Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2012 Dec;14(6):713–725. doi: 10.1007/s11920-012-0317-4

Interventions Addressing Social Impairment in Autism

Connie Kasari 1,, Stephanie Patterson 1
PMCID: PMC3492515  NIHMSID: NIHMS412039  PMID: 23055002

Abstract

Children with an autism spectrum disorder have significant impairment in social skills. This area of development has also been the focus of many intervention studies. In this article we review intervention studies published over the past two years. Three topical areas were addressed in current interventions: social skills knowledge, peer relationships, and joint attention/joint engagement. Younger children most often received interventions on joint attention/joint engagement and older, higher functioning children received interventions on social knowledge and peer relationship development. Both single subject research designs and group designs were reviewed. One advancement was that more randomized controlled trials were reported, as well as effectiveness trials in the community. Study quality was also rated. More group than single subject designs were rated as adequate or strong in quality. Overall, moderate to large effects were found for interventions targeting joint attention/joint engagement and peer relationships with mixed effects on interventions targeting social skills knowledge. Future studies should focus on isolating the active ingredients of interventions and include broader participant representation.

Keywords: Interventions, Social impairment, Autism, Autism spectrum disorder, Social skills, Randomized controlled trials, Single subject research designs, Joint attention, Joint engagement, Peer relationships, Social knowledge, Psychiatry

Introduction

Social impairment may be the most complex and impenetrable core challenge facing children with autism [1, 2]. While many behavioral and comprehensive interventions have shown promise in addressing a range of developmental difficulties of children with autism including cognitive ability and functional behavior, these interventions have had the least effect on improving social behavior. The National Institute of Mental Health [3] and Interagency Autism Coordinating Committee [4] have identified the development of interventions to address social impairment in individuals with autism as a high priority.

Intervening to improve social impairment in autism can be perplexing. While it is common to target social behaviors that are absent/limited (e.g., initiations) or social behaviors that occur so frequently they become inappropriate (e.g., excessive question asking), interventions may also be required to target the quality of social behaviors. That is, a child may initiate social interactions with others frequently, but the quality of the initiation is so poor that the initiation is ignored or avoided. Also puzzling is the pattern of strengths and weaknesses in social skills, prompting Frith and Happe [5] to describe this pattern as ‘fine cuts along a hidden seam’. Children can be quite good, for example, at requesting help via gestures, but quite poor at commenting or sharing interest using gesture. Both types of gestures develop at the same time in typically developing children, but sharing gestures require more consideration of others than mere requesting skills [6]. Thus, children with autism present with qualitatively complex strengths and weaknesses, requiring interventions that are targeted, individualized, and that include flexible targets that change over time.

Social Impairment in Autism: What is the Problem?

The difficulties common for young children with ASD center on two key problems: a) their ability to engage jointly with others (joint attention/joint engagement), and b) the amount and quality of their interactive skills to enter into or maintain interactions with peers. Young children with autism are identified by their apparent lack of awareness of others, evidenced by little coordination of attention between an object or event and another person (joint attention). For example, they may be so focused on playing with a toy that it is difficult for another person to join in and share the play (lacking joint engagement). When young children do engage with others it is often to request help obtaining a favorite item. In community contexts such as the classroom or playground, the child with autism may be unaware of other children, instead playing in the sand alone, or running across the park. If the child is interested in other children, s/he may not have the skill set to begin or maintain an interaction.

Older children with autism, particularly those who are high functioning and in school continue to have significant social challenges, including a limited number of social encounters with others. Challenges in developing better social relationships have been linked to problems in recognizing subtle social cues and emotional states in others, and an inability to take the other’s perspective, leading to egocentric responses in social situations. Children often report few friendships, and in adolescence endorse feelings of loneliness at school [7]. By their own account, developing friendships is often cited as the most important goal for children, adolescents and adults with ASD [8]. For the older child with ASD, major social difficulties center on a) skill development involving perception, knowledge and understanding of others, and b) development of peer relationships, including friendships.

Many interventions have been developed and tested over the past 20 years to address these impairments in younger and older children. Four reviews of these interventions published prior to 2010 [912] noted that most social skills interventions were tested in clinics with groups of children unfamiliar with each other. The social skills curricula in these programs had common elements (e.g., greetings, making eye-contact, initiating conversations) but there was not a uniformly accepted set of curricular skills, nor were there data suggesting that skills learned in these groups were transferred to natural contexts for children, such as school [1011]. A concern has been that the skills addressed in these programs may not be the actual skills needed in everyday interactions at school [9]. In other words, polite manners (e.g., shaking hands, making eye-contact) may be useful when introduced to an adult but may be less typical when trying to enter a game or conversation on the playground. According to one review, two specific social skills interventions have sufficient evidence to be considered evidence based practices, social skills groups and video modeling [12]. Based on the state of the science, Reichow and Volkmar [12] suggested that future research needs to include parent-mediated social skills interventions for adolescents and adults, interventions for more cognitively impaired individuals, and interventions involving siblings. In the current review of social skills studies between 2010 and 2012, we note that interventions involving parents and siblings and including participants who are more cognitively impaired were published suggesting that the field of social skills intervention is rapidly expanding.

Methods

Search Strategy

Eight electronic databases covering education, medicine and psychology were searched in July 2012. The search was restricted to materials published in peer -reviewed journals between January 2010 and July 2012. Keyword search terms spanned three areas including those related to autism (autis* or pervasive develop* or Asperger*), social skills (social skill* or social interaction* or joint attention or social communication or social behavior*) and intervention (intervention or social skills training or parent* or peer* or teacher or para*).

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

A set of inclusion and exclusion criteria was applied to the manuscripts obtained from the search. Included studies:

  1. Used a quantitative experimental design including group or single subject research designs (SSRDs). Studies using pre-experimental designs (e.g., one group pre/post, case studies) were excluded.

  2. Included participants of any age diagnosed with an autism spectrum disorder including autistic disorder/autism, pervasive developmental disorder or Asperger’s syndrome. Studies including a mix of participants with autism and participants with other diagnoses were excluded.

  3. Examined an intervention for which a social skill was the primary outcome.

  4. For studies using SSRD, graphical data for primary outcomes were presented such that Improvement Rate Difference (IRD) [13] could be calculated.

  5. Published in the English language.

  6. Published in a peer-reviewed journal between 2010 and 2012.

Rating Methodological Quality

The methodological quality of the studies was rated using a protocol developed by Reichow, Volkmar, and Cicchetti [14]. Separate protocols were used for studies using single subject research designs (SSRD) and those using group designs. The SSRD scale consists of six primary indicators, including participant, intervention and outcome description, baseline stability, visual analysis and experimental control. Primary indicators could receive a rating of ‘high’, ‘adequate’ or ‘unacceptable’. Six secondary indicators related to inter-observer agreement, interventionist fidelity, blinding of observers, generalization/maintenance and social validity were also rated as present or absent. Improved Rate Difference (IRD) [13] was calculated for studies using SSRDs to determine the size of the effect. IRD has a long history of use in the medical literature, where it is termed “risk difference” [13].

Group designs were rated using a similar protocol which included items specific to group trials with a total of six primary indicators including description of comparison conditions and statistical data analysis and a total of eight secondary indicators including randomization and reporting of effect size. Two independent raters assessed the studies. Twenty-five percent of the studies were double coded. Interclass correlations indicate high reliability (α=.84).

Results

The systematic search of the literature produced 2498 citations. Titles and abstracts of the articles were examined. After removal of irrelevant articles, reviews, duplicates and grey literature, a total of 34 articles were included in the review. Seventeen articles used SSRD [1518, 19•, 20, 21•, 22, 23•, 2431] and seventeen used group designs [32, 33•, 34, 35•, 36•, 37, 38, 39••, 40••, 41••, 42, 43, 44•, 45•, 4649], a shift from previous reviews in which there were few group designs.

Methodological Quality

Single subject research designs

Studies using SSRDs were primarily of ‘weak’ overall quality as assessed using the Reichow et al. [14] rating scale. Only three studies were rated ‘adequate’ [19•, 21•, 23•] while no studies were rated as ‘strong’. Scores for each study are listed in Table 1. This overall rating is derived from the combination of ‘high’, ‘acceptable’ and ‘weak’ ratings received across the six primary quality indicators. Studies rated as ‘weak’ overall either: 1) scored ‘high’ on fewer than four primary indicators, 2) obtained no secondary indicators or 3) scored ‘unacceptable’ one or more primary indicators. Studies most frequently received a rating of ‘weak’ by obtaining an “unacceptable” rating for one primary quality indicator often including participant description, visual analysis and experimental control indicators.

Table 1.

Participant information, outcomes and methodological quality

Study Participants Intervention Measures Outcomes (overall IRD or effect size) Primary (P)
and
secondary
(S) quality
indicators
& overall
rating
Single subject research designs
Banda & Hart
[15], 2010
(USA)
N=2; School age;
female
Peer Training Frequency Count Initiations to Peer (small)
Responses (small)
Sharing Behavior (small)
P- 4/6
S- 2/6
Weak
Banda et al.
[16], 2010
(USA)
N=2; School age;
male
Peer Training Frequency Count Initiations to Peer (large)
Responses to Peer (large)
P- 4/6
S- 2/6
Weak
Buggey [17],
2012
(USA)
N=3; Preschool;
male
Video Modeling Frequency Count Initiations in stage 1 (no effect)
Initiations in stage 2 (no effect)
P- 2/6
S- 2/6
Weak
Buggey et al.
[18], 2011
(USA)
N=4; Preschool;
male (2), female (2)
Video Modeling Frequency Count Initiations (moderate); Maintenance (small) P- 4/6
S- 3/6
Weak
Charlop et al.
[19•], 2010
(USA)
N=3, School age;
male
Video Modeling Percent of
Opportunities
Verbalization (large); Gen (large)
Intonation (large); Gen (large)
Gesture (large); Gen (large)
Facial Expression (moderate); Gen (large)
P- 4/6
S- 3/6
Adequate
Dykstra et al.
[20], 2012
(USA)
N=3; Preschool;
male (1), female (2)
Advancing Social
Communication
and Play (ASAP)
Intervals with
behavior present
Social interaction (SI), requests (BR) and joint attention (JA)
in group only instruction: group (small), individual
(moderate)
SI, BR & JA in full instruction: group (small), individual
(large)
Play in group only instruction: group (moderate), individual
(moderate)
Play in full instruction: group (small), individual (large)
P- 2/6
S- 1/6
Weak
Jull & Mirenda
[21•], 2011
(Canada)
N=4; Preschool (2);
male
Parent Facilitated
Play Dates
Intervals with
behavior present
Parents’ correct strategy use: large
Intervals with peer interaction: large
P- 5/6
S- 2/6
Adequate
L. Koegel et al.
[22], 2012
(USA)
N=3; Preschool;
male
Facilitated Social
Play with
Initiations Training
Intervals with
behavior present;
event recording of
initiations; ratings
of affect
Intervention without initiations:
Social behavior (large); Gen (small)
Initiations (small); Gen (no effect)
Affect (large); Gen (small)
Intervention with initiations:
Social behavior:(large); Gen (large)
Initiations (large); Gen (large)
Affect (large); Gen (large)
P- 1/6
S- 4/6
Weak
R. Koegel et
al. [23•], 2012
(USA)
N=3; School age;
male
Clubs Based on
Perseverative
Interests
Intervals with
behavior present;
Frequency
Engaged with peers: (large)
Initiations (large)
P- 4/6
S- 2/6
Adequate
Marzullo-
Kerth et al.
[24], 2011
(USA)
N=4; School age;
male
Video Modeling Intervals with
behavior present
Sharing during training (large); Maintenance (large)
Sharing generalization (large); Maintenance (large)
Nonsharing trials (no effect); Maintenance (no effect)
P-3/6
S-1/6
Weak
Parker &
Kamps [25],
2011
(USA)
N=2; School age;
male (1), female (1)
Social Scripts and
Task Analysis
Intervals with
behavior present;
Completed steps
Completed task analysis steps (large)
Peer directed verbalizations (moderate)
P- 1/6
S- 1/6
Weak
Reichow &
Sabornie [26],
2009
(USA)
N=1; School age;
male
Social Stories (SS)
and Cue Fading
(CF)
Frequency Verbal greeting with SS (large)
Verbal greeting with CF (large)
Verbal greeting to peer with SS (large)
Verbal greeting to peer with CF (large)
Effect CF over SS for all greetings (small)
P- 5/6
S- 1/6
Weak
Robinson [27],
2011
(USA)
N=4; Preschool (1),
School age (3); male
Modeling and
Video Feedback by
Paraprofessionals
Percentage correct,
Frequency, Likert
rating
Paraprofessional fidelity (large); Gen (large); Main (large)
Social communication (large); Gen (large); Main (large)
Affect (small); Gen (small); Main (small)
P- 2/6
S- 4/6
Weak
Schneider &
Goldstein [28],
2010
(USA)
N=3; School age;
male
Social Stories (SS,
n=4) and Visual
Schedules (VS,
n=1)
Percentage of
interval with
behavior present
On task behavior with SS (small)
On task behavior with VS (large)
On task VS over SS (moderate)
P- 2/6
S- 3/6
Weak
State & Kern
[29], 2012
(USA)
N=1; School age;
male
Video Feedback
and Self-
Monitoring
Percentage of
interval with
behavior present
Inappropriate noises-teacher (small); peers (moderate)
Inappropriate interactions-teacher (no effect); peers (no
effect)
Appropriate interactions-teacher (small); peers (no effect)
P- 3/6
S- 2/6
Weak
Tetreault &
Lerman [30],
2010
(USA)
N=3; Preschool (1),
School age (2); male
(2), female (1)
Video Modeling +
Edible Reinforcers
and Prompting
Frequency Correct exchanges (moderate)
Generalization of correct exchanges (small)
Correct eye contact (moderate)
Correct scripted responses (moderate)
P- 2/6
S- 1/6
Weak
Wichnick et al.
[31], 2010
(USA)
N=3; Preschool (1),
School age (2); male
(2), female (1)
Script Fading Percentage of
interval with
behavior present
Unscripted initiations (large)
Scripted initiations (large)
Generalized initiations (moderate)
P- 2/6
S- 1/6
Weak
Group design studies
Begeer et al.
[32], 2011
(Netherlands)
N=36; School age
male (33), female (3)
Theory of Mind
(ToM) Training-
Treatment vs.
Waitlist Delayed
Intervention
ToM Test; Index of
Empathy; CSBQ; LEAS-
C
Treatment group showed significantly greater increases
on ToM test than control (d=.75), primarily first order
ToM (d=1.00). No effect on second order ToM, emotion
recognition, imitation or self reported empathy. No
differential treatment effect on emotional awareness.
P- 3/6
S- 4/8
Weak
Castorina &
Negri [33•],
2011
(Australia)
N=21; School age,
male
Social Skills
Training- Sibling
vs. No Sibling vs.
Control Group
CASP; SSRS Children in program had significant higher CASP scores
at exit (ηp2= .59) and follow up (ηp2= .59). No
difference between groups in parent reported social
skills.
P- 4/6
S- 3/8
Adequate
Chu & Pan
[34], 2012
(Taiwan)
N=42 (21 ASD, 21
TD); School age;
ASD-male (20),
female (1), TD- male
(6), female (15)
Peer and Sibling
Assisted Swim
Program vs.
Swim
Control Group
HAAR Aquatic Skills
Checklist; Observations
of Social Interaction and
Participation
Increase in social interaction behavior as well as
instances of physical participation in the activity in
sibling and peer conditions than control. All children
improved their aquatic skills.
P- 2/6
S- 3/8
Weak
DeRosier et
al. [35•],
2011
(USA)
N=55; School age;
male (53), female (2)
SS.GRIN-HFA
Treatment versus
SS.GRIN control
Achieved Learning
(ALQ); SRS; Social Self
Efficacy; Social
Dissatisfaction
Statistically significant differential effects in parent
reported social skills (SRS- range in ES d=.67 to.94;
ALQ d=.75) and social self efficacy (d=.51) in favor of
the treatment group. No significant effect of treatment
on children’s reported social self efficacy or social
dissatisfaction.
P- 5/6
S- 4/8
Adequate
Frankel et al.
[36•], 2010
(USA)
N=68; School age;
male (58), female
(10)
Children’s
Friendship
Training (CFT)
vs. Delayed
Intervention
ASSQ; Loneliness Scale;
Piers-Harris Self Concept
Scale; QPQ; SSRS; PEI-
Teacher
Children in CFT reported significantly being less lonely
and more popular, results were not maintained at follow
up. Parents reported increased in number of play dates
and via the SSRS reported increased in self-control and
assertion as well as lower internalizing and externalizing
behavior scores, all maintained at follow up. No effects
were reported by teachers.
P- 4/6
S- 3/8
Adequate
Gantman et
al. [37], 2012 (USA)
N=17; Adolescents,
male (12), female (5)
UCLA Peers vs.
Delayed
Treatment
SRS; SSRS; SELSA;
Empathy Quotient; SSI;
Quality of Socialization;
Test of Young Adult
Social Skills Knowledge
Treatment group showed significant gains over control
in adolescents’ self reported social emotional loneliness,
knowledge of social skills, empathizing and social
responsiveness as well as increases in cooperation and
assertion (SSRS). Caregiver reported frequency of “get
togethers” also increased.
P- 3/6
S- 2/8
Weak
Hopkins et al.
[38], 2011
(USA)
N=49; School age;
male (44), female (5)
Face Say vs.
Control “Tux
Paint” Computer
Emotion and Facial
Recognition; SSRS;
Social Skills Observation
(SSO)
Significant increase over control group in emotion
recognition using photos for both low functioning (LFA)
and high functioning (HFA) groups as well as with
drawings for HFA group. Increases in facial recognition
for children with HFA. Treatment group showed
increases in SSRS scores (LFA-d=1.01, HFA-d=.29)
and SSO (LFA-d=.81, HFA-d=1.34).
P- 3/6
S- 3/8
Weak
Kaale et al.
[39••], 2012
(Norway)
N=61; Preschool;
male (48), female
(13)
Joint Attention
Intervention vs.
Treatment as
usual
ESCS; Observed Duration
of Joint Engagement &
Joint Attention Skills
Significant increase in frequency of JA skills in teacher-
child interaction (d=.44), no effect on joint engagement,
but significant increase in joint engagement during
parent-child interaction
(d=.67). No significant increases
in JA skills during standardized assessment (ESCS).
P- 6/6
S- 4/8
Strong
Kasari et al.
[40••], 2010
(USA)
N=38; Toddlers;
male (29), female (9)
JASPER vs.
Delayed
Intervention
Observed Joint
Engagement, Play Types,
Joint Attention Skills;
Caregiver Quality of
Involvement
Immediate treatment group showed significant increases
in time jointly engaged (d=.97), decreases in object only
engagement (d=1.09), responsiveness to joint attention
(d=.74), types of functional play (d=.88) compared to
waitlist. Initiations of joint attention and diversity of
symbolic play did not differ.
P- 6/6
S- 6/8
Strong
Kasari et al.
[41••], 2012
(USA)
N=60; School age;
male (54), female (6)
Child Assisted,
Peer Assisted,
Child Plus Peer,
vs. Control
Social Network Salience
(SNS); Playground
Observation; Teacher
Perception-Social Skills
Child and Peer group showed greatest increases in SNS
over child (d=1.12), peer (d=.69) and control (d=1.18),
maintained over child (d=.97) at follow up. Solitary
engagement significantly decreased for children in Peer
at follow up (d=0.94) and joint engagement increased
(d=.77). Students in Peer also received increased
friendship nominations (d=.74) post intervention.
Teachers reported significant change in Peer students’
social skills post intervention (d=.44).
P- 6/6
S- 6/8
Strong
Koenig et al.
[42], 2010
(USA)
N=44; School age;
male (34), female (10)
Group Social
Skills Intervention
vs. Waitlist
CGI- Improvement; SCI CGI scores were significantly greater for the treatment
group over waitlist. No significant gains were made on
the SCI.
P- 4/6
S- 3/8
Weak
Landa et al.
[43], 2011
(USA)
N=48; Toddlers;
male (40), female (8)
Interpersonal
Synchrony vs.
Non-Interpersonal
Synchrony
CSBS; Observed Socially
Engaged Imitation; MSEL
Expressive Language
(EL) and Visual
Reception (VR)
Treatment group showed significantly higher rate of
change in socially engaged imitation (SEI: d=.48) pre to
post and greater total SEI at follow up only (d=.86). No
effect on MSEL EL or VR. No effect on initiations of
joint attention or shared positive affect.
P- 3/6
S- 3/8
Weak
Laugeson et
al. [44•],
2012
(USA)
N=28; School age
and adolescents; male
(23), female (5)
UCLA Peers vs.
Delayed
Intervention
SSRS; SRS; QPQ;
TASSK-R
Parents of students in the treatment group reported
greater social skills, reduction in ASD symptoms (SRS)
and increase in “get togethers” over delayed treatment.
Significant increase in knowledge of social skills was
reported (TASSK-R) after treatment and via teacher
report at follow up only.
P- 4/6
S- 2/8
Adequate
Lawton &
Kasari [45•],
2012a
(USA)
N=52; Preschoolers;
male (40), female
(12)
Joint Attention
(JA) vs. Symbolic
Play (SP) vs.
Control
ESCS Increases in joint attention with shared positive affect as
well as joint attention with shared positive affect and an
utterance were found 6 month and 12 month follow up
for both JA and SP conditions while no effect was found
for control participants.
P- 5/6
S- 5/8
Adequate
Lawton &
Kasari [46],
2012b
(USA)
N=17; Preschoolers;
Gender not reported
JASPER vs.
Delayed
Intervention
ESCS; Classroom
Observations
Children showed significant increases in initiations of
joint attention (d=1.85) during classroom observation
including significant increases in points (d=2.02) and
shows (d=1.85) to share. Only increases in shows were
found during structured assessment (d=2.02). Large
effects were also found for decreases in object
engagement (d=1.41) and increases in supported joint
engagement (d= 1.24)
P- 3/6
S- 6/8
Weak
Lerner et al.
[47], 2010
(USA)
N=17; School age &
adolescents; male
(14), female (3)
SDARI
Intervention vs.
Control
CBCL; SSRS; SRS;
Emory Dyssemia Index;
DANVA-2; BDI-Y
Overall participants displaced decreases in EDI and
CBCL scores. No effects found via SSRS. Treatment
group showed greater decrease in errors in identifying
emotions and decrease on CBCL social problems.
P- 2/6
S- 1/8
Weak
Wong &
Kwan [48]
2010
(China)
N=17; Toddlers;
male (16), female (1)
Autism 1-2-3 vs.
no intervention
(crossover trial)
ADOS; RFRLRS; SPT;
PSI
No between group differences in ADOS subscale scores,
symbolic play, parent stress or parent observations of
language or social behavior. Combined treatment groups
showed decreases in ADOS reciprocal social interaction
subscale score, symbolic play, parent report of language
and social behavior as well as total parent stress score.
P- 1/6
S- 2/8
Weak
Young &
Posselt [49],
2012
(Australia)
N=25; Preschoolers
& School age; gender
not reported
Transporters vs.
Control
Television Show
NEPSY-II (affect
recognition); Face Task;
SCQ
Participants in the Transporters group experienced
significantly greater increases in affect recognition than
control participants (ηp2= .53). The Transporters group
also experienced a large gain in emotion recognition
(NEPSY: d=1.7, Face Task: d=.92). No differential
effect was found on social peer interest, gaze aversion or
eye contact.
P- 1/6
S- 2/8
Weak

Overall, this group of studies provided replicable descriptions of the dependent variables (17 studies), implemented manualized interventions (13), reported high inter-rater agreement (10) and examined either generalization or maintenance of treatment effects (10). Yet, these studies generally failed to thoroughly describe both participants and interventionists, include blinded raters, measure interventionist fidelity or demonstrate experimental control for each dependent variable of interest.

Group designs

Studies using group designs were of mixed quality. Scores for each study are in Table 1. Three studies obtained a rating of ‘strong’ achieving ‘high’ quality ratings on all primary indicators [39••, 40••, 41••] while five studies were considered ‘adequate’, achieving ratings of ‘high’ on four to five primary indicators and demonstrating at least two of the secondary indicators [33•, 35•, 36•, 44•, 45•]. The remaining nine studies obtained ‘weak’ ratings, scoring ‘high’ on fewer than four primary indicators or obtaining an ‘unacceptable’ rating for at least one primary indicator.

Altogether this group of studies demonstrated a strong link between research questions and data analysis (17 studies), implemented manualized interventions (12), provided replicable definitions of dependent variables (14), demonstrated less than 30% attrition balanced across groups (17) and implemented random assignment (14). However, the studies often failed to include adequately powered statistical analysis, describe comparison conditions with replicable precision, include blind raters, report effect sizes of at least .40 across 75% or more of the outcomes and record interventionist fidelity.

Discussion of Study Outcomes

Based on the studies reviewed, several intervention targets were addressed in the social skills interventions. These included: 1) knowledge and conceptual understandings, 2) peer relationships/friendships, and 3) joint attention/joint engagement.

Interventions to Improve Knowledge and Conceptual Understandings

Many of the interventions were aimed at improving: 1) knowledge of discrete skills such as emotion recognition or appropriate social behaviors, or 2) concepts such as theory of mind. These interventions are based on the theory that increasing knowledge of social behavior will translate into better social interactions in authentic real life situations. These types of interventions generally are carried out in groups and led by an instructor in a clinical setting with direct teaching of social skills. A number of studies also augmented these group interventions with video modeling and feedback [17, 18, 19•, 24, 27, 29, 30].

Studies in this review yielded similar results to previously published studies on knowledge, perceptions, and understandings. The outcome measures for each intervention were mostly linked to the content of the intervention. A child would, for example, be taught to recognize emotions in pictures or video, and then tested on their knowledge of emotions from a similar battery of pictures. For example, in a study aimed at teaching theory of mind (i.e., understanding others have thoughts different from oneself), children improved their conceptual theory of mind after intervention on a theory of mind test [32]. These results did not appear to generalize to other settings. In this study, similar outcomes were not observed on parent reported social behavior [32]. Similarly, Castorina and Negri [33•] found that children did better on a test of social skills after intervention (regardless of the treatment conditions: with or without siblings) but parents did not report social behavior changes. These targeted treatments typically had little effect on parents’, teachers’ or children’s report of their own social behaviors. Social skills in natural settings as an intervention outcome rarely were observed. One exception was an RCT [38] in which video instruction of emotion recognition increased social skills observed by trained research assistants during recess or free time at school for higher functioning children (but not lower functioning children).

Summary

Overall, interventions aimed at increasing social knowledge demonstrate increased knowledge, but the effects do not appear to transfer to real life settings. Future studies may want to merge didactic knowledge modules with practice modules in authentic community settings to support the generalization of conceptual skills to natural interactions.

Interventions Addressing Peer Relationships

Traditionally, children with ASD have been prompted to initiate and respond to peers, and studies in this review continued this tradition [15, 16]. For example, for one study the outcomes of interest were prompted initiations, responses and sharing between peers [16]. Maintenance and generalization data were not collected but one might not expect skills to maintain in the absence of an adult to prompt behaviors, and one might question an outcome that is prompted and not spontaneous. In several other studies, researchers focused on the context for teaching children to initiate and respond to peers. Three studies used play dates and club activities to teach children with ASD to initiate and respond to peers, and to develop friendships [21•, 22, 23•]. Two of these three studies [21•, 23•] recorded spontaneous interactions [21•] or initiations [23•] rather than prompted initiations. Differentiation of spontaneous versus prompted social initiations is a conceptual improvement that provides evidence toward children’s ability to generate social behaviors on their own initiative rather than responding to prompting by an adult. While these studies suggest some success of this teaching approach on child outcomes, an important advancement during this review period was the number of RCTs incorporating the same elements as in the SSRDs (teaching of peer initiations/responses using the context of play dates).

Three RCTs using a wait-list design based on the same model of parent-mediated social skills intervention [50] demonstrated positive outcomes for children and adolescents [36•, 37, 44•]. Studies delivered weekly group based social skills intervention for 14 weeks focusing on social information “critical social situations” including establishing social networks with peers, entering and negotiating social interactions, and engaging in peer play and social “get togethers” in community settings [35•]. In all three studies parents reported more “get togethers” with friends and better social skills post intervention. In two of the studies, participants also reported less loneliness [36•, 37]. A limitation of these findings, however, is that the main outcomes were based on parent report and parents were involved in the intervention; thus as informants they are not unbiased. While parents reported more hosted get-togethers, only one study reported increased numbers of get togethers in which the child was a guest [37]. Generalization to natural settings was not reported; however, Frankel et al. [36•] reported that in a subsample of children, those who engaged in more hosted play dates were also more engaged on school playgrounds. This finding suggests that increasing experiences with peers through facilitated play dates may have benefits in the school context.

Another RCT was conducted in the school setting representing a departure from the other studies during this review, which were carried out in clinic settings [41••]. This study randomized children with ASD who were high functioning and in general education classrooms to combinations of two different interventions designed to improve peer interactions. One intervention focused on teaching social skills to the child with ASD directly. This intervention addressed the top three challenges the child demonstrated on the playground at school. The other intervention focused on teaching three typical peers in the child with ASD’s classroom on how to engage any child having a social challenge on the playground. Sixty children with ASD were randomized to receive one of the interventions, to receive both of the interventions, or to receive neither of the interventions. The intervention was 12 sessions, carried out at school, with the primary outcome measure based on the number of peer connections from a peer social network measure. Results indicated that children with ASD who received an intervention involving classroom peers received more friend nominations, were more connected to social groups at school, and were less isolated on the playground. Their teachers also reported improved classroom social skills. Thus, this intervention has potential for improving social engagement at school; however, the intervention was implemented by researchers on the school campus and not transferred to school staff. Sustainability is less likely without transfer of the implementation of an intervention to the school personnel, and these types of community trials should be the focus of future research.

Summary

Previously identified gaps [12] were addressed by studies of adequate quality included in this review. Tests of parent-mediated interventions for adolescents resulted in increases in social skills knowledge and interactions between teens [44•], siblings were included (although no significant effects were attributed to their presence) [33•, 34] and some studies included cognitively delayed individuals [38]. Greater numbers of RCTs were reported during this review period, some with reasonably large sample sizes (>50), and rigorous designs (RCTs) and one was also implemented in an authentic community setting.

Interventions Addressing Joint Attention/Joint Engagement in Young Children

A number of interventions directly addressed core social deficits in young children, namely the development of joint attention/joint engagement between the child and an adult (parent or teacher). Joint attention skills refer to gestures used to share an experience with another person, including coordinated looking as well as showing, pointing and giving objects to share. Joint engagement refers to the amount of time dyads are engaged together in shared activities. Both joint attention skills and joint engagement are impaired in young children with ASD. Two advancements are noted for this review. First, most studies focusing on joint attention/joint engagement used RCTs in which an experimental intervention was compared to practice as usual. Only one SSRD study was reported, a change from previous reviews in which most studies examining joint attention used SSRDs. Another advancement is that the majority of studies included effectiveness trials focused on teacher delivery of intervention in natural settings, a dramatic departure from earlier clinic-based studies.

Preschoolers

Three effectiveness studies [20, 39••, 46] tested adaptations of a joint attention and play intervention that had previously shown clinic-based efficacy [51, 52]. Dykstra et al [20] implemented a SSRD while both Kaale et al. [39••] and Lawton and Kasari [46] carried out RCTs in which teachers in specialized and non-specialized preschool settings implemented the intervention. All three studies demonstrated effects on social behavior with some variation that should inform future studies. For example, Dykstra et al [20] compared group instruction versus group plus 1:1 instruction for three participants using a multiple baseline design. While children appeared to benefit from the combined group and 1:1 interventions, this study is limited in two notable ways. First, joint attention skills were combined with social interaction and requesting skills, and functional and symbolic play acts were combined in the data analysis. Given that children with ASD have specific impairments in joint attention skills (more so than requesting skills) this study does not provide specific information on which skills improved. The same is true for functional and symbolic play skills. The combination of these two types of play skills into one play category does not inform whether the difficult-to-change domain of symbolic play skills actually changed or if the results were driven by functional play acts. Second, the conclusions one can make based on the small sample are limited. Although children appeared to benefit from group plus 1:1 instruction, the question remains whether receiving 1:1 instruction prior to group may have led to different conclusions. In this study group instruction was delivered first and then paired with 1:1. Due to the lack of variation in the order of treatment, it is not clear whether the reverse (1:1 followed by group intervention) would have equal or potentially greater effects.

The two RCTs examining a similar intervention model provide greater information on skills that could be changed in a brief and targeted teacher-mediated intervention. Both studies implemented a developmentally informed behavioral intervention that specifically focused on the teaching of joint attention and play skills to preschool aged children with ASD in the classroom. One study received a rating of strong quality [39••], and the other weak (primarily due to lack of participant description; Lawton and Kasari [46]) although both yielded significant findings. Kaale et al. [39••] yielded significant effects on children’s initiations of joint attention in the classroom, and joint engagement with parents suggesting generalization from classroom instruction to parents who were not involved in the intervention. Similarly, Lawton and Kasari [46] also found significant effects for children’s initiations of joint attention in the classroom. Given that spontaneous initiations are particularly difficult to teach, both of these studies are highly significant given they were mediated in real world settings by community staff. These studies are a significant advancement from clinic based trials, single subject designs, and interventions that focused more on requesting skills than joint attention initiations.

Toddlers

Two additional studies tested intervention effects on toddlers. One intervention was mediated through interventionists and the other through parents, both in university, clinic settings. While the interventionist-delivered treatment did not show effects on joint attention skills, it did result in differences in toddler’s imitation skills [43]. Similarly the parent-mediated intervention did not result in changes in child joint attention initiations but did show effects on joint attention responses and improved joint engagement [40••]. Children decreased their focus on objects only and increased their joint engagement with their parent and objects. Increasing engagement between parent and child may be especially important to the development of social and communication skills. Given the better outcomes on joint attention initiations for preschool aged children, these studies on toddlers highlight potentially critical developmental differences in intervention effects. It may be that younger children are not quite ready for developing joint attention initiations or may require greater density of intervention, or different sequencing of interventions. The results also suggest that treatment delivery may be an important consideration with parent-mediated interventions reporting more changes on core deficits than group delivery by trained interventionists.

Summary

Altogether, interventions addressing joint attention/joint engagement appear to facilitate significant improvements in skills that are core developmental challenges for children with ASD. Future studies should further explore pairing different treatment deliveries (e.g., parent and therapist, or group and individual instruction) for maximum benefit. At this juncture, joint attention interventions demonstrate both efficacy and effectiveness, and are primed for further deployment into community settings.

Conclusions

The last two years of intervention research on social skills have produced important advancements. One is the greater focus on core deficits, including joint attention, joint engagement, social initiations and responses, and development of friendships. Other advancements included the increased number of group designs with many employing randomized controlled trials and the expansion of topics, including studies addressing adolescents, parent-mediated approaches, sibling involvement, and interventions conducted in real world settings. While impressive in the range of topics addressed, future research still needs to tackle four specific areas. First, we need study designs that can address the active ingredients of interventions (e.g., dose required to get effect, agent of change, such as parent, peer, teacher or sibling). We need to move beyond testing an intervention against practice as usual, and compare interventions that differ on critical elements.

A second issue is our understanding of meaningful and sustainable outcomes. Outcome measures remain limited. There is an over-reliance on potentially biased informants (i.e., involved in the intervention or unblinded), outcomes that result from teaching to the test or the result of prompting rather than spontaneous initiations. Outcomes need to provide confidence that meaningful change has occurred (improved social relationships, sustained social interactions).

Third, while we used a rating system to identify quality indicators of studies, this system yielded weak ratings for most studies. Several studies received a weak rating due to limited description of participants while otherwise receiving high quality ratings for key study elements considered important to an intervention trial including data analytic procedures, intervention/outcome description, fidelity and treatment integrity. Moreover, although a study received a strong quality rating, participants may not have achieved significant sustainable benefit from the intervention. Thus, having a means to evaluate the quality of intervention research is a significant advancement and future efforts should further consider whether the intervention merits adoption.

Finally, we continue to focus on select samples of children, often high functioning, and middle class who have the resources to participate in a study at a clinic. The vast majority of children with ASD are not represented in research—children with co-morbidities, non-English speaking children, minimally verbal children, and ethnically and culturally diverse samples. Future studies should include underserved and underrepresented populations of children with ASD thus broadening our understanding of intervention effectiveness for children with ASD.

Acknowledgments

The preparation of this paper was supported by HRSA AIR-B UA3MC11055. SP was supported by the Canadian Institutes for Health Research - Doctoral Foreign Study Award and an Autism Speaks Weatherstone Fellowship, #7036.

Footnotes

Disclosure C. Kasari: grants from Autism Speaks and National Institutes of Health; S. Patterson: royalties from Jessica Kingsley Publishers and compensation for travel/accommodations from Canadian Institutes for Health Research.

References

Papers of particular interest, published recently, have been highlighted as:

• Of importance

•• Of major importance

  • 1.Kasari C, Rotheram-Fuller E. Peer relationships of children with autism: Challenges and Interventions. In: Hollander E, Anagnostou E, editors. Clinical Manual for the Treatment of Autism. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2007. pp. 235–257. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Wing L. Asperger’s syndrome: A clinical account. Psychological Medicine. 1981;11:115–129. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700053332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.National Institute of Mental Health. Bethesda, MD: Author; 2004. Congressional appropriations report on the state of autism research. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Interagency Autism Coordinating Committee. Autism spectrum disorder research: Portfolio analysis report. [Accessed August 2012]; Available at http://iacc.hhs.gov/portfolioanalysis/2010/2010_portfolio_analysis.pdf.
  • 5.Frith U, Happe F. Autism: Beyond theory of mind. Cognition. 1994;50:115–132. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90024-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Mundy P, Block J, Delgado C, et al. Individual Differences and the Development of Joint Attention in Infancy. Child Dev. 2007;78:938–954. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01042.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Bauminger N, Kasari C. Brief report: Theory of mind in high-functioning children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 1999;29:81–86. doi: 10.1023/a:1025974701090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Orsmond GI, Krauss MW, Seltzer MM. Peer relationships and social and recreational activities among adolescents and adults with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34:245–256. doi: 10.1023/b:jadd.0000029547.96610.df. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bellini S, Peters JK, Benner L, et al. A meta-analysis of school-based social skills interventions for children with autism spectrum disorders. Remedial Spec Educ. 2007;28:153–162. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Rao PA, Beidel DC, Murray MJ. Social skills interventions for children with Aspegers syndrome or high functioning autism: A review and recommendations. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:353–361. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0402-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Williams-White S, Keonig K, Scahill L. Social skills development in children with autism spectrum disorders: A review of the intervention research. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:1858–1868. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0320-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Reichow B, Volkmar F. Social skills interventions for individuals with autism: Evaluation for evidence-based practices within a best evidence synthesis framework. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40:149–166. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0842-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Parker RI, Vannest KJ, Brown L. The improvement rate difference for single-case research. Exceptional Children. 2009;75:135–150. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Reichow B, Volkmar FR, Cicchetti DV. Development of the evaluative method for evaluating and determining evidence-based practices in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:1311–1319. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0517-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Banda DR, Hart SL. Increasing peer-to-peer social skills through direct instruction of two elementary school girls with autism. JORSEN. 2010;10:124–132. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Banda DR, Hart SL, Gitz L. Impact of training peers and children with autism on social skills during center time activities in inclusive classrooms. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2010;4:619–625. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Buggey T. Effectiveness of video self-modeling to promote social initiations by 3-year-olds with autism spectrum disorders. Focus Autism Other Dev Disabl. 2012;27:102–110. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Buggey T, Hoomes G, Sherberger ME, et al. Facilitating social initiations of preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders using video self-modeling. Focus Autism Other Dev Disabl. 2011;26:25–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Charlop MH, Carpenter MH, Greenberg AL. Teaching socially expressive behaviors to children with autism through video modeling. Education and Treatment of Children. 2010;33:371–393. Received ‘adequate’ rating on quality indicators for single subject research designs. Taught 3 children to use more socially expressive vocalizations and gestures through video-modeling intervention.
  • 20.Dykstra JR, Boyd BA, Watson LR, et al. The impact of the advancing social-communication and play (ASAP) intervention on preschoolers with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2012;16:27–43. doi: 10.1177/1362361311408933. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Jull S, Mirenda P. Parents as play date facilitators for preschoolers with autism. J Posit Behav Interv. 2011;13:17–30. Received ‘adequate’ rating on quality indicators for single subject research designs. Taught parents of 2 children to facilitate play dates between their child with ASD and a typical peer. Parents learned strategies and children engaged in more synchronous interactions and children spent more time interacting with peers. Parents maintained strategies over one year follow up.
  • 22.Koegel LK, Kuriakose S, Singh AK, et al. Improving generalization of peer socialization gains in inclusive school settings using initiations training. Behav Modif. 2012;36:361–377. doi: 10.1177/0145445512445609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Koegel RL, Fredeen R, Kim S, et al. Using perseverative interests to improve interactions between adolescents with autism and their typical peers in school settings. J Posit Behav Interv. 2012;14:133–141. doi: 10.1177/1098300712437043. Received ‘adequate’ rating on quality indicators for single subject research designs. Used the perseverative interests of 3 adolescents to design lunch clubs at school as context for increasing social engagement and initiations. Engagement increased immediately with variable increases in initiations among the participants.
  • 24.Marzullo-Kerth D, Reeve SA, Reeve KF, et al. Using multiple-exemplar training to teach a generalized repertoire of sharing to children with autism. J Appl Behav Anal. 2011;44:279–294. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2011.44-279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Parker D, Kamps D. Effects of task analysis and self-monitoring for children with autism in multiple social settings. Focus Autism Other Dev Disabl. 2011;26:131–142. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Reichow B, Sabornie EJ. Brief report: Increasing verbal greeting initiations for a student with autism via a Social Story intervention. Journal on Developmental Disabilities. 2009;39:1740–1743. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0814-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Robinson SE. Teaching paraprofessionals of students with autism to implement pivotal response treatment in inclusive school settings using a brief video feedback training package. Focus Autism Other Dev Disabl. 2011;26:105–118. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Schneider N, Goldstein H. Using social stories and visual schedules to improve social appropriate behaviors with children with autism. J Posit Behav Interv. 2010;12:149–160. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.State TM, Kern L. A comparison of video feedback and in vivo self-monitoring on the social interactions of an adolescent with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Behaviour Education. 2012;21:18–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Tetreault AS, Lerman DC. Teaching social skills to children with autism using point-of-view video modeling. Education and Treatment of Children. 2010;33:395–419. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wichnick AM, Vener SM, Keating C, et al. The effect of a script-fading procedure on unscripted social initiations and novel utterances among young children with autism. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2010;4:51–64. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Begeer S, Gevers C, Clifford P, et al. Theory of mind training in children with autism: A randomized controlled trial. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41:997–1006. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1121-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Castorina LL, Negri LM. The inclusion of siblings in social skills training groups for boys with Asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41:73–81. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1023-x. Received ‘adequate’ rating on quality indicators for group designs. Small pilot RCT demonstrating increases in emotion identification and non-verbal social cues for children with direct training on social skills with and without siblings present in groups. Compared to wait list control, both treatment groups improved and maintained skills over 3 month follow up. Increased identification of cues did not translate to social skills as rated by parent and teacher.
  • 34.Chu C, Pan C. The effect of peer- and sibling-assisted aquatic program on interaction behaviors and aquatic skills of children with autism spectrum disorder and their peers/siblings. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2012;6:1211–1223. [Google Scholar]
  • 35. DeRosier ME, Swick DC, Davis NO, et al. The efficacy of a social skills group intervention for improving social behaviors in children with high functioning autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41:1033–1043. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1128-2. Received ‘adequate’ rating on quality indicators for group designs. Adapted social skills program for high functioning children with ASD with group instruction involving parents in some sessions. Parents in treatment group reported increased social skills compared to control parents. Children did not report differences by group.
  • 36. Frankel F, Myatt R, Sugar C, et al. A randomized controlled study of parent-assisted children’s friendship training with children having autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40:827–842. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0932-z. Received ‘adequate’ rating on quality indicators for group designs. Focus on high functioning children with ASD in 2nd to 5th grade. Group child and parent intervention addressing social skills, play dates, and friendships. Parents in treatment group reported improved social skills and increased play dates. Children reported less loneliness and more popularity. At 3 month follow up results for treatment group still improved from baseline.
  • 37.Gantman A, Kapp SK, Orenski K, et al. Social skills training for young adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders: A randomized controlled pilot study. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42:1094–1103. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1350-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Hopkins IM, Gower MW, Perez TA, et al. Avatar assistant: Improving social skills in students with an ASD through a computer-based intervention. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41:1543–1555. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1179-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Kaale A, Smith L, Sponheim E. A randomized controlled trial of a preschool-based joint attention intervention for children with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatr. 2012;53:97–105. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02450.x. Received a’ strong’ rating on quality indicators. An effectiveness trial of joint attention and play intervention delivered by nursery school teachers yielded effects on joint attention initiations with teachers and joint engagement with parents.
  • 40. Kasari C, Gulsrud AC, Wong C, et al. Randomized controlled caregiver mediated joint engagement intervention for toddlers with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40:1045–1056. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0955-5. Received a ‘strong’ rating on quality indicators. Small randomized controlled trial of parent mediated joint attention and play intervention improved joint engagement between parents and toddlers, and child joint attention responses and functional play acts.
  • 41. Kasari C, Rotheram-Fuller E, Locke J, et al. Making the connection: Randomized controlled trial of social skills at school for children with autism spectrum disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatr. 2012;53:431–439. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02493.x. Received a ‘strong’ rating on quality indicators. Partial effectiveness trial in the schools (researchers delivered the intervention) with significant effects of intervention conditions involving typical peers from the child with ASD’s class. Multi-informant outcome measures including peers (social connections), teacher report, and playground observations.
  • 42.Koenig K, White SW, Pachler M, et al. Promoting social skill development in children with pervasive developmental disorders: A feasibility and efficacy study. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40:1209–1218. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0979-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Landa R, Holman KC, O’Neil AH, et al. Intervention targeting development of socially synchronous engagement in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder: A randomized controlled trial. J Child Psychol Psychiatr. 2011;52:13–21. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02288.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Laugeson EA, Frankel F, Gantman A, et al. Evidence-based social skills training for adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: The UCLA PEERS program. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42:1025–1036. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1339-1. Received ‘adequate’ rating on quality indicators. Randomized trial of group based and parent mediated social skills program for teens with significant effects on parent reported social skills, and more get-togethers at home between teens. Teens tested significantly better on social knowledge post intervention.
  • 45. Lawton K, Kasari C. Brief report: Longitudinal improvements in the quality of joint attention in preschool children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012a;42:307–312. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1231-z. Received ‘adequate’ rating on quality indicators. Longitudinal assessment of children involved in a randomized trial of joint attention and play interventions with concomitant improvements in shared positive affect with the joint attention intervention.
  • 46.Lawton K, Kasari C. Teacher-implemented joint attention intervention: Pilot randomized controlled study for preschoolers with autism. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2012b;80:687–693. doi: 10.1037/a0028506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Lerner MD, Mikami AY, Levine K. Socio-dramatic affective-relational intervention for adolescents with Asperger syndrome and high functioning autism: Pilot study. Autism Res. 2011;15:21–42. doi: 10.1177/1362361309353613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Wong VCN, Kwan QK. Randomized controlled trial for early intervention for autism: A pilot study of the autism 1-2-3 project. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40:677–688. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0916-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Young RL, Posselt M. Using The Transporters DVD as a learning tool for children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42:984–991. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1328-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Frankel F, Myatt R. Children’s friendship training. New York: Brunner-Routledge; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Kasari C, Freeman S, Paparella T. Joint attention and symbolic play in young children with autism: A randomized controlled intervention study. J Child Psychol Psychiatr. 2006;6:611–620. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01567.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Kasari C, Paparella T, Freeman S, et al. Language outcome in autism: Randomized comparison of joint attention and play. J Child Psychol Psychiatr. 2008;76:125–137. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.1.125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES