Skip to main content
Croatian Medical Journal logoLink to Croatian Medical Journal
. 2012 Oct;53(5):450–460. doi: 10.3325/cmj.2012.53.450

Relations of the Big-Five personality dimensions to autodestructive behavior in clinical and non-clinical adolescent populations

Marina Kotrla Topić 1, Marina Perković Kovačević 2, Boris Mlačić 1
PMCID: PMC3490456  PMID: 23100207

Abstract

Aim

To examine the relationship between the Big-Five personality model and autodestructive behavior symptoms, namely Autodestructiveness and Suicidal Depression in two groups of participants: clinical and non-clinical adolescents.

Methods

Two groups of participants, clinical (adolescents with diagnosis of psychiatric disorder based on clinical impression and according to valid diagnostic criteria, N = 92) and non-clinical (high-school students, N = 87), completed two sets of questionnaires: the Autodestructiveness Scale which provided data on Autodestructiveness and Suicidal Depression, and the International Personality Item Pool (IPIP), which provided data on the Big -Five personality dimensions.

Results

Clinical group showed significantly higher values on the Autodestructiveness scale in general, as well as on Suicidal Depression, Aggressiveness, and Borderline subscales than the non-clinical group. Some of the dimensions of the Big-Five personality model, ie, Emotional Stability, Conscientiousness, and Agreeableness showed significant relationship (hierarchical regression analyses, P values for β coefficients from <0.001 to 0.021) with Autodestructivness and Suicidal Depression, even after controlling for the sex and group effects or, when analyzing Suicidal Depression, after controlling the effect of other subscales.

Conclusion

The results indicate that dimensions of the Big-Five model are important when evaluating adolescent psychiatric patients and adolescents from general population at risk of self-destructive behavior.


In the past few years, autodestructive behavior has been acknowledged as a serious disease and cost factor in an increasing number of different medical cases (1). Autodestructive tendencies can manifest in different ways – as acute or chronic, latent or overt, conscious or subconscious (2). In studies on autodestructive behavior, researchers usually focus on intentional, self-directed destruction of body tissue without suicidal intent (1,3-8). Autodestructive behavior is often referred to as “self-destructive behavior,” “self-injurious behavior,” “self-injury,” “self-harm,” “self-mutilation,” or “cutting” (8).

For a long time it was considered that autodestructive behavior was related primarily to emotional disturbances (9,10). Therefore, most of the research on this topic was done using clinical populations. Only in the last decade or so, the research began focusing on other populations, noticing that there are high rates of different kinds of autodestructive behavior among youth and young adults in general population. This finding brought out the importance of other factors relevant for the development of autodestructive behavior, besides emotional disturbance, such as different social and cultural factors (4). It also highlighted some concerns regarding instruments designed for capturing and measuring symptoms of this kind of behavior in clinical and in non-clinical populations.

Psychopathology and personality assessment

When assessing personality characteristics of psychiatric patients, clinicians frequently use questionnaires aimed at discovering psychopathological symptoms (11), but some researchers argue that many kinds of abnormal behavior, including autodestructive behavior, can be viewed as exaggerated versions of normal personality traits. This would mean that in some patients with diagnosed psychological disorders, psychopathology could be seen as falling on a continuum with normal personality instead of being regarded as a distinct departure from normal (12,13). One of a number of alternative dimensional models of personality disorders (14) and other psychopathological symptoms that has sustained a great deal of attention is the Big-Five personality model (15,16).

The Big-Five personality model

The emergence of this model is considered as one of the most significant developments in the history of personality psychology (17). There are several types of this model, but the fundamental model was originally associated with studies of personality traits used in natural language. The model has five broad dimensions: Extraversion/Introversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Emotional Stability/Neuroticism, and Intellect (18). Each of the lexical Big-Five factors consists of nine facets (19).

A number of studies have examined the relationship between the Big-Five personality model and mental health problems in clinical and normal-range populations. Focusing most often on personality disorders in adult populations, their results point to a strong relationship between mental health problems and four domains of the Big-Five model: Neuroticism, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness (20-22), with less consistent data regarding the role of Intellect. Recently studies have moved their focus to developmental antecedents of personality pathology in adolescence, or even childhood, trying to identify the role of general personality according to the Big-Five model in describing personality disorders and their precursors (16,22-25). Since then, the use of the Big-Five personality model and its questionnaires has been suggested to be a valid adjunct to usual clinical instruments when it comes to diagnosing and planning a treatment. The question that remains to be answered is the nature of the relationship of the Big-Five and other mental health problems, namely autodestructive behavior. The systematic research in this area is scarce, a reason for which might be that the manifestations of autodestructive behavior are often incorporated in a wide area of different mental health problems and not studied separately. For example, autodestructive behavior is often studied in the context of attempted suicide; therefore it is sometimes difficult to discern autodestructive behavior from other mental health problems. The relationship between autodestructive behavior and suicide is a complex one, but it is generally considered that there is a strong correlation between these two phenomena, as well as between autodestructive behavior and depression.

Since autodestructive behavior is strongly related to suicidal ideation and depression, the findings on the relationship between these two phenomena and the Big-Five can give valid guidelines for the research on the Big-Five and autodestructive behavior symptoms. Studies show that suicidal ideation, as one type of autodestructive behavior symptom, is associated with low Emotional Stability, low Extraversion, and low Agreeableness (26). The studies investigating depression, which is often closely related to autodestructive behavior, show that a high level of Neuroticism was significantly associated with recurrence of depression (27). As for the relationship of other factors with depression, research points to a negative association of depression with Extraversion (28,29) and Emotional Stability (29), and a positive association of depression with Openness to Experience (30). Also, depressed individuals score low on Conscientiousness (31).

This study addressed the adolescent population. While there is substantial research that shows age-related changes in some of the Big-Five dimensions, especially in the adolescence (32,33), studies on adolescents’ psychiatric problems and their relations with the Big-Five are rare or non-existing. Furthermore, those rare studies point to inconsistent findings, with some reporting significant negative correlations between depressive symptomatology or similar internalized behavior syndromes and two Big-Five dimensions: Emotional Stability and Extraversion (34), or Emotional Stability and Conscientiousness (35), as well all five dimensions (36).

Our main research question pertained to how well the Big-Five personality model could account for Autodestructive behavior symptoms, namely Autodestructiveness and Suicidal Depression in adolescence.

Method

Participants

The sample consisted of two groups, clinical and non-clinical. The clinical group consisted of 92 adolescents (52 female) who were treated in ambulatory care or hospitalized at the Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Ward of the Psychiatric clinic at the Clinical Hospital Osijek, and who agreed to participate in the research. The participants from the clinical group were all given a diagnosis of psychiatric disorder. The diagnosis was based on the clinical impression and according to valid diagnostic criteria (37). Their mean age was 15.77 years (range 13-20). They were treated for different problems diagnosed according to DSM-IV criteria (37): behavioral disorders (38%), anxious disorders (23%), depressive disorders (16%), psychotic disorders (7%), eating disorders (5%), and other disorders (10%). The non-clinical group consisted of 87 adolescents (65 female) from two Osijek high schools. Their mean age was 15.57 years (range 13-18). The high schools were chosen randomly from a list of Osijek high schools, and in each school we randomly selected one class of students, the only important factor being that participants match the clinical group in age. From those two classes, all the students whose parents gave their consent, were included in the research.

Procedure

The clinical group data were collected by one of the authors during the standard psychological testing procedure at the Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Ward. The approval for the data collection was given by the participants’ parents who accompanied them to the treatment, and participants themselves were explained the purpose of the research. The non-clinical group data were collected during school hours by the authors. Parents of the non-clinical participants gave their consent prior to the study by signing the letter given to them by their child. The administration of the questionnaires for both groups lasted about 20 minutes. Ethical approval was received from the Ethics Committee of the Ivo Pilar Institute of Social Sciences, and from the Ethics Committee of the Clinical Hospital Osijek.

Measures

Autodestructiveness scale (2). The scale consists of 107 items for which respondents gave “yes” or “no” answers, depending if the statement could be applied to them or not. Each “yes” answer was later given 1 point, and the final score for the respondent on the scale was the sum of “yes” ratings for all of the items. The internal consistency reliability (coefficient α) for this measure was 0.94, in accordance with previous research that also showed comparable reliability (38). The total result on this scale pointed to the extent of disturbance of the autodestructive drive control (2) and it was referred to as Autodestructiveness.

Previous factor analyses of the items from this scale (2) pointed to four factors, which corresponded to four subscales: Suicidal Depression, Anxiety, Aggressiveness, and Borderline subscale. In previous research, each subscale showed acceptable reliability (38). Besides the total score on Autodestructiveness scale, results on these subscales were often considered for clinical purposes, especially since each of the subscales coincides to specific DSM-IV disorder classification.

The subscale of Suicidal Depression consists of 32 items and measures the feelings of inferiority, hopelessness, lack of tolerance to frustration, lack of self-control, suicidal ideas, and suicide attempts. Anxiety subscale consists of 32 items measuring fear, tendency to worry, feeling of personal adequacy, lack of self-esteem, and tendency to avoid activities by over exaggerating potential risks and dangers. Aggressiveness subscale consists of 22 items measuring psychopathic aggressive behavior, impulsivity, lack of self-control and empathy, tendency to engage in high risk activities, and self-hurt types of behavior. Borderline subscale consists of 21 items measuring disturbances in interpersonal relationships, lack of altruism and empathy, instability of emotional relationships, specific autistic system of thought, and tendency to autodestructive behavior, including suicide attempts.

International Personality Item Pool (IPIP) (39). To measure the Big-Five domains we used the Croatian translation (39) of the IPIP Big-Five domains (40), with 50 items (short form). Participants were asked to read each of the 50 items and then rate how well they believed it described them on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (very inaccurate) to 5 (very accurate) as in the original instrument (40). Previous research confirmed the stability of the five-factor structure of IPIP50 and satisfactory scale reliability on a sample of Croatian adolescents (41). The IPIP50 consists of 10 statements for each of the five factors of the Big-Five model: Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Emotional Stability, and Intellect. High results on Extraversion point to pronounced engagement with other people and with the external world. Extraverts are sociable, outgoing, energetic, lively, and prefer to be around people, while introverts are reserved, sober, and quiet (42). Agreeableness reflects individual differences in peoples’ interest in needs and well-being of others, therefore high agreeableness is marked by altruism, caring, and emotional support (43). Conscientiousness reflects the way in which we control, regulate, and direct our impulses, and high conscientiousness points to a reliable, well organized, responsible, and hard-working person (44). Emotional Stability/Neuroticism is marked by a tendency to get less easily upset and less emotionally reactive. High emotional stability indicates calm, composed, and relaxed individuals who do not react with intense emotions (45). High results on the Intellect dimension point to an imaginative person who enjoys variety, novelty, and change, and who has intellectual and artistic interests.

Statistical analysis

Data were analyzed using SPSS software for Windows, version 20.0 (2012., IBM Corporation, Software Group, Armonk, NY, USA). The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test showed that all the scales had normal distribution in both groups, clinical and non-clinical. After the reliability analyses of all the variables encompassed by the study, which was done using Cronbach α coefficients in total sample and in both samples individually, we first tested the differences between clinical and the non-clinical group in Autodestructiveness scale and all its subscales, as well as in Big-Five scales, using t tests. We then calculated Pearson correlation coefficients for all the variables in the study. To answer the main research questions of the study, we performed a set of three hierarchical regression analyses with Autodestructiveness as the criterion variable (one for the total sample and one for each of the two groups), and other three hierarchical regression analyses with Suicidal Depression as the criterion variable. Hierarchical regression analyses enabled us to determine the order in which variables are entered into the regression equation, allowing us then to examine the contribution of variables entered later, above and beyond the first group of independent variables.

Results

Reliability analyses, descriptive statistics, and differences in Mean Scale Scores

A majority of the scales had acceptable to high reliabilities (Table 1), however a few scales in the subsamples fell below the desired value of 0.7 and these results should be interpreted with caution. Those few scales that had internal consistencies lower than reported in the literature could have resulted with lower correlations between the observed constructs. In the case of Autodestructivness (total score) and all of its subscales, Cronbach α coefficients were somewhat lower in the non-clinical than in clinical sample, but still had acceptable values. The Big-Five scales had somewhat higher reliabilities in the non-clinical sample, with the exception of Conscientiousness (Table 1).

Table 1.

Cronbach α reliabilities for all variables encompassed in the study, for the whole sample, and separately for the clinical and non-clinical group

Scale Whole sample Clinical group Non-clinical group
Autodestructiveness scale and subscales:



Autodestructiveness (total score)
0.94
0.96
0.90
Suicidal Depression
0.91
0.93
0.86
Anxiety
0.84
0.89
0.78
Aggressiveness
0.77
0.88
0.56
Borderline
0.77
0.80
0.62
Big-five factors:



Extraversion
0.77
0.74
0.80
Agreeableness
0.70
0.70
0.71
Conscientiousness
0.78
0.85
0.66
Emotional Stability
0.85
0.83
0.86
Intellect 0.66 0.65 0.69

The participants from the clinical group showed significantly higher values on the Autodestructiveness scale in general, as well as on Suicidal Depression, Aggressiveness, and Borderline subscales, than the non-clinical group. There were no significant differences between the two groups in the Big-Five dimensions (Table 2).

Table 2.

Descriptive statistics for the total sample and separately for the two groups of participants, along with t test results for the differences between the groups*

Scale Range M±σ Groups Range M±σ t P
Autodestructiveness scale and subscales:







Autodestructiveness (total score)
5-95
40.18 ± 20.5
clinical
5-95
44.73 ± 22.7
3.131
0.002
non-clinical
10-80
35.38 ± 16.7
Suicidal Depression
0-30
9.89 ± 7.4
clinical
1-30
11.97 ± 8.3
4.020
<0.001
non-clinical
0-22
7.69 ± 5.6
Anxiety
1-39
16.24 ± 7.2
clinical
1-29
16.64 ± 7.4
0.720
0.473
non-clinical
2-39
15.86 ± 7.0
Aggressiveness
0-28
7.01 ± 5.3
clinical
0-22
7.89 ± 5.7
2.332
0.021
non-clinical
0-28
6.07 ± 4.6
Borderline
0-19
7.03 ± 3.9
clinical
0-19
8.23 ± 4.3
4.430
<0.001
non-clinical
0-13
5.76 ± 3.0
Big-Five factors:







Extraversion
14-50
33.76 ± 6.8
clinical
14-46
33.66 ± 6.9
-0.214
0.831
non-clinical
18-50
33.87 ± 6.7
Agreeableness
21-50
38.74 ± 5.4
clinical
21-50
38.85 ± 5.7
0.265
0.791
non-clinical
27-50
38.63 ± 5.2
Conscientiousness
11-49
34.42 ± 7.1
clinical
11-49
33.68 ± 8.2
-1.436
0.153
non-clinical
21-48
35.21 ± 5.7
Emotional Stability
11-47
29.41 ± 8.3
clinical
11-46
28.66 ± 8.5
-1.252
0.213
non-clinical
12-47
30.21 ± 8.0
Intellect 21-50 35.53 ± 5.5 clinical
21-48
35.46 ± 5.9
-0.165 0.869
non-clinical 25-50 35.60 ± 5.1

*Significant values are in bold.

Relations between the Autodestructiveness scale and Big-Five dimensions

The only Big-Five dimension that significantly negatively correlated with Autodestructiveness and all of its subscales was Emotional Stability (Table 3). In other words, less emotionally stable participants in general showed more autodestructiveness, but also more anxiety and aggressiveness, more borderline symptoms and higher suicidal depression. Other two Big-Five dimensions that showed significant negative correlations with the total Autodestructiveness and all but one Autodestructiveness subscales were Extraversion and Conscientiousness. Higher Extraversion pointed to generally lower Autodestructiveness, but also lower Suicidal Depression and Anxiety, and fewer Borderline symptoms, or, looking inversely, Introversion was associated with almost every aspect of Autodestructiveness. Similarly, higher Conscientiousness pointed to generally lower Autodestructiveness, but also lower Suicidal Depression and Aggressiveness, and fewer Borderline symptoms.

Table 3.

Pearson correlations coefficients (r) between Autodestructiveness scale and its subscales, and the International Personality Item Pool scales, for the total sample (P values in parentheses)*

Suicidal depression Anxiety Aggressiveness Borderline Extraversion Agreeableness Conscientiousness Emotional stability Intellect
Autodestructiveness (total score)
0.919 (<0.001)
0.838 (<0.001)
0.789 (<0.001)
0.871 (<0.001)
-0.264(<0.001)
-0.042 (0.572)
-0.286 (<0.001)
-0.716 (<0.001)
-0.054 (0.473)
Suicidal Depression
1
0.684 (<0.001)
0.631 (<0.001)
0.792 (<0.001)
-0.286 (<0.001)
-0.095 (0.207)
-0.286 (<0.001)
-0.685 (<0.001)
-0.085 (0.256)
Anxiety

1
0.475 (<0.001)
0.596 (<0.001)
-0.357 (<0.001)
0.056 (0.460)
-0.071 (0.345)
-0.686 (<0.001)
-0.004 (0.962)
Aggressiveness


1
0.702 (<0.001)
0.004 (0.957)
-0.063 (0.403)
-0.391 (<0.001)
-0.435 (<0.001)
-0.043 (0.567)
Borderline



1
-0.186 (0.013)
-0.060 (0.425)
-0.294 (<0.001)
-0.591 (<0.001)
-0.056 (0,457)
extraversion




1
0.285 (<0.001)
-0.046 (0.545)
0.342 (<0.001)
0.299 (<0.001)
Agreeableness





1
0.263 (<0.001)
-0.005 (0.994)
0.312 (<0.001)
Conscientiousness






1
0.178 (0.017)
0.172 (0.021)
Emotional Stability 1 0.007 (0.924)

*Significant values are in bold.

On the other hand, Agreeableness and Intellect showed no significant correlations with the Autodestructiveness scale and its subscales.

If we consider the correlations between subscales of the Autodestructiveness scale, we can see that all the subscales were in moderate to high significant positive correlations with each other (from 0.475 to 0.919, P < 0.01). Big-Five dimensions showed different patterns of correlation with each other, with all the significant correlations being positive and relatively low or moderate (Table 3).

The Big-Five dimensions as predictors of autodestructive behavior symptoms

To test the main hypotheses of the study we first performed three hierarchical multiple regression analyses with Autodestructiveness as the criterion – one for the total sample and one for each of the two groups. For the total sample we entered sex in the first step and group in the second step. The third step included the five dimensions of the Big-Five model of personality, with stepwise selection. Sex and group significantly contributed to the explanation of Autodestructiveness (Table 4). However, after controlling for sex and group, two Big-Five dimensions still substantially contributed to the explanation of Autodestructiveness. The first Big-Five dimension that accounted for additional 44% of variance of the criterion variable was Emotional Stability, while Conscientiousness also significantly contributed to explaining Autodestructiveness. This finding indicates that Emotional Stability and Conscientiousness are important personality dimensions for the explanation of Autodestructiveness for both male and female adolescents, and for both the clinical and the non-clinical group. The described model accounted for 57% of variance of Autodestructiveness.

Table 4.

Regression analyses predicting Autodestructiveness in the total sample from five dimensions of the Big-Five personality model, after controlling for sex and group*

Autodestructiveness
β (P) ΔR2
Step 1

0.030
sex
-0.174 (0.020)

F(1,177) =
5.530, P = 0.020
Step 2

0.071
group
-0.272 (<0.001)

F(1,176) =
13.992, P = <0.001
Step 3

0.441
Emotional Stability
-0.686 (<0.001)

F(1,175) =
169.048, P = <0.001

Step 4

0.025
conscientiousness
-0.164 (0.002)

F(1,174) =
10.094, P = 0.002
all variables: R2 = 0.568


F(4,174) = 57.227, P = <0.001

*Significant values are in bold.

The second two hierarchical multiple regressions were performed separately for the clinical and the non-clinical group. The criterion was again Autodestructiveness and again the first step included sex, and the next step included the Big-Five dimensions, with stepwise selection. The results show that the findings we observed at the level of the total sample repeated in the clinical subsample (Table 5 and Table 6). After we controlled for the effects of sex, two Big-Five dimensions still significantly contributed to the explanation of Autodestructiveness: Emotional Stability and Conscientiousness. This model accounted for 62% of variance of Autodestructiveness, somewhat more than in the total sample. In the case of non-clinical group, sex did not significantly contribute to the explanation of Autodestructiveness, and only one Big-Five dimension was the significant predictor of this criterion, namely Emotional Stability. This model accounted for 45% of variance of Autodestructiveness, somewhat lower than in the case of clinical group or the total sample.

Table 5.

Regression analyses predicting Autodestructiveness in the clinical sample from five dimensions of the Big-Five personality model, after controlling for sex*

Autodestructiveness
β (P) ΔR2
Step 1

0.103
sex
-0.320 (0.002)

F(1,90) =
10.296, P = 0.002
Step 2

0.475
Emotional Stability
-0.727 (<0.001)

F(1,89) =
99.963, P = <0.001

Step 3

0.043
conscientiousness
-0.215 (0.002)

F(1,88) =
9.982, P = 0.002
all variables: R2 = 0.620


F(3,88) = 47.945, P = <0.001

*Significant values are in bold.

Table 6.

Regression analyses predicting Autodestructiveness in the non-clinical sample from five dimensions of the Big-Five personality model, after controlling for sex*

Autodestructiveness
β (P) ΔR2
Step 1

0.006
sex
-0.079 (0.469)

F(1,85) =
0.530, P = 0.469
Step 2

0.444
Emotional Stability
-0.671 (<0.001)

F(1,84) =
67.749, P = <0.001

all variables: R2 = 0.450


F(2,84) = 34.348, P = <0.001

Another variable that we investigated in relation to Big-Five model was Suicidal Depression. We performed another three hierarchical multiple regression analyses, one for the total sample and one for each of the two groups, with Suicidal Depression as the criterion. For the total sample in the first step we entered sex, in the second step we entered group, in the third step the remaining three subscales of the Autodestructiveness scale, and in the last step the five dimensions of the Big-Five model, with stepwise selection. The result (Table 7) show that after controlling for sex, group, and subscales effects, two Big-Five dimensions showed independent and significant contribution to the explanation of Suicidal Depression: Emotional Stability and Agreeableness. However, the major predictors were the three Autodestructiveness subscales, especially Borderline, which accounted for most of the Suicidal Depression variance. This model accounted for 75% of variance of Suicidal Depression.

Table 7.

Regression analyses predicting Suicidal Depression in the total sample from three subscales of Autodestructiveness and five dimensions of the Big-Five personality model, after controlling for sex and group*

Suicidal Depression
β (P) ΔR2
Step 1

0.032
sex
-0.180 (0.016)

F(1,177) =
5.900, P = 0.016
Step 2

0.109
group
-0.336 (<0.001)

F(1,176) =
22.263, P = <0.001
Step 3

0.577
anxiety
0.313 (<0.001)

aggressiveness
0.136 (0.020)

borderline
0.467 (<0.001)

F(3,173) =
117.924, P = <0.001

Step 4

0.024
Emotional Stability
-0.225 (<0.001)

F(1,172) =
16.084, P = <0.001
Step 5

0.008
agreeableness
-0.091 (0.021)

F(1,171) =
5.395, P = 0.021
all variables: R2 = 0.750


F(7,171) = 73.241, P = <0.001

*Significant values are in bold.

In the case of clinical and non-clinical group, in the hierarchical multiple regression analyses, in the first step we entered sex, in the second the remaining three subscales of the Autodestructiveness scale, and in the last step we entered the five dimensions of the Big-Five model of personality, again with stepwise selection (Table 8 and Table 9). In the clinical group, this model accounted for 78% of variance of Suicidal Depression, and the only Big-Five dimension significantly contributing to this explanation, after controlling for sex, Anxiety, Aggressiveness, and Borderline subscales was Agreeableness. Again, the major predictors of the Suicidal Depression were the three Autodestructiveness subscales, especially Anxiety. Results in the non-clinical group were somewhat different – sex and Aggressiveness were not significant predictors of Suicidal Depression, and the only Big-Five dimension significantly contributing to Suicidal Depression explanation, after controlling for Anxiety and Borderline subscales, was Emotional Stability. This model accounted for 67% of variance of Suicidal Depression. Once more, the major predictors of Suicidal Depression were Autodestructiveness subscales, especially Borderline.

Table 8.

Regression analyses predicting Suicidal Depression in the clinical sample from three subscales of Autodestructiveness and five dimensions of the Big-Five personality model, after controlling for sex*

Suicidal Depression
β (P) ΔR2
Step 1

0.111
sex
-0.333 (0.001)

F(1,90) =
11.235, P = 0.001
Step 2

0.649
anxiety
0.382 (<0.001)

aggressiveness
0.301 (<0.001)

borderline
0.293 (0.002)

F(3,87) =
78.600, P = 0.001

Step 3

0.019
agreeableness
-0.141 (0.007)

F(1,86) =
7.507, P = 0.007
all variables: R2 = 0.780


F(5,86) = 60.850, P = <0.001

*Significant values are in bold.

Table 9.

Regression analyses predicting Suicidal Depression in the non-clinical sample from three subscales of Autodestructiveness scale and five dimensions of the Big-Five personality model, after controlling for sex*

Suicidal Depression
β (P) ΔR2
Step 1

0.011
sex
-0.106 (0.329)

F(1,85) =
0.965, P = 0.329
Step 2

0.605
anxiety
0.359 (<0.001)

aggressiveness
-0.065 (0.470)

borderline
0.568 (<0.001)

F(3,82) =
43.181, P = <0.001

Step 3

0.079
Emotional Stability
-0.371 (<0.001)

F(1,81) =
21.107, P = <0.001
all variables: R2 = 0.696


F(5,81) = 37.081, P = <0.001

*Significant values are in bold.

Discussion

The results showed that clinical group had significantly higher results than non clinical group for the Autodestructiveness in general, as well as for the most subscales of the Autodestructiveness scale. The only subscale that showed no significant difference between these two groups was the Anxiety subscale. Given that all the participants from the clinical group had some of the DSM-IV diagnoses, it was expected that they would have higher results on these measures than the non-clinical group. On the other hand, the two groups showed no differences in mean results on any of the five dimensions from the Big-Five model.

The correlations between the Big-Five dimensions and the Autodestructiveness scale showed that, Emotional Instability, Introversion, and low Conscientiousness were the key dimensions related to autodestructive behavior. Our results are comparable to the results of previous studies that basically show that high Emotional Instability and Introversion are important variables when it comes to depressive symptomatology (34,46). For example, del Barrio et al (34) found negative correlations between depression and Emotional Stability and Extraversion in a sample of 423 adolescents. Similarly, Carrasco and del Barrio (36) found negative correlations of depressive symptomatology with Emotional Stability and Extraversion, but also with the remaining three dimensions of the Big-Five personality model in children and adolescents. Furthermore, highlighting the importance of Conscientiousness, John et al (45) found that boys aged 13 to 15 years with internalizing problems, whose salient features involved anxiety, somatic complaints, and social withdrawal, scored higher on Emotional Instability and Conscientiousness than non-internalizing boys.

Using hierarchical multiple regression analyses we investigated which, if any, of the Big-Five dimensions contributed to the explanation of Autodestructiveness after controlling for the sex and group effects in the total sample, and the effect of sex in separate subsamples. In the total sample, the results showed that there were two dimensions that significantly contributed to the explanation of Autodestructiveness and these were Emotional Stability and Conscientiousness. This is an interesting finding considering that Extraversion, which correlated significantly with the Autodestructiveness scale, did not prove to be a significant predictor of this variable. Furthermore, in the case of clinical sample the same two Big-Five dimensions, namely Emotional Stability and Conscientiousness, showed significant contribution to the explanation of Autodestructiveness, but in the non-clinical sample only Emotional Stability remained a significant predictor. This points to somewhat different patterns of relationships of Big-Five dimensions and Autodestructiveness in adolescents with and without psychiatric diagnoses.

We performed yet another set of hierarchical multiple regression analyses trying to explain Suicidal Depression based on the Big-Five personality model, after controlling for sex, group, and other Autodestructiveness subscales’ effects in the total sample, and the effects of sex and other Autodestructiveness subscales in separate subsamples. Once more, in the total sample, result showed that there were two dimensions that significantly contributed to the explanation of Suicidal Depression: Emotional Stability and Agreeableness. In the case of separate samples, Emotional Stability remained as the only significant predictor of Suicidal Depression in the non-clinical sample, while Agreeableness showed to be the only significant predictor in the clinical sample. It is important to highlight that these Big-Five dimensions proved to be significant predictors of Suicidal Depression, even after controlling for the effects of other subscales of the Autodestructiveness scale, which explained a large portion of variance of the criteria variable (from 58% to 65%). Furthermore, these results again point to different patterns of relationships between the Big-Five and Suicidal Depression in adolescents with and without psychiatric diagnoses, after controlling for other relevant factors.

Some limitations of the present study have to be acknowledged. The first limitation concerns different DSM-IV diagnoses of the participants in the clinical sample, as it is well documented that autodestructive behavior is regarded as a symptom of some personality disorders, namely borderline personality disorder (47). The second limitation is that in the non-clinical group there was a clear excess of female participants (74.7%). This was due to the fact that the high schools in which the participants were recruited in general had more female students.

However, none of these limitations could fundamentally affect the main outcomes of this study. Since women scored lower on Emotional Stability (48,49), and there were no substantial differences in the pattern of relations between Emotional Stability and Autodestructiveness scales and its subscales between the clinical and non-clinical group, we can conclude that sex composition of the two groups of participants had no effect on the main finding of this study.

In summary, the present research demonstrated that the Big-Five personality model can be a valid addition to the process of evaluating autodestructive behavior symptoms in adolescent psychiatric patients and adolescents without clinical diagnoses. In the performed models of hierarchical multiple regression analyses, the Big-Five model was found to account for a significant proportion of variance in Autodestructiveness and Suicidal Depression, after controlling for effects of sex, group, and in the case of Suicidal Depression, other Autodestructiveness subscales. The proposed models explained from 57% to 75% of total variance of Autodestructiveness and Suicidal Depression. Emotional Stability was the only dimension of the Big-Five model that proved to be a significant predictor in all cases, except in the case of predicting Suicidal Depression in the clinical sample. Other dimensions that showed to be important in relations of the Big-Five and autodestructive behavior were Conscientiousness in the case of Autodestructiveness, and Agreeableness in the case of Suicidal Depression. Previous studies using multiple regression analysis showed that in general population Emotional Stability and Agreeableness accounted for up to 48.3% of the variance in suicide rates, after controlling for demographics and depression (50).

In an applied perspective, our results imply that after further research, the Big-Five personality model and the IPIP questionnaire could be used as a valid addition in describing and screening autodestructive behavior in adolescent clinical and non-clinical populations.

*Significant values are in bold.

Acknowledgments

Funding This research was supported by the scientific project of the Ministry of Science, Education and Sports: Personality traits, natural language and cognitive development.

Ethical approval was received from the Ethics Committee of the Ivo Pilar Institute of Social Sciences, and from the Ethical Committee of the Clinical Hospital Osijek.

Declaration of authorship MKT participated in planning and designing of the study, planning and designing of the data acquisition, conducting data acquisition process, analyzing data, and presentation and interpretation of the results and in drafting the final version of the article. MPK participated in planning and designing of the study, planning and designing of the data acquisition, conducting data acquisition process, and planning data analysis. BM participated in interpreting the personality model, interpreting the results, and drafting and critical revision of the article.

Competing interests All authors have completed the Unified Competing Interest form at www.icmje.org/coi_disclosure.pdf (available on request from the corresponding author) and declare: no support from any organization for the submitted work; no financial relationships with any organizations that might have an interest in the submitted work in the previous 3 years; no other relationships or activities that could appear to have influenced the submitted work.

References

  • 1.Kocalevent RD, Fliege H, Rose M, Walter M. Autodestructive syndromes: A literature review. Psychother Psychosom. 2005;74:202–11. doi: 10.1159/000085143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Dautović M. Autodestructivenes scale manual [in Croatian]. Jastrebarsko: Naklada Slap; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Gilbert P, McEwan K, Irons C, Bhundia R, Christie R, Broomhead C, et al. Self-harm in a mixed clinical population: The roles of self-criticism, shame, and social rank. Br J Health Clin Psychol. 2010;49:563–76. doi: 10.1348/014466509X479771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Heath NL, Ross S, Toste JR, Charlebois A, Nedecheva T. Retrospective analysis of social factors and nonsuicidal self-injury among young adults. Can J Behav Sci. 2009;41:180–6. doi: 10.1037/a0015732. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kerr PL, Muehlenkamp JJ, Turner JM. Nonsuicidal self-injury: A review of current research for family medicine and primary care physicians. J Am Board Fam Med. 2010;23:240–59. doi: 10.3122/jabfm.2010.02.090110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Long M, Jenkins M. Counsellors’ perspectives on self-harm and the role of the therapeutic relationship for working with clients who self-harm. Counselling and Psychotherapy Research. 2010;10:192–200. doi: 10.1080/14733140903474293. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Nock MK, Prinstein MJ. A functional approach to the assessment of self-mutilative behavior. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2004;72:885–90. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.5.885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Whitlock J. Self-injurious behavior in adolescents. PLoS Med. 2010;7:e1000240. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Pattison EM, Kahan M. The deliberate self-harm syndrome. Am J Psychiatry. 1983;140:867–72. doi: 10.1176/ajp.140.7.867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.van der Kolk BA, Perry C, Herman JL. Childhood origins of self-destructive behavior. Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148:1665–71. doi: 10.1176/ajp.148.12.1665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Dautović M. The autodestructiveness scale and the MMPI-201 in the assessment of suicide. Suvremena Psihologija. 2001;4:111–20. [in Croatian] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Miller JD, Lyman DR, Widiger TA, Leukefeld C. Personality disorders as extreme variants of common personality dimensions: Can the five factor model adequately represent psychopathy? J Pers. 2001;69:253–76. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Widiger TA. The DSM-III-R categorical personality diagnoses: A critique and an alternative. Psychol Inq. 1993;4:75–90. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0402_1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Widiger TA, Simonsen E. Alternative dimensional models of personality disorder: Finding a common ground. J Pers Disord. 2005;19:110–30. doi: 10.1521/pedi.19.2.110.62628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Decuyper M, De Pauw S, De Fruyt F, De Bolle M, De Clercq BJA. Meta-Analysis of Psychopathy-, Antisocial PD- and FFM Associations. Eur J Pers. 2005;23:531–65. doi: 10.1002/per.729. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Decuyper M, De Clercq B, De Bolle M, De Fruyt F. Validation of FFM PD counts for screening personality pathology and psychopathy in adolescence. J Pers Disord. 2009;23:587–605. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2009.23.6.587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.De Raad B. The Big Five personality factors: The psycholexical approach to personality. Göttingen (Germany): Hogrefe & Huber Publishers; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Goldberg LR. An alternative “description of personality:” The Big-Five factor structure. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1990;59:1216–29. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.59.6.1216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.International Personality Item Pool. Available from: http://ipip.ori.org/. Accessed: October 12, 2012.
  • 20.Dyce JA. The Big Five factors of personality and their relatiooship to personality disorders. J Clin Psychol. 1997;53:587–93. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679(199710)53:6&#x0003c;587::AID-JCLP7&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-H. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Soldz S, Budman SH, Demby A, Merry J. Representation of personality disorders in circumplex and five-factor space: Explorations with a clinical sample. Psychol Assess. 1993;5:41–52. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.5.1.41. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Saulsman LM, Page AC. The five-factor model and personality disorder empirical literature: A meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2004;23:1055–85. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2002.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Decuyper M, De Bolle M, De Fruyt F, De Clercq B. General and maladaptive personality dimensions and the assessment of callous-unemotional traits in adolescence. J Pers Disord. 2011;25:681–701. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2011.25.5.681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lynam DR, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Raine A, Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M. Adolescent psychopathy and the big five: results from two samples. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2005;33:431–43. doi: 10.1007/s10648-005-5724-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Salekin RT, Leistico AM, Trobst KK, Schrum CL, Lochman JE. Adolescent psychopathy and personality theory–the interpersonal circumplex: expanding evidence of a nomological net. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2005;33:445–60. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-5726-Y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Kerby DS. CART analysis with unit-weighted regression to predict suicidal ideation from Big Five traits. Pers Individ Dif. 2003;35:249–62. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00174-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Steunenberg B, Braam AW, Beekman AT, Deeg DJ, Kerkhof JF. Evidence for an association of the big five personality factors with recurrence of depressive symptoms in later life. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2009;24:1470–7. doi: 10.1002/gps.2291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Trull TJ, Sher KJ. Relationship between the five-factor model of personality and Axis I disorders in nonclinical sample. J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103:350–60. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.103.2.350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Jylha P, Isometsa E. The relationship of neuroticism and extraversion to symptoms of anxiety and depression in the general population. Depress Anxiety. 2006;23:281–9. doi: 10.1002/da.20167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Wolfenstein M, Trull TJ. Depression and openness to experience. J Pers Assess. 1997;69:614–32. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6903_14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Anderson KW, McLean PD. Conscientiousness in depression: Tendencies, predictive utility, and longitudinal stability. Cognit Ther Res. 1997;21:223–38. doi: 10.1023/A:1021836830389. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Soto CJ, John OP, Gosling SD, Potter J. Age differences in personality traits from 10 to 65: Big Five domains and facets in a large cross-sectional sample. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011;100:330–48. doi: 10.1037/a0021717. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Branje SJT, Van Lieshout CFM, Gerris JRM. Big Five personality development in adolescence and adulthood. Eur J Pers. 2007;21:45–62. doi: 10.1002/per.596. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Del Barrio V, Moreno-Rosset C, Lopez-Martinez R, Olmedo M. Anxiety, depression and personality structure. Pers Individ Dif. 1997;23:327–35. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00030-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.John OP, Caspi A, Robins RW, Moffitt TE, Stouthamer-Loeber M. Child Dev. The “Little five”: exploring the nomological network of the five-factor model of personality in adolescent boys. Child Dev. 1994;65:160–78. doi: 10.2307/1131373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Carrasco Ortiz MA, del Barrio MV. Temperamental and personality variables in child and adolescent depressive symptomatology. Psicothema. 2007;19:43–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Subotić S, Brajša-Žganec A, Merkaš M. School stress and certain personality characteristics as predictors of suicidality in adolescents. Psihologijske Teme. 2008;17:111–31. [in Croatian] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Mlačić B, Goldberg LR. An analysis of a cross-cultural personality inventory: The IPIP Big-Five factor markers. J Pers Assess. 2007;88:168–77. doi: 10.1080/00223890701267993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Goldberg LR. A broad-bandwidht, public-domain, personality inventory measuring the lower-level facets of several Five-Factor models. Personality Psychology in Europe. 1999;7:7–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Mlačić B, Milas G, Kratohvil A. Adolescent personality and self-esteem – an analysis of self-reports and parental-ratings. Društvena istraživanja. 2007;16:87–88. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Watson D, Clark LA. Extraversion and its positive emotional core. In: Hogan R, Johnson J, Briggs S, editors. Handbook of personality psychology. San Diego (CA): Academic Press; 1997. p. 767-793. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Graziano WG, Eisenberg N. Agreeableness: A dimension of personality. In: Hogan R, Johnson J, Briggs S, editors. Handbook of Personality Psychology. San Diego (CA): Academic Press; 1997. p. 95-824. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hogan J, Ones DS. Conscientiousness and integrity at work. In: Hogan R, Johnson J, Briggs S, editors. Handbook of personality psychology. San Diego (CA): Academic Press; 1997. p. 849-870. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.John OP, Naumann LP, Soto CJ. Paradigm shift to the integrative Big-Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues. In: John OP, Robins RW, Pervin LA, editors. Handbook of personality: Theory and research. New York (NY): Guilford Press; 2008. p. 114-158. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Williams DG. Effects of psychoticism, extraversion and neuroticism in current mood: A statistical review of six studies. Pers Individ Dif. 1990;11:615–30. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(90)90045-S. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Costa PT, Jr, Terracciano A, McCrae RR. Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: Robust and surprising findings. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;81:322–31. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Schmitt DP, Realo A, Voracek M, Allik J. Why can't a man be more like a woman? Sex differences in Big Five personality traits across 55 cultures. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2008;94:168–82. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.McCann SJ. Suicide, Big Five personality factors, and depression at the American state level. Arch Suicide Res. 2010;14:368–74. doi: 10.1080/13811118.2010.524070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Croatian Medical Journal are provided here courtesy of Medicinska Naklada

RESOURCES