Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Nov 5.
Published in final edited form as: J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012 Nov 5;60(11):2075–2080. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.04195.x

Residential Proximity to Major Roadway and Cognitive Function in Community-Dwelling Seniors: Results from the MOBILIZE Boston Study

Gregory A Wellenius 1, Luke D Boyle 1,2, Brent A Coull 3, William P Milberg 4, Alexandros Gryparis 5, Joel Schwartz 6, Murray A Mittleman 7, Lewis A Lipsitz 8
PMCID: PMC3498530  NIHMSID: NIHMS398757  PMID: 23126566

Abstract

Background/Objectives

Long-term exposure to ambient air pollution has been associated with decreased cognitive function, but the effects of traffic pollution on the elderly have not been studied in detail. Accordingly, the objective of this study was to evaluate the association between residential distance to major roadway, as a marker of long-term exposure to traffic pollution, and cognitive function in seniors.

Design, Setting, Participants

A prospective cohort study of 765 community-dwelling seniors with median follow-up of 16.8 months.

Measurements

We administered the Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE), Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R), Trail Making Test (TMT), category and letter fluency tests, and clock-in-the-box test (CIB) during home visits on 2 occasions. We calculated the residential distance to nearest major roadway and used generalized estimating equations to evaluate the association between performance on each test and residential distance to major roadway adjusting for participant demographics, education, socioeconomic status and past medical history.

Results

Decreasing distance to major roadway was associated with statistically significantly poorer performance on the immediate and delayed recall components of the HVLT-R, TMT part B, TMT delta, and the letter and category fluency tests. Generally, participants residing <100 m from a major roadway performed worst. Performance improved monotonically with increasing distance.

Conclusions

In this cohort of community-dwelling seniors, residential proximity to major roadways was associated with poorer performance on cognitive tests of verbal learning and memory, psychomotor speed, language and executive functioning. If causal, these results add to the growing evidence that living near major roadways is associated with adverse health outcomes.

Keywords: air pollution, traffic, roadway, cognitive function, elderly

INTRODUCTION

Long-term exposure to fine particulate matter air pollution has been associated with increased risk of cardiovascular mortality, coronary heart disease, and cerebrovascular events.1, 2 These effects are likely mediated by a combination of pathophysiologic changes including increased systemic inflammation, oxidative stress, and vascular endothelial injury, with pollution from traffic identified as a key source.3 Long-term exposure to traffic pollution has been associated with increased atherosclerosis of the carotid arteries4 and narrowing of the retinal arterioles,5 suggesting that traffic pollution can adversely affect both the large cerebral arteries and the cerebral microvasculature. Considering the importance of a vascular etiology in cognitive impairment,6 it is plausible that long-term exposure to traffic pollution may be detrimental to cognitive function. However, few studies have specifically evaluated this hypothesis in the elderly.7, 8 Given the increasing prevalence of mild cognitive impairment, the substantial impact even mild cognitive decline can have on the number of people unable to live independently, and the increasing urbanization of cities worldwide, further clarifying the potential effects of traffic pollution on cognitive function is important. Accordingly, we evaluated the association between residential distance to major roadway as a marker of long-term exposure to traffic pollution and cognitive function within a prospective, community-based cohort study of healthy aging.9

METHODS

Study Design

Between 2005 and 2008, the MOBILIZE Boston Study (MBS) recruited 765 non-institutionalized men and women aged ≥65 years, able to communicate in English, residing <5 miles (8.0 km) from the study clinic, able to walk 20 feet (6.1 m) without personal assistance, and having a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score of ≥18, as previously described.9 All participants provided written informed consent. This analysis was approved by the Institutional Review Boards at Hebrew Senior Life and Brown University.

During an in-home interview, trained staff administered tests of cognitive function and obtained detailed information on participant age, race, sex, education, household income, medical history, current medications, physical activity, and smoking history. Performance of physical activities was determined using the Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE).10 During a follow-up clinic examination, we measured height and weight, collected a venous blood sample, and measured supine blood pressure as previously described.9 A second assessment consisting of an in-home interview and clinic examination was performed a median of 16.8 months after the baseline assessment.

Neuropsychological Measures

We administered six neuropsychological tests to each participant during the home interview, as previously described.11

The MMSE is widely used in clinical and research settings to screen for dementia and provides a global assessment of an individual’s cognitive function. The MMSE is scored on a 30-point scale, with low scores indicating worse performance.

The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R)12 is a 12-item word list learning test in which individuals are presented three learning and recall trials followed by a delayed recall trail and a 24-item word recognition test. The HVLT-R produces three scores: the sum of correct responses in each of three learning trials (HVLT-R Learning); the number of items correctly recalled after the delay (HVLT-R Delayed Recall); and the number of recognition items correctly identified (HVLT-R Recognition). In older adults, the encoding procedure requires working and verbal memory processes and executive functioning.13

The Trailmaking Test (TMT) part A consists of number targets to be connected in order, providing an estimate of attention and psychomotor speed. TMT Part B includes number and letter targets that are to be connected in alternating sequence (e.g., 1-A-2-B-3-C), providing an estimate of set-shifting and executive function. Performance is based on the time required to complete each task up to a maximum of 5 minutes. Shorter times indicate better performance. The TMT is frequently applied in the clinical setting and is sensitive to the presence of frontal lobe pathology and cerebrovascular risk.14 As in previous studies,11 to control for the effect of motor function and information processing speed we calculated the TMT delta as the time to perform part B minus the time to perform part A.

The Clock-in-a-Box Test (CIB) requires participants to read and follow written directions by drawing a clock in a specific location on the response form and setting the clock to the correct time.15 Performance on this test is based on eight features associated with working memory and organization and planning of the drawing, with higher scores indicating better performance. Performance on the CIB has been shown to be predictive of performance on standardized measures of working memory and executive function.11, 15

In the Letter Fluency test, participants were asked to name as many words as possible beginning with given letters (F, A, and S) for 60 seconds each. An additional 60-second trial was given to assess category fluency (animal naming). Performance was based on the number of items generated for each trial, with higher scores indicating better performance. Both tests assess language and executive functioning.11

Exposure Assessment

We calculated residential distance to major roadway as a marker of long-term exposure to traffic pollution. We used ArcGIS (version 9.2; ESRI, Inc., Redlands, CA) to geocode participant addresses and calculate the Euclidean distance from residence to the nearest major roadway, defined as roads having US Census Feature Class Code A1 (primary highway with limited access) or A2 (primary road without limited access), as in previous studies.16 In a secondary analysis, we estimated daily outdoor black carbon levels (a marker of traffic pollution) at each participant’s residential address using a validated spatial-temporal land-use regression model, as previously described.17 To create a metric of long-term black carbon exposure for the present study, we average destimated residential black carbon over the 365 days preceding each cognitive assessment.

Statistical Analysis

As in a previous study,7 we dichotomized MMSE scores as below or above the lowest quartile (MMSE <26 versus ≥26) and used generalized estimating equation (GEE) models with a logit link function and an exchangeable correlation matrix to estimate the association between residential exposure to traffic pollution and the odds of having an MMSE <26 while accounting for within-subject correlation. For all other outcomes, we used GEE models with an identity link function. In all analyses we adjusted for age, sex, race (white versus other), history of stroke, history of smoking (ever versus never), physical activity, education (3 categories), visit (baseline versus first follow-up) body mass index, household income (below versus above median), season of home interview (4 categories), and two census track-level indicators of neighborhood socioeconomic status (percent of population that is non-white and percent of population with college degree or above). Age, physical activity, and body mass index were each modeled using natural cubic splines with 3 degrees of freedom. We initially modeled residential distance to roadway as a linear continuous variable, but subsequently considered the following categories: ≤100, >100 to 250, >250 to 500, >500 to 1000, and >1000 m. Tests for linear trend were performed by assigning the median distance to each category and including the term as a continuous variable in the regression model.

We evaluated whether the association between residential distance to major roadway and test performance varied by age (≤77 versus > 77) or education (high school or less versus some college or more) by adding an interaction term to the main effects model.

Analyses were performed using SAS (v9.2; SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC) and R statistical software (R v2.10). A two-sided p value of <0.05 was considered statistically significant.

RESULTS

MOBILIZE Boston Study participants were elderly, and predominantly white and female (Table 1). Residential distance to major roadway varied from near 0 meters to just over 3 km, with 10% of participants living within 100 m of a major roadway.

Table 1.

Baseline characteristics of 765 participants from the MOBILIZE Boston Study.

Characteristic n (%) or mean ± SD
Female 489 (63.9)
Age, years 78.1 ± 5.4
White 593 (77.5)
Educational Level
 High School or Less 261 (34.2)
 College or Vocational School 266 (34.8)
 Graduate School 237 (31.0)
Ever Smoker 427 (55.8)
Body Mass Index, kg/m2 27.3 ± 5.1
History of Stroke 76 (9.9)
Dyslipidemia 361 (47.2)
Diabetes Mellitus 153 (20.0)
Hypertension 598 (78.2)
Physical Activity* 104.7 (66.4)
*

assessed using the using the Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE) 10.

In models adjusting for participant demographics, medical history, education, and socioeconomic status, decreasing residential distance to major roadway was associated with poorer performance on the immediate and delayed recall components of the HVLT-R, TMT part B, TMT delta, and both the letter and category fluency tests (Table 2, Supplemental Table 1). Generally, performance on these tests was worst for those participants residing within 100 m from a major roadway and improved monotonically with increasing distance (Table 3). Distance to major roadway was not associated with performance on the HVLT-R recognition, TMT part A, or CIB tests, nor with risk of having an MMSE score <26 (odds ratio (OR): 1.07; 95% confidence interval (CI): 0.84, 1.36; p=0.60).

Table 2.

Change in score on tests of cognitive function associated with an interquartile range decrease (851.2 m) in residential distance to major roadway.*

Mean Score ± SD Change in Score (95% CI) p-value
Lower Scores Indicate Poorer Performance
 Hopkins Verbal Learning Test
  Immediate Recall 20.8 ± 5.8 −0.6 (−1.1, −0.1) 0.015
  Delayed Recall 6.3 ± 3.5 −0.4 (−0.7, −0.1) 0.011
  Recognition 11.4 ± 2.2 0.07 (−0.12, 0.25) 0.47
 Letter Fluency 37.5 ± 14.7 −1.4 (−2.7, −0.2) 0.022
 Category Fluency 15.6 ± 5.3 −0.7 (−1.1, −0.3) 0.002
 Clock in the box 6.4 ± 1.5 −0.04 (−0.15, 0.07) 0.47
Higher Scores Indicate Poorer Performance
 Trailmaking Test, seconds
  Part A 56.3 ± 34.2 2.1 (−0.7, 4.9) 0.14
  Part B 141.1 ± 78.1 10.5 (4.0, 17.1) 0.002
  Delta 87.9 ± 63.7 7.5 (2.2, 12.8) 0.006
*

All models are adjusted for age, sex, race, history of stroke, history of smoking, education, visit number, body mass index, physical activity, household income, percent of neighborhood population that is non-white, and percent of neighborhood population with college degree or above.

Trailmaking Test Delta is calculated as the time to complete Part B minus the time to complete Part A of the test.

Table 3.

Change in score on tests of cognitive function associated with categories of residential distance to major roadway.

Residential Distance to Major Roadway
Ptrend
<100 m (n=77) 100–250 m (n=85) 250–500 m (n=133) 500–1000 m (n=224) >1000 m (n=237)
Lower Scores Indicate Poorer Performance
 Hopkins Verbal Learning Test
  Immediate Recall −1.6 (−2.9, − 0.3) −0.8 (−2.0, 0.4) −1.0 (−2.0, 0.1) 0.1 (−0.8, 1.0) 0 0.011
  Delayed Recall −1.1 (−1.9, − 0.3) −0.8 (−1.6, −0.1) −0.3 (−1.0, 0.3) −0.1 (−0.7, 0.4) 0 0.006
  Recognition 0.2 (−0.3, 0.8) 0.1 (−0.4, 0.5) 0.2 (−0.2, 0.7) 0.2 (−0.2, 0.6) 0 0.27
 Letter Fluency −1.5 (−4.7, 1.8) −2.4 (−5.4, 0.5) −2.4 (−5.0, 0.2) −1.9 (−4.2, 0.4) 0 0.053
 Category Fluency −0.8 (−1.9, 0.3) −1.2 (−2.4, 0.0) −0.9 (−1.8, 0.1) −0.3 (−1.1, 0.5) 0 0.016
 Clock-in-the-Box −0.1 (−0.4, 0.2) −0.1 (−0.4, 0.2) 0.0 (−0.2, 0.3) 0.0 (−0.3, 0.2) 0 0.66
Higher Scores Indicate Poorer Performance
 Trailmaking Test
  Part A 7.4 (−2.2, 16.9) 1.9 (−5.7, 9.5) −0.6 (−5.8, 4.5) 4.1 (−1.2, 9.5) 0 0.26
  Part B 15.2 (−1.6, 32.0) 13.7 (−4.5, 31.8) 11.0 (−3.2, 25.2) 9.5 (−2.0, 21.0) 0 0.025
  Delta 6.9 (−6.5, 20.3) 12.4 (−2.6, 27.5) 10.2 (−1.3, 21.8) 5.0 (−4.4, 14.4) 0 0.041

Ptrend denotes the P-value from the test for linear trend.

Trailmaking Test Delta is calculated as the time to complete Part B minus the time to complete Part A of the test.

Proximity to major roadway was associated with increased risk of having an MMSE score <26 among participants with at least some college education (OR: 1.54; 95% CI: 1.10, 2.17) and participants aged ≤77 years (OR: 1.34; 95% CI (1.01, 1.76), but not among older participants or those with less formal education (Supplemental Table 2). The association between distance to roadway and performance on the HVLT-R delayed recall and category fluency tests varied according to age with the larger association observed among participants aged ≤77 years.

Annual residential black carbon levels ranged from 0.15 to 0.98 μg/m3 with a median of 0.36 μg/m3. An interquartile range increase in residential black carbon levels (0.11 μg/m3) was associated with a higher risk of having an MMSE score <26 (OR: 1.15; 95% CI: 0.99, 1.34; p=0.063), and worse performance on the HVLT-R immediate recall (p=0.046), but was not associated with statistically significant changes in performance on any other neuropsychological tests (Supplemental Table 3).

DISCUSSION

In this cohort of community-dwelling seniors, residential distance to major roadway was associated with poorer performance on cognitive tests of verbal learning and memory, psychomotor speed, language and executive functioning, with most tests showing evidence of a graded dose-response relationship. To put these results into context, the decrease in performance associated with an interquartile range decrease in distance to major roadway was similar to that associated with an increase in age of 2 (for tests of verbal memory) to 4 years (for the TMT and category naming tests). Some of the observed associations differed across subgroups defined by achieved education, but we found no statistical evidence that the associations differed by age. Results were generally in the same direction but not statistically significant when we instead considered residential black carbon levels as a marker of traffic pollution.

Prior studies suggest that traffic pollution18, 19 is likely deleterious to neurodevelopment in children and that urban air pollution is associated with cognitive decline in older adults.20 However, fewer studies have evaluated the effects of traffic pollution on cognitive impairment in the elderly. Ranft et al.8 found that among 399 women aged 68–79 years in Germany those living <50 m from a busy roadway performed worse on the total score calculated from the CERAD-Plus test battery, as well as the Stroop color word test and a test of olfactory function. The observation that performance on the Stroop test – which provides an assessment of executive function and selective attention – is reduced is consistent with the findings in the current study. Power et al.7 found that predicted 1-year average residential black carbon was associated with higher risk of having an MMSE score in the lowest quartile among 680 men from the Normative Aging Study. We similarly found an association between performance on the MMSE and long-term average residential black carbon levels. We also found that MMSE performance was associated with distance to roadway among participants with at least some college education. Power et al.7 also found that residential black carbon levels were associated with a lower summary score derived from a battery of 7 cognitive tests, but did not report the results for individual tests making further comparison to the current study difficult.

While residential black carbon and distance to nearest major roadway both serve as markers of traffic pollution, they are not equivalent. Between-person differences in estimated residential black carbon predominantly reflect differences in air pollution from traffic on roads within 100 m of the home which is dominated by automobiles moving at slower speeds. On the other hand, residential distance to major roadway likely reflects pollution originating from automobile and truck traffic on busy highways. Thus, our results might suggest that cognitive function is more strongly associated with pollution arising from automobiles and trucks on busy highways than that arising from automobile traffic on local roads. Indeed, independent effects of traffic pollution over different spatial scales have been previously reported.21

Short-term and long-term exposure to ambient particulate matter has been associated with increased risk of cerebrovascular events, and traffic pollution seems to be particularly important.2, 22, 23 Traffic pollution exposure has also been associated with increased systemic inflammation and oxidative stress,24, 25 which may in turn promote cerebral micro vascular dysfunction and increase the brain’s susceptibility to injury.6 Histologic evidence of cerebral micro vascular damage, vascular inflammation, and disruption of the blood brain barrier has been observed in dogs from areas with high versus low levels of urban air pollution.26,27 Excess prefrontal white matter hyperintense lesions on magnetic resonance imaging have been observed in dogs and children residing in high versus low pollution areas of Mexico.27 Whether long-term exposure to ambient pollutants is associated with other vascular pathologies remains unknown.

Non-vascular mechanisms of cognitive impairment are also plausible. Toxicologic studies suggest that inhaled ultrafine particles (diameter <100nm, primarily from traffic) and/or transition metals may translocate from the respiratory system directly to the central nervous system via the olfactory neuronal pathway.28, 29 Dogs from the polluted areas also showed increased DNA damage in the olfactory bulb and hippocampus, as well as increased expression of injury-responsive proteins (NFκB, iNOS, amyloid precursor protein and amyloid β) in neurons and/or glial cells.29

Our study has some potential limitations. First, despite controlling for socioeconomic status at both the individual and neighborhood levels, we cannot exclude the possibility of residual confounding. Second, since participants with MMSE <18 were excluded from the study our results may not be generalizable to individuals with impaired cognitive function. Third, the length of time at the current address for each study participant is unknown, possibly leading to some exposure misclassification which would have biased our health effects estimates towards the null. Additionally, indoor levels of pollutants of ambient origin may be substantially lower than outdoor levels, likely further biasing our health effect estimates towards the null. Finally, our study is unable to separate the effects of traffic air pollution from traffic noise, which has also been associated with cognitive impairment in children.30 On the other hand, important strengths of our study include a large, representative cohort of community-dwelling seniors, repeated measures within individuals, and a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological assessments administered in the home.

In conclusion, in this cohort of community-dwelling elderly participants, markers of long-term exposure to traffic pollution were associated with cognitive impairment across a number of different domains, suggesting that traffic pollution could be exerting effects on specific areas of the brain. Additional studies are needed to confirm these findings, to identify the components of traffic pollution most responsible, and to identify the pathophysiologic changes in brain structure or function underlying these associations. If causal, these results add to the growing evidence that living near major roadways is associated with adverse health.

Supplementary Material

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to the MOBILIZE Boston Study research team and participants for their time, effort, and dedication.

Sources of Funding

The project described was supported by grants AG004390, AG25037, ES015774, ES009825, and ES000002 from the National Institutes of Aging (NIA) and Environmental Health Sciences (NIEHS), NIH, and grants R832416 and RD83479801 from the US Environmental Protection Agency (US EPA). Dr. Lipsitz holds the Irving and Edyth S. Usen and Family Chair in Geriatric Medicine at Hebrew Senior Life. The contents of this report are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the sponsoring institutions.

Role of the Sponsors: The sponsors had no role in the design or conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, or interpretation of the data; or preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.

Footnotes

Author Disclosures

The authors declare that they have no competing financial interests.

Author Contributions:

Dr. Wellenius had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: Wellenius, Mittleman. Acquisition of data: Milberg, Lipsitz, Gryparis. Analysis and interpretation of data: Wellenius, Boyle, Coull, Schwartz, Mittleman, Milberg. Drafting of manuscript: Wellenius, Boyle. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Wellenius, Boyle, Mittleman, Schwartz, Coull, Lipsitz. Obtained funding: Wellenius, Mittleman, Lipsitz.

References

  • 1.Laden F, Schwartz J, Speizer FE, et al. Reduction in fine particulate air pollution and mortality: Extended follow-up of the Harvard Six Cities study. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2006;173:667–672. doi: 10.1164/rccm.200503-443OC. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Miller KA, Siscovick DS, Sheppard L, et al. Long-term exposure to air pollution and incidence of cardiovascular events in women. N Engl J Med. 2007;356:447–458. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa054409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Brook RD, Rajagopalan S, Pope CA, 3rd, et al. Particulate matter air pollution and cardiovascular disease: An update to the scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2010;121:2331–2378. doi: 10.1161/CIR.0b013e3181dbece1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kunzli N, Jerrett M, Mack WJ, et al. Ambient air pollution and atherosclerosis in Los Angeles. Environ Health Perspect. 2005;113:201–206. doi: 10.1289/ehp.7523. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Adar SD, Klein R, Klein BE, et al. Air Pollution and the microvasculature: A cross-sectional assessment of in vivo retinal images in the population-based multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis (MESA) PLoS Med. 2010;7:e1000372. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Gorelick PB, Scuteri A, Black SE, et al. Vascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia: A statement for healthcare professionals from the american heart association/american stroke association. Stroke. 2011;42:2672–2713. doi: 10.1161/STR.0b013e3182299496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Power MC, Weisskopf MG, Alexeeff SE, et al. Traffic-related air pollution and cognitive function in a cohort of older men. Environ Health Perspect. 2011;119:682–687. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1002767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Ranft U, Schikowski T, Sugiri D, et al. Long-term exposure to traffic-related particulate matter impairs cognitive function in the elderly. Environ Res. 2009;109:1004–1011. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2009.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Leveille SG, Kiel DP, Jones RN, et al. The MOBILIZE Boston Study: Design and methods of a prospective cohort study of novel risk factors for falls in an older population. BMC Geriatr. 2008;8:16. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-8-16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Washburn RA, Smith KW, Jette AM, et al. The Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE): Development and evaluation. J Clin Epidemiol. 1993;46:153–162. doi: 10.1016/0895-4356(93)90053-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Eggermont LH, Milberg WP, Lipsitz LA, et al. Physical activity and executive function in aging: the MOBILIZE Boston Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2009;57:1750–1756. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2009.02441.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Shapiro AM, Benedict RH, Schretlen D, et al. Construct and concurrent validity of the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-revised. Clin Neuropsychol. 1999;13:348–358. doi: 10.1076/clin.13.3.348.1749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Glisky EL, Kong LL. Do young and older adults rely on different processes in source memory tasks? A neuropsychological study. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2008;34:809–822. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.809. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Pugh KG, Kiely DK, Milberg WP, et al. Selective impairment of frontal-executive cognitive function in african americans with cardiovascular risk factors. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51:1439–1444. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2003.51463.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Grande L, Milberg W, Rodolph J, et al. A Timely Screening for Executive Functions and Memory. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2005;11:S9–10. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Auchincloss AH, Roux AV, Dvonch JT, et al. Associations between Recent Exposure to Ambient Fine Particulate Matter and Blood Pressure in the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA) Environ Health Perspect. 2008;116:486–491. doi: 10.1289/ehp.10899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Gryparis A, Coull BA, Schwartz J, et al. Semiparametric latent variable regression models for spatiotemporal modelling of mobile source particles in the greater Boston area. J Roy Stat Soc C-App. 2007;56:183–209. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Suglia SF, Gryparis A, Wright RO, et al. Association of black carbon with cognition among children in a prospective birth cohort study. Am J Epidemiol. 2008;167:280–286. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Freire C, Ramos R, Puertas R, et al. Association of traffic-related air pollution with cognitive development in children. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2010;64:223–228. doi: 10.1136/jech.2008.084574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Weuve J, Puett RC, Schwartz J, et al. Exposure to particulate air pollution and cognitive decline in older women. Arch Intern Med. 2012;172:219–227. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2011.683. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Tonne C, Melly S, Mittleman M, et al. A case-control analysis of exposure to traffic and acute myocardial infarction. Environ Health Perspect. 2007;115:53–57. doi: 10.1289/ehp.9587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Wellenius GA, Burger MR, Coull BA, et al. Ambient air pollution and the risk of acute ischemic stroke. Arch Intern Med. 2012;172:229–234. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2011.732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Maheswaran R, Elliott P. Stroke mortality associated with living near main roads in England and wales: A geographical study. Stroke. 2003;34:2776–2780. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000101750.77547.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Rioux CL, Tucker KL, Mwamburi M, et al. Residential traffic exposure, pulse pressure, and c-reactive protein: Consistency and contrast among exposure characterization methods. Environ Health Perspect. 2010;118:803–811. doi: 10.1289/ehp.0901182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Delfino RJ, Staimer N, Tjoa T, et al. Circulating biomarkers of inflammation, antioxidant activity, and platelet activation are associated with primary combustion aerosols in subjects with coronary artery disease. Environ Health Perspect. 2008;116:898–906. doi: 10.1289/ehp.11189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Calderon-Garciduenas L, Azzarelli B, Acuna H, et al. Air pollution and brain damage. Toxicol Pathol. 2002;30:373–389. doi: 10.1080/01926230252929954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Calderon-Garciduenas L, Mora-Tiscareno A, Ontiveros E, et al. Air pollution, cognitive deficits and brain abnormalities: A pilot study with children and dogs. Brain Cogn. 2008;68:117–127. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Oberdorster G, Sharp Z, Atudorei V, et al. Translocation of inhaled ultrafine particles to the brain. Inhal Toxicol. 2004;16:437–445. doi: 10.1080/08958370490439597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Calderon-Garciduenas L, Maronpot RR, Torres-Jardon R, et al. DNA damage in nasal and brain tissues of canines exposed to air pollutants is associated with evidence of chronic brain inflammation and neurodegeneration. Toxicol Pathol. 2003;31:524–538. doi: 10.1080/01926230390226645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Stansfeld SA, Berglund B, Clark C, et al. Aircraft and road traffic noise and children’s cognition and health: A cross-national study. Lancet. 2005;365:1942–1949. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)66660-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

RESOURCES