and many factors could alter the tasks performance. In our study, we used very strict inclusion criteria. The participants were free of medications; normal brain MRI without silent infarcts and leucoencephalopathy was a mandatory prerequisite to avoid influences of other factors (Tsivgoulis et al. 2009; Nooyens et al. 2010).

In conclusion, education was found to influence tests performance during follow-up examinations. This effect was present during the 1-year repeated follow-up examinations in a series of verbal and nonverbal tasks supporting a slower decline in higher educated subjects. Our findings are preliminary; inclusion of more subjects and extension of the follow-up assessment beyond the 12 months would be an answer to the difficult question how long this “protective” effect persists.

Acknowledgments

Dr. G. Tsivgoulis has been supported by European Regional Development Fund – Project FNUSA-ICRC (No. CZ.1.05/1.1.00/02.0123).

Conflict of Interest

None declared.

References

  1. Afgin AE, Massarwa M, Schechtman E, Israeli-Korn SD, Strugatsky R, Abuful A, et al. High prevalence of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease in Arabic villages in northern Israel: impact of gender and education. J. Alzheimer Dis. 2012;29:431–439. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-111667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alexander GE, Furey ML, Grady CL, Pietrini P, Brady DR, Mentis MJ, et al. Association of premorbid intellectual function with cerebral metabolism in Alzheimer's disease: implications for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Am. J. Psych. 1997;154:165–172. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.2.165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anchisi D, Borroni B, Franceschi M, Kerrouche N, Kalbe E, Beuthien-Beumann B, et al. Heterogeneity of brain glucose metabolism in mild cognitive impairment and clinical progression to Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 2005;62:1728–1733. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.11.1728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Anstey K, Christensen H. Education, activity, health, blood pressure and apolipoprotein E as predictors of cognitive change in old age: a review. Gerontology. 2000;46:163–177. doi: 10.1159/000022153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, Evans DA, Mendes de Leon CF, Arnold SE, et al. Education modifies the relation of AD pathology to level of cognitive function in older persons. Neurology. 2003;60:1909–1915. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000069923.64550.9f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Le Carret N, Auriacombe S, Letenneur L, Bergua V, Dartigues JF, Fabrigoule C. Influence of education on the pattern of cognitive deterioration in AD patients: the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Brain Cogn. 2005;57:120–126. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.08.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray K, Rosenberg-Thompson S, Carusi DA, Gornbein J. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology. 1994;44:2308–2314. doi: 10.1212/wnl.44.12.2308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Filley CM, Cullum CM. Education and cognitive function in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol. 1997;10:48–51. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1975;2:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Friedland RP, Fritsch T, Smyth KA, Koss E, Lerner AJ, Chen CH, et al. Patients with Alzheimer's disease have reduced activities in midlife compared with healthy control-group members. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2001;98:3440–3445. doi: 10.1073/pnas.061002998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Fritsch T, McClendon MJ, Smyth KA, Lerner AJ, Chen CH, Petot GJ, et al. Effects of educational attainment on the clinical expression of Alzheimer's disease: results from a research registry. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Demen. 2001;16:369–376. doi: 10.1177/153331750101600606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Garibotto V, Borroni B, Kalbe E, Herholz K, Salmon E, Holtoff V, et al. Education and occupation as proxies for reserve in aMCI converters and AD: EDG-PET evidence. Neurology. 2008;71:1342–1349. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000327670.62378.c0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gauthier S, Reisberg B, Zaudig M, Petersen RC, Ritchie K, Broich K, et al. Mild cognitive impairment. Lancet. 2006;367:1262–1270. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68542-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hamilton M. Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness. Br. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 1967;6:278–296. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1967.tb00530.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Huppert FA, Brayne C, Gill C, Paykel ES, Beardsall L. CAMCOG: a concise neuropsychological test to assist dementia diagnosis: socio-demographic determinants in an elderly population sample. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 1995;34:529–541. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1995.tb01487.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Huppert F, Jorm AF, Brayne C, Girling DM, Barkley C, Beardsall L, et al. Psychometric properties of the CAMCOG and its efficacy in the diagnosis of dementia. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 1996;3:201–214. [Google Scholar]
  17. Ince PG. Pathological correlates of late-onset dementia in a multicenter community-based population in England and Wales. Lancet. 2001;357:169–175. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(00)03589-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kaplan E, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. Boston naming test. Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  19. Kaplan RF, Cohen RA, Moscufo N, Guttmann C, Chasman J, Buttaro M, et al. Demographic and biological influences on cognitive reserve. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2009;31:868–876. doi: 10.1080/13803390802635174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Karrasch M, Laine M. Age, education and test performance on the Finnish CERAD. Acta Neurol. Scand. 2003;108:97–101. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0404.2003.00037.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Katzman R, Terry R, DeTeresa R, Brown T, Davies P, Fuld P, et al. Clinical, pathological, and neurochemical changes in dementia: a subgroup with preserved mental status and numerous neocortical plaques. Ann. Neurol. 1988;23:138–441. doi: 10.1002/ana.410230206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kemppainen NM, Aalto S, Karrasch M, Någren K, Savisto N, Oikonen V, et al. Cognitive reserve hypothesis: Pittsburgh compound B and fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography in relation to education in mild Alzheimer's disease. Ann. Neurol. 2008;63:112–118. doi: 10.1002/ana.21212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Lièvre A, Alley D, Crimmins EM. Educational differentials in life expectancy with cognitive impairment among the elderly in the United States. J. Aging Health. 2008;20:456–477. doi: 10.1177/0898264308315857. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Meguro K, Shimada M, Yamaguchi S, Ishizaki J, Ishii H, Shimada Y, et al. Cognitive function and frontal lobe atrophy in normal elderly adults: implications for dementia not as aging-related disorders and the reserve hypothesis. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2001;55:565–572. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.2001.00907.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, Fillenbaum G, van Belle G, Mellits ED, et al. The consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer's disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1989;9:1159–1165. doi: 10.1212/wnl.39.9.1159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Nooyens A, Baab C, Spijkerman A, Verschuren WM. Type 2 diabetes and cognitive decline in middle-aged men and women: the Doetinchem cohort study. Diabetes Care. 2010;33:1964–1969. doi: 10.2337/dc09-2038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Perneczky R, Drzezga A, Diehl-Schmid J, Schmid G, Wohlschläger A, Kars S, et al. Schooling mediates brain reserve in Alzheimer's disease: findings of fluoro-deoxy-glucose-positron emission tomography. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 2006;77:1060–1063. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.094714. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch. Neurol. 1999;56:303–308. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, Mohs RC, Morris JC, Rabins PV, et al. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Neurol. 2001;58:1985–1992. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.12.1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Roe CM, Xiong C, Miller JP, Morris JC. Education and Alzheimer disease without dementia: support for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Neurology. 2007;68:223–228. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000251303.50459.8a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Roe CM, Mintun MA, D'Angelo G, Xiong C, Grant EA, Morris JC. Variation of education effect with carbon 11-labeled Pittsburgh compound B uptake. Arch. Neurol. 2008;65:1467–1471. doi: 10.1001/archneur.65.11.1467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Rolstad S, Nordlund A, Eckerström C, Gustavsson MH, Blennow K, Olesen PJ, et al. High education may offer protection against tauopathy in patients with mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2010;21:221–228. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-091012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Scarmeas N, Albert SM, Manly JJ, Stern Y. Education and rates of cognitive decline in incident Alzheimer's disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 2006;77:308–316. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2005.072306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Del Ser T, Hachinski B, Merskey H, Munoz DG. An autopsy-verified study of the effects of education on degenerative dementia. Brain. 1999;122:2309–2319. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.12.2309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Snowdon D, Kemper S, Mortimer JA, Greiner LH, Wekstein DR, Markesbery WR. Linguistic ability in early life and cognitive function and Alzheimer's disease in late life. JAMA. 1996;275:528–532. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Solé-Padullésa C, Bartrés-Faza D, Junquéa C, Vendrell P, Rami L, Clemente IC, et al. Brain structure and function related to cognitive reserve variables in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol. Aging. 2009;30:1114–1124. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Stern Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2002;8:448–460. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2015–2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Stern Y, Andrews H, Pittman J, Sano M, Tatemichi T, Lantigua R, et al. Diagnosis of dementia in a heterogeneous population – development of a neuropsychological paradigm-based diagnosis of dementia and quantified correction for the effects of education. Arch. Neurol. 1992a;49:453–460. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1992.00530290035009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Stern Y, Alexander GE, Prohovnik I, Mayeux R. Inverse relationship between education and parietotemporal perfusion deficit in Alzheimer's disease. Ann. Neurol. 1992b;32:371–375. doi: 10.1002/ana.410320311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Stern Y, Alexander GE, Prohovnik I, Stricks L, Link B, Lennon MC, et al. Relationship between lifetime occupation and parietal flow: implications for a reserve against Alzheimer's disease pathology. Neurology. 1995;45:55–60. doi: 10.1212/wnl.45.1.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Stern Y, Albert S, Tang MX, Tsai WY. Rate of memory decline in AD is related to education and occupation: cognitive reserve? Neurology. 1999;53:1942–1947. doi: 10.1212/wnl.53.9.1942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Teri L, McCurry SM, Edland SD, Kukull WA, Larson EB. Cognitive decline in Alzheimer's disease: a longitudinal investigation of risk factors for accelerated decline. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 1995;50:49–55. doi: 10.1093/gerona/50a.1.m49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Tsivgoulis G, Alexandrov AV, Wadley VG, Unverzagt FW, Go RC, Moy CS, et al. Association of higher diastolic blood pressure levels with cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2009;73:589–595. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b38969. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Vliet EC, Manly J, Tang MX, Marder K, Bell K, Stern Y. The neuropsychological profiles of mild Alzheimer's disease and questionable dementia as compared to age-related cognitive decline. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2003;9:720–732. doi: 10.1017/S1355617703950053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Gilley DW, Beckett LA, Barnes LL, Evans DA. Premorbid reading activity and patterns of cognitive decline in Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 2000;57:1718–1723. doi: 10.1001/archneur.57.12.1718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Wilson RS, Li Y, Aggarwal NT, Barnes LL, McCann JJ, Gilley DW, et al. Education and the course of cognitive decline in Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2004;63:1198–1202. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000140488.65299.53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Wilson RS, Hebert LE, Scherr PA, Barnes LL, Mendes de Leon CF, Evans DA. Educational attainment and cognitive decline in old age. Neurology. 2009;72:460–465. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000341782.71418.6c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Zahodne LB, Glymour MM, Sparks C, Bontempo D, Dixon RA, MacDonald SW, et al. Education does not slow cognitive decline with aging: 12-year evidence from the Victoria longitudinal study. J. Int. Neyropsychol. Soc. 2011;17:1039–1046. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711001044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Brain and Behavior are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES