Skip to main content
Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry logoLink to Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
. 2012 Nov;21(4):282–288.

Efficacy of Methylphenidate in ADHD Children across the Normal and the Gifted Intellectual Spectrum

Natalie Grizenko 1,2,, David Dong Qi Zhang 3, Anna Polotskaia 2,4, Ridha Joober 1,2,5
PMCID: PMC3490529  PMID: 23133462

Abstract

Objective:

This study evaluates whether attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) children with a borderline intelligence quotient (IQ) (70≤FSIQ<80), normal IQ (80≤FSIQ<120) and high IQ (FSIQ≥120) respond differently to psychostimulant treatment.

Method:

502 children, aged 6 to 12 years, with an IQ range from 70 to 150 participated in a two-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover methylphenidate (MPH) trial.

Results:

In addition to differences in socioeconomic background and parental education, higher IQ children were found to present with less severe symptoms. No significant differences were found with regards to treatment response.

Conclusion:

ADHD children within the normal and high levels of intellectual functioning all respond equally to psychostimulant treatment, and that proper medication management is necessary for all children with the disorder.

Keywords: ADHD, IQ, methylphenidate response

Introduction

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a psychiatric behavioral disorder that presents with the core deficits of inattention, impulsivity and hyperactivity and often leads to significant impairments in school and overall functioning. The use of stimulant drugs, such as methylphenidate (MPH) is only efficacious in 70% of ADHD patients (Spencer et al., 1996). It is therefore important to differentiate between responders and non-responders to stimulants.

There are conflicting reports in the present literature as to the effects of intellectual functioning on stimulant treatment response in children who function within the normal intellectual spectrum. The relationship of intelligence to responder status has been reviewed to be inexistent or at most minimal (Gray & Kagan, 2000). Some studies have failed to demonstrate significant intelligence quotient (IQ) differences between responders and non-responders to MPH (Mayes, Crites, Bixler, Humphrey, & Mattison, 1994; Zeiner, Bryhn, Bjercke, Truyen, & Strand, 1999). However, the small sample sizes and absence of parental evaluation of outcome in Mayes et al.’s study and lack of objective laboratory ratings in Zeiner et al.’s study may limit their findings.

On the other hand, an equally important body of literature has demonstrated a significant positive influence of IQ on the response to MPH. Results reported by the Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD (MTA) (n = 579, IQ mean = 101, SD = 14.7) have shown in a subgroup a positive relationship between child IQ and MPH response (Owens et al., 2003). Several other studies have also reported that higher IQs are associated with better responses to MPH (Buitelaar, Van der Gaag, Swaab-Barneveld, & Kuiper, 1995; Van der Oord, Prins, Oosterlaan, & Emmelkamp, 2008). Nonetheless the findings of these studies may be limited by their small sample sizes (n<70) and by their exclusive use of parent and teacher ratings of behavior to evaluate treatment response. In a study conducted with a group of 336 children, in which 89% had IQs between 80 and 120, Thomson & Varley reported that higher IQ levels positively predicted the response to MPH. However full scale IQ (FSIQ) from the Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R) was available for only 155 children (Thomson & Varley, 1998).

In contrast to the above findings, lower IQ has also been found to predict a better response to MPH (Taylor, Schachar, Thorley, Wieselberg, & Everitt, 1987), but the small sample size (n=39) and the absence of objective laboratory rating scales may limit the generalizability and validity of their results.

As for the children functioning in the extremes of the intellectual spectrum, a small but consistent body of literature has demonstrated the reduced efficacy of MPH among children with an intellectual quotient (IQ) of less than 70 compared to those whose IQ is greater than 70 (Aman, Buican, & Arnold, 2003; Aman, Marks, Turbott, Wilsher, & Merry, 1991). In contrast, with regards to ADHD children functioning in the higher intellectual spectrum of giftedness (IQ above 120) (Antshel, 2008), most studies do not have a large enough high IQ sample size (often <10) to examine specifically how gifted children respond to MPH compared to lower IQ children. At present the scarcity of reliable literature does not point to a conclusion on the efficacy of psychostimulants in children with higher intellectual abilities.

No clear consensus has been reached on IQ as a predictor of the response to MPH in children with normal and high intellectual abilities. However determining whether IQ can influence stimulant treatment outcome in ADHD patients is important, as it may significantly impact clinical decision-making.

We conducted a two-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover, randomized MPH trial with a large sample size of 502 children using parent, teacher as well as laboratory evaluations of outcome. The aim of our study was to compare the responses to MPH treatment of children functioning in the high, normal and borderline levels of the intellectual spectrum.

Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited from the Severe Disruptive Behavior Disorder Program and from the outpatient clinics at the Douglas Mental Health University Institute, a psychiatric teaching hospital in Montreal, Canada.

The present study consisted of 502 children 6 to 12 years old (mean = 9.05; SD = 1.86). It included 393 boys and 109 girls. 46.1% of the subjects came from a family with an income of more than $40,000 CAD per year, 11.9% between $30,000 and $40,000 CAD, 14.4% between $20,000 and $30,000 CAD and 27.7% less than $20,000 CAD. The fathers of the participants received on average 12.44 years of education (range 3 to 27 years, SD = 3.4) and, the mothers received on average 13.06 years (range 5 to 27 years, SD = 3.16).

The children were diagnosed as ADHD according to the criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV) (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) by two experienced child psychiatrists. The diagnosis was based on clinical interviews with a psychiatrist, school reports, the Conners Global Index Teacher version (CGI-T) (Conners, Sitarenios, Parker, & Epstein, 1998b) and the Conners Global Index Parent version (CGIP) (Conners, Sitarenios, Parker, & Epstein, 1998a). Within the sample, 51.7% of the children had a combined subtype of ADHD, 38.0% had the inattentive subtype, and 10.3% had the hyperactive subtype.

Exclusion criteria included an IQ of less than 70, a history of Tourette’s syndrome, pervasive developmental disorder, psychosis, and previous intolerance or allergic reaction to MPH. 38.9% of children in our sample had been on some medication in the past, but all medications were stopped for two weeks before the start of our clinical trial.

Baseline IQ scores were obtained from the full scale IQ (FSIQ) of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Third Edition (WISC-III) (Wechsler, 1991) from 1999 to 2004 and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) from 2004 to 2011 (Wechsler, 2003).The correlation between WISC-IV and WISC-III FSIQ in normal children has been found to be reliably high (r = 0.89) (Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen, 2006). FSIQs ranged from 70 to 150 (mean = 96.31; SD = 13.34). According to their FSIQ, the children were classified into three groups: borderline IQ (70 ≤FSIQ<80; n = 45; mean = 74.98; SD = 3.20), average IQ (80 ≤FSIQ<120; n = 430; mean = 96.68; SD = 9.98), and superior IQ (FSIQ ≥120; n = 27; mean = 126.07; SD = 6.98).

The participant’s mothers reported whether they smoked tobacco or consumed alcohol during their pregnancy.

Other baseline measures included: the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983), a 113-item questionnaire for parents that measures internalizing and externalizing behavior; the Conners’ Global Index for Parents (CGI-P) and the Conners’ Global Index for Teachers (CGI-T), filled out, respectively, by parents and teachers. Both CGI-T and CGI-P were used to determine the frequency of 10 types of ecologically relevant behavior.

The parents of the children signed informed consent, and the subjects agreed to participate in the trial.

Stimulant Trial

The core of this study consisted of a two-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover, randomized MPH trial. The Research and Ethics Board of the Douglas Institute approved the trial. After an initial week of baseline assessments, all subjects received either placebo or 0.5 mg/kg of body weight of MPH divided into two equal doses administered daily in the morning and at noon for one week. The participants were then crossed over in the second week. A research psychologist who had no contact with the patients completed the randomization. All capsules, MPH and placebo, were prepared by a clinical pharmacist who was not otherwise involved in the study. No important adverse events or side effects were noted.

Assessments of Outcome

During the medication trial weeks, after observing each child for five days at school, teachers were asked to evaluate the participants’ behavior at school by completing the CGI-T. Parents were asked to evaluate behavior at home by completing the CGI-P on the Sunday after giving the medication to the children during the weekend. The subtracted difference between the CGI-T and CGI-P scores of the medication and of the placebo weeks was used as an outcome measure. Scoring change on the restless-impulsive (RI) and on the emotional lability (EL) subscales of both CGI-T and CGI-P were also examined. In addition the children were assessed in the laboratory on the third of day of each week with the Restricted Academic Situation Scale (RASS), a measure that assesses off task, fidgeting, vocalizing, playing with objects and being out of seat behavior while doing math problems (Barkley, 1990). The RASS was given before and 45 minutes after the administration of the medication. Detailed explanations of our clinical diagnosis and stimulant trial procedures can be found in (Grizenko, Paci, & Joober, 2010).

Statistical Analysis

IQ groups were compared using cross-tabulations and calculated significance using χ2 tests for categorical variables. For continuous variables, analysis of variance (ANOVA) and independent samples T-tests were used.

Results

ANOVA analysis of the demographic characteristics of our sample yielded significant differences between the three IQ groups with regards to their income groups (p<0.001), the fathers’ education durations (p<0.001) as well as the mothers’ education durations (p<0.001) (see Table 1). Subsequent independent samples T analysis between the groups all proved to be significant. Generally higher IQ subjects came from families with a higher income and had parents with more education than subjects of lower IQ. No difference was found with regards to in utero tobacco and alcohol exposure between the three groups.

Table 1.

Demographics of ADHD subjects according to IQ level

Borderline (Bord.) IQ Average (Av.) IQ Superior (Sup.) IQ Statistics Degrees of freedom (df) Pa
IQ range 70 ≤ FSIQ < 80 80 ≤ FSIQ < 120 FSIQ ≥ 120
N (%) 45 (9.0) 430 (85.7) 27 (5.4)
Age (SD) 8.85 (2.03) 9.07 (1.85) 9.22 (1.67) F = 0.39 2 0.675
  Bord. vs Av. IQ t = −0.754 473 0.451
  Av. vs Sup. IQ t = −0.406 455 0.685
  Bord. vs Sup. IQ t = −0.797 70 0.428
Male/female (%) 35/10 (77.8/22.2) 334/96 (77.7/22.3) 24/3 (88.9/11.1) χ2 = 1.887 2 0.389
  Bord. vs Av. IQ χ2 = 0.000 1 0.987
  Av. vs Sup. IQ χ2 = 1.883 1 0.170
  Bord. vs Sup. IQ χ2 = 1.408 1 0.235
Income groupb (SD) 3.86 (1.67) 4.62 (1.56) 5.46 (1.02) F = 8.60 2 0.000
  Bord. vs Av. IQ t = −3.007 443 0.003
  Av. vs Sup. IQ t = −2.596 424 0.010
  Bord. vs Sup. IQ t = −4.256 65 0.000
Father’s years of education (SD) 10.76 (3) 12.51 (3.29) 15.22 (3.28) F = 12.68 2 0.000
  Bord. vs Av. IQ t = −2.944 346 0.003
  Av. vs Sup. IQ t = −3.808 336 0.000
  Bord. vs Sup. IQ t = −5.271 54 0.000
Mother’s years of education (SD) 11.45 (2.791) 13.23 (3.14) 14.6 (3.37) F = 8.74 2 0.000
  Bord. vs Av. IQ t = −3.438 414 0.001
  Av. vs Sup. IQ t = −2.111 399 0.035
  Bord. vs Sup. IQ t = −4.086 63 0.000

χ2 for Chi-square, F for Anova, t for T-test

a

Significance set at p = 0.05

b

Income groups set at 1 for < Can$ 6 000, 2 for Can$ 6–10 000, 3 for Can$ 10–20 000, 4 for Can$ 20–30 000, 5 for Can$ 30–40 000, 6 for > Can$ 40 000.

Baseline CBCL total T score differed markedly upon ANOVA analysis between the IQ groups (p=0.010) (see Table 2). Subjects from the superior IQ group display less behavior problems compared to average IQ (p=0.014) and borderline IQ children (p=0.002).Subsequent detailed ANOVA analysis of CBCL subscales scores revealed significant differences between the groups with regards to social problems (p=0.001), attention problems (p=0.003) and delinquent behavior (p=0.025).

Table 2.

Baseline clinical characteristics according to IQ level

Borderline (Bord.) IQ Average (Av.) IQ Superior (Sup.) IQ Statistics Degrees of freedom (df) Pa
IQ range 70 ≤ FSIQ < 80 80 ≤ FSIQ < 120 FSIQ ≥ 120
ADHD subtype: inattentive/hyperactive/combined (%) 14/6/25 (31.1/13.3/55.6) 164/42/223 (38.2/9,8/52) 12/4/11 (44.4/14.8/40.7) χ2 = 2.661 4 0.616
  Bord. vs Av. IQ χ2 = 1.154 2 0.562
  Av. vs Sup. IQ χ2 = 1.515 2 0.469
  Bord. vs Sup. IQ χ2 = 1.598 2 0.450
CBCLb total T score (SD) 70.58 (6.95) 68.46 (8.69) 64.15 (10.44) F = 4.69 2 0.010
  Bord. vs Av. IQ t = 1.578 462 0.115
  Av. vs Sup. IQ t = 2.468 444 0.014
  Bord. vs Sup. IQ t = 3.138 70 0.002
CGI-Pb total baseline score (SD) 75.31 (11.24) 72.27 (11.35) 69.28 (9.65) F = 2.29 2 0.103
  Bord. vs Av. IQ t = 1.597 427 0.111
  Av. vs Sup. IQ t = 1.286 413 0.199
  Bord. vs Sup. IQ t = 2.209 62 0.031
CGI-Tb total baseline score (SD) 71.34 (13.78) 69 (12.59) 69.19 (13.79) F = 0.58 2 0.558
  Bord. vs Av. IQ t = 1.086 438 0.278
  Av. vs Sup. IQ t = −0.074 426 0.941
  Bord. vs Sup. IQ t = 0.613 62 0.542

χ2 for Chi-square, F for Anova, t for T-test

a

Significance set at p = 0.05

b

CBCL: Child Behavior Checklist; CGI-P: Conners Global Index - Parent version; CGI-T: Conners Global Index - Teacher version

Independent Samples

T-tests demonstrated that average IQ children display less social (p=0.001) and attention (p=0.006) problems than their borderline IQ counterparts, and that superior IQ children scored less than borderline IQ children on the sub-scales of externalizing behavior (p=0.048), social problems (p=0.001), thought problems (p=0.010) and attention problems (p=0.002). Superior IQ subjects differed from the average IQ subjects on only the subscale of delinquent behavior, with the average IQ subjects displaying more problems (p=0.009).On the baseline CGI-P test, parents of superior IQ subjects rated their children’s symptoms as being less severe compared to subjects with a borderline IQ (p=0.031). Baseline CGI-T scores did not differ significantly between the IQ groups (p=0.558).

ANOVA analysis of the improvements in CGI-P, CGI-T and RASS scores following medication did not show significant differences between the IQ groups (see Table 3). Subsequent independent samples T-tests showed a trend but no statistically significant difference in change on the CGI-P total and RI scores between the superior IQ and borderline IQ subjects, with the high IQ children generally showing less improvement than borderline IQ children (p=0.085 and p=0.071).

Table 3.

Response to medication according to IQ level

Borderline (Bord.) IQ Average (Av.) IQ Superior (Sup.) IQ Statistics Degrees of freedom (df) Pa
IQ range 70 ≤ FSIQ < 80 80 ≤ FSIQ < 120 FSIQ ≥ 120
Change in CGI-Pbtotal score (SD) 8.13 (14.16) 4.56 (14.41) 1.91 (11.57) F = 1.563 2 0.211
  Bord. vs Av. IQ t = 1.474 417 0.141
  Av. vs Sup. IQ t = 0.848 400 0.397
  Bord. vs Sup. IQ t = 1.754 59 0.085
Change in CGI-P restless-impulsive score (SD) 8.36 (13.40) 5.40 (14.26) 1.95 (12.43) F = 1.506 2 0.223
  Bord. vs Av. IQ t = 1.241 417 0.215
  Av. vs Sup. IQ t = 1.109 400 0.268
  Bord. vs Sup. IQ t = 1.838 59 0.071
Change in CGI-P emotional lability score (SD) 5.26 (17.12) 1.78 (15.15) 1.86 (11.71) F = 0.927 2 0.396
  Bord. vs Av. IQ t = 1.346 417 0.179
  Av. vs Sup. IQ t = 0.024 400 0.981
  Bord. vs Sup. IQ t = 0.825 59 0.412
Change in CGI-Tb total score (SD) 8.57 (13.80) 9.85 (13.18) 9.82 (12.40) F = 1.59 2 0.853
  Bord. vs Av. IQ t = 0.562 402 0.575
  Av. vs Sup. IQ t = 0.011 387 0.991
  Bord. vs Sup. IQ t = 0.349 57 0.728
Change in CGI-T restless-impulsive score (SD) 8.11 (12.62) 9.35 (12.23) 8.82 (10.76) F = 0.185 2 0.831
  Bord. vs Av. IQ t = 0.585 402 0.559
  Av. vs Sup. IQ t = 0.198 38 0.843
  Bord. vs Sup. IQ t = 0.220 57 0.826
Change in CGI-T emotional lability score (SD) 6.22 (15.10) 7.78 (13.59) 7.91 (14.24) F = 0.221 2 0.802
  Bord. vs Av. IQ t = 0.660 402 0.510
  Av. vs Sup. IQ t = 0.043 387 0.965
  Bord. vs Sup. IQ t = 0.425 57 0.672
Change in RASSb (SD) 27.14 (40.07) 27.07 (30.72) 29.47 (23.32) F = 0.75 2 0.928
  Bord. vs Av. IQ t = −0.014 464 0.989
  Av. vs Sup. IQ t = 0.399 448 0.690
  Bord. vs Sup. IQ t = 0.274 68 0.785

χ2 for Chi-square, F for Anova, t for T-test

a

Significance set at p = 0.05

b

CGI-P: Conners Global Index – Parent version; CGI-T: Conners Global Index – Teacher version; RASS: Restricted Academic Situation Scale

Discussion

The major result of this study is that there was no statistically significant difference in the response to MPH for children in the borderline, average and superior IQ levels.

The results of this study suggest that ADHD children, depending on their cognitive ability, differ in their socioeconomic background as well as the amount of education received by their parents. Not surprisingly, higher IQ children tend to have more educated parents than children with lower IQ, and these parents in turn tend to earn more for the family.

Higher IQ children generally presented with a less severe symptomatology than children of lower IQs. Borderline IQ children had the greatest level of attention, social and externalizing problems at baseline. Our results are in line with previous findings of a negative phenotypic correlation between IQ and ADHD symptoms scores (Simonoff, Pickles, Wood, Gringras, & Chadwick, 2007). At present the mechanism of this relationship has not been elucidated, although Simonoff et al. found no evidence that inappropriate expectations by raters or confounding associations with other psychiatric problems could account for it.

With regards to MPH treatment, ADHD children with different IQs do equally well by improving on parent, teacher and laboratory ratings. However some degree of variation was observed. When mean scoring changes are examined, superior IQ children (mean = 1.91; SD = 11.57) show less improvement on the CGI-P total score when compared to average (mean = 4.56; SD = 14.41) and borderline IQ subjects (mean = 8.13; SD = 14.16), but all three groups of children seem to improve equally on teacher ratings of the CGI. In addition, a trend was found when examining the improvements of superior IQ children compared to borderline counterparts on CGI-P total and RI scores (p=0.085 and p=0.071). The rather large standard deviations might explain the lack of significance. The discrepancy between the parent and teacher ratings of improvement suggests that the improvements due to medication in children with a superior IQ are minimized by their parents who may not notice important changes in their child’s behavior at home. In this sense parental ratings seem to be more sensitive to changes in the hyperactivity of their child rather than to subtle improvements in attention deficits. On the other hand, medication effects are noticed by teachers who see obvious improvements in their performances at school as well as in their general behavior. Furthermore, it is important to note that children with borderline IQ, according to parents on the CBCL and CGI-P, tend to be seen as having more behavioral and attentional problems than the others. This may also explain why the change in the CGI-P score with medication was greater for children in the borderline intelligence group. What is important though is that the teachers, who saw all children equally problematic on the CGI-T at baseline reported a CGI-T change score on medication as very similar across IQ levels. Therefore, the magnitude of amelioration may be affected by the perceived initial severity of symptomatology.

A limitation of our study lies in the lack of different MPH doses in our medication trial. Different doses of the medication may have allowed us to see different improvement effects in the children. It is also important to point out that our sample is clinically referred and not population-based. Even though our sample is one of the largest in the literature to examine the effects of IQ in response to medication in a double-blind placebo-controlled MPH trial we do have relatively few children in the gifted range. Furthermore our trial did not examine the effect of long-acting psychostimulants other than methylphenidate.

Conclusion

To our knowledge, this study is the first large double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover methylphenidate trial study to be conducted with the specific focus of looking at responses among children with different intellectual functioning levels in the normal and gifted range. The results of this study point to the conclusion that ADHD children, despite individual variations in intellectual ability, all respond in a similar fashion to methylphenidate. Therefore a proper medication treatment plan is warranted for all ADHD children.

Acknowledgements/Conflicts of Interest

The authors would like to thank Johanne Bellingham, Sandra Robinson, Jacqueline Richard, Phuong-Thao Nguyen and Marie-Ève Fortier for their devoted help with data collection. This study was supported by grants from The Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) and the Fonds de la recherche en santé du Québec (FRSQ). The authors have no conflict of interest to declare.

References

  1. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock C. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist and Revised Child Beahvior Profile. Burlington: Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  2. Aman MG, Buican B, Arnold LE. Methylphenidate treatment in children with borderline IQ and mental retardation: Analysis of three aggregated studies. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology. 2003;13(1):29–40. doi: 10.1089/104454603321666171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aman MG, Marks RE, Turbott SH, Wilsher CP, Merry SN. Clinical effects of methylphenidate and thioridazine in intellectually subaverage children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1991;30(2):246–256. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199103000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Antshel KM. Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in the context of a high intellectual quotient/giftedness. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2008;14(4):293–299. doi: 10.1002/ddrr.34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barkley RA. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment. New York: Guilford; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  6. Buitelaar JK, Van der Gaag RJ, Swaab-Barneveld H, Kuiper M. Prediction of clinical response to methylphenidate in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1995;34(8):1025–1032. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199508000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Conners CK, Sitarenios G, Parker JD, Epstein JN. The revised Conners’ Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-R): factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1998a;26(4):257–268. doi: 10.1023/a:1022602400621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Conners CK, Sitarenios G, Parker JD, Epstein JN. Revision and restandardization of the Conners Teacher Rating Scale (CTRS-R): Factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1998b;26(4):279–291. doi: 10.1023/a:1022606501530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Gray JR, Kagan J. The challenge of predicting which children with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder will respond positively to methylphenidate. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2000;21(5):471–489. [Google Scholar]
  10. Grizenko N, Paci M, Joober R. Is the inattentive subtype of ADHD different from the combined/hyperactive subtype? Journal of Attention Disorders. 2010;13(6):649–657. doi: 10.1177/1087054709347200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Mayes SD, Crites DL, Bixler EO, Humphrey FJ, 2nd, Mattison RE. Methylphenidate and ADHD: Influence of age, IQ and neurodevelopmental status. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 1994;36(12):1099–1107. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1994.tb11811.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Owens EB, Hinshaw SP, Kraemer HC, Arnold LE, Abikoff HB, Cantwell DP, Wigal T. Which treatment for whom for ADHD? Moderators of treatment response in the MTA. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2003;71(3):540–552. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.71.3.540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Simonoff E, Pickles A, Wood N, Gringras P, Chadwick O. ADHD symptoms in children with mild intellectual disability. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2007;46(5):591–600. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e3180323330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Spencer T, Biederman J, Wilens T, Harding M, O’Donnell D, Griffin S. Pharmacotherapy of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder across the life cycle. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1996;35(4):409–432. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199604000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Strauss E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms and Commentary. 3 ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  16. Taylor E, Schachar R, Thorley G, Wieselberg HM, Everitt B. Which boys respond to stimulant medication? A controlled trial of methylphenidate in boys with disruptive behaviour. Psychological Medicine. 1987;17(1):121–143. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700013039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Thomson JB, Varley CK. Prediction of stimulant response in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology. 1998;8(2):125–132. doi: 10.1089/cap.1998.8.125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Van der Oord S, Prins PJ, Oosterlaan J, Emmelkamp PM. Treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children. Predictors of treatment outcome. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2008;17(2):73–81. doi: 10.1007/s00787-007-0638-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third Edition: Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  20. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  21. Zeiner P, Bryhn G, Bjercke C, Truyen K, Strand G. Response to methylphenidate in boys with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Acta Paediatrica. 1999;88(3):298–303. doi: 10.1080/08035259950170060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

RESOURCES