Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Mar 21.
Published in final edited form as: Science. 2012 Sep 21;337(6101):1505–1510. doi: 10.1126/science.1224648

NEIGHBORHOOD EFFECTS ON THE LONG-TERM WELL-BEING OF LOW-INCOME ADULTS

Jens Ludwig a,b,1, Greg J Duncan c, Lisa A Gennetian d, Lawrence F Katz b,e, Ronald C Kessler f, Jeffrey R Kling b,g, Lisa Sanbonmatsu b
PMCID: PMC3491569  NIHMSID: NIHMS403688  PMID: 22997331

Abstract

Nearly 9 million Americans live in extreme-poverty neighborhoods, places that also tend to be racially segregated and dangerous. Yet the effects on the well-being of residents of moving out of such communities into less-distressed areas remain uncertain. Using data from Moving to Opportunity, a unique randomized housing mobility experiment, we find that moving from a high-poverty to lower-poverty neighborhood leads to long-term (10 to 15 year) improvements in adult physical and mental health and subjective well-being, despite not affecting economic self-sufficiency. A 1 standard deviation decline in neighborhood poverty (13 percentage points) increases subjective well-being by an amount equal to the gap in subjective well-being between people whose annual incomes differ by $13,000, a large amount given that the average control group income is $20,000. Subjective well-being is more strongly affected by changes in neighborhood economic disadvantage than racial segregation, which is important because racial segregation has been declining since 1970 but income segregation has been increasing.


Nearly 9 million people in the United States live in “extreme-poverty” neighborhoods in which at least 40 percent of residents have incomes below the federal poverty threshold, which for 2011 equaled about $23,000 for a family of four (1, 2). Such neighborhoods also tend to be racially segregated with high rates of crime and disorder and low-quality public services (3). Studies dating back as far as the 17th century have shown that people living in distressed neighborhoods have greater criminal involvement and fare worse on educational, economic, and health outcomes than those living in less-distressed areas (36). These patterns have generated long-standing concern that distressed neighborhood environments might themselves adversely affect people’s lives and “doubly-disadvantage” their low-income residents.

But much uncertainty remains about the degree to which variation across neighborhoods in people’s outcomes reflects the independent causal effects of neighborhood environments per se instead of the propensity of different types of people to live in different areas. Even the most detailed data collection effort may be unable to measure adequately all of the individual- or family-level characteristics that influence both neighborhood selection and life outcomes. This type of “selection bias” can substantially distort non-experimental estimates of “neighborhood effects” (7). Yet determining the importance of changes in people’s neighborhood environments for their life outcomes is a central issue for the social and medical sciences and social policy.

An understanding of the mechanisms through which neighborhood environments affect people’s lives is a crucial issue for policy design. Much of the debate among researchers has focused on the relative importance of residential racial segregation versus economic segregation. Nearly 70 years ago Gunnar Myrdal argued that racial segregation enabled policymakers to reduce the quality of public services to blacks without harming whites (8), a concern echoed by the 1968 National Advisory Commission on Civil Disorders (the “Kerner Report”) (9). Douglas Massey and Nancy Denton subsequently argued in their widely-cited 1993 book American Apartheid that “residential segregation has been instrumental in creating a structural niche within which a deleterious set of attitudes and behaviors – a culture of segregation – has arisen and flourished” (10, p. 8).

In contrast, William Julius Wilson’s landmark 1987 book The Truly Disadvantaged argued that the flight of black working- and middle-class families out of ghettos in the 1960s and 1970s was harmful to the families who remained behind not because of any increased racial segregation, but rather because this exodus removed “mainstream role models that help keep alive the perception that education is meaningful, that steady employment is a viable alternative to welfare, and that family stability is the norm, not the exception” 11, p.49. Subsequent work has examined other pathways through which spatially-concentrated disadvantage might affect people’s lives such as declines in “collective efficacy” – the willingness and ability of community residents to work together to support shared norms (3, 5).

Distinguishing the effects of changes in racial versus income segregation also helps answer the question of whether the problem of harmful neighborhood effects on disadvantaged populations is getting better or worse over time, given opposing recent trends in U.S. residential segregation by race and income. Specifically, racial segregation in America peaked in 1970 and has been declining over the past 40 years, to levels not seen since 1910 (12), whereas income segregation has been increasing since 1970 (13, 14.

This paper examines the long-term effects of moving into a less-distressed neighborhood environment on the well-being of low-income adults using new data from a unique, large-scale randomized social experiment – the U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development’s (HUD) Moving to Opportunity (MTO) demonstration. Via random lottery, MTO offered some public housing families but not others the chance to move into a less-distressed area (see supplementary materials section 1). MTO randomization generates large, persistent differences in neighborhood conditions across otherwise comparable groups of families and enables us to attribute differences in post-baseline outcomes across groups to the MTO-assisted moves.

Unlike many social experiments that follow people for short periods, we focus on long-term effects through in-person data collected 10–15 years after randomization. We have shown elsewhere that MTO moves have long-term beneficial effects on a narrow but important set of physical health measures, related to extreme obesity and diabetes (15). The implications for how neighborhoods affect the overall quality of the lives of participating families were not addressed in that work.

In the current report, we use data from the MTO experiment to examine the long-term effects of moving to less distressed neighborhoods on broad measures of the well-being of low-income adults. We examine “objective” outcomes (economic self-sufficiency, physical health, and mental health) that have been the traditional focus of this literature. We also take a new approach in examining experimental neighborhood effects on a comprehensive measure of people’s quality of life as they perceive it, using adult self-reports of subjective well-being (SWB). And we investigate the relative importance of racial segregation vs. income segregation in affecting the SWB of low-income adults.

The Moving to Opportunity experiment

From 1994 to 1998 MTO enrolled 4,604 low-income public housing families living in high-poverty neighborhoods within five U.S. cities: Baltimore, Boston, Chicago, Los Angeles, and New York. Families were randomized into three groups: i) the Low-Poverty Voucher (LPV) group, which received housing vouchers that subsidize private-market rents, but could only be used in census tracts with 1990 poverty rates below 10 percent; ii) the Traditional Voucher (TRV) group which received regular housing vouchers without any MTO relocation constraint; and iii) a control group which received no assistance through MTO. Some 48% of the adults assigned to the LPV group and 63% of those assigned to the TRV group managed to relocate using an MTO voucher (the MTO “compliance rate”). Because the effects of LPV and TRV assignment on neighborhood conditions converge over time, and to maximize statistical power, we initially present results that pool the two treatment groups together. (Separate estimates for the LPV and TRV groups are in tables S1-S4).

Data from baseline surveys collected from all MTO adults shows these families were quite economically disadvantaged when they applied for MTO (table 1). Most household heads were African-American or Hispanic females; fewer than 40% had completed high school. By far the most common reason applicants reported signing up for MTO was to get away from gangs and drugs, with around three-quarters reporting this as one of their top two reasons for wanting to move.

Table 1.

Baseline characteristics (1994–98) of adults interviewed as part of long-term survey (N=3,273), by randomized MTO treatment status. Mean values represent shares, except for age and income; missing values have been imputed (except income). Values are weighted to account for changes over time in treatment assignment likelihood and for the follow-up survey sampling design (see supplementary materials section 1).

Control group
mean
MTO treatment
(voucher) groups mean
N=1139 N=2134
Gender and age
Female 0.978 0.984
Age as of December 31, 2007 (years) 44.5 44.6
Race and ethnicity
African-American (any ethnicity) 0.660 0.640
Hispanic ethnicity (any race) 0.304 0.325
Other demographic characteristics
Never married 0.637 0.623
Working 0.245 0.270
High school diploma 0.361 0.367
General Educational Development (GED) 0.199 0.169*
Receiving Aid to Families with Dependent Children (AFDC) 0.763 0.752
Household characteristics
Household income (2009 dollars) $12,438.64 $12,833.64
Site
Baltimore 0.135 0.136
Boston 0.205 0.203
Chicago 0.205 0.206
Los Angeles 0.226 0.225
New York 0.229 0.229
Neighborhood characteristics
Household member was crime victim in last six months 0.416 0.425
Very dissatisfied with neighborhood 0.467 0.478
Primary or Secondary Reason for Wanting to Move
To get away from gangs and drugs 0.779 0.770
Better schools for children 0.481 0.516*
To get a bigger or better apartment 0.457 0.440
To get a job 0.069 0.058
***

= P<.01,

**

= P<.05,

*

= P<.10 on two-tailed t-test of difference between MTO treatment and control groups.

As one would expect from a properly-conducted random assignment, the distribution of baseline characteristics is balanced between the treatment and control groups. Among the 21 baseline characteristics reported in table 1 just two treatment-control differences are significant at P<.10 and none is significant at the P<.05 threshold. An F-test fails to reject the null hypothesis that treatment-control differences in the baseline variables shown in table 1 are jointly zero (P=.462).

Measures

To measure long-term effects of changing neighborhoods on adults in the MTO demonstration, the Institute for Social Research at the University of Michigan—under subcontract with our research team—collected in-person data from participants 10–15 years after random assignment (hereafter “long-term survey;” see supplementary materials section 2 for details). Interviewers were blinded to the MTO group assignments of participating families. The effective response rate for MTO adults was 90% and was similar across randomized MTO groups.

To measure the neighborhood conditions in which families were living during the follow-up study period, we linked address information for MTO adults to census tract-level data on population characteristics from the 1990 and 2000 decennial censuses and the 2005–09 American Community Surveys. Our main results focus on duration-weighted tract characteristics averaged over the entire post-randomization study period, since people’s life outcomes may depend on cumulative exposure to neighborhood environments not just current neighborhood conditions (16). The long-term surveys also asked MTO participants to self-report on conditions of the neighborhoods and housing units in which they were living at the time.

To measure neighborhood effects on traditional “objective” measures of well-being, we construct summary indices of long-term adult outcomes in the domains of economic self-sufficiency, physical health, and mental health. We focus on adults in part because of our interest in well-being over the long term, which may not yet be evident for the MTO children. Our outcome indices are constructed from a set of individual outcomes that are re-scaled so that higher values represent “better” outcomes and then converted to Z-scores using the control group distribution. Aggregating outcomes improves statistical power to detect impacts and reduces risk of “false positives” from examining numerous outcomes (7). To reduce the risk of false positives due to data mining, we examine outcome indices that were pre-specified for the interim (5-year) MTO follow-up (7).

We also examine a self-reported measure of comprehensive subjective well-being (SWB)—the first time the effect of neighborhoods on SWB has been assessed in an experimental analysis. Our primary measure of SWB is based on responses to the following question from the General Social Survey (GSS) that we included on our long-term follow-up survey of MTO adults: “Taken all together, how would you say things are these days – would you say that you are very happy, pretty happy, or not too happy?” (17). This type of happiness question yields results similar to those from questions about general life satisfaction; both provide global retrospective assessments of how people think their lives are going and are increasingly used to assess public policy impacts (18). We use the same 3-point response scale as the GSS to benchmark MTO against national samples; tradeoffs with this scaling are discussed in the supplementary materials. Another reason we focus on adults is because more is known about measuring SWB of adults than youth (19). SWB was not included in the interim MTO survey but was added to the long-term survey to be one of the key summary measures of the net impacts on families from moving to a less-distressed neighborhood. MTO controls are slightly happier than adults in national surveys with similar socio-demographic characteristics (table S2).

Methods

We begin by presenting intention-to-treat (ITT) estimates that capture the effect of being offered the chance to use an MTO voucher to move into a different neighborhood. These estimates are calculated as the difference in average outcomes for families assigned to treatment versus those assigned to the control condition. ITT estimation assumes that randomization was carried out correctly, that there is no selective attrition in measuring outcomes across groups, and that MTO’s effect on a given family is independent of the treatment status of other families.

We can also use the MTO experimental data to estimate the relationship between outcomes and some specific neighborhood attributes W as in equation (1). Ordinary least squares estimation of (1) may yield biased estimates because of possible correlation of W with unmeasured individual characteristics (ε) that influence both neighborhood selection and outcomes, Y. We instead use two-stage least squares to generate instrumental variables (IV) estimates, where in the first stage equation we use interactions of MTO random assignment and indicators for which MTO site families live in at baseline as instrumental variables to generate predicted values of W that are then substituted for the actual value in the second stage eq. (1) (7). The equation also includes a set of baseline characteristics, Xincluding indicators for MTO demonstration site and numerous participant socio-demographic characteristics, to improve the precision of our estimates.

Y=π0+Wπ1+Xπ2+ε (1)

IV estimation of equation (1) essentially fits a “dose-response” model, and asks whether those treatment groups and sites that experience relatively larger gains in specific elements of W as a result of treatment assignment also experience relatively larger gains in the outcome of interest. This estimation approach assumes this is the only reason why the effect of treatment assignment on outcomes varies across randomized groups and demonstration sites. It also assumes the only pathway through which the instruments affect the outcomes of interest is by affecting the neighborhood measures included in equation (1). Given the large number of neighborhood attributes affected by MTO moves, this approach cannot isolate the effect of a specific attribute. We instead view any single variable used in W to be a summary measure of neighborhood environment (for example, tract poverty captures the effects of moving to an area with a lower poverty rate and other aspects of neighborhood economic disadvantage that co-vary with tract poverty).

In a model that relates Y to a single neighborhood measure W with the only covariates (X) being the indicators for the MTO cities, the IV estimation of equation (1) is equivalent to fitting a regression line through the 15 data points that correspond to the average values of Y and W for each of the three randomized MTO groups in the five demonstration sites relative to the site overall mean. Below we present several visual instrumental variables graphs that show the data and logic behind our IV estimates.

Results

Table 2 shows that MTO does indeed generate sizable and sustained differences in average neighborhood conditions of the individuals across randomly-assigned groups, despite the fact that only around half the adults assigned to treatment used a MTO voucher to relocate. One year after random assignment the average control group family is living in a census tract with a poverty rate of 50%, compared to 34% for the average family assigned to treatment (standard error of the difference ±0.7%). This difference in tract poverty across randomized groups narrows over time, mostly because tract poverty rates decline for controls over time. This decline is driven by control families increasingly moving into lower-poverty neighborhoods on their own, as opposed to their baseline neighborhoods gentrifying around them. Averaged over the entire study period, assignment to treatment reduces average tract poverty rates by 8.2 percentage points (standard error ±0.5%), or about one-fifth of the control group average of 40%. This is equal to about two-thirds of a standard deviation reduction in tract poverty in the national tract-poverty distribution.

Table 2.

MTO effects on post-randomization housing and neighborhood conditions of adult participants interviewed in long-term survey. Table shows average outcomes for control group adults and intention-to-treat (ITT) contrast of outcomes for adults assigned to treatment (pooling the low-poverty and traditional voucher groups) rather than control. Housing and neighborhood conditions measured from long-term survey data and Census tract-level data interpolated from the 1990 and 2000 decennial censuses and the 2005–09 American Community Survey. ITT calculated using OLS regression controlling for baseline covariates, using weights (see Table 1 note, and supplemental materials sections 1 and 5).

MTO treatment
(voucher) groups
vs. control
Control
mean
ITT SE N
Census tract characteristics
   Share poor at different points in time
     1 year post-random assignment 0.499 −0.160*** (0.007) 3224
     5 years post-random assignment 0.399 −0.089*** (0.007) 3208
     10–15 years post-random assignment (May 2008) 0.311 −0.034*** (0.007) 3206
Share poor for all addresses since random
assignment (duration-weighted)
   Share poor 0.396 −0.082*** (0.005) 3270
   Share poor, z-score using U.S. tract poverty
distribution
2.082 −0.666*** (0.041) 3270
   Share poor, z-score using MTO control group tract
poverty distribution
0.000 −0.653*** (0.040) 3270
Duration-weighted poverty rate is
   Less than 20% 0.054 0.196*** (0.013) 3270
   Less than 30% 0.242 0.237*** (0.018) 3270
   Less than 40% 0.512 0.206*** (0.018) 3270
Share minority
   10–15 years post-random assignment (May 2008) 0.844 −0.024** (0.009) 3206
   All addresses since random assignment (duration-
weighted)
0.880 −0.046*** (0.006) 3270
Residential mobility
     Number of moves after random assignment 2.165 0.584*** (0.068) 3273
Self-reports on long-term (10–15 year) follow-up
surveys about neighborhood and housing conditions
   Feel unsafe during day 0.196 −0.039** (0.015) 3262
   Number of housing problems (0–7) 2.051 −0.380*** (0.076) 3267
   Likely or very likely to report kids spraying
graffiti (collective efficacy)
0.589 0.064*** (0.020) 3255
   One or more friends with college degree 0.532 0.049** (0.020) 3203
***

= P<.01,

**

= P<.05,

*

= P<.10 on two-tailed t-test.

Table 2 also shows that MTO had more modest effects on neighborhood racial composition. Assignment to treatment reduces the average neighborhood minority share experienced by participants over the study period by 4.6 percentage points (standard error ±0.6%), a small share of the control group’s average of 88 percent, although there are larger treatment-control differences in this variable in some sites than others (the source of variation we use for our instrumental variables estimates; see supplementary materials section 3.3). Table 2 further indicates MTO generated sustained effects on neighborhood safety and other neighborhood social processes such as collective efficacy that are thought to be important in changing behavior (3, 5).

Because moving itself is part of the MTO treatment, which could have independent effects on people’s life outcomes, it is important to keep in mind that the control group averaged 2.165 moves over the study period (table 2). Treatment assignment increases the number of moves over 10–15 years by .584 (standard error ±.068).

Figure 1 shows that the opportunity to move through MTO had mixed (null to positive) long-term effects on objective measures of well-being of the type that have been the traditional focus of the neighborhood effects literature. ITT effects are not statistically significant on economic outcomes for adults in MTO households 10–15 years after random assignment. Effects on a broad index of physical health measures are in the direction of better health (ITT effect of +0.063 standard deviations, standard error ±.039) but are not quite statistically significant (P=.107; unless otherwise noted, all remaining statistical results come from t-tests). Effects on mental health are marginally significant (P=.084) in the direction of better health (ITT effect of +.070 standard deviations, standard error ±.041). The final bar of Figure 1 shows, though, that ITT effects are more strongly beneficial for SWB, with the offer to move to a less-disadvantaged area increasing SWB by +.098 standard deviations (standard error ±.039, P=.013).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Impact on each outcome of assignment to the MTO treatment (voucher) groups for adults interviewed in long-term survey. The squares represent the intent-to-treat (ITT) estimate for the effect of being assigned to MTO treatment (pooling low-poverty and traditional voucher groups) rather than control, for the outcomes listed on the x-axis: economic self-sufficiency, physical health, mental health, and subjective well-being (see Table 2 note, and supplemental materials sections 1, 4, and 5). The box whiskers represent the 95th percent confidence interval around the estimates.

Figure 2 shows the basic intuition behind our instrumental variables estimates, which try to distinguish between the effects on SWB of neighborhood economic disadvantage (as represented by tract poverty rate) versus racial segregation (as measured by tract share minority). The x-axis of Fig. 2A represents the average tract poverty rate MTO adults experience over the study period, while the y-axis represents SWB, both in standardized (Z-score) form. The data points are the average tract poverty and SWB for adults broken out by MTO randomized group and demonstration site. The slope of this line is essentially our IV estimate of the relationship between SWB and tract poverty. A 1 standard deviation decrease in tract poverty (a 13 percentage point change) is associated with increased SWB equal to .141 standard deviations (standard error ±.054, P=.0009; table S5).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Instrumental variable estimation of the relationship between subjective well-being (SWB) and average (duration-weighted) tract poverty rate (panel A), tract share minority (panel B), tract poverty controlling for minority share (panel C), and tract minority share controlling for tract poverty (panel D). The y-axis is a 3-point happiness scale (1=not too happy, 2=pretty happy, 3=very happy) expressed in standard deviation units relative to the control group. Share poor is the fraction of census tract residents living below the poverty threshold. Share minority is the fraction of census tract residents who are members of racial or ethnic minority groups. Tract shares are linearly interpolated from the 1990 and 2000 decennial census and 2005–09 American Community Survey and are weighted by the time respondents lived at each of their addresses from random assignment through May 2008. Share poor and minority are z-scores, standardized by the control group mean and standard deviation. The points represent the site (Bal = Baltimore, Bos = Boston, Chi = Chicago, LA = Los Angeles, NY = New York City) and treatment group (LPV = low-poverty voucher, TRV = traditional voucher, C = control group). The slope of the line is equivalent to a 2SLS estimate of the relationship between subjective well-being and the mediator shown in each panel, using interactions of indicators for MTO treatment group assignment and demonstration site as instruments for the mediator (controlling for site indicator main effects).

The remaining panels of Figure 2 suggest that poverty concentration is more important than racial segregation in affecting the SWB of MTO adults. SWB does not have a statistically significant relationship with the minority composition of the tracts in which MTO families reside (P=.478), as illustrated by the relatively flat line in Fig. 2B. The size of the increase in SWB from a 1 standard deviation reduction in tract poverty nearly doubles once we control for tract minority share in the same model (from .141 to .261 standard deviations, standard error ±.093, P=.005; table S9), as seen by comparing Figs. 2A and 2C. In contrast, holding neighborhood poverty constant, a 1 standard deviation decrease in neighborhood minority share makes MTO adults if anything worse off -.279 standard deviations, standard error ±.169, P=.098), shown by the positive slope in Fig. 2D. The conclusion that a decline in neighborhood economic disadvantage has a more beneficial result for SWB than does a comparably-sized decline in neighborhood minority composition comes from the fact that we can reject the null hypothesis that the slopes illustrated by Figs. 2C and 2D are equal (P=.030; table S9).

Results are qualitatively similar if we estimate models that assume that outcomes are only affected by current neighborhood conditions, measured at the start of the survey period, May 2008 (tables S6, S10, Figs. S4-S7).

Discussion

To what extent does moving to a less distressed neighborhood environment affect people’s well-being? In this paper we present results from a large-scale randomized social experiment (MTO) designed to address this question that has been of long-standing concern to the social and medical sciences and to policymakers. Random assignment in MTO overcomes concerns with selection bias by generating differences in the average neighborhood conditions experienced by otherwise comparable groups of people. MTO is unique in terms of the long duration of the follow-up data collection that has been carried out with participants spanning 10–15 years after randomization.

MTO has strong internal validity, but the MTO findings may not generalize to all U.S. families. Although the MTO sample is comparable to other urban minority samples in high-poverty urban areas that have been studied in this literature (20, 21), the sorts of families living in such extreme-poverty areas are very disadvantaged relative to other American adults. MTO was carried out during a time when concentrated poverty and crime rates were declining, and HUD’s HOPE VI program was demolishing many public housing projects across the country. MTO’s impacts also do not necessarily identify the effects of larger-scale mobility programs (22).

Keeping these caveats in mind, we find that over the long term (10–15 years) the chance to move to less distressed neighborhoods in MTO has no detectable long-term effects on adult economic self-sufficiency. In a previous paper we showed that MTO had important long-term effects on two particularly important physical health measures that predict long-term disease risk; namely extreme obesity and diabetes (15). We report here that MTO’s impact on a broader index of physical health was in the same direction (towards improved health), but was not quite statistically significant, while we find a marginally significant beneficial impact of moving to a less distressed neighborhood on a broad index of mental health.

This mixed pattern of MTO impacts for traditional, objective measures of well-being echo what was found in the interim (5-year) follow up of MTO families (7, 23). These mixed results have been disappointing to many observers, in part because the Congressional legislation authorizing the MTO demonstration explicitly mentioned the goal of improving some outcomes that were unaffected (such as adult earnings). Similar mixed findings are apparent in recent quasi-experimental studies of other housing mobility programs (2426). These mixed results have led influential observers like Yale Law School professor Robert Ellickson, who is generally sympathetic to the value of housing vouchers over project-based housing programs, to argue that (27, p. 439) “recently published studies have begun to destabilize the former consensus that a poor adult or child is significantly disadvantaged by residing among other poor people … the case for dismantling an entire poor neighborhood … is hardly so plain.”

Yet the results reported here might lead to quite a different conclusion in that we find sizable positive effects of moving from a more-distressed to a less-distressed neighborhood on subjective well-being, a measure that represents a comprehensive assessment by the participants themselves of the extent to which their lives have been affected. Our results suggest that living in distressed neighborhoods has more important adverse impacts, and escaping from such neighborhoods more important positive effects, on the well-being of low-income adults than was revealed by previous experimental and quasi-experimental studies of neighborhood effects that focused on traditional measures of socioeconomic and health outcomes. Whether or not the MTO vouchers imposed additional locational constraints on families does not appear to matter much for the positive effects of such moves on well-being (table S4).

Although “happiness” has no natural metric, one can still interpret the magnitude of our results by noting that a 1 standard deviation reduction in neighborhood poverty (about 13 percentage points) is associated with an increase in SWB that is about two-thirds of the gap in SWB between U.S. blacks and whites [which is around one-quarter of a standard deviation in favor of whites (28)], and about equal to the remaining gap in SWB between families with annual incomes that differ by $13,000 after conditioning on a standard set of control variables that differ by income and affect happiness (supplementary materials sections 3.3). This is a large amount, equal to about two-thirds of the average income of MTO control group families in our long-term survey ($20,000).

Subject self-reports of SWB have the potential to provide an informative summary measure of the overall impact of neighborhood conditions on people’s lives. Although SWB measures are being used with increased frequency in the social sciences and policy analysis, SWB has not been the focus of much previous “neighborhood effects” research. The proper interpretation of self-reports about SWB remains the topic of some debate. Previous studies show different measures of self-reported SWB to be correlated in expected ways with objective indicators of well-being such as life events, biological indicators (e.g., smiling frequency; brain activity), and reports from significant others about the person’s happiness at both the individual and group levels (29, 30; supplemental materials section 2.3. We also corroborate our findings for SWB by examining the effects of MTO moves on related measures of psychological distress (table S4).

As noted in the introduction, it is also important for both science and policy to understand why changes in neighborhood environments affect the well-being of low-income adults. Isolating mechanisms with the MTO data is challenging and our statistical power to do so is somewhat limited. We focus on distinguishing the effects of residential income segregation versus racial segregation because this is a key scientific question, because different policies may be required to address segregation by income versus race, and because racial segregation has declined the last 40 years while income segregation has substantially increased.

Our results suggest that changes in neighborhood poverty are more important than racial segregation in affecting the SWB of low-income adults in MTO. (We interpret neighborhood poverty as a marker for a collection of correlated neighborhood characteristics across the neighborhoods in which the MTO families reside.) The supplementary materials show the same qualitative pattern holds for adult physical and mental health outcomes as well.

The rise in U.S. residential income segregation since 1970 raises the possibility that the problem of harmful neighborhood effects on people’s well-being may be getting worse rather than better over time. Increased poverty concentration in America does not seem to be due simply to increases in overall income inequality (31). The average tract poverty rate for families in the bottom quintile of the U.S. income distribution increased over the past 40 years by about 2.4 percentage points (from 17.6% to 20.0%). If the results from our MTO sample generalize to other very low-income families, the increase in poverty concentration over the past 40 years reduced the well-being of the bottom quintile of the income distribution by an amount that may be equivalent to a decline in annual household income of about $1,400 (about 8%). If our estimates are correct, the $1,400 dollar-equivalent for the decline in well-being for families in the bottom quintile caused by increased poverty concentration from 1970–2007 is about equal in size to the total gain in real annual family income of $1,300 that the bottom quintile has experienced over roughly the past 40 years from $15,336 in 1969 to $16,622 in 2007 ((32), converted to 2009 dollars; see supplementary materials section 3.3).

Our findings are also germane to debates about the proper objectives for public policy. For example, one recent review of U.S. anti-poverty programs notes that their effectiveness depends “at least in part, on whether the programs do, in fact, reduce poverty” (33 p. 12. By that standard, MTO-type policy efforts to improve the neighborhood conditions of poor families would not be part of an effective anti-poverty strategy, as the program failed to produce detectable impacts on family income (7, 23). But if the goal is the broader one of improving the well-being of poor families, then policies that seek to ameliorate the adverse effects of dangerous, distressed neighborhoods on poor families are worthy of careful consideration.

Supplementary Material

01

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Support for this research was provided by a contract from the U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development (HUD; C-CHI-00808) and grants from National Science Foundation (SES- 0527615), the National Institute for Child Health and Human Development (R01-HD040404, R01-HD040444), the Centers for Disease Control (R49-CE000906), the National Institute of Mental Health (R01-MH077026), the National Institute for Aging (P20-AG012180, R56- AG031259, and P01-AG005842-22S1), the National Opinion Research Center’s Population Research Center (through R24-HD051152-04 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development), University of Chicago’s Center for Health Administration Studies, the U.S. Department of Education/Institute of Education Sciences (R305U070006), the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation, the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation, the Russell Sage Foundation, the Smith Richardson Foundation, the Spencer Foundation, and the Annie E. Casey Foundation. This paper was also supported by a visiting scholar award from the Russell Sage Foundation to Jens Ludwig and a Robert Wood Johnson Investigator Award in Health Policy Research to Ludwig and Greg Duncan. Outstanding assistance with the data preparation and analysis was provided by Joe Amick, Ryan Gillette, Ijun Lai, Jordan Marvakov, Nicholas Potter, Matt Sciandra, Fanghua Yang, Sabrina Yusuf, and Michael Zabek. The survey data collection effort was led by Nancy Gebler of the University of Michigan’s Survey Research Center under subcontract to our research team. Helpful comments were provided by Ingrid Gould Ellen, Alan Krueger, Devah Pager, Genevieve Pham-Kanter, Todd Richardson, Mark Shroder, Richard Suzman, Justin Wolfers and seminar participants at Princeton University and the Russell Sage Foundation. MTO data was provided by HUD. The data used in this paper will be made available through the Interuniversity Consortium on Political and Social Research (ICPSR) at the University of Michigan and the National Bureau of Economic Research. Any errors and all opinions are our own. The views expressed in this work are those of the authors and should not be interpreted as those of the Congressional Budget Office or HUD.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Kneebone E, Nadeau C, Berube A. The Re-Emergence of Concentrated Poverty: Metropolitan Trends in the 2000s. 2011 available at http://www.brookings.edu/~/media/Files/rc/papers/2011/1103_poverty_kneebone_nadeau_berube/1103_poverty_kneebone_nadeau_berube.pdf).
  • 2.U.S. Census Bureau. [accessed July 12 2012];Poverty Thresholds for 2011 by Size of Family and Number of Related Children Under 18 Years. 2012 http://www.census.gov/hhes/www/poverty/data/threshld/thresh11.xls.
  • 3.Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW, Earls F. Neighborhoods and violent crime: a multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science. 1997;277:918–924. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5328.918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Macintyre SA, Ellaway A. Neighborhoods and Health: An Overview. In: Kawachi I, Berkman LF, editors. Neighborhoods and Health. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003. pp. 20–42. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Sampson RJ, Morenoff JD, Gannon-Rowley T. Assessing “Neighborhood Effects”: Social Processes and New Directions in Research. Annual Review of Sociology. 2002;28:443–478. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Kawachi I, Berkman LF, editors. Neighborhoods and Health. Oxford University Press; New York: 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kling JR, Liebman JB, Katz LF. Experimental Analysis of Neighborhood Effects. Econometrica. 2007;75:83–119. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Myrdal G. An American Dilemma: The Negro Problem and Modern Democracy. Volume 1. Harper Brothers; New York: 1944. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.U.S. National Advisory Commission on Civil Disorders. The Kerner Report. 1968 http://www.eisenhowerfoundation.org/docs/kerner.pdf).
  • 10.Massey DS, Denton N. American Apartheid: Segregation and the Making of the Underclass. Harvard University Press; Cambridge, MA: 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Wilson WJ. The Truly Disadvantaged: The Inner City, the Underclass, and Public Policy. Chicago: University Press; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Glaeser EL, Vigdor JL. The End of the Segregated Century: Racial Separation in America’s Neighborhoods, 1980–2010 (Manhattan Institute Civic Report No. 66, New York. 2012 http://www.manhattan-institute.org/pdf/cr_66.pdf).
  • 13.Watson T. Inequality and the Measurement of Residential Segregation By Income in American Neighborhoods. Review of Income and Wealth. 2009;55:820–844. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Reardon SF, Bischoff K. More Unequal and More Separate: Growth in the Residential Segregation of Families by Income, 1970–2009 (Report prepared for the Russell Sage Foundation US2010 project, New York, 2011. http://www.s4.brown.edu/us2010/Data/Report/report111111.pdf) [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ludwig J, et al. Neighborhoods, obesity, and diabetes-a randomized social experiment. The New England Journal of Medicine. 2011;365:1509–1519. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa1103216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Sampson RJ. Great American City: Chicago and the Enduring Neighborhood Effect. University of Chicago Press; Chicago: 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Smith TW, Marsden P, Hout M, Kim J. General social surveys, 1972–2010 [machine-readable data file] Sponsored by National Science Foundation. NORC ed. Chicago: National Opinion Research Center [producer] Storrs, CT: The Roper Center for Public Opinion Research, University of Connecticut; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Dolan P, Layard R, Metcalfe R. Measuring Subjective wellbeing for public policy: Recommendations on measures (Special Paper No. 23) 2011 available at http://cep.lse.ac.uk/pubs/download/special/cepsp23.pdf.
  • 19.Huebner ES. Research on assessment of life satisfaction of children and adolescents. Social Indicators Research. 2004;66:3–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Sampson RJ, Sharkey P, Raudenbush SW. Durable effects of concentrated disadvantage on verbal ability among African-American children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2008;105:845–852. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0710189104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ludwig J. Working Paper, Harris School of Public Policy Studies, University of Chicago. 2012. The Long-Term Results From the Moving to Opportunity Residential Mobility Demonstration. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Sampson RJ. Moving to Inequality: Neighborhood Effects and Experiments Meet Social Structure. American Journal of Sociology. 2008;114:189–231. doi: 10.1086/589843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Sanbonmatsu L, et al. Moving to Opportunity for Fair Housing Demonstration Program: Final Impacts Evaluation. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development, Office of Policy Development and Research; 2011. http://www.huduser.org/publications/pdf/MTOFHD_fullreport_v2.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Jacob BA. Public housing, housing vouchers and student achievement: Evidence from public housing demolitions in Chicago. American Economic Review. 2004;94:233–258. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Oreopoulos P. The Long-Run Consequences of Living in a Poor Neighborhood. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2003;118:1533–1575. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.DeLuca S, Duncan GJ, Keels M, Mendenhall R. Gautreaux mothers and their children: an update. Housing Policy Debate. 2010;20:7–25. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ellickson R. The Mediocrity of Government Subsidies to Mixed-Income Housing Projects. In: Ingram G, Hong Y-H, editors. Property Rights and Land Policies. Cambridge, MA: Lincoln Institute of Land Policy; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Stevenson B, Wolfers J. Subjective and objective indicators of racial progress. Working Paper. University of Pennsylvania Wharton School; 2010. available at http://bpp.wharton.upenn.edu/betseys/papers/Happiness_Race.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kahneman D, Krueger AB. Developments in the Measurement of Subjective Well-Being. Journal of Economic Perspectives. 2006;20:3–24. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Oswald AJ, Wu S. Objective confirmation of subjective measures of human well-being: evidence from the U.S.A. Science. 2010;327:576–579. doi: 10.1126/science.1180606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Reardon SF, Bischoff K. Income inequality and income segregation. American Journal of Sociology. 2011;116:1092–1153. doi: 10.1086/657114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.U.S. Census Bureau. [accessed May 03 2012];Table F-3. Mean Income Received by Each Fifth and Top 5 Percent of Families, All Races: 1966 to 2010. 2010 http://www.census.gov/hhes/www/income/data/historical/families/2010/F03AR_2010.xls.
  • 33.Ben-Shalom Y, Moffitt RA, Scholz JK. Working Paper 17042. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research; 2011. An Assessment of the Effectiveness of Anti-Poverty Programs in the United States. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Goering J, et al. Moving to Opportunity for fair housing demonstration program: Current status and initial findings. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Goering J, Feins JD, Richardson T. In: What have we learned about housing mobility and poverty deconcentration? In Choosing a better life? Evaluating the Moving to Opportunity social experiment. Goering J, Feins J, editors. The Urban Institute Press; 2003. pp. 3–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.U.S. Census Bureau, Geography Division. [accessed December 1, 2011];Census Tracts and Block Numbering Areas. 2000 http://www.census.gov/geo/www/cen_tract.html.
  • 37.Orr L, et al. Moving to Opportunity for Fair Housing Demonstration Program: Interim Impacts Evaluation. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development, Office of Policy Development and Research; 2003. http://www.huduser.org/Publications/pdf/MTOFullReport.pdf) [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Fortson JG, Sanbonmatsu L. Child Health and Neighborhood Conditions: Results from a Randomized Housing Voucher Experiment. Journal of Human Resources. 2010;45:840–864. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Kling JR, Ludwig J, Katz LF. Neighborhood Effects on Crime for Female and Male Youth: Evidence from a Randomized Housing Voucher Experiment. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2005;120:87–130. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Sanbonmatsu L, Kling JR, Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J. Neighborhoods and Academic Achievement: Results from the Moving to Opportunity Experiment. Journal of Human Resources. 2006;41:649–691. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Groves RM, Fowler FJ, Couper MP, Lepkowski JM, Singer E. Survey Methodology. ed. 7. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.The American Association of Public Opinion Research. Standard Definitions: Final Dispositions of Case Codes and Outcome Rates for Surveys. 7th Edition 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Idler EL, Kasl SV. Self-ratings of health: do they also predict change in functional ability? The journals of gerontology Series B Psychological sciences and social sciences. 1995;50:S344–S353. doi: 10.1093/geronb/50b.6.s344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Idler EL, Benyamini Y. Self-rated health and mortality: a review of twenty-seven community studies. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1997;38:21–37. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Delpierre C, Lauwers-Cances V, Datta GD, Lang T, Berkman L. Using self-rated health for analysing social inequalities in health: a risk for underestimating the gap between socioeconomic groups? Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health. 2009;63:426–432. doi: 10.1136/jech.2008.080085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Dowd JB, Zajacova A. Does self-rated health mean the same thing across socioeconomic groups? Evidence from biomarker data. Annals of Epidemiology. 2010;20:743–749. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2010.06.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Wiener JM, Hanley RJ, Clark R, Van Nostrand JF. Measuring the activities of daily living: comparisons across national surveys. Journal of Gerontology. 1990;45:S229–S237. doi: 10.1093/geronj/45.6.s229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Institute for Social Research, Health and retirement study: Physical measures and biomarkers. 2008 http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/modules/meta/2008/core/qnaire/online/2008PhysicalMeasuresBiomarkers.pdf.
  • 49.Chobanian AV, et al. Seventh report of the Joint National Committee on Prevention, Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Pressure. Hypertension. 2003;42:1206–1252. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000107251.49515.c2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Furukawa TA, Kessler RC, Slade T, Andrews G. The performance of the K6 and K10 screening scales for psychological distress in the Australian National Survey of Mental Health and Well-Being. Psychological Medicine. 2003;33:357–362. doi: 10.1017/s0033291702006700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Kessler RC, et al. Screening for serious mental illness in the general population. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:184–189. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.2.184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Kessler RC, Ustün TB. The World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative Version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 2004;13:93–121. doi: 10.1002/mpr.168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Kessler RC, Merikangas KR. The National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R): background and aims. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 2004;13:60–68. doi: 10.1002/mpr.166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Alwin DF. Information transmission in the survey interview: Number of categories and the reliability of attitude measurement. Sociological Methods. 1992;22:83–118. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Andrews FM, Withey SB. Social indicators of well-being: Americans’ perceptions of life quality. New York: Plenum; 1976. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Jacoby J, Matell MS. Three-point Likert scales are good enough. Journal of Marketing Research. 1971;8:495–500. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Lehmann DR, James H. Are three-point scales always good enough? Journal of Marketing Research. 1972;9:444–446. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Ferrer-i-Carbonell A, Frijters P. How Important is Methodology for the estimates of the determinants of Happiness? The Economic Journal. 2004;114:641–659. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Krueger AB, Schkade DA. The Reliability of Subjective Well-Being Measures. Journal of Public Economics. 2008;92:1833–1845. doi: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2007.12.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Lucas RE, Diener E, Suh EM. Discriminant validity of well-being measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996;71:616–628. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.71.3.616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Sandvik E, Diener E, Seidlitz L. Subjective Well-Being: The Convergence and Stability of Self-Report and Non-Self-Report Measures. Journal of Personality. 1993;61:317–342. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Diener E, Suh EM. Kahneman D, Diener E, Schwartz N, editors. Well-Being: the Foundations of Hedonic Psychology. 1999:434–450. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Diener E, Suh EM, Lucas RE, Smith HL. Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin. 1999;125:276–302. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Frey BS, Stutzer A. Happiness and economics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Blanchflower DG, Oswald AJ. Well-being over time in Britain and the USA. Journal of Public Economics. 2002;88:1359–1386. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Layard R. Happiness: Lessons from a New Science. London: Penguin; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Steptoe A, Wardle J, Marmot M. Positive affect and health-related neuroendocrine, cardiovascular, and inflammatory processes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2005;102:6508–6512. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0409174102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Di Tella R, MacCulloch R. Some Uses of Happiness Data in Economics. Journal of Economic Perspectives. 2006;20:25–46. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Stevenson B, Wolfers J. Happiness Inequality in the United States. The Journal of Legal Studies. 2008;37:S33–S79. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Stevenson B, Wolfers J. In: Brookings Papers on Economic Activity, Spring 2008. Elmendorf DW, Gregory N, Mankiw, Summers LH, editors. vol 39. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press; 2008. pp. 1–102. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Urry HL, et al. Making a life worth living: neural correlates of well-being. Psychological Science. 2004;15:367–372. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00686.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Diener E, Lucas RE. In: Well-being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology. Kahneman D, Diener E, Schwarz N, editors. vol. 37. Guilford Press; 1999. pp. 213–229. [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Van Praag BMS. Ordinal and cardinal utility: An integration of the two dimensions of the welfare concept. Journal of Econometrics. 1991;50:69–89. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Angrist JD. Estimation of Limited Dependent Variable Models With Dummy Endogenous Regressors. Journal of Business and Economic Statistics. 2001;19:2–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Rubin DB. Comment on “Randomization Analysis of Experimental Data: The Fisher Randomization Test,” by D. Basu. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1980;75:591–593. [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Sobel ME. What Do Randomized Studies of Housing Mobility Demonstrate? Causal Inference in the Face of Interference. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 2006 Dec;101 [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Ludwig J, et al. What Can We Learn about Neighborhood Effects from the Moving to Opportunity Experiment? American Journal of Sociology. 2008;114:144–188. [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Bloom HS. Accounting for No-Shows in Experimental Evaluation Designs. Evaluation Review. 1984;8:225–246. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Angrist JD, Imbens GW, Rubin DB. Identification of Causal Effects Using Instrumental Variables. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1996;91:444–455. [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Angrist JD, Pischke J-S. Mostly Harmless Econometrics: An Empiricist’s Companion. Princeton University Press; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Ludwig J, Kling JR. Is Crime Contagious? The Journal of Law and Economics. 2007;50:491–518. [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Rivers D, Vuong Q. Limited information estimators and exogeneity tests for simultaneous probit models. Journal of Econometrics. 1988;39:347–366. [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Murray MP. Avoiding Invalid Instruments and Coping with Weak Instruments. Journal of Economic Perspectives. 2006;20:111–132. [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Fuller WA. Some properties of a modification of the limited information estimator. Econometrica. 1977;45:939–953. [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Stock J, Yogo M. In: Identification and Inference for Econometric Models: Essays in Honor of Thomas J. Rothenberg. Andrews DWK, Stock JH, editors. Cambridge University Press; 2005. pp. 80–108. [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Hahn J, Hausman JA, Kuersteiner G. Estimation with Weak Instruments: Accuracy of Higher Order Bias and MSE Approximations. Econometrics Journal. 2004;7:272–306. [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Kennedy SD, Leger ML. Final Comprehensive Report of the Freestanding Housing Voucher Demonstration. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development, Office of Policy Development and Research; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Rubinowitz LS, Rosenbaum JE. Crossing the class and color lines: From public housing to white suburbia. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Feins JD, Shroder M. Moving to opportunity: The demonstration’s design and its effects on mobility. Urban Studies. 2005;42:1275–1299. [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Shroder M. Locational constraint, housing counseling, and successful lease-up in a randomized housing voucher experiment. Journal of Urban Economics. 2002;51:315–338. [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Staiger D, Stock J. Instrumental variables regression with weak instruments. Econometrica. 1997;65:557–586. [Google Scholar]
  • 92.U.S. Census Bureau. [accessed May 04 2012];1970 Census Data. Prepared by Social Explorer. http://www.socialexplorer.com.ezpprod1.hul.harvard.edu/pub/reportdata/htmlresults.aspx?ReportId=R10244914.
  • 93.U. S. Census Bureau. [accessed May 04 2012];American Community Survey 2005–2009. Prepared by Social Explorer. http://www.socialexplorer.com.ezpprod1.hul.harvard.edu/pub/reportdata/htmlresults.aspx?ReportId=R10244926.
  • 94.U.S. Census Bureau. [accessed May 03 2012];Table F-1. Income Limits for Each Fifth and Top 5 Percent of Families (All Races): 1947 to 2010. 2010 http://www.census.gov/hhes/www/income/data/historical/families/2010/F01AR_2010.xls.
  • 95.Jacob BA, Ludwig J, Miller DL. The effects of housing and neighborhood conditions on child mortality. Working Paper 17369. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research; 2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

01

RESOURCES