Abstract
There is limited research on the association between participation in cognitively stimulating activity and cognitive function in older Hispanics. The main purpose of the present study was to explore whether frequency of cognitive activity and its association with cognitive function in Hispanics is comparable to that of non-Hispanics. In a multiethnic cohort of 1571 non-demented older adults, we assessed past and current cognitive activity, availability of cognitive resources in the home in childhood and middle age, and five domains of cognitive function. The measures of cognitive activity and cognitive resources had adequate reliability and validity in our subset of Hispanic participants (n = 81). Hispanics reported lower levels of education, lower frequency of cognitive activity and less cognitive resources than non-Hispanic White (n = 1102) and non-Hispanic Black (n = 388) participants. Despite these differences the strength of the association between cognitive activity and cognitive function was comparable across ethnic groups. Because Hispanics have lower frequency of cognitive activity, the benefit of cognitive activity to late life cognitive function may be potentially larger in this segment of the population. Thus, interventions aimed at increasing frequency of participation in cognitively stimulating activity may offer a potential target to reduce cognitive impairment in Hispanics.
Keywords: Cognition, Aging, Epidemiologic studies, Cross-sectional studies, Latinos, Leisure activity
INTRODUCTION
Several studies in older adults have shown more frequent participation in cognitive activities to be related to better cognitive function (Christensen & Mackinnon, 1993; Hultsch, Hammer, & Small, 1993; Lachman, Agrigoroaei, Murphy, & Tun, 2010; Wilson et al., 1999), reduced cognitive decline (Hultsch, Hertzog, Small, & Dixon, 1999; Wilson et al., 2003), and reduced risk of dementia (Stern & Munn, 2010; Verghese et al., 2003; Wang, Karp, Winblad, & Fratiglioni, 2002; Wilson, Mendes de Leon, et al., 2002). Although the mechanisms underlying these associations are not well understood, cognitive stimulating activity has been posited to contribute to cognitive reserve. The concept of cognitive reserve proposes that individual differences in the efficiency and flexibility of cognitive systems might explain individual differences in the effect of brain pathology on functioning. Participation in cognitive and other leisure activities, along with education, and occupational attainment, are at least some of the variables that might contribute to cognitive reserve (Stern, 2002, 2006).
Most studies on the association between cognitive activity and cognitive function in non-demented older adults have been in predominantly non-Hispanic Whites (e.g., Hultsch et al., 1999; Verghese et al., 2003; Wang et al., 2002; Wilson et al., 2005), and a small number in primarily non-Hispanic Blacks (Barnes, Wilson, Mendes de Leon, & Bennett, 2006; Dotson, Schinka, Brown, Mortimer, & Borenstein, 2008). Studies in geographically defined biracial samples have found less frequent engagement in cognitive activity in Blacks than Whites (Wilson et al., 1999; Wilson, Bennett, et al., 2002), but similar associations of cognitive activity to cognitive decline (Wilson et al., 2003) and incident dementia (Wilson, Bennett, et al., 2002). Very few studies have examined the association between cognitive activity and cognition in older Hispanics (Herrera et al., 2011; Scarmeas, Levy, Tang, Manly, & Stern, 2001). In a multiethnic cohort (Scarmeas et al., 2001), Hispanics reported less frequent engagement in leisure activity as compared to non-Hispanic Blacks and Whites in a measure that included cognitive, physical and social activities, with no significant differences between Blacks and Whites. After adjusting for ethnicity and other demographic variables, higher frequency of engagement in cognitive activity in late life was associated with reduced risk of incident dementia. Another study explored frequency of engagement in leisure activities, some of which were cognitive activities, in a demographically matched sample of Hispanic and non-Hispanic White older women (Herrera et al., 2011). Participants in both groups reported similar frequency of engagement in most cognitive activities, except use of computers, which was lower in Hispanics. Leisure activities were differentially associated with better cognition in Hispanic and non-Hispanic White women and the findings were interpreted as evidence that Hispanic ethnicity might impact the relationship between cognitive activity and cognitive function (Herrera et al., 2011). Overall, the scant data on older Hispanics indicates that frequency of engagement in cognitive activity, at least in some cases, might be lower in Hispanics than other ethnic groups. However, whether there might still be a significant association between cognitive activity and cognitive function despite lower activity levels remains an unanswered question.
According to data from the U.S. Census Bureau, Hispanics represent the ethnic group with the highest growth within the older adult population of the United States. They are expected to become the largest minority group by 2050, representing 20% of older adults (U.S. Census Bureau, 2008). Despite these statistics, research on cognitive aging in Hispanics has been limited. The few available studies on a geographically defined population showed that although Hispanics might be at higher risk for mild cognitive impairment (Manly et al., 2008) and dementia (Gurland et al., 1999; Tang et al., 2001), this might not be the case for all subgroups of Hispanics (Haan et al., 2003). Projected increases in the numbers of elderly Hispanics, and the higher risk for cognitive problems in this group, highlight the importance of understanding factors that might impact age-related cognitive impairment in this segment of the population. Cognitive activity might prove to be an important one.
There were two main purposes to the present study. First, we were interested in exploring whether frequency of cognitive activity and its association with cognitive function in Hispanics was comparable to that of other ethnic groups, that is, non-Hispanic Whites and Blacks. Based on prior findings (Herrera et al., 2011; Scarmeas et al., 2001), we hypothesized that while Hispanics might engage in cognitive activity less often, the association between more frequent engagement in cognitive activity and better cognition would be comparable to that of other ethnic groups. Understanding the association between cognitive activity and cognitive function in older Hispanics requires appropriate measures of the constructs in this group. The cognitive battery used in this study has been previously validated for a Hispanic population (Krueger, Wilson, Bennett, & Aggarwal, 2009). Thus, the second purpose of the current study was to examine the psychometric properties of a previously developed cognitive activity scale (Wilson et al., 2005).
METHOD
Participants
Participants were subjects from the Memory and Aging Project (MAP) or the Minority Aging Research Study (MARS) who were free of dementia, identified themselves as being Hispanic, non-Hispanic White, or non-Hispanic Black, and had valid measures of global cognitive function and lifetime cognitive activity. Details of these studies have been described elsewhere (Arvanitakis, Bennett, Wilson, & Barnes, 2010; Bennett et al., 2005). Briefly, these are ongoing longitudinal studies of older adults with similar study designs, recruitment techniques and data collection procedures (Boyle, Barnes, Buchman, & Bennett, 2009; James, Boyle, Buchman, Barnes, & Bennett, 2011). Participants undergo assessment of risk factors and annual clinical evaluations. Clinical evaluations include a medical history, neurological examination, and cognitive and motor function testing. The diagnosis of dementia is done by a physician on the basis of the results of the clinical evaluation, and following the criteria set forth by the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association (McKhann et al., 1984).
Of the 1856 people enrolled in MAP and MARS, eight identified their ethnicity as something other than Hispanic or non-Hispanic White or Black, 86 had dementia, three did not have valid measures of global cognition and 162 were missing the lifetime cognitive activity measure, which was not collected in MAP until 4 years after study onset, so these participants were excluded from further analyses. This left 1571 older adults (73.45% female). Participants had a mean age of 78.36 years (SD = 7.23) and a mean education of 14.48 years (SD = 3.33). Study procedures were approved by an Internal Review Board.
Materials and Procedures
Participants completed all measures in their primary language (English or Spanish), as assessed by self-report.
Assessment of cognitive activity frequency
Participants completed a scale assessing frequency of cognitive activity in four life epochs: childhood (ages 6 and 12), transition to adulthood (18 years), middle of adulthood (40 years), and current age. Details about the scale have been published previously (Wilson et al., 2005). Briefly, items included activities such as reading newspapers, magazines, or books; visiting a museum or library; attending a concert, play or musical; writing letters; and playing games (see Appendix A for specific items). We computed an overall measure of past cognitive activity by averaging scores from the 30 items for childhood, age 18 and age 40. Current cognitive activity frequency scores represented the average of nine items. Both past and current activity scores ranged from 1 to 5, with higher scores indicating more frequent cognitive activity. This scale has been shown to have appropriate psychometric characteristics in a smaller sample drawn from MAP (Wilson et al., 2005). A similar scale, but which included a couple of additional items, was also found to have sound psychometric properties in a separate non-Hispanic Black cohort (Barnes et al., 2006).
Appendix A.
Descriptive Information on Items in Cognitive Activity Scale for Hispanics
Reference life epoch | Item | Activity frequencya M (SD) |
---|---|---|
Past | ||
Age 6 | Play games | 3.26 (1.67) |
Read to | 2.03 (1.50) | |
Told story | 2.84 (1.54) | |
Age 12 | Visit library | 1.84 (1.18) |
Time reading | 2.90 (1.02) | |
Read newspaper | 2.40 (1.63) | |
Read magazine | 2.61 (1.46) | |
Read book | 3.90 (1.49) | |
Write letter | 1.59 (0.97) | |
Play games | 3.04 (1.52) | |
Homework | 2.99 (1.12) | |
Age 18 | Visit library | 2.10 (1.37) |
Visit museum | 2.58 (1.46) | |
Attend concert | 2.60 (1.59) | |
Time reading | 2.65 (1.14) | |
Read newspaper | 3.22 (1.71) | |
Read magazine | 3.33 (1.39) | |
Read book | 3.04 (1.60) | |
Write letter | 2.02 (1.25) | |
Play games | 2.26 (1.32) | |
Extracurricular activity | 2.06 (1.12) | |
Age 40 | Visit library | 2.02 (1.25) |
Visit museum | 2.94 (1.31) | |
Attend concert | 2.78 (1.57) | |
Time reading | 2.52 (1.15) | |
Read newspaper | 3.57 (1.72) | |
Read magazine | 3.15 (1.42) | |
Read book | 2.78 (1.53) | |
Write letter | 1.86 (1.08) | |
Play games | 2.38 (1.28) | |
Currrent | Visit library | 1.68 (0.94) |
Visit museum | 2.63 (1.19) | |
Attend concert | 2.62 (1.48) | |
Time reading | 2.62 (0.98) | |
Read newspaper | 3.68 (1.61) | |
Read magazine | 3.05 (1.31) | |
Read book | 3.11 (1.57) | |
Write letters | 1.76 (0.95) | |
Play games | 2.37 (1.41) |
Note.
Activity frequency ranged from 1 (least frequent) to 5 (most frequent).
Assessment of cognitive resources
Participants completed measures of cognitive resources present in the home at age 12 and 40 thought to support a cognitively active lifestyle. Details of the measures have been published previously (Barnes et al., 2006; Wilson et al., 2005). Participants were asked to indicate whether they had each of seven items available (newspaper subscription, magazine subscription, dictionary, encyclopedia, atlas, globe, and library card) and to estimate the number of books in the home. Scores on each of the measures ranged from zero to eight, with higher scores indicating more resources.
Assessment of cognitive function
Participants were administered a battery of tests measuring five cognitive domains: episodic memory, semantic memory, working memory, perceptual speed, and visuospatial ability (Wilson, Beckett, et al., 2002). The Mini-Mental State Examination (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975) was used to describe the cohort but not in analyses.
Episodic memory measures included: immediate and delayed recall scores of story A from Logical Memory (Wechsler, 1987) and of the East Boston Story (Albert et al., 1991; Wilson, Beckett, et al., 2002), and Word List Memory, Recall, and Recognition (Morris et al., 1989). The semantic memory measures were: a 15-item version (Morris et al., 1989) of the Boston Naming Test (Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, 1983), and verbal fluency (Morris et al., 1989; Wilson, Beckett, et al., 2002). Working memory measures included: Digit Span Forward and Backward (Wechsler, 1987), and Digit Ordering (Cooper & Sager, 1993; Wilson, Beckett, et al., 2002). Perceptual speed tests included: the oral Symbol Digit Modalities Test (Smith, 1982), Number Comparison (Ekstrom, French, Harman, & Kermen, 1976; Wilson, Beckett, et al., 2002), and the number of words and colors correctly named in a modified version of the Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test (Trenerry, Crosson, DeBoe, & Leber, 1989). Visuospatial ability was measured with a 15-item form of the Judgment of Line Orientation (Benton, Sivan, Hamsher, Varney, & Spreen, 1994), and a 16-item form of Standard Progressive Matrices (Raven, Court, & Raven, 1992).
As described in prior studies (Wilson, Beckett, et al., 2002), composite scores for the five cognitive domains were created by converting raw scores of the measures comprising each cognitive domain into z scores, and averaging the z scores to obtain the cognitive domain score. A global cognition score was computed in similar manner using all the measures comprising the five cognitive domains. This battery was found to provide conceptually similar measures of cognition in Spanish-speakers (Krueger et al., 2009).
Demographic variables
Demographic information, including age, self and parental education, race, and ethnicity, was gathered by participants’ self report. Race was ascertained by asking the following question as worded in the US Census: “With which group do you most closely identify yourself?” and giving the following options: White; Black, Negro, African-American; Native American, Indian; Eskimo; Aleut; and, Asian or Pacific Island. Hispanic ethnicity was determined by asking “Are you of Spanish/Hispanic/Latino origin?”
Adaptation of measures into Spanish
The adaptation of the cognitive function battery into Spanish has been described elsewhere (Krueger et al., 2009). Briefly, a combination of the back translation method and group consensus was used in an effort to achieve a neutral Spanish language and culturally appropriate version of the tests, while maintaining their similarity to the English version. Cognitive activity frequency and cognitive resources scales were translated to Spanish and reviewed by a committee comprised of bilingual consultants who were familiar with neuropsychology and research on aging. In an effort to make these measures easily understood by groups of Hispanics of various origins, the committee members were of several regions in Latin America including Mexico, the Caribbean, Central America, and South America.
Statistical Analyses
We examined group differences in demographics, cognitive activity frequency, availability of cognitive resources, and cognitive function across ethnic groups using analyses of variance (ANOVAs). We also performed analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) on these measures adjusting for demographics (age, sex, and education). Follow-up independent samples t-tests were performed in those cases where analyses indicated significant group differences, and Tukey’s honestly significant difference was applied in all post hoc tests. We examined the relation of past and current cognitive activity and cognitive resources at age 12 and 40 with overall cognition and five cognitive domains in a series of linear regression models adjusting for age, sex, education, and ethnicity in our overall sample. To examine whether there were racial/ethnic differences in the association between cognitive activity and cognitive function we repeated the regression model for global cognition with terms for the interaction between ethnicity and cognitive activity (past and current) in addition to demographics (age, sex, education, ethnicity), with Hispanics serving as the reference group. Comparable analyses were run on cognitive resources scores (at age 12 and 40). Given demographic differences between groups, we matched a subgroup of non-Hispanic Whites and Blacks on age and sex to the 81 Hispanics, using two to one matching procedures. The much lower level of education in Hispanics prevented us from matching groups on this variable. We then repeated the previous analyses exploring the interaction with ethnicity on this smaller subsample.
In our sample of Hispanics, we investigated the relation of demographics with cognitive activity and cognitive resources scores using Pearson product moment correlation coefficients for continuous variables and independent sample t tests for categorical ones. To explore whether the association between education and cognitive activity was comparable in Hispanics and non-Hispanics, we ran a linear regression model in our overall sample on current cognitive activity scores adjusting for demographics and with added terms for the interactions between ethnicity and education. To examine the relation between cognitive activity and cognitive resources, and the relation of these variables with cognition in Hispanics we computed Pearson product moment correlation coefficients in our sample of Hispanics.
Models were validated graphically and analytically. Programming was done in SAS (SAS Institute Inc., 2000).
RESULTS
Demographic Variables
Table 1 shows demographic characteristics of the sample by ethnic group. There were significant differences on demographic characteristics by ethnicity, with Hispanics being younger than non-Hispanic Whites, and reporting fewer years of self and parental education than both non-Hispanic Whites and Blacks.
Table 1.
Group comparisons (Hispanic [n= 81], non-Hispanic White [n= 1102], non-Hispanic Black [n= 388]) on demographic variables
Variable | Hispanic
|
Non-Hispanic White
|
Non-Hispanic Black
|
Group Comparisonsa
|
Pairwise Comparisonsb
|
||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M (SD)/% | M (SD)/% | M (SD)/% | F/χ2(p) | H-W | H-B | B-W | |
Age (years) | 71.76 (8.76) | 80.77 (6.76) | 72.88 (6.36) | 234.44 (<.001) | <.001 | .366 | <.001 |
Own education (years) | 10.47 (4.95) | 14.78 (2.92) | 14.46 (3.44) | 68.83 (<.001) | <.001 | <.001 | .215 |
Parental education (years) | 5.56 (4.46) | 10.05 (3.69) | 9.32 (3.72) | 53.70 (<.001) | <.001 | <.001 | .004 |
Women | 69.14 | 74.50 | 73.45 | 1.19 (.549) | – | – | – |
Note.
Results from Analyses of Variance (ANOVAs), F ( p), and Chi Square test, χ2 ( p) on demographic characteristics by ethnic group (Hispanic [H], non-Hispanic White [W] and non-Hispanic Black [B]).
Results from follow-up independent samples t-tests with Tukey’s correction.
In a crude analysis of the Hispanic participants, 33 (40.74%) were born in the continental United States. Within foreign-born Hispanics, 39.58% were born in Mexico, 31.26% in the Caribbean islands, 14.58% in South America, and 14.58% in Central America. Those who were born outside the United States had been living in the US for a mean of 36.38 years (SD = 18.81; range, 2–76). Forty-eight participants (59.26%) identified themselves as primarily Spanish-speaking. Most Hispanics identified their primary race as White (89.87%), six as Black (7.59%), and two as Indian (2.53%).
Level of Cognitive Activity Frequency and Availability of Cognitive Resources Across Ethnic Groups
As shown in Table 2 Hispanics reported lower frequency of engagement in past and current cognitive activity and less availability of cognitive resources than both non-Hispanic groups. After adjusting for demographics, results for cognitive resources were unchanged. Analyses accounting for demographics showed that Hispanics’ frequency of past cognitive activity was similar to that of non-Hispanic Whites (p = .431) but less than non-Hispanics Blacks (p <.001), while engagement in current cognitive activity was comparable in Hispanics and non-Hispanic Blacks (p = .328), but less frequent than in non-Hispanic Whites (p <.001). Hispanics scored lower than one or both of our non-Hispanic groups across all cognitive domains except episodic memory and perceptual speed (Table 2).
Table 2.
Cognitive activity, cognitive resources, and cognitive function in Hispanics (n= 81), non-Hispanic Whites (n= 1102), and non-Hispanic Blacks (n= 388)
Variable | Hispanic
|
Non-Hispanic White
|
Non-Hispanic Black
|
ANOVAa
|
Pairwise Comparisonsb
|
||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | F (p) | H-W | H-B | W-B | |
Cognitive activity | |||||||
Past | 2.64 (0.73) | 3.13 (0.56) | 3.25 (0.62) | 36.12 (<.001) | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 |
Current | 2.62 (0.64) | 3.23 (0.64) | 3.07 (0.64) | 36.03 (<.001) | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 |
Cognitive resources | |||||||
Age 12 | 1.97 (1.99) | 4.36 (2.00) | 3.68 (2.17) | 60.51 (<.001) | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 |
Age 40 | 4.23 (2.37) | 6.08 (1.57) | 6.21 (1.67) | 51.08 (<.001) | <.001 | <.001 | .359 |
MMSE | 27.45 (2.41) | 27.99 (2.05) | 27.90 (2.09) | 2.62 (.073) | – | – | – |
Global Cognition | −0.10 (0.62) | 0.12 (0.56) | −0.01 (0.52) | 12.07 (<.001) | .002 | .360 | <.001 |
Episodic memory | 0.06 (0.61) | 0.08 (0.71) | 0.12 (0.59) | 0.52 (.597) | – | – | – |
Semantic memory | −0.07 (0.80) | 0.15 (0.71) | −0.02 (0.76) | 9.83 (<.001) | .023 | .818 | <.001 |
Working memory | −0.38 (0.88) | 0.14 (0.72) | −0.04 (0.73) | 24.86 (<.001) | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 |
Perceptual speed | −0.09 (0.95) | 0.11 (0.78) | −0.07 (0.79) | 8.83 (<.001) | .070 | .972 | <.001 |
Visuospatial ability | −0.32 (0.89) | 0.20 (0.72) | −0.28 (0.84) | 65.51 (<.001) | <.001 | .866 | <.001 |
Note.
Results from analysis of variance (ANOVA) on cognitive activity, cognitive resources, and cognitive function scores by ethnic group (Hispanic [H], non-Hispanic White [W] and non-Hispanic Black [B]).
Results from follow-up independent samples t-tests with Tukey’s correction.
Relation Between Cognitive Activity Frequency, Cognitive Resources, and Cognitive Function Across Ethnic Groups
Consistent with previous reports (Wilson et al., 2005), results of a series of linear regression models adjusting for demographics showed that higher frequency of cognitive activity (past and current) was associated with better global cognition in old age (Estimates = 0.15 and 0.21, respectively, both ps <.001), and each of our five cognitive domains (Estimates ranged from 0.11 to 0.39, ps from .010 to<.001) in our overall sample.
Findings from similar analyses on global cognition scores with added terms for the interaction between ethnicity and cognitive activity, showed no significant interactions, indicating comparable associations between cognitive activity and cognitive function across ethnic groups (Table 3). We repeated the previous analyses in a subgroup of non-Hispanic Whites and Blacks matched on age and sex to the 81 Hispanics (non-Hispanic Whites [n = 162; Age M = 72.29, SD = 7.53]; and Blacks [n = 162, Age M = 72.01, SD = 7.85]; sex [69% female in all groups]), and results were substantially similar.
Table 3.
Relation of ethnicity and cognitive activity to late life cognitive function
Model term | Estimate | SE | P |
---|---|---|---|
Current Cognitive Activity | .27 | .08 | .001 |
Current Cognitive Activity X Whites | −.04 | .09 | .635 |
Current Cognitive Activity X Blacks | −.12 | .09 | .185 |
Past Cognitive Activity | .18 | .07 | .015 |
Past Cognitive Activity X Whites | −.03 | .08 | .699 |
Past Cognitive Activity X Blacks | −.05 | .08 | .544 |
Note. Results were based on linear regression models on global cognition scores that controlled for demographics (age, sex, education, and ethnicity) and had Hispanics as the reference group.
Although estimates were relatively small, having more cognitive resources in the home both at age 12 and 40 was significantly associated with better cognitive function in late life (Estimates = 0.02 to 0.06, all ps <.04), with one exception: resources at age 12 was not significantly associated with episodic memory (Estimate = 0.01; SE = 0.00; p = .159). Similar models on global cognition with added terms for the interaction between ethnicity and cognitive resources, showed no significant interactions between ethnicity and cognitive resources at either age 12 or 40 (ps = .092 to .808). Analyses on the matched subgroup of Hispanics and non-Hispanics resulted in substantially similar findings.
Psychometric Information About Cognitive Activity and Cognitive Resources Measures in Hispanics
Table 4 shows psychometric data on the cognitive activity and cognitive resources measures in Hispanic participants alone. There were few item responses missing in our scales (range: 0–5 for cognitive activity, and 0–2 for cognitive resources). As can be seen in Table 4, scores for past and current cognitive activity, and cognitive resources at age 40 were approximately normally distributed. However, Hispanic participants reported having a relatively small number of cognitive resources in the home at age 12, and scores for this scale were positively skewed, Median = 1.38, range = 0 to 7.50. Internal consistency was adequate, but lower for current cognitive activity. Appendix A lists descriptive statistics for Hispanics on individual items comprising the cognitive activity scale.
Table 4.
Psychometric information on cognitive activity and cognitive resources scales in Hispanics (N= 81)
Characteristic | Cognitive activity
|
Cognitive resources
|
||
---|---|---|---|---|
Past | Current | Age 12 | Age 40 | |
Skewness | −0.07 | 0.09 | 0.88 | −0.04 |
Coefficient alpha | 0.91 | 0.64 | 0.77 | 0.81 |
Correlationsa | ||||
Age | −0.01(.919) | 0.04 (.744) | 0.05 (.650) | −0.03 (.767) |
Education (self) | 0.65 (<.001) | 0.63 (<.001) | 0.67 (<.001) | 0.61 (<.001) |
Education (parental) | 0.45 (<.001) | 0.46 (<.001) | 0.59 (<.001) | 0.44 (<.001) |
Differencesb | ||||
Male sex | 0.61 (.540) | 1.24 (.220) | −0.33 (.745) | 1.31 (.193) |
Language tested | 3.23 (.002) | 4.19 (<.001) | 4.43 (<.001) | 5.49 (<.001) |
Place of birth | 1.65 (.103) | 3.65 (<.001) | 2.92 (.005) | 3.29 (.002) |
Note.
Results from Pearson product moment correlation coefficients, r (p).
Results from independent samples t-tests, t (p).
In our subgroup of Hispanics, cognitive activity and cognitive resources were not related to age, and there were no significant sex differences. However, they were significantly correlated with level of own and parental education (Table 4). Results of analyses exploring whether the association between own education and cognitive activity (past and current) was comparable in Hispanics and non-Hispanics, showed that the terms for the interaction between ethnicity and education were not significant (Estimates = both 0.02; ps = .296 and .345), suggesting similar associations between education and cognitive activity across ethnic groups.
Within our sample of Hispanics, those tested in Spanish reported significantly less frequent past (M = 2.44; SD = 0.68) and current cognitive activity (M = 2.41; SD = 0.57), as well as cognitive resources at age 12 (M = 1.25; SD = 1.61) and age 40 (M = 3.21; SD = 2.03) as compared to those tested in English (cognitive activity: past M = 2.94, SD = 0.70; current M = 2.98, SD = 0.61; cognitive resources: Age 12: M = 3.05, SD = 2.03; Age 40: M = 5.73, SD = 2.02). However, there were also significant differences by place of birth, with those born outside the United States reporting lower frequency of engagement in cognitive activity in late life (M = 2.43; SD = 0.59) and less cognitive resources at both age 12 (M = 1.45; SD = 1.65) and age 40 (M = 3.56; SD = 2.93) than those born in the United States (current cognitive activity: M = 2.95, SD = 0.60; cognitive resources: Age 12: M = 2.71, SD = 2.20; Age 40: M = 5.22, SD = 2.33). There were no group differences for past cognitive activity. Most of the participants tested in Spanish reported being foreign-born and having lower levels of own and parental education than those tested in English (ps <.001).
Appendix B shows the percentage of Hispanics that reported having each of the items comprising the cognitive resources scale at ages 12 and 40. Data are presented on the entire group of Hispanics and on Hispanics by place of birth (United States-born or foreign-born). Of note, certain items, such as having a magazine subscription or a library card were hardly ever present in the home of foreign-born Hispanic participants at age 12.
Appendix B.
Cognitive Resources Scale Items Present in the Home at Ages 12 and 40 for Hispanics (n = 81)
Scale Item | % Yes Age 12
|
%Yes Age 40
|
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
All Hispanics | U.S.-borna | Foreign-bornb | All Hispanics | U.S.-borna | Foreign-bornb | |
Subscription to magazines | 20.25 | 37.50 | 5.06 | 45.68 | 72.73 | 27.08 |
Subscription to daily newspaper | 23.75 | 39.39 | 12.77 | 41.98 | 69.70 | 22.92 |
Globe | 15.00 | 21.21 | 10.64 | 38.27 | 45.45 | 33.33 |
World atlas | 15.19 | 12.50 | 17.02 | 48.15 | 57.58 | 41.67 |
Library card | 22.22 | 39.39 | 10.42 | 51.85 | 72.73 | 37.50 |
Encyclopedia | 28.75 | 36.36 | 23.40 | 70.37 | 78.79 | 64.58 |
Dictionary | 58.75 | 69.70 | 51.06 | 92.59 | 87.88 | 95.83 |
≥50 books | 14.81 | 18.18 | 12.50 | 41.98 | 42.42 | 41.67 |
Percentage of Hispanics born in the United States (n= 33) that reported having specific cognitive resources available.
Percentage of foreign-born Hispanics (n= 48) that reported having specific cognitive resources available
Table 5 shows significant correlations between the measures of cognitive activity frequency and cognitive resources, with global cognition and most domains of cognitive function in our Hispanic sample. Scores on the cognitive resources scales at age 12 and 40 were also significantly correlated with both past (rs = 0.66 and 0.61, respectively) and current (rs = 0.63 and 0.65, respectively) cognitive activity frequency (ps <.001).
Table 5.
Relation of cognitive activity and cognitive resources to cognitive function in Hispanicsa (N= 81)
Cognitive function | Cognitive activity
|
Cognitive resources
|
||
---|---|---|---|---|
Past | Current | Age 12 | Age 40 | |
Global cognition | 0.46 (<.001) | 0.47 (<.001) | 0.46 (<.001) | 0.41 (<.001) |
Episodic memory | 0.31 (.005) | 0.40 (<.001) | 0.35 (.001) | 0.36 (<.001) |
Semantic memory | 0.39 (<.001) | 0.42 (<.001) | 0.43 (<.001) | 0.39 (<.001) |
Working memory | 0.44 (<.001) | 0.41 (<.001) | 0.51 (<.001) | 0.33 (.003) |
Processing speed | 0.39 (<.001) | 0.45 (<.001) | 0.22 (.052) | 0.31 (.005) |
Visuospatial skills | 0.25 (.025) | 0.17 (.144) | 0.35 (.002) | 0.16 (.160) |
Note.
Results from Pearson product moment correlation coefficients, r (p).
DISCUSSION
In this analysis of two cohort studies of older Hispanics, non-Hispanic Whites, and non-Hispanic Blacks without dementia, we found that Hispanics had lower levels of education, reported engaging in cognitive activity less frequently and reported having less cognitive resources available to them as compared to non-Hispanic Whites and Blacks. Despite these group differences, the association of cognitive activity and cognitive resources to cognitive function in Hispanics was comparable to that of non-Hispanic Whites and Blacks.
Our findings are in line with results from a population based study from a geographically defined area in non-Hispanic Blacks and Whites showing that the association between participating in cognitively stimulating activities and cognitive function was comparable across these two racial groups (Wilson, Bennett, et al., 2002; Wilson et al., 2003). However, current findings are somewhat inconsistent with those of a prior study in Hispanics, which suggested that Hispanic ethnicity might affect this association (Herrera et al., 2011). There were significant differences between that study and ours in both the measures of cognitive activity and the outcome of cognitive function. For example, the Herrera et al. study used a self-administered cognitive test which was designed to be used for screening of dementia (i.e., Cognitive Assessment Screening Test), and thus might be less sensitive to detect differences in cognition than our cognitive composite measures. Furthermore, they were interested in measuring leisure activity, instead of cognitive activity specifically. Their measure of leisure activity had only a few items involving cognitive activity (e.g., reading and doing puzzles, writing, using a computer); and analyses were performed on individual items. Finally, their sample was limited to only women. Thus, differences between their study and ours may be difficult to interpret.
The findings of a similar association between cognitive activity and cognition across ethnic groups despite lower cognitive activity in Hispanics potentially have important implications. They suggest that increasing cognitive activity might benefit late life cognition in Hispanics more than non-Hispanics because Hispanics start with a lower activity level and have more room for improvement. However, this assumes that the dynamic range of the cognitive activity scale used in this study is similar for Hispanics and non-Hispanics. Thus, although speculative, this hypothesis might be worth exploring in studies aimed at increasing frequency of cognitive activity in Hispanics and measuring the subsequent impact on cognition.
But, why might Hispanics report less frequent engagement in cognitive activity than other ethnic groups? One explanation might be related to the demographics associated with Hispanic ethnicity. Hispanics had lower levels of own and parental education than both of our non-Hispanic groups, and these variables were associated with frequency of cognitive activity. However, after adjusting for demographic variables (age, sex, and education) some of the group differences in cognitive activity and cognitive resources remained significant, indicating that other factors might be playing a role. It might also be the case that our measures of cognitive activity do not fully capture the cognitive experience of Hispanics. For example, activities that might have an important cognitive component and that might be common in a Hispanic culture such as cooking, listening to music, and fixing things around the house, might be items worth adding to cognitive activity scales for Hispanics. Alternatively, at least part of what might be driving the low frequency of cognitive activity in Hispanics might be the scarcity of cognitive resources available to them. Consistent with his hypothesis, availability of cognitive resources was also significantly lower in Hispanics than both of our non-Hispanic groups, even after adjusting for demographics. Furthermore, availability of cognitive resources in childhood and adulthood was associated with past and current cognitive activity in Hispanics after adjusting for demographics. However, caution is warranted in inferring causality in the associations from our study.
Hispanics’ limited cognitive resources were particularly notable in childhood and in those who were primarily Spanish-speaking and born outside the United States. For instance, only 5 to 10% of Hispanics born outside the United States reported having a library card, subscription to magazines or a globe in their home at age 12. Given that place of birth was confounded with language tested and level of own and parental education in our Hispanic sample, it is difficult to provide clear explanations for these findings. They do raise the possibility that there might be features of the environment, particularly of foreign-born Hispanics that might support cognitive activity but are not captured in our measures. For example, it might be more appropriate to ask about having access to magazines rather than having a magazine subscription in Hispanics. Alternatively, Hispanics might be at a true disadvantage in terms of availability of cognitive resources compared to other groups.
Despite the fact that our measures of cognitive activity and cognitive resources can certainly be improved upon to be used for Hispanics, particularly those from more disadvantaged backgrounds, we found our measures to have sound psychometric properties in Hispanics. This is consistent with a prior analysis in MAP that did not focus on race (Wilson et al., 2005) and a prior study in non-Hispanic Blacks (Barnes et al., 2006). In Hispanics, there were only a small number of item responses missing in our cognitive activity and cognitive resources scales, indicating that items might be acceptable for this segment of the population. Furthermore, our measures had adequate internal consistency and were positively related to own and parental education, as well as various cognitive domains, supporting their construct validity in Hispanics. Hispanics who were tested with the Spanish version of our scales achieved lower scores than those who completed the corresponding English versions. Language use was confounded with level of education and place of birth, which highlights the importance of examining variables that might differ within subgroups of Hispanics (e.g., place of birth or testing language). Overall, results provide initial evidence of adequate reliability and validity of our cognitive resources measures in Hispanics.
The present study has important limitations. First, our findings on Hispanics are based on a small sample of volunteers. Hispanics represent a heterogenous group, which is highlighted in our sample by the diversity of our Hispanic participants in variables such as country of origin, time living in the United States, race, and language use. Thus, our findings might not necessarily apply to all subgroups of Hispanics, or to Hispanics living outside the United States. It will be important to explore in greater detail the effects of variables that differentiate subgroups of Hispanics, particularly in light of results from the present study indicating that language tested and country of origin might impact frequency of cognitive activity and availably of cognitive resources. Second, our group of Hispanics was not matched on level of education to our non-Hispanic groups, primarily because of the much lower level of education in Hispanics. We attempted to control for these differences in our statistical analyses since we did not want to restrict our Hispanic sample to those with higher levels of education. Future studies might help better understand the extent to which group differences in cognitive activity and cognitive resources are driven by ethnicity and other associated demographic variables. Third, because of large differences in sample sizes between ethnic groups in this study, our power for detecting a difference in the relation of cognitive activity to cognitive function between Hispanics and Blacks was substantially lower than the analogous power for the difference between Hispanics and Whites. Fourth, we used self-report measures for cognitive activity and resources, which might have introduced differential reporting bias across ethnic groups. The fact that the relationship between education and cognitive activity was similar across ethnic groups provides some evidence against this. Lastly, we used a retrospective design in our assessment of cognitive activity and cognitive resources, which might introduce recall bias and distortion. However, we excluded persons with dementia, and cognitive activity has been found to be associated with cognitive decline and dementia in longitudinal studies of non-Hispanics using a similar design (e.g., Hultsch et al., 1999; Verghese et al., 2003; Wang et al., 2002; Wilson, Bennett, et al., 2002; Wilson et al., 2007).
There are also important strengths of the present study. First, the study included the use of previously established, psychometrically sound measures of both past and present cognitive activity and cognitive resources (Wilson et al., 2005). Second, data are derived from two well-characterized epidemiologic cohorts with an essentially identical study design and methods. Third, detailed neuropsychological data were available and summarized to yield previously established composite measures of five cognitive domains and global cognition. Furthermore, these measures were found to be psychometrically valid in Hispanics in prior research (Krueger et al., 2009).
In conclusion, we found that despite Hispanics having lower levels of education, cognitive resources, and cognitive activity compared to non-Hispanics, the association between cognitive activity and cognition was similar across ethnic groups. Because Hispanics engage less frequently in cognitive activity than other groups, the potential for increasing the frequency of cognitive activity might be greater in Hispanics, resulting in a potentially a larger impact on cognition in this group. Our findings also indicate that although our cognitive activity frequency scale can certainly be improved upon, it is a psychometrically sound measure that might prove to be useful in research aimed at understanding individual differences in late-life cognitive function in Hispanics. Future studies could look into expanding our measures to fully capture the experience of cognitive activity in Hispanics, and determine whether interventions aimed at increasing the level of cognitive activity in older Hispanics without dementia might help reduce the risk for cognitive impairment and cognitive decline in this group.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank the participants of the Minority Aging Research Study and the Rush Memory and Aging Project for their invaluable contributions. We thank Charlene Gamboa, MPH; Traci Colvin, MPH; Tracey Nowakowski, Barbara Eubeler, Karen Lowe-Graham, MS and Mary Futrell for study recruitment and coordination, and John Gibbons, MS, and Greg Klein for data management, and the staff of the Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center. This research was supported by National Institute on Aging Grants R01AG22018, R01AG17917, and the Illinois Department of Public Health.
Footnotes
The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.
References
- Albert M, Smith L, Scherr P, Taylor J, Evans D, Funkenstein H. Use of brief cognitive tests to identify individuals in the community with clinically diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Neuroscience. 1991;57:167–178. doi: 10.3109/00207459109150691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arvanitakis Z, Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Barnes LL. Diabetes and cognitive systems in older Black and White persons. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders. 2010;24(1):37–42. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181a6bed5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes L, Wilson R, Mendes de Leon C, Bennett D. The relation of lifetime cognitive activity and lifetime access to resources to late-life cognitive function in older African Americans. Aging, Neuropsychology & Cognition. 2006;13:516–528. doi: 10.1080/138255890969519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Buchman AS, Mendes de Leon C, Bienias JL, Wilson RS. The Rush Memory and Aging Project: Study design and baseline characteristics of the study cohort. Neuroepidemiology. 2005;25:163–175. doi: 10.1159/000087446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benton AL, Sivan AB, Hamsher K, Varney NR, Spreen O. Contributions to neuropsychological assessment. 2. New York: Oxford University Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Boyle PA, Barnes LL, Buchman AS, Bennett DA. Purpose in life is associated with mortality among community-dwelling older persons. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2009;71:574–579. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181a5a7c0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christensen H, Mackinnon A. The association between mental, social and physical activity and cognitive performance in young and old subjects. Age and Ageing. 1993;22:175–182. doi: 10.1093/ageing/22.3.175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper JA, Sager HJ. Incidental and intentional recall in Parkinson’s disease: An account based on diminished attentional resources. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1993;15:713–731. doi: 10.1080/01688639308402591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dotson VM, Schinka JA, Brown LM, Mortimer JA, Borenstein AR. Characteristics of the Florida Cognitive Activities Scale in older African Americans. Assessment. 2008;15:72–77. doi: 10.1177/1073191107307509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ekstrom RB, French JW, Harman HH, Kermen D. Manual for kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service; 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gurland BJ, Wilder DE, Lantigua R, Stern Y, Chen J, Killeffer EHP, Mayeux R. Rates of dementia in three ethnoracial groups. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 1999;14:481–493. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haan MN, Mungas DM, Gonzalez HM, Ortiz TA, Acharya A, Jagust WJ. Prevalence of dementia in older Latinos: The influence of Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus, stroke and genetic factors. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2003;51:169–177. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2003.51054.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herrera AP, Meeks TW, Dawes SE, Hernandez DM, Thompson WK, Sommerfeld DH, Jeste DV. Emotional and cognitive health correlates of leisure activities in older Latino and Caucasian women. Psychology, Health & Medicine. 2011;16:661–674. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2011.555773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hultsch DF, Hammer M, Small BJ. Age differences in cognitive performance in later life: Relationships to self-reported health and activity life style. Journal of Gerontology. 1993;48:P1–P11. doi: 10.1093/geronj/48.1.p1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hultsch DF, Hertzog C, Small BJ, Dixon RA. Use it or lose it: Engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychology and Aging. 1999;14:245–263. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.14.2.245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- James BD, Boyle PA, Buchman AS, Barnes LL, Bennett DA. Life space and risk of Alzheimer disease, mild cognitive impairment, and cognitive decline in old age. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2011;19:969–969. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e318211c219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan EF, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Krueger KR, Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Aggarwal NT. A battery of tests for assessing cognitive function in older Latino persons. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders. 2009;23:384–388. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31819e0bfc. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lachman ME, Agrigoroaei S, Murphy C, Tun PA. Frequent cognitive activity compensates for education differences in episodic memory. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2010;18:4–10. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181ab8b62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manly JJ, Tang MX, Schupf N, Stern Y, Vonsattel JP, Mayeux R. Frequency and course of mild cognitive impairment in a multiethnic community. Annals of Neurology. 2008;63:494–506. doi: 10.1002/ana.21326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan E. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS/ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology. 1984;34:939–944. doi: 10.1212/wnl.34.7.939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, Hughes JP, van Belle G, Fillenbaum G, Clark C. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1989;39:1159–1165. doi: 10.1212/wnl.39.9.1159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raven JC, Court JH, Raven J. Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary: Standard progressive matrices. Oxford, UK: Oxford Psychologists Press; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Scarmeas N, Levy G, Tang MX, Manly J, Stern Y. Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology. 2001;57:2236–2242. doi: 10.1212/wnl.57.12.2236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith A. Symbol Digit Modalities Test manual-revised. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Stern C, Munn Z. Cognitive leisure activities and their role in preventing dementia: A systematic review. International Journal of Evidence-Based Healthcare. 2010;8:2–17. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-1609.2010.00150.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stern Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2002;8:448–460. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stern Y. Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders. 2006;20:112–117. doi: 10.1097/01.wad.0000213815.20177.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang MX, Cross P, Andrews H, Jacobs DM, Small S, Bell K, Mayeux R. Incidence of AD in African-Americans, Caribbean Hispanics, and Caucasians in northern Manhattan. Neurology. 2001;56:49–56. doi: 10.1212/wnl.56.1.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trenerry MR, Crosson B, DeBoe J, Leber WR. The Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Census Bureau. United States population projections: National population projections based on census 2000. 2008 Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/population/www/projections/summarytables.html.
- Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, Hall CB, Derby CA, Kuslansky G, Buschke H. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. New England Journal of Medicine. 2003;348:2508–2516. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa022252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang HX, Karp A, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. Late-life engagement in social and leisure activities is associated with a decreased risk of dementia: A longitudinal study from the Kungsholmen Project. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2002;155:1081–1087. doi: 10.1093/aje/155.12.1081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson RS, Barnes LL, Krueger KR, Hoganson G, Bienias JL, Bennett DA. Early and late life cognitive activity and cognitive systems in old age. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2005;11:400–407. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson RS, Beckett LA, Barnes LL, Schneider JA, Bach J, Evans DA, Bennett DA. Individual differences in rates of change in cognitive abilities of older persons. Psychology and Aging. 2002;17:179–193. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Beckett LA, Morris MC, Gilley DW, Bienias JL, Evans DA. Cognitive activity in older persons from a geographically defined population. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 1999;54B(3):155–160. doi: 10.1093/geronb/54b.3.p155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Bienias JL, Aggarwal NT, Mendes de Leon CF, Morris MC, Evans DA. Cognitive activity and incident Alzheimer’s disease in a population-based sample of older persons. Neurology. 2002;59:1910–1914. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000036905.59156.a1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Bienias JL, Mendes de Leon CF, Morris MC, Evans DA. Cognitive activity and cognitive decline in a biracial community population. Neurology. 2003;61:812–816. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000083989.44027.05. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson RS, Mendes de Leon CF, Barnes LL, Schneider JA, Bienias JL, Evans DA, Bennett DA. Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer Disease. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2002;287:742–748. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.6.742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson RS, Scherr PA, Schneider JA, Tang Y, Bennett DA. Relation of cognitive activity to risk of developing Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2007;69:1911–1920. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000271087.67782.cb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]