Abstract
Contact zones between subspecies or closely related species offer valuable insights into speciation processes. A typical feature of such zones is the presence of clinal variation in multiple traits. The nature of these traits and the concordance among clines are expected to influence whether and how quickly speciation will proceed. Learned signals, such as vocalizations in species having vocal learning (e.g. humans, many birds, bats and cetaceans), can exhibit rapid change and may accelerate reproductive isolation between populations. Therefore, particularly strong concordance among clines in learned signals and population genetic structure may be expected, even among continuous populations in the early stages of speciation. However, empirical evidence for this pattern is often limited because differences in vocalisations between populations are driven by habitat differences or have evolved in allopatry. We tested for this pattern in a unique system where we may be able to separate effects of habitat and evolutionary history. We studied geographic variation in the vocalizations of the crimson rosella (Platycercus elegans) parrot species complex. Parrots are well known for their life-long vocal learning and cognitive abilities. We analysed contact calls across a ca 1300 km transect encompassing populations that differed in neutral genetic markers and plumage colour. We found steep clinal changes in two acoustic variables (fundamental frequency and peak frequency position). The positions of the two clines in vocal traits were concordant with a steep cline in microsatellite-based genetic variation, but were discordant with the steep clines in mtDNA, plumage and habitat. Our study provides new evidence that vocal variation, in a species with vocal learning, can coincide with areas of restricted gene flow across geographically continuous populations. Our results suggest that traits that evolve culturally can be strongly associated with reduced gene flow between populations, and therefore may promote speciation, even in the absence of other barriers.
Introduction
A fundamental property of contact zones between subspecies or closely related species is the presence of stable and concordant steep clines in multiple traits. This can arise either from secondary contact of formerly isolated divergent populations or environmentally induced evolutionary divergence along environmental gradients, and can be a stage in the process of speciation [1], [2], [3]. The clines will not remain concordant for long unless the traits are genetically or functionally linked, or if there is a partial barrier to gene flow at or near the centre of the clines, or if the same environmental gradient affects all traits equally [1], [3], [4], [5]. Cline concordance is more likely to be maintained, and the probability of speciation increased, if some of the concordant clines involve traits associated with mate choice and breeding. For example, if vocal signals are used in mate choice, and if vocal traits form steep clines, the individuals from either side of the cline may be less likely to mate with each other if they were to disperse into each other’s ranges [3], [6], [7], [8]. Such partial isolation would be expected to steepen the vocal clines and simultaneously promote divergence of other traits, making speciation more likely [1], [5].
Divergence between populations may be more rapid if such signals are learned rather than genetically determined [3], [7]. Therefore, we expect strong divergence in learned signals relative to other signals, particularly during the early stages of speciation. In several taxa, including higher primates, cetaceans, bats and many birds, vocalizations are learned and are important mating signals [6], [9], [10], [11], [12], [13]. A major prediction is strong concordance between vocal clines and reduced gene flow in such taxa. In humans there are positive associations between vocal signals and genetic variation between populations (e.g. [14], [15], [16], [17]), but there is very limited empirical evidence for concordance between learned vocal variation and population genetic structure in the three groups of birds that display vocal learning [6], [18]: oscines [19], [20], [21], [22], parrots [23], [24] and hummingbirds (no studies to our knowledge). Furthermore, results are often confounded by the influences of habitat, morphology, evolutionary history, or a low level of population genetic structure. Here we report on a parrot species complex in which vocal variation and microsatellite-inferred genetic clines are concordant with each other but not with known plumage, mitochondrial DNA or habitat clines.
Studies of several parrot species to date confirm that their calls are learned throughout their lifetime and fulfil a variety of social functions [13], [25], [26]. Evidence from several species has demonstrated that parrot contact calls are used in mate choice, pair bonding and mate recognition [26], [27], [28], [29], [30], [31]. Australian parrots in the crimson rosella complex (Platycercus elegans) form steep plumage colour and genetic clines, however these clines are discordant [32], [33], [34]. This mismatch occurs close to the area where two phenotypically divergent subspecies, the predominantly red crimson rosella (P. e. elegans) and the contrastingly yellow rosella (P. e. flaveolus), meet in riparian habitats along a major river system and form a narrow hybrid zone [33]; Fig. 1). To the west of this zone for several hundred kilometres, yellow rosellas occur in riparian open woodlands surrounded by grasslands and low open woodlands, whereas to the zone’s east crimson birds essentially inhabit dense woodlands and forests. However, according to our recent microsatellite analysis, yellow birds up to ca 200 km west of the hybrid zone were consistently more similar genetically to crimson populations elsewhere in south-eastern Australia [33].
We intensively sampled these closely related but genetically divergent populations of P. elegans for vocal characters in and on either side of the area where we have previously identified discordance between microsatellite and phenotypic diversity [33]. Our broad aim was to provide insight into the relationships between culturally transmitted and genetic traits, and the role that learned signals may play in population divergence and incipient speciation. A more specific aim was to test whether clinal variation and divergence in vocal characters correlate more strongly with known mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA), plumage and habitat clines, or with previously documented, cryptic population genetic structure in P. elegans.
Materials and Methods
Ethics Statement
This study conformed to the laws for animal experimentation in Australia (Deakin University Animal Ethics Committee approval A33/2008; New South Wales National Parks and Wildlife Service Licence Number S11436 and South Australian Department for Environment and Heritage Permit Number Y24784), and was carried out on privately owned properties with permission.
Study Sites
We recorded 182 free-living individuals from 12 sites along the Murray-Murrumbidgee river system in Australia (Fig. 1). These sites spanned over 1200 km of continuous habitat from Cadell (South Australia), in the western part of the range of the yellow rosella subspecies (P. e. flaveolus), to Canberra (Australian Capital Territory), which lies within the range of the crimson rosella subspecies (P. e. elegans). These sites encompassed two genetic clusters identified by a previous Geneland analysis of 14 microsatellite loci where they were termed CentralGL and EasternGL and two mtDNA (ND2) haplotype groups in earlier work by our group [33] (shown in Fig. 1). The difference in microsatellite markers between individuals at different recording sites was confirmed with an analysis using STRUCTURE [35]. The average distance between adjacent recording sites was >10 km to increase independence between recording sites and to minimize the chance of unknowingly including the same individuals in the analysis more than once [36].
Recording and Acoustic Analyses
We analyzed the P. elegans contact call, which is the most common call produced by rosellas and is highly variable (Figure 1; [36]). Contact calls are commonly studied in parrot communication, and are thought to facilitate a range of social activities and be involved mate preferences (e.g. [25], [26], [27], [28], [29], [30], [31], [37], [38], [39]). We analyzed 556 contact calls recorded from 182 individuals. Sites were visited over the period 2004–2009, and recordings were made during daylight hours (0700–2000 h) in December and January using the methods described in earlier work by our group [36], [40].
Sound files (uncompressed wav format, sampling rate 44.1 kHz, 16-bit) were analyzed with Raven Pro 1.4 (Cornell Lab of Ornithology, New York, USA) and Sound Analysis Pro (SAP) 1.04 [41]. Calls were processed and analyzed in random order and blindly with respect to their population of origin. Up to four clear calls per individual (3.05±1.12) were chosen on the basis of amplitude and background noise, and band pass filtered using Raven to remove noise <0.5kHz. Spectrograms and waveforms of the calls were produced using Raven (windows setting: Blackman; size: 400 samples; DFT size: 2048 samples; overlap: 90%) and Sound Analysis Pro (standard settings; FFT window = 1024 samples; overlap = 85%; spectrum range = 18 kHz). For each contact call we measured five acoustic variables that reflect temporal and structural properties of the call [36]. These are: (1) call duration, measured in Raven using the waveform, (2) peak frequency, the frequency with greatest amplitude as measured automatically in Raven, (3) fundamental frequency, the fundamental frequency of harmonically rich notes that formed the longest part of the call measured manually in Raven, (4) mean frequency modulation (FM), measured automatically in SAP, which indicates the deviation of frequencies in the call (in degrees) in comparison to a constant frequency, and (5) peak frequency pattern, which is the temporal location of the peak frequency within each call, measured automatically in Raven, and presented as a percentage of the total duration of the call. We used an individual mean for each of the five acoustic variables in statistical analyses. Information on intra-individual differences between calls is available in Ribot et al. (2009).
Statistical Analyses
The five acoustic variables were tested separately, as combining the five acoustic variables using a principal component (PC) analysis would only allow for a small reduction in variables (PC 1 explained 32.9% of the variation, PC 2 explained 24.3%, PC 3 explained 20.3%) and the pairwise correlation coefficients between the acoustic variables were typically low (0.033< r <0.378). P-values are two-tailed and results were considered significant when P<0.05.
We used the cline fitting software C-fit [42] to analyze the acoustic and microsatellite clines. We used the vocalization data directly. For the microsatellite data we used STRUCTURE to estimate the probability of inferred ancestry for each individual, with parameters k = 2 (2 groups assumed), 74 individuals, 15 loci, 100000 burn-in period, 100000 replications, LOCPRIOR model (longitudes). The overall proportion of membership in the two inferred clusters was 0.48 and 0.52, with an estimated allele frequency divergence among populations of 0.0315, and r = 0.86. The probability of each individual being in group 1 formed the cline we analyzed with C-fit (Figure 1). C-fit fits cline models to data assuming a unimodal (one species), bimodal (two species with little or no hybridization) or a trimodal (two species or semispecies with hybridization) model. Models can either be fit to single trait clines, or simultaneously to two or more clines. The Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) is then used to decide which is the best fitting model [42]. We fit clines to the acoustic variables and the microsatellite clines separately, to test which modal model fitted these traits best. We tested the two vocal traits with steep clines jointly for comparison with the sum of the AIC of the single-fitted traits to test for concordance of the two vocal traits [42]. We also tested for concordance between the microsatellite and two steep vocal trait clines.
Mantel tests, implemented in IBD 1.52 (1000 randomizations; [43]), were used to analyze acoustic distances versus geographic distances between the 12 recording sites. For call distances, we calculated Euclidean distances between the recording sites using the means of each acoustic variable for each site. Geographic distances between recording sites were calculated following the major path of the river and suitable habitat (1281 km).
Results
Call Structure
In most cases, contact calls consisted of harmonically rich notes (92%, n = 514), with the fundamental frequency, where measured, typically falling into one of two frequency ranges, namely 0.7–1.0 kHz or 2.0–3.5 kHz (95%, n = 489).
Acoustic Variation Across the Microsatellite Contact Zone
Fundamental frequency, mean frequency modulation (FM) and peak frequency pattern varied clinally across the transect (Fig. 2). Fundamental frequency and peak frequency pattern formed steep clines which were roughly concordant with the steep microsatellite cline (Fig. 2): C-fit analysis [42] showed that, although the two steep vocal trait clines and the microsatellite clines were roughly concordant in position and highly discordant with plumage and mtDNA, their fitted midpoints were still significantly different (Table 1). The position order is (from West to East): fundamental frequency, microsatellite score and peak frequency pattern, a gap, mtDNA haplotype group, and then plumage coloration (Fig. 2).
Table 1. Tests for fit of unimodal, bimodal and trimodal cline models for each variable (A), tests for common position of two vocalization traits with steep clines (B), and tests for common position of the vocalization clines and the microsatellite cline (C).
Test | Variable(s) | Model | df | AIC |
(A) Clines of single traits | Fundamental frequency | Unimodal | 7 | 209.34 |
Bimodal | 8 | −23.3 | ||
Trimodal | 14 | −52.36* | ||
Peak frequency pattern | Unimodal | 7 | −111.94 | |
Bimodal | 8 | −145.63* | ||
Trimodal | 11 | −145.13* | ||
Duration | Unimodal | 7 | −691.09 | |
Bimodal | 8 | −785.66* | ||
Trimodal | 14 | −784.53* | ||
Peak frequency | Unimodal | 7 | 469.55 | |
Bimodal | 8 | 462.59* | ||
Trimodal | 14 | 487.73 | ||
Mean frequency modulation | Unimodal | 7 | 1117.03 | |
Bimodal | 8 | 1112.7* | ||
Trimodal | 14 | 1119.74 | ||
Microsatellite assignment | Unimodal | 6 | −59.6 | |
Bimodal | 8 | −143.91* | ||
Trimodal | 14 | −136.2 | ||
Trimodal (hybrid deficiency in east) | 11 | −204.2* | ||
(B) Common position of vocal clines | Fundamental frequency and Peak frequency pattern | Unimodal, Common | 7 | 668.47 |
Unimodal, Common position only | 13 | 113.7 | ||
Unimodal, Common position+slope | 12 | 113.42 | ||
Bimodal, Common | 8 | 431.82 | ||
Bimodal, Common position only | 16 | −120.77 | ||
Bimodal, Common position+slope | 15 | −133.86 | ||
Trimodal, Common | 14 | 417.38 | ||
Trimodal, Common position only | 27 | −139.39 | ||
Trimodal, Common position+slope | 26 | −135.66 | ||
Sum of AIC for vocal traits fitted singly | Trimodal | −197.49* | ||
(C) Common position of vocal and microsatellite clines | Peak frequency pattern and microsatellite assignment | Trimodal, Joint fits | 14 | −194.59 |
Peak frequency pattern+microsatellite assignment | Sums of single trait fits | −281.7* | ||
Fundamental frequency and microsatellite assignment | Trimodal, Joint fits | 14 | 281.41 | |
Fundamental frequency+microsatellite assignment | Sums of single trait fits | −188.56* |
For clines of single traits (A), unimodal models were never the best fits. A trimodal model fit best for fundamental frequency, a bimodal model fit best for peak frequency and mean FM, bimodal and trimodal models fit equally well for peak frequency pattern and duration. For microsatellite assignment, a bimodal model fit best but a trimodal model with a deficiency of hybrids in the east was better still.
For tests of common position of the vocal clines (B), individual models fit better than pooled models. A trimodal model with an assumption of common position was the best joint fit.
For tests of common position of vocal clines and the microsatellite cline (C), individual models fit better than joint models.
indicates best fitting models.
C-fit analysis of both the vocalization and microsatellite clines (Table 1) showed that unimodal models never fit as well as the alternative bimodal and/or trimodal models, indicating that some form of hybridization is occurring, rather than these traits forming simple clines. The trimodal models fit best for fundamental frequency, the trimodal and bimodal models fit equally well for peak frequency pattern and duration, and the bimodal model had the best fit for peak frequency and mean FM. Unlike the vocal traits, the best fitting models for microsatellites depended upon more assumptions. The best fitting model for microsatellites was trimodal when we constrained the model [42] to have deficiency of hybrids in the east. This model fits better than any other model, including deficiencies in the middle or west, or no deficiencies (Table 1; Fig. S1 and S2). Trimodal models suggest that backcrossing is limited and the populations are partially isolated [42].
A Mantel test using all five acoustic parameters at each site provided evidence for significant isolation-by-distance across the transect (Fig. 3). This test is based on non- straight line (i.e. river-course) geographic distance between recording sites; similar results were obtained when using straight line geographic distances along the river (r = 0.41, P = 0.025). Mantel tests conducted on each acoustic variable separately (Fig. S3) indicated a pattern of isolation-by-distance which occurred in fundamental frequency and time of peak frequency in call (r>0.337, P<0.05), and possibly mean FM (r = 0.338, P<0.1).
Discussion
In this study we showed that learned vocal variation in P. elegans is spatially correlated with microsatellite-based genetic population structure far more strongly than with plumage coloration and mtDNA diversity over a 1300 km transect [33]. To our knowledge, this is the first demonstration of this pattern. Moreover, our study reveals some of the clearest evidence to date for a relationship between presumably neutral genetic variation and vocal variation in any species that displays vocal learning. This supports the notion that vocal learning of birds may play an important role in speciation [20], [44], [45], [46], [47].
C-fit allows identification of clines, tests of concordance, and comparison of unimodal, bimodal and trimodal models [42]. Based on our C-fit analysis, we confidently reject the unimodal scenario. This is because, for all vocal traits considered and for the microsatellite-inferred genetic cline, the bimodal, and in some cases the trimodal, models provided clearly better fits to the data (Table 1). The better fit with the bimodal and/or trimodal models shown in this study suggests that the contact zone, rather than forming simple clines, has two or three sets of genotypes: parental types and hybrids [42]. This is what one would expect during incipient speciation. A trimodal model would best fit the microsatellite genotypes as in the vocal traits where hybrids are starting to become distinct rather than being lost in a continuum of intergrades. At a slightly later stage of speciation reproductive isolation would increase and reduce the incidence of hybrids, leading to the bimodal model being a better fit than the trimodal model. The microsatellites appear to be somewhere between the two with a good fit to either the bimodal or trimodal model and a deficiency to the east. The latter may result from interaction with the other two clines to the east.
There are at least two possible explanations for steep vocal trait clines being roughly congruent with the steep, presumably neutral microsatellite cline, but very discordant with both plumage coloration and mtDNA variation. The simplest hypothesis is that secondary contact occurred between eastern and western populations and that the vocalization and microsatellite clines occur roughly where the contact first occurred. This could result in unimodal, bimodal or trimodal models fitting best depending on the extent of gene flow between the formerly isolated populations. The clines in plumage coloration and mtDNA may have moved eastward until they reached the present transition between open riparian woodland and higher elevation forest [48], [49], [50]; different visual backgrounds then potentially would have caused geographical selection gradients balanced by gene flow and a temporarily moving cline which stopped at the centre of the habitat gradient. This would constitute habitat matching (8, 16–19) for plumage. Environmentally dependent variation in colour vision [51], [52] may also contribute to plumage colour variation due to sensory drive. In addition, mtDNA clines are often displaced relative to nuclear-based clines due to sex-related differences in dispersal or introgression [53], [54]. Under this scenario of a moving hybrid zone for plumage coloration, the microsatellite and vocal clines remain as a “ghost of introgression past” [50]. Unlike P. elegans colour variation, habitat matching (8, 16–19) is unlikely to explain the vocal clines because acoustic variation was not concordant with known habitat or topographical variation: steep environmental gradients occur much further east of the vocalization and microsatellite clines, where crimson and yellow birds currently meet [48], [49].
A second hypothesis is that the vocal traits differentiated at random and that this promoted pre-zygotic reproductive isolation arising from mate choice, which is among the functions demonstrated for parrot contact calls [26], [27], [29], [30]. This would subsequently promote differentiation in the microsatellites and other traits on either side of the cline. If the other traits were involved in genetic compatibility then this would accelerate concordant differentiation in both sets of traits [7]. This would most likely result in the bimodal or trimodal models fitting best, because reproductive isolation has already started to occur. The colour traits may have already responded to the environmental gradient further east, although this does not explain the location of mtDNA divergence. It is invalid to assume that different kinds of traits necessarily respond to hybridization in the same way [55]. Based on our present data, it is difficult to distinguish these two hypotheses because it depends on the level of reproductive isolation that has evolved. Importantly, our analysis of vocal and microsatellite clines suggests that the clines signal an early step in the process of speciation before complete reproductive isolation has occurred; this would be true regardless of the extent to which either of the two historical alternatives outlined above may apply.
To date, the studies by Wright et al. [23], [24] on yellow-naped amazon parrots (Amazona auropalliata) are among the most comprehensive in examining associations between avian vocal variation and genetic population structure. However, these studies did not find any evidence that geographic variation in contact calls [38] was related to genetic differences between populations, measured with mtDNA [23] and microsatellites [24]. While they detected very little neutral genetic variation between populations using microsatellite data, Wright et al. [24] suggested that the most likely reason for these results is because parrots are open-ended vocal learners and can learn the dialects of new populations after they disperse. A study of mountain white-crowned sparrows [44] indicated that acoustic differences between populations explained a significant amount of microsatellite variation between populations. However, the amount of genetic variation associated with dialects was extremely small (<1%) and much lower than that explained by variation among individuals within recording sites (>98%). Further, a subsequent study by Soha et al. [19] in another subspecies of white-crowned sparrows did not detect a significant relationship between population genetic structure and acoustics at all. They suggested that this could be due to recent population divergence, as they also observed little genetic differentiation overall [19].
A likely explanation for the lack of concordance between dialects and population genetic structure reported in these and other previous studies [19], [20], [21], [22], [23], [24], is that most studies have examined populations with clear vocal dialects but little or no population genetic structure, or used too few or inappropriate molecular markers to detect existing genetic variation. Thus, most previous studies set out to test whether existing dialect boundaries limit gene flow and thus promote genetic differentiation. In some cases, an insufficient amount of time may have passed since the formation of dialects for genetic differences to become evident (e.g. [19], [20]). Learned avian acoustic signals have the potential to change rapidly in birds [6], [7], [56], [57], so markers with relatively rapid mutation rates are more likely to correspond with recent divergence in acoustic signals and events in recent population history. Investigating vocal variation in a species with previously observed distinct genetic variation between populations, as we have done in this study, may provide a more reliable way of testing whether vocal variation and genetic differences are linked.
Furthermore, habitat can determine which acoustic features of vocalizations will be favoured [58], [59], either directly through its effect on which types of sounds are transmitted the best, or indirectly through selection on the morphology of sound-producing structures [60]. Most studies so far have concluded that the social relations between individuals and habitat-dependent selection are more important factors affecting vocal divergence between populations than population genetic structure [10]. However, acoustic traits that are less susceptible to ecological change (e.g. those that are less affected by habitat structure, those with a stronger genetic basis and those more dependent on syringeal structure; [61]) could still be expected to show an association with genetic structure.
Conclusions
The hypothesis that learned signals, such as many avian and mammal vocalizations, play a role in promoting population divergence and ultimately speciation remains contentious, and to date clear empirical evidence is still largely lacking. A key prediction of this hypothesis, however, is the concordant geographical patterns of signal variation and gene flow in the early stages of speciation. In this study we show that in P. elegans, rapid geographical change in contact calls coincides with a steep, microsatellite-inferred (and presumably neutral) genetic cline across geographically continuous populations. Furthermore, these clines occur ca 100 km west of steep clines in mtDNA, plumage coloration and habitat. Our study provides new evidence that vocal variation, in a species with vocal learning, coincides with zones of relatively reduced gene flow even in the absence of barriers associated with morphological, habitat or mtDNA differences between populations. Our findings suggest that traits that may rapidly evolve culturally can be strongly associated with recent genetic divergence between populations, and therefore support the hypothesis that such traits can play a key role in promoting speciation. Whether these differences in calls are being used by individuals to discriminate between populations in the area where the genetic discontinuity was identified is unknown, and will require further research.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
We thank J. H. Bennett, P. Bird, J. Bradbury, S. Cooper, S. Donnellan, E. Krebs, R. Magrath, D. Paton, M. Penck, and R. Sinclair, for stimulating discussions about Platycercus elegans. We also thank L. Raijmakers for help in the field, and C. Sherman for help with the analysis. We are grateful to the many landowners and managers who allowed us to work on their properties, and to the University of Adelaide for material support. We thank T. Lenormand for giving us a copy of CFit and for advice in using this software.
Funding Statement
This work was funded by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (grant B01568 to ATDB, KLB and MLB), a Leverhulme Fellowship (ATDB), the Lady Emily Smyth Agricultural Research Station (RFHR and ATDB), a University of Bristol postgraduate scholarship (RFHR), and Deakin University. All work conformed to the laws of Australia and the United Kingdom. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Endler JA (1977) Geographic variation, speciation, and clines. Princeton: Princeton University Press. [PubMed]
- 2.Coyne JA, Orr HA (2004) Speciation. Sunderland: Sinauer associates.
- 3.Price T (2008) Speciation in birds. Greenwood Village, CO: Roberts and Company Publishers.
- 4. Barton NH (1979) The dynamics of hybrid zones. Heredity 43: 1–359. [Google Scholar]
- 5. Barton NH, Hewitt GM (1985) Analysis of hybrid zones. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 16: 113–148. [Google Scholar]
- 6. Slabbekoorn H, Smith TB (2002) Bird song, ecology and speciation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 357: 493–503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Edwards SV, Kingan SB, Calkins JD, Balakrishnan CN, Jennings WB, et al. (2005) Speciation in birds: genes, geography, and sexual selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102: 6550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Irwin DE, Bensch S, Price TD (2001) Speciation in a ring. Nature 409: 333–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Slabbekoorn H, Smith TB (2002) Bird song, ecology and speciation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 357: 493–503%U http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/cgi/doi/410.1098/rstb.2001.1056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 10.Marler P, Slabbekoorn H (2004) Nature’s Music: The science of birdsong. San Diego: Elsevier Academic Press.
- 11. Behr O, von Helversen O (2004) Bat serenades - complex courtship songs of the sac-winged bat (Saccopteryx bilineata). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 56: 106–115. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Janik VM (1999) Origins and implications of vocal learning in bottlenose dolphins. In: Box HO, Gibson KR, editors. Mammalian social learning: comparative and ecological perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 308–325.
- 13. Berg KS, Delgado S, Cortopassi KA, Beissinger SR, Bradbury JW (2012) Vertical transmission of learned signatures in a wild parrot. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 279: 585–591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Hunley K, Dunn M, Lindstrom E, Reesink G, Terrill A, et al. (2008) Genetic and linguistic co-evolution in Northern Island Melanesia. Plos Genetics 4: e1000239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Cavalli-Sforza LL (1997) Genes, peoples, and languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 94: 7719–7724. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Wang S, Lewis CM, Jakobsson M, Ramachandran S, Ray N, et al. (2007) Genetic variation and population structure in Native Americans. Plos Genetics 3: 2049–2067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Barbujani G, Sokal RR (1990) Zones of sharp genetic change in Europe are also linguistic boundaries. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 87: 1816–1819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Catchpole CK, Slater PJB (2008) Bird Song: Biological Themes and Variations. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
- 19. Soha JA, Nelson DA, Parker PG (2004) Genetic analysis of song dialect populations in Puget Sound white-crowned sparrows. Behavioral Ecology 15: 636–646. [Google Scholar]
- 20. Nicholls JA, Austin JJ, Moritz C, Goldizen AW (2006) Genetic population structure and call variation in a passerine bird, the satin bowerbird, Ptilonorhynchus violaceus . Evolution 60: 1279–1290. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Lougheed SC, Handford P (1992) Vocal dialects and the structure of geographic variation in morphological and allozymic characters in the rufous-collared sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis . Evolution 46: 1443–1456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Baker MC (1975) Song dialects and genetic differences in white-crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys). Evolution 29: 226–241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Wright TF, Wilkinson GS (2001) Population genetic structure and vocal dialects in an amazon parrot. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 268: 609–616. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Wright TF, Rodriguez AM, Fleischer RC (2005) Vocal dialects, sex-biased dispersal, and microsatellite population structure in the parrot Amazona auropalliata . Molecular Ecology 14: 1197–1205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Wanker R, Sugama Y, Prinage S (2005) Vocal labelling of family members in spectacled parrotlets, Forpus conspicillatus . Animal Behaviour 70: 111–118. [Google Scholar]
- 26. Hile AG, Plummer TK, Striedter GF (2000) Male vocal imitation produces call con vergence during pair bonding in budgerigars, Melopsittacus undulatus . Animal Behaviour 59: 1209–1218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Moravec ML, Striedter GF, Burley NT (2006) Assortative pairing based on contact call similarity in budgerigars, Melopsittacus undulatus . Ethology 112: 1108–1116. [Google Scholar]
- 28. Eda-Fujiwara H, Kanesada A, Okamoto Y, Satoh R, Watanabe A, et al. (2011) Long-term maintenance and eventual extinction of preference for a mate’s call in the female budgerigar. Animal Behaviour 82: 971–979. [Google Scholar]
- 29. Moravec ML, Striedter GF, Burley NT (2010) ‘Virtual Parrots’ Confirm Mating Preferences of Female Budgerigars. Ethology 116: 961–971. [Google Scholar]
- 30. Balsby TJS, Adams DM (2011) Vocal similarity and familiarity determine response to potential flockmates in orange-fronted conures (Psittacidae). Animal Behaviour 81: 983–991. [Google Scholar]
- 31. Berg KS, Delgado S, Okawa R, Beissinger SR, Bradbury JW (2011) Contact calls are used for individual mate recognition in free-ranging green-rumped parrotlets, Forpus passerinus . Animal Behaviour 81: 241–248. [Google Scholar]
- 32. Irwin DE, Irwin JH, Price TD (2001) Ring species as bridges between microevolution and speciation. Genetica 112: 223–243. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Joseph L, Dolman G, Donnellan S, Saint KM, Berg ML, et al. (2008) Where and when does a ring start and end? Testing the ring-species hypothesis in a species complex of Australian parrots. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 275: 2431–2440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Berg ML, Bennett ATD (2010) The evolution of plumage colouration in parrots: a review. Emu 110: 10–20. [Google Scholar]
- 35. Pritchard JK, Stephens M, Donnelly P (2000) Inference of Population Structure Using Multilocus Genotype Data. Genetics 155: 945–959. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Ribot RFH, Berg ML, Buchanan KL, Komdeur J, Joseph L, et al. (2009) Does the ring species concept predict vocal variation in the crimson rosella, Platycercus elegans, complex? Animal Behaviour 77: 581–593. [Google Scholar]
- 37. Saunders D (1983) Vocal repertoire and individual vocal recognition in the Short-billed White-tailed Black Cockatoo, Calyptorhynchus funereus latirostris Carnaby . Australian Wildlife Research 10: 527–536. [Google Scholar]
- 38. Wright TF (1996) Regional dialects in the contact call of a parrot. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 263: 867–872. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bradbury JW (2003) Vocal communication in parrots. In: De Waal FBM, Tyack PL, editors. Animal social complexity: Intelligence, culture, and individualized societies Cambridge: Harvard University Press. 293–316.
- 40. Ribot RFH, Berg ML, Buchanan KL, Bennett ATD (2011) Fruitful use of bioacoustic alarm stimuli as a deterrent for Crimson Rosellas (Platycercus elegans). Emu 111: 360–367. [Google Scholar]
- 41. Tchernichovski O, Nottebohm F, Ho CE, Bijan P, Partha Pratim M (2000) A procedure for an automated measurement of song similarity. Animal Behaviour 59: 1167–1176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Gay L, Crochet PA, Bell DA, Lenormand T (2008) Comparing clines on molecular and phenotypic traits in hybrid zones: a window on tension zone models. Evolution 62: 2789–2806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Bohonak A (2002) IBD (isolation by distance): a program for analyses of isolation by distance. Journal of Heredity 93: 153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. MacDougall-Shackleton EA, MacDougall-Shackleton SA (2001) Cultural and genetic evolution in mountain white-crowned sparrows: Song dialects are associated with population structure. Evolution 55: 2568–2575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Patten MA, Rotenberry JT, Zuk M (2004) Habitat selection, acoustic adaptation, and the evolution of reproductive isolation. Evolution 58: 2144–2155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Irwin DE, Thimgan MP, Irwin JH (2008) Call divergence is correlated with geographic and genetic distance in greenish warblers (Phylloscopus trochiloides): a strong role for stochasticity in signal evolution? Journal of Evolutionary Biology 21: 435–448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Balaban E (1988) Cultural and genetic variation in swamp sparrows (Melospiza georgiana): I. Song Variation, genetic Variation, and their relationship. Behaviour 105: 250–291. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Higgins PJ (1999) Handbook of Australian, New Zealand & Antarctic Birds. Vol. 4, Parrots to Dollarbird. South Melbourne: Oxford University Press.
- 49.Forshaw JM, Cooper WT (2002) Australian parrots. Robina: Alexander Editions.
- 50. Rheindt FE, Edwards SV (2011) Genetic introgression: An integral but neglected component of speciation in birds. Auk 128: 620–632. [Google Scholar]
- 51. Knott B, Berg ML, Morgan ER, Buchanan KL, Bowmaker JK, et al. (2010) Avian retinal oil droplets: dietary manipulation of colour vision? Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 277: 953–962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Knott B, Bowmaker J, Berg M, Bennett AD (2012) Absorbance of retinal oil droplets of the budgerigar: sex, spatial and plumage morph-related variation. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 198: 43–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Prugnolle F, De Meeus T (2002) Inferring sex-biased dispersal from population genetic tools: a review. Heredity 88: 161–165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Avise JC (2004) Molecular markers, natural history, and evolution. Sunderland: Sinauer Associates.
- 55. Brumfield RT, Jernigan RW, McDonald DB, Braun MJ (2001) Evolutionary implications of divergent clines in an avian (Manacus: Aves) hybrid zone. Evolution 55: 2070–2087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Derryberry EP (2009) Ecology Shapes Birdsong Evolution: Variation in Morphology and Habitat Explains Variation in White-Crowned Sparrow Song. American Naturalist 174: 24–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Holland J, McGregor PK, Rowe CL (1996) Changes in microgeographic song variation of the Corn Bunting Miliaria calandra . Journal of Avian Biology 27: 47–55. [Google Scholar]
- 58. Morton ES (1975) Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds. American Naturalist 109: 17–34. [Google Scholar]
- 59. Wiley R, Richards D (1978) Physical constraints on acoustic communication in the atmosphere: implications for the evolution of animal vocalizations. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 3: 69–94. [Google Scholar]
- 60. Podos J (2001) Correlated evolution of morphology and vocal signal structure in Darwin’s finches. Nature 409: 185–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. McCracken KG, Sheldon FH (1997) Avian vocalizations and phylogenetic signal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 94: 3833–3836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.