Summary
This study is a systematic review of mind-body interventions that used immune outcomes in order to: 1) characterize mind-body medicine studies that assessed immune outcomes, 2) evaluate the quality of mind-body medicine studies measuring immune system effects, and 3) systematically evaluate the evidence for mind-body interventions effect on immune system outcomes using existing formal tools. 111 studies with 4,777 subjects were reviewed. The three largest intervention type categories were Relaxation Training (n=25), Cognitive Based Stress Management (n=22), and Hypnosis (n=21). Half the studies were conducted with healthy subjects (n=51). HIV (n=18), cancer (n=13) and allergies (n=7) were the most prominent conditions examined in the studies comprising of non-healthy subjects. Natural killer cell and CD4 T lymphocyte measures were the most commonly studied outcomes. Most outcome and modality categories had limited or inconclusive evidence. Relaxation training had the strongest scientific evidence of a mind-body medicine affecting immune outcomes. Immunoglobulin A had the strongest scientific evidence for positive effects from mind-body medicine. Issues for mind-body medicine studies with immune outcomes are discussed and recommendations are made to help improve future clinical trials.
Keywords: immunology, complementary and alternative medicine, relaxation, cognitive based stress management, hypnosis, psychoneuroimmunology, lymphocytes, cytokines, HIV, cancer
INTRODUCTION
An increasing number of people in the United States are using complementary and alternative medicine with mind-body medicine being the most commonly used form (Barnes et al., 2004). Mind-body medicine focuses on the relationships between the brain, mind, body, and behavior, and their effect on health and disease. According to the National Center for Complementary and Alternative Medicine, it encompasses a large group of therapies such as hypnosis, meditation, yoga, biofeedback, tai chi, and visual imagery ("Mind-Body Medicine: An Overview"). Positive benefits of mind-body medicine are observed in numerous conditions(Ernst et al., 2007) including headaches (Wahbeh et al., 2008), coronary artery disease (Rutledge et al., 1999), chronic pain (Astin et al., 2003), mood, quality-of-life, and coping improvement. These therapies have also been shown to ameliorate disease and treatment-related symptoms, such as chemotherapy-induced nausea, vomiting, and pain in patients with cancer (Mundy et al., 2003). Mind-body modalities are commonly incorporated into treatment plans due to the low physical and emotional risk, the relatively low cost, and their ability to enable patients to take a more active role in their treatment.
The key premise of mind-body medicine is that a person’s mental state influences their physical health. The exact mechanisms underlying the health-promoting effects are unknown. However, evidence exists supporting the brain and central nervous system’s influence on immune function and thus potentially on immune outcomes (Irwin, 2008). The study of these interactions, psychoneuroimmunology, has been a growing field since its introduction by Robert Ader and Nicholas Cohen in 1975 (Ader & Cohen, 1975). Despite increased research studies and reviews, there have been limited studies examining mind-body medicine and immune outcomes and thus, this topic is the focus of this review.
Psychoneuroimmunology researchers are posed with the challenging problem of selecting appropriate immune outcome for their studies, with a multitude of available immune outcomes to select for any given study. Assessing all aspects of the immune system in a single study is usually not feasible (Robinson et al., 2002). Thus, most investigators attempt to measure multiple immune measures relevant to the research question. It is unclear as of yet which if any immune outcomes are most sensitive to mind-body medicine effects in general, or if they are only study-specific. It may also be that mind-body medicine’s influence on immune outcomes is dependent upon the health and/or disease states of the participants. Physicians often observe immune system improvements in health on a clinical level. However, being able to demonstrate these improvements through rigorous research methods is challenging.
When embarking upon this systematic review, the research questions were in regards to immune system effects of mind-body medicine, namely, “What is the state of research literature in mind-body medicine and immune measures”; and “What is the evidence for mind-body therapies affecting immune outcomes?” knowing that generalizations about these immune outcomes may not actually be able to be made. Additionally, the authors hoped to gain insight into the most effective mind-body medicine and most sensitive immune outcomes for future trials. The study objectives were to: 1) characterize mind-body medicine studies that assessed immune outcomes, 2) evaluate the quality of these studies, and 3) systematically evaluate the evidence for mind-body interventions effect on immune system outcomes.
METHODS
Literature search and retrieval
Comprehensive searches were conducted by a research librarian using MEDLINE® (1950-10/25/2007), PsycINFO® (1967-10/25/2007), CINAHL® (1982-2/13/2007), Alt HealthWatch (1984-10/26/2007), AMED (ca. 1980 to 10/26//2007), Cochrane Library: CENTRAL (10/25/2007), Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (10/25/2007), Database of Abstracts of Reviews of Effects (10/25/2007), and Health Technology Assessment Database (10/30/2007). Search terms included immune system terms and mind-body medicine terms dependent on the search strategy required for each database (a comprehensive description of the search strategy is available from the first author). The search parameters for subject type and modalities were kept broad to maximize recall because it was anticipated that the inclusion criteria “immune outcomes” would be limiting. Reference lists of relevant studies were also reviewed for eligible papers.
Selection of studies
Inclusion criteria included: 1) any published scientific literature regardless of peer review or paper type in any language; 2) all participant types; 3) modalities comprising mind-body interventions (hypnosis, imagery, meditation, mental healing, mind-body relations, all relaxation techniques, biofeedback, cognitive-behavioral therapies, group support, autogenic training, spirituality, and prayer, and emotional disclosure); 4) a sample size greater or equal to five; 5) study designs including randomized controlled trials (RCT’s), non-randomized controlled trials (NRCT’s), prospective and retrospective observational studies with controls, case-control studies, and uncontrolled pre-post studies; and 5) studies examining any measurable immune outcome. Because the study focused specifically on mind-body effects, interventions that incorporated body movement as the primary therapy (i.e. yoga, qi-gong, tai-chi) were excluded as exercise is known to influence immune function (Pedersen & Toft, 2000; Gleeson & Bishop, 2005; Radom-Aizik et al., 2007).
The primary author reviewed titles and abstracts according to study inclusion and exclusion criteria. A second reader reviewed and confirmed included and excluded studies. The full-text of studies meeting criteria, and those with insufficient information to determine eligibility from the abstract were retrieved.
Assessment of methodological quality
Numerous authors have noted difficulty in finding appropriate quality assessment tools (Juni et al., 1999; Juni et al., 2001; Glasziou et al., 2004). After reviewing numerous instruments, a Quality Assessment Tool modeled after the “Aid to the Evaluation of Therapeutic Studies” developed by Reisch et al, (Reisch et al., 1989) and modified as recommended by Deeks (Deeks et al., 2003) was used to rate the quality of the studies. This instrument was chosen because it included all relevant constructs (blinding, randomization, adequate reporting, etc.), provided a quantitative score, and adjusted for study design. The instrument adjusts for study design by removing questions about randomization, comparisons between groups and blinding for NRCT and uncontrolled trials from the total score. The result is an adjusted score on a scale of 0–100, 100 being a higher quality study.
Two reviewers assessed the methodological quality of studies independently during data extraction with a third reviewer designated to resolve disagreements through consensus. Descriptive statistics of the quality scores were evaluated. Scores were analyzed between studies before and after the year 2001 due to the release of another meta-analysis, which made recommendations for improving study design and reporting for this field (Miller & Cohen, 2001).
Additionally, an immunologist (HZ) reviewed the immune outcomes used for each study to determine appropriateness of outcomes. As the foundation of immunology knowledge has grown, certain outcomes have been found to be in vitro artifact (T suppressor cells, etc.), normal levels have been defined (cytokines), and many more receptors have been identified. Each outcome was assessed on a case by case basis for appropriate utilization depending on subject type, length of intervention, and assay method. For example, visualization had positive evidence for neutrophil adherence, but was rated as inappropriate by the immunologist (HZ). Neutrophils in vivo are attached to vessel walls with adhesion molecules. When measuring in serum, neutrophil concentrations are low and include activated neutrophils. In vitro, neutrophil adherence refers to the adhesion to the plastic labware, does not include adhesion molecules, and is irrelevant to health and immune function.
Data Collection
Uniformly trained research staff from the Helfgott Research Institute (Portland, Oregon) collected study data using a pre-tested data extraction form. A single reviewer extracted data and another independent reviewer verified the accuracy and completeness of the data extraction. Any discrepancies were resolved by consensus. The following data were collected: study design, number of treatment arms, setting, participant type, primary health condition (including acute or chronic), inclusion and exclusion criteria, number of subjects, mean age, study population (i.e., students, women), intervention type, length of each session, frequency of treatment, length of treatment period, total exposure time, home practice details, group or individual practice, type of outcome, sample type (i.e. blood, saliva), when outcomes were measured, means or mean differences when available, and p-values for each measure. If there were multiple time-points of measurement, values were taken from the time-points immediately before and after the intervention. For example, if outcomes were measured after a 4 week intervention and also 6 weeks later, only the results at 4 weeks were included. The same data were also extracted on any and all control groups. All study data were managed with Microsoft Excel™ and an Access™ relational database (Microsoft Corporation, Redmond, Washington). Statistical tests were conducted in SPSS 16.0 (SPSS, Inc, Chicago, Illinois).
Study Classification
Twelve intervention study categories were developed (Table 1). Brief descriptions of the categories are listed in Table 1. A full description of these therapies is beyond the scope of this paper and has already been discussed in the following review articles (Astin et al., 2003). Also, rather than collapse these categories into larger ones such as Relaxation or Psychotherapy-like, the categories were kept more specific to assess the finer distinctions between modalities. Many studies included more than one mind-body modality making the classification unclear. In these cases, studies were categorized according to the reported purpose of the study.
Table 1.
Category | Description | n | # of Studies |
---|---|---|---|
Relaxation | A therapy that promotes muscular and mental relaxation thought to increase parasympathetic activation and decrease sympathetic activation resulting in a slower heart rate, lower blood pressure, slower breath rate, and reducing muscle tension (Benson, 1997; Jerath et al., 2006; Ducla-Soares et al., 2007). | 1070 | 25 |
CBSM | Training and education in cognitive restructuring, assertiveness skills behavior change strategies, and stress response with training in one or many of the following: progressive muscle relaxation, autogenic training, meditation, guided imagery and breathing exercises, stress management techniques (Penedo et al., 2006). | 1361 | 22 |
Hypnosis | Attention and focused concentration with a relative suspension of peripheral awareness (Spiegel & Spiegel, 1987) documented by fMRI (Faymonville et al., 2006; Raz et al., 2006) and EEG (de Pascalis, 1999) studies. | 618 | 21 |
Visualization | Patient’s imagination used to visualize a specific health outcome (Simonton & Simonton, 1975; Gawain & Uchida, 1982). | 315 | 10 |
CBT | Psychotherapy based on the idea that thoughts cause emotions and behaviors and aims to change the way a person thinks in order to improve emotion and behaviors (Schnurr et al., 2007). | 375 | 9 |
Disclosure | Verbal or written expression of emotional experience (Pennebaker & Seagal, 1999). | 303 | 7 |
Support | Group intervention that stated support was the major component (Simoni et al., 2007). | 435 | 6 |
MBSR | Structured group program that employs instruction and practice in mindfulness meditation, education and discussions, and intensive home practice (Kabat-Zinn, 1982). | 134 | 3 |
Biofeedback | Measures physiological markers like heart rate, breathing rate, electromyography, electroencephalography, or electrodermal activity and displays the results back to the patient to aid in self-modulation (Schwartz & Andrasik, 2005). | 47 | 2 |
Humor | Induction of laughter in the patient through various mediums (Bennett & Lengacher, 2008). | 73 | 2 |
Meditation | Self-observation of mental activity, attentional focus training, and cultivating an attitude that highlights process rather than content (Ospina et al., 2007). | 20 | 2 |
Music | Listening to music or singing (Hilliard, 2005). | 26 | 2 |
CBSM-cognitive based stress management; CBT-cognitive based therapy; MBSR-mindfulness-based stress reduction.
We considered reducing the selection criteria and/or focusing the review on just a specific outcome or disease state. However, the variability between studies was such that grouping them into smaller categories resulted in groups of one or two studies. This undermined our purpose of coming to broader generalizations about the outcomes. Thus, we decided to continue with the broader selection and conducted a qualitative rather than quantitative review.
Evidence Grading
Due to study heterogeneity, a meta-analysis was not possible. The heterogeneity resulted from differences in subject type, intervention and implementation variation, and immune outcome type. The Natural Standard evidence-based validated grading rationale™ was used to provide a general understanding of the available evidence to guide future research, rather than attempt to definitively evaluate whether mind-body modalities affect immune outcomes. The Natural Standard evidence-based validated grading rationale™ is an objective grading criteria derived from validated instruments for evaluating study quality, including the 5-point scale developed by Jadad et al., in which a score below 4 is considered to indicate lesser quality methodologically (Jadad et al., 1996) http://www.nlm.nih.gov/medlineplus/druginfo/natural/grading.html (Table 2).
Table 2.
Level of Evidence Grade | Criteria |
---|---|
A (Strong Scientific Evidence) | Statistically significant evidence of benefit from >2 properly randomized trials (RCTs), OR evidence from one properly conducted RCT AND one properly conducted meta-analysis, OR evidence from multiple RCTs with a clear majority of the properly conducted trials showing statistically significant evidence of benefit AND with supporting evidence in basic science, animal studies, or theory. |
B (Good Scientific Evidence) | Statistically significant evidence of benefit from 1–2 properly randomized trials, OR evidence of benefit from ≥1 properly conducted meta-analysis OR evidence of benefit from >1 cohort/case-control/non-randomized trials AND with supporting evidence in basic science, animal studies, or theory. |
C (Unclear or conflicting scientific evidence) | Evidence of benefit from ≥1 small RCT(s) without adequate size, power, statistical significance, or quality of design by objective criteria, OR conflicting evidence from multiple RCTs without a clear majority of the properly conducted trials showing evidence of benefit or ineffectiveness, OR evidence of benefit from ≥1 cohort/case-control/non-randomized trials AND without supporting evidence in basic science, animal studies, or theory, OR evidence of efficacy only from basic science, animal studies, or theory. |
D (Fair Negative Scientific Evidence) | Statistically significant negative evidence (i.e., lack of evidence of benefit) from cohort/case-control/non-randomized trials, AND evidence in basic science, animal studies, or theory suggesting a lack of benefit. |
F (Strong Negative Scientific Evidence) | Statistically significant negative evidence (i.e. lack of evidence of benefit) from ≥1 properly randomized adequately powered trial(s) of high-quality design by objective criteria. |
Lack of Evidence | Unable to evaluate efficacy due to lack of adequate available human data. |
Resultant grades reflect the level of available scientific evidence in support of the effects of a given therapy for a specific indication. For this study, grades were assessed with only higher quality papers defined as a quality assessment score greater than 72, which was the median quality assessment score of all the studies. Gradings were then repeated with all studies, regardless of quality score, to determine if quality influenced the results. For some immune outcomes, the desired direction of change of each outcome will vary depending on the disease being studied. For grading purposes, a statistically significant positive outcome was defined as a p-value of less than .05 and in the direction of change hypothesized by the investigators.
RESULTS
Study Characterization
A total of 914 studies were selected for review. Three hundred and forty were duplicates, 216 were excluded due to outcome measures, 80 due to design, 145 due to intervention, and 19 due to mind-body intervention being movement-based. In addition, one could not be located, and three could not be interpreted. One hundred and eleven studies with a total 4,777 combined subjects were reviewed to provide evidence regarding the state of research on mind-body medicine and immune outcomes (included studies are listed after references). The studies were published between 1964 and 2007, with 47 studies published after 2000. Seventy-three percent (81) were RCTs, 13% (14) were non-randomized controlled trials (NRCTs), 10% (11) were pre-post studies, and 4% (5) were cross-over controls. Seventy-seven percent incorporated some method of blinding (excluding pre-post studies). The three largest intervention type categories were Relaxation Training (23%), CBSM (20%), and Hypnosis (19%) (Table 2). Sixty-seven percent incorporated one modality and the remaining used two or more (e.g., relaxation with visualization). The average number of subjects in each study was 52±46 (range 5–303). Of the studies that reported mean ages (73%), the total mean age for participants across all intervention types was 36 ±14 years. Five studies were with children (under 18 yrs of age). Forty-five percent involved healthy subjects, 52% involved patients with chronic disease, and 3% involved patients with acute disease. Among the subjects with chronic and acute disease, a diverse range of conditions was examined (Table 3).
Table 3.
Condition | Studies |
---|---|
Healthy | 51 |
HIV | 18 |
Cancer (breast, prostate, ovarian, malignant melanoma) | 13 |
Allergy (urticaria, asthma, eczema) | 7 |
Mental health (anorexia, depression, insomnia, panic disorder) | 5 |
Upper respiratory infections | 4 |
Herpes Simplex Virus | 3 |
Multiple sclerosis, Rheumatoid Arthritis | 2 |
Chronic Fatigue Syndrome, Facial Pain, Coronary Artery Disease, Tinnitus, Ulcerative Colitis, Decreased WBC, Hemopoietic Stem Cell Transplantation | 1 each |
Forty-eight percent of the studies used group interventions rather than individual therapy (40%) and 12% did not report how the intervention was administered. Reporting of the actual intervention varied. Seventy-five percent of the studies reported intervention session length, weekly frequency, and duration. Mean sessions were 78 minutes (range 9–360 minutes) with sessions ranging from 1 to 7 days per week, and interventions lasting 1 to 52 weeks long (mean 10.75±11.48). Fifty-one percent incorporated home practice which occurred between intervention sessions, half of which reported home practice details (mean session time 26 ±15 minutes).
Ninety percent of the studies were controlled with a varying number of arms (74 with two arms, 22 with three arms, 2 with four arms, and two with five arms). The different types of control groups included no treatment (37%), treatment with a different therapy (active control) (31%), waitlist (19%), standard of care (5%), and 8% did not report control type. A majority of the studies with three arms had a no treatment and an active control group, while the remaining studies had two different active control groups. Fifteen studies with an active control group did not report control session details in terms of frequency and duration. Of those that did, the time for the active control matched the time for the intervention.
Quality Assessment
The quality assessment items are listed in Supplemental Table A, along with the number of studies that included each criterion. The mean total quality scores representing different study designs were pre-post: 67±11 (range 43–82), cross-sectional: 67± 10 (52–76), NRCT: 69± 13(4–78), RCT: 73± 08 (46–96), and 70 ± 10 (4–96) for all studies. When analyzed using a one-way ANOVA, these scores were significantly different (F(3,110) = 3.69, p<.02). Bonneferoni post-hoc analysis attributes these to differences between RCT’s and NRCT’s. Other study design category differences were not significant. The mean quality score for studies published after 2001 was 74±7, whereas before 2001 the figure was 69±10 (p<.02, t=2.46).
All assessed studies stated the purpose of the study, recruited subjects and collected data prospectively, and gave the total number of subjects. Ninety-seven percent of the studies defined the outcome variables prior to the study, asked suitable research questions, had standardized and consistent laboratory and other outcomes and described the evaluation methods adequately. Ninety-three percent of studies used an appropriate treatment to answer the research questions, and clearly identified and appropriately used statistical tests. Eighty percent adequately described the treatment, 66% obtained informed consent, 34% reported collecting data on treatment compliance, 11% reported conducting sample size calculations or having adequate numbers of subjects to detect differences, and 4% reported on adverse events.
Immune Outcomes
Out of the 357 immune outcomes assessed in all the studies, 38 or 10% were assessed by the immunologist as not appropriate for the specific study. These markers included lymphocyte reactivity to mitogen (n=25), neutrophil adherence (n=3), immunoglobulin G (n=3), immunoglobulin M (n=3), suppressor T cells (n=2), T lymphocytes (n=1), IFN-γ (n=1) and IgA (n=1). These particular studies were eliminated for a variety of reason. Some studies measured outcomes at inappropriate times, some had inappropriate outcomes for the disease studied, and others had protein levels that were significantly outside normal ranges suggesting mislabeled unit measures, or operator error. Sixteen were assessed as unclear if appropriate and the rest were assessed as appropriate.
The most common immune outcomes assessed in the mind-body intervention publications were natural killer cell outcomes (n=61), CD4 T lymphocyte (n=38), immunoglobulin A (IgA) (n=24), CD8 T lymphocyte (n=23), and delayed-hypersensitivity skin tests (n=21). Using the Natural Standard evidence-based validated grading rationale™ (Table 2), immune outcomes were assessed for strength of scientific evidence to be affected by mind-body medicine. Evaluating immune outcomes regardless of intervention type, only IgA demonstrated strong scientific evidence for positive effects resulting from mind-body interventions (Table 4). IgA included both serum and saliva measures. When examined separately, only salivary IgA had positive evidence (7 positive, 1 negative, high quality studies) whereas, serum IgA had negative evidence (1 positive, 3 negative, high quality studies) (Table 4D). All other outcomes were scored unclear, conflicting, lacking evidence, or with negative evidence according to the grading criterion. Because the health of the subjects probably plays a large role in immune outcomes, grading was repeated separating healthy subject and patient population studies. Also, grading was repeated for all the studies, regardless of study quality. These additional assessments did not change the strength or direction of the evidence.
Table 4.
A | Blood Cell Count | p<.05 | p>.05 | Grade** |
---|---|---|---|---|
Basophil | 0 (2)* | 0 (1) | L | |
Eosinphil | 1(1) | 0 (2) | C | |
Granulocytes | 1 (1) | 2 (2) | C | |
Lymphocytes | 4 (8) | 8 (10) | D | |
Macrophage | 1 (1) | 0 (0) | C | |
Monocyte | 1 (3) | 2 (8) | D | |
White blood cell | 2 (4) | 4 (10) | D | |
Neutrophil | 1 (7) | 2 (4) | D | |
Thrombocytes | 0 (0) | 0 (1) | L |
B | Cytokines Soluble Factors |
p<.05 | p>.05 | Grade |
---|---|---|---|---|
IFN-y | 2 (4) | 1 (1) | C | |
IL-10 | 1 (1) | 0 (3) | C | |
IL-1β | 2 (2) | 2 (3) | C | |
IL-2 | 2 (3) | 1 (2) | C | |
IL-4 | 1 (1) | 0 (2) | C | |
IL-6 | 0 (0) | 2 (4) | D | |
TNF-α | 1 (3) | 2 (4) | D | |
Neuropeptide Y | 0 (0) | 1 (0) | C | |
C Reactive Protein | 1 (1) | 2 (2) | D |
C | Cell Activation | p<.05 | p>.05 | Grade |
---|---|---|---|---|
CD3 | 3 (3) | 5 (6) | D | |
CD4 T Lymph | 6 (12) | 14 (25) | F | |
CD4/CD8 Ratio | 2 (3) | 3 (7) | D | |
CD8 T Lymph | 3 (7) | 9 (15) | F | |
CD19 (B cells) | 2 (2) | 3 (5) | D | |
CD23 | 1 (1) | 0 (0) | C | |
CD56 (NK cells) | 7 (25) | 16 (33) | F |
D | Immunoglobulin | p<.05 | p>.05 | Grade |
---|---|---|---|---|
IgA | 7 (16) | 5 (8) | A | |
IgE | 1 (1) | 0 (0) | C | |
IgG | 0 (2) | 1 (2) | C | |
IgM | 2 (2) | 0 (1) | C |
E | Reactivity to Antigen | p<.05 | p>.05 | Grade |
---|---|---|---|---|
Lymphocyte reactivity-ConA, Candida, CMV, MLR, PHA, PWM, VZ*** | 4 (12) | 9 (18) | F | |
Flu frequency, duration | 0 (2) | 1 (1) | C | |
Viral titer | 3 (7) | 6 (7) | D | |
Skin Delayed Hypersensitivity | 0 (10) | 2 (11) | D | |
Erythrocyte Sedimentation Rate | 0 (0) | 1 (1) | C | |
Colitis Activity Index | 0 (0) | 1 (1) | C |
Numbers outside of parentheses represent number of outcomes in each category with a quality assessment score greater than 72. Numbers in parentheses represent the number of outcomes in each category for all studies regardless of quality assessment score.
Grades were made according to the Natural Standard evidence-based validated grading rationale™ as depicted in Table 1. (A=strong scientific evidence, B=good scientific evidence, C=unclear or conflicting scientific evidence, D = fair negative scientific evidence, F = strong negative scientific evidence, L = lack of evidence)
ConA= Concanavalin A; CMV= Cytomegalovirus; MLR= Mixed lymphocyte reaction; PHA = phytohemagglutinin; PWM = Pokeweed Mitogen; VZ = Varicella Zoster Virus.
The effect of interventions on all immune outcomes combined revealed that relaxation training had strong scientific evidence. Biofeedback, Humor, and Meditation lacked adequate data to grade due to a small number of studies included and all others had unclear or conflicting data (Table 5).
Table 5.
Intervention Code | p<.05 | p>.05 | Grade** |
---|---|---|---|
Biofeedback | 0 (4)* | 0 (8) | L |
CBSM*** | 19 (25) | 26 (33) | F |
CBT | 2 (6) | 28 (37) | F |
Disclosure | 1 (5) | 4 (13) | F |
Humor | 0 (3) | 0 (0) | L |
Hypnosis | 15 (28) | 16 (38) | D |
MBSR | 8 (9) | 22 (22) | F |
Meditation | 0 (1) | 0 (2) | L |
Music | 1 (1) | 0 (1) | C |
Relaxation training | 16 (44) | 14 (39) | A |
Support | 5 (13) | 5 (12) | C |
Visualization | 5 (12) | 4 (10) | C |
Numbers outside of parentheses represent number of outcomes in each category with a quality assessment score greater than 72. Numbers in parentheses represent the number of outcomes in each category for all studies regardless of quality assessment score.
Grades were made according to the Natural Standard evidence-based validated grading rationale™ as depicted in Table 1. (A=strong scientific evidence, B=good scientific evidence, C=unclear or conflicting scientific evidence, D = fair negative scientific evidence, F = strong negative scientific evidence, L = lack of evidence).
CBSM-cognitive based stress management; CBT-cognitive based therapy; MBSR-mindfulness-based stress reduction.
DISCUSSION
Study Characterization
It was not surprising that relaxation therapy, CBSM, and hypnosis were the most studied as they have been practiced for a longer duration. Music, disclosure, and humor were included as mind-body medicine based on the concept that their effects are mostly likely mediated through the mind however, there were limited studies on these interventions. During the literature search, studies on humor and music were unexpectedly found, although these search terms were not specifically included. There was discussion within the team on whether to include the studies in the review. It was decided to include the studies although additional searches were not conducted to search for humor and music studies because it was not part of the original study design.
A majority of the studies were small RCT’s. As expected, most of the studies did not incorporate double-blinding in the traditional sense, where both the subject and investigator were blinded. The nature of mind-body interventions precludes blinding the subject to their group assignment. Efforts were made by most investigators to include some sort of blinding through data entry, laboratory personnel, and assessment. Surprisingly, most studies did not include adverse events reporting. Whether this is because of a reporting failure or lack of adverse events is unknown. Even though mind-body medicine is a low-risk therapy, reporting adverse events data is essential. Approximately half of the studies utilized healthy subjects, which often results in negative trials. Interpreting efficacy from a negative trial using healthy subjects is problematic because the immune system response may be different in a healthy versus patient participant. Regardless, no difference in evidence grades was found when healthy subject studies were graded separately from patient population studies.
Dosing parameters in mind-body medicine are still as of yet undetermined (Caspi & Burleson, 2005). Often details of the actual intervention and home practice were not reported, nor were compliance measures assessed. Total exposure time may influence results and is important data to capture and should be reported in future studies.
It was encouraging that 90% of the studies used a control group. Many used an active and non-active control and when incorporated the active control group exposure time matched the intervention group time. For mind-body studies where placebo may play a pivotal role, both an active and non-active control group should be included (Crow et al., 1999; Oken et al., 2006). The active control and non-active control group protocols should duplicate the time, attention, and home practice of the experimental group. In this way, non-specific effects like placebo and expectancy can be assessed.
Quality Assessment
As expected, the RCT’s had a higher mean quality score than the other study designs. Poor reporting was the major contributor in most quality score deductions. Surprisingly, only 66% of the studies noted obtaining informed consent. It is assumed that consent was obtained but was not reported, however, this is an unnecessary omission. Additionally, many studies did not report power calculations or rationale behind subject number. When a study does not report whether a power calculation was done or that adequate subject numbers were present to detect differences between groups, we must infer that these trials were not adequately powered or that they failed to report power calculations. Either way, the reader is left with uncertainty. Pre-clinical studies not attempting to definitively assess efficacy may not need to report power calculations, yet the objectives of the study as a pilot should be clearly stated.
The quality assessment scores significantly improved for studies published post-2001 after the release of the Miller meta-analysis (Miller & Cohen, 2001). The Miller review examined psychological interventions’ effect on immune outcomes, with a comprehensive review of the therapies, and discussion of recommendations for future trials including subject selection, choosing appropriate immune system measures, designing methodologically rigorous studies, and testing meditational pathways. The Miller and Cohen review reported similar findings in immune system outcomes and the need for improved methods in these trials (Miller & Cohen, 2001). Regardless of whether the improvement in study quality was a direct consequence of the publication or some other guidelines in quality study design and reporting, the results are hopeful. This study builds upon the Miller review by examining studies conducted through October 2007, expanding the intervention categories, using alternative grading criteria, and contributing additional recommendations for future trials.
Immune Outcomes
The variability in intervention application is one of the main limitations to accurately synthesizing data regarding mind-body modalities’ effects on the immune system. Meta-analytic methods were not used for this study because of heterogeneity not only between the intervention groups but also within the intervention groups. The intervention’s implementation consisted of different session lengths, frequency and duration and thus could not be directly compared. For example, relaxation training was held three times a week for 45 minutes for three weeks in one study and once a week for 20 minutes for four weeks along with focused breathing in another. Even if these two studies had identical immune measures, the results could not be combined for meta-analysis because of the application differences. Comparisons may have been conducted if effect sizes were uniformly reported, but they were not.
The study variability also highlights the fact that mind-body medicine research has a paucity of pre-clinical trials where dose response, optimal dose, and preliminary efficacy are established. Investigators often attempt to conduct a Phase 3 definitive assessment of therapy efficacy in an under-powered RCT. Unfortunately, these studies undermine the field because they often yield negative results. Pre-clinical studies must be conducted to move the field forward. Most of the studies reviewed were small RCT’s conducted and could be considered pilots. Most studies did not include power calculations and thus it is uncertain whether the studies could be considered definitive.
Only IgA showed strong evidence for being affected by mind-body medicine. This measure may not be ideal for every intervention or patient group but has shown strong evidence of effects resulting from mind-body interventions. Salivary IgA had positive evidence whereas serum IgA did not possibly reflecting the faster rate of change of salivary IgA and the less stressful collection method. Salivary IgA may be more reliable for mind-body intervention studies. The Miller and Cohen review reported similar findings on IgA (Miller & Cohen, 2001). Overall, relaxation training demonstrated the strongest evidence for a mind-body intervention to influence immune outcomes overall. Incorporating some type of relaxation training into mind-body medicine therapies may help improve health outcomes through immune system mediation.
Interpreting immune outcome results includes multiple factors to consider. The direction of change of the immune outcomes can be different for the same outcome with different populations. For example, an increase in IFNγ may be positive in a population of people with a viral infection. The same increase in IFNγ would be considered negative in a population with a Th1-mediated autoimmune disease. Also, the immune outcome must be relevant to the research question and be able to be changed within the time-frame of the intervention. Immune outcome changes may differ in healthy versus patient populations and must be considered when making conclusions. Another issue in interpreting these findings is in the sensitivity, reliability, and validity of immune markers. Are the results of these studies truly negative due to lack of effect on immunity or because the markers employed lack sensitivity or are improperly used? Some immune outcomes reliability and validity are not well-established and thus using these markers may not be viable. Immune markers are also influenced by nutrition, exercise, caffeine, sleep, and pharmaceuticals. The depth of controlling for or reporting these variables was limited in many of the evaluated studies. Further research is required to assess appropriate, sensitive, reliable, and valid immune outcome measures in mind-body medicine. Additionally, the immune outcome choice may not be relevant to the disease studied or sensitive to the intervention.
One major issue we experienced in conducting the review was the definition of mind-body medicine and which modalities should be included. Movement-based practices such as yoga and tai chi were excluded although some may argue that they should have been included in the study. However, the results would have been inconclusive because the immune changes may have been a result of the increased movement rather than the change of mental state. Also, some modalities such as music and humor may not be considered mind-body medicine. Although they may change mental states for some, it arguable whether they are actually a mind-body medicine.
Various biases must be considered when reviewing these results. There is a language bias in the study because although we attempted to include all languages we were unable to translate three of the articles and thus did not include them in the study. There is also a possibility of publication bias as we only included published papers. We were unable to conduct a funnel plot analysis because the gradings were qualitative. Publication bias may be present although usually publication bias presents itself as greater positive trials being published rather than negative ones as was evidenced in this review (149 positive and 208 negative outcome measures).
Another limitation of the study is its qualitative rather than quantitative nature. Ideally, a traditional meta-analysis would have been conducted. However, the extent of mind-body research is not yet vast enough to include multiple studies of similar design to allow for grouped analysis. Because of this, the results from this study must be viewed with a cautionary note that these are observed qualitative trends rather than conclusions.
In order to help improve future mind-body-immune studies, the following recommendations are made:
Follow CONSORT guidelines for study design and manuscript preparation even in NRCT or uncontrolled trials. A new set of guidelines have recently been created for non-pharmacological treatments and is applicable to mind-body interventions (Boutron et al., 2008).
Create a dialogue with investigators studying similar interventions and attempt to create consensus on intervention session length, frequency, and duration through pre-clinical studies examining dosing parameters.
Combine resources between investigators to conduct larger, possibly multiple site studies.
Use appropriate control groups to account for non-specific effects.
Conduct and report on power calculations for definitive studies and/or report study is exploratory.
Continue assessing appropriate, sensitive, reliable, and valid immune outcomes.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
The author would like to thank Susan Norris, MD, Mary McDonagh, MD, Andrew Hamilton, MSLIS, Erin Connelly, Khivan Singh, Tracy Edinger, ND, Helfgott Research Institute at the National College of Naturopathic Medicine, and Oregon Health & Sciences University for their support of this project. This publication was made possible with support from the Oregon Clinical and Translational Research Institute (OCTRI), UL1 RR024140 from the National Center for Research Resources (NCRR), a component of the National Institutes of Health (NIH), and NIH Roadmap for Medical Research, and NIH T32 AT002688.
References
- Ader R, Cohen N. Behaviorally conditioned immunosuppression. Psychosom Med. 1975;37(4):333–340. doi: 10.1097/00006842-197507000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Astin JA, Shapiro SL, Eisenberg DM, Forys KL. Mind-body medicine: state of the science, implications for practice. J Am Board Fam Pract. 2003;16(2):131–147. doi: 10.3122/jabfm.16.2.131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes PM, Powell-Griner E, McFann K, Nahin RL. Complementary and alternative medicine use among adults: United States, 2002. Adv Data. 2004;(343):1–19. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett MP, Lengacher C. Humor and Laughter May Influence Health: III. Laughter and Health Outcomes. Evid Based Complement Alternat Med. 2008;5(1):37–40. doi: 10.1093/ecam/nem041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benson H. The relaxation response: therapeutic effect. Science. 1997;278(5344):1694–1695. doi: 10.1126/science.278.5344.1693b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boutron I, Moher D, Altman DG, Schulz KF, Ravaud P. Extending the CONSORT statement to randomized trials of nonpharmacologic treatment: explanation and elaboration. Ann Intern Med. 2008;148(4):295–309. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-148-4-200802190-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspi O, Burleson KO. Methodological challenges in meditation research. Adv Mind Body Med. 2005;21(1):4–11. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crow R, Gage H, Hampson S, Hart J, Kimber A, Thomas H. The role of expectancies in the placebo effect and their use in the delivery of health care: a systematic review. Health Technol Assess. 1999;3(3):1–96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Pascalis V. Psychophysiological correlates of hypnosis and hypnotic susceptibility. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 1999;47(2):117–143. doi: 10.1080/00207149908410026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deeks JJ, Dinnes J, D'Amico R, Sowden AJ, Sakarovitch C, Song F, et al. Evaluating non-randomised intervention studies. Health Technol Assess. 2003;7(27):iii–x. 1–173. doi: 10.3310/hta7270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ducla-Soares JL, Santos-Bento M, Laranjo S, Andrade A, Ducla-Soares E, Boto JP, et al. Wavelet analysis of autonomic outflow of normal subjects on head-up tilt, cold pressor test, Valsalva manoeuvre and deep breathing. Exp Physiol. 2007;92(4):677–686. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.2007.038026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ernst E, Pittler MH, Wider B, Boddy K. Mind-body therapies: are the trial data getting stronger? Altern Ther Health Med. 2007;13(5):62–64. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faymonville ME, Boly M, Laureys S. Functional neuroanatomy of the hypnotic state. J Physiol Paris. 2006;99(4–6):463–469. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gawain S, Uchida Y. Creative visualization. Toronto ; New York: Bantam Books; 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Glasziou P, Vandenbroucke JP, Chalmers I. Assessing the quality of research. Bmj. 2004;328(7430):39–41. doi: 10.1136/bmj.328.7430.39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gleeson M, Bishop NC. The T cell and NK cell immune response to exercise. Ann Transplant. 2005;10(4):43–48. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hilliard RE. Music Therapy in Hospice and Palliative Care: a Review of the Empirical Data. Evid Based Complement Alternat Med. 2005;2(2):173–178. doi: 10.1093/ecam/neh076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Irwin MR. Human psychoneuroimmunology: 20 years of discovery. Brain Behav Immun. 2008;22(2):129–139. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2007.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jadad AR, Moore RA, Carroll D, Jenkinson C, Reynolds DJ, Gavaghan DJ, et al. Assessing the quality of reports of randomized clinical trials: is blinding necessary? Control Clin Trials. 1996;17(1):1–12. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(95)00134-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jerath R, Edry JW, Barnes VA, Jerath V. Physiology of long pranayamic breathing: neural respiratory elements may provide a mechanism that explains how slow deep breathing shifts the autonomic nervous system. Med Hypotheses. 2006;67(3):566–571. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2006.02.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juni P, Altman DG, Egger M. Systematic reviews in health care: Assessing the quality of controlled clinical trials. Bmj. 2001;323(7303):42–46. doi: 10.1136/bmj.323.7303.42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juni P, Witschi A, Bloch R, Egger M. The hazards of scoring the quality of clinical trials for meta-analysis. Jama. 1999;282(11):1054–1060. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.11.1054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kabat-Zinn J. An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: theoretical considerations and preliminary results. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 1982;4(1):33–47. doi: 10.1016/0163-8343(82)90026-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller GE, Cohen S. Psychological interventions and the immune system: a meta-analytic review and critique. Health Psychol. 2001;20(1):47–63. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.20.1.47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mind-Body Medicine: An Overview. 2007 May; NCCAM Publication No. D239., from http://nccam.nih.gov/health/backgrounds/mindbody.htm.
- Mundy EA, DuHamel KN, Montgomery GH. The efficacy of behavioral interventions for cancer treatment-related side effects. Semin Clin Neuropsychiatry. 2003;8(4):253–275. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oken BS, Zajdel D, Kishiyama S, Flegal K, Dehen C, Haas M, et al. Randomized, controlled, six-month trial of yoga in healthy seniors: effects on cognition and quality of life. Altern Ther Health Med. 2006;12(1):40–47. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ospina MB, Bond K, Karkhaneh M, Tjosvold L, Vandermeer B, Liang Y, et al. Meditation practices for health: state of the research (AHRQ) Evid Rep Technol Assess (Full Rep) 2007;(155):1–263. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pedersen BK, Toft AD. Effects of exercise on lymphocytes and cytokines. Br J Sports Med. 2000;34(4):246–251. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.34.4.246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Penedo FJ, Molton I, Dahn JR, Shen BJ, Kinsinger D, Traeger L, et al. A randomized clinical trial of group-based cognitive-behavioral stress management in localized prostate cancer: development of stress management skills improves quality of life and benefit finding. Ann Behav Med. 2006;31(3):261–270. doi: 10.1207/s15324796abm3103_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennebaker JW, Seagal JD. Forming a story: the health benefits of narrative. J Clin Psychol. 1999;55(10):1243–1254. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679(199910)55:10<1243::AID-JCLP6>3.0.CO;2-N. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radom-Aizik S, Leu SY, Cooper DM, Zaldivar F., Jr Serum from exercising humans suppresses t-cell cytokine production. Cytokine. 2007;40(2):75–81. doi: 10.1016/j.cyto.2007.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raz A, Fan J, Posner MI. Neuroimaging and genetic associations of attentional and hypnotic processes. J Physiol Paris. 2006;99(4–6):483–491. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reisch JS, Tyson JE, Mize SG. Aid to the evaluation of therapeutic studies. Pediatrics. 1989;84(5):815–827. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson FP, Mathews HL, Witek-Janusek L. Issues in the design and implementation of psychoneuroimmunology research. Biol Res Nurs. 2002;3(4):165–175. doi: 10.1177/10900402003004002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutledge JC, Hyson DA, Garduno D, Cort DA, Paumer L, Kappagoda CT. Lifestyle modification program in management of patients with coronary artery disease: the clinical experience in a tertiary care hospital. J Cardiopulm Rehabil. 1999;19(4):226–234. doi: 10.1097/00008483-199907000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schnurr PP, Friedman MJ, Engel CC, Foa EB, Shea MT, Chow BK, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder in women: a randomized controlled trial. Jama. 2007;297(8):820–830. doi: 10.1001/jama.297.8.820. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz MS, Andrasik F. Biofeedback, Third Edition: A Practitioner's Guide. NY, NY: The Guildford Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Simoni JM, Pantalone DW, Plummer MD, Huang B. A randomized controlled trial of a peer support intervention targeting antiretroviral medication adherence and depressive symptomatology in HIV-positive men and women. Health Psychol. 2007;26(4):488–495. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.26.4.488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simonton OC, Simonton S. Belief systems and management of the emotional aspects of malignancy. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology. 1975;(7):29–47. [Google Scholar]
- Spiegel H, Spiegel D. Trance and treatment : clinical uses of hypnosis (Reprint ed.) Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Wahbeh H, Elsas SM, Oken BS. Mind-body interventions: applications in neurology. Neurology. 2008;70(24):2321–2328. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000314667.16386.5e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Included Studies
- Antoni MH, Baggett L, et al. Cognitive-behavioral stress management intervention buffers distress responses and immunologic changes following notification of HIV-1 seropositivity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1991b;59(6):906–915. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.59.6.906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antoni MH, Cruess DG, et al. Cognitive-behavioral stress management intervention effects on anxiety, 24-hr urinary norepinephrine output, and T-cytotoxic/suppressor cells over time among symptomatic HIV-infected gay men. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68(1):31–45. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.68.1.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antoni MH, Cruess DG, et al. Increases in a marker of immune system reconstitution are predated by decreases in 24-h urinary cortisol output and depressed mood during a 10-week stress management intervention in symptomatic HIV-infected men. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2005;58(1):3–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2004.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antoni MH, Cruess DG, et al. Stress management and immune system reconstitution in symptomatic HIV-infected gay men over time: effects on transitional naive T cells (CD4(+)CD45RA(+)CD29(+)) American Journal of Psychiatry. 2002;159(1):143–145. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.1.143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antoni MH, LaPerriere A, et al. Stress and immunity in individuals at risk for AIDS. Stress Medicine. 1991a;7(1):35–44. [Google Scholar]
- Auerbach JE, Oleson TD, et al. A behavioral medicine intervention as an adjunctive treatment for HIV-related illness. Psychology & Health. 1992;6(4):325–334. [Google Scholar]
- Bakke AC, Purtzer M, et al. The effect of hypnotic-guided imagery on psychological well-being and immune function in patients with prior breast cancer. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2002;53(6):1131–1137. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(02)00409-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barling NR, Raine SJ. Some effects of hypnosis on negative affect and immune system response. Australian Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis. 2005;33(2):160–177. [Google Scholar]
- Bennett MP, Zeller JM, et al. The effect of mirthful laughter on stress and natural killer cell activity. Alternative Therapies in Health & Medicine. 2003;9(2):38–45. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berger JA, O'Brien WH. Effect of a cognitive-behavioral stress management intervention on salivary IgA, self-reported levels of stress, and physical health complaints in an undergraduate population. International Journal of Rehabilitation & Health. 1998;4(3):129–152. [Google Scholar]
- Bongartz W, Lyncker I, et al. The influence of hypnosis on white blood cell count and urinary levels of catecholemines and vanillyl mandelic acid. Hypnos. 1987;14:52–61. [Google Scholar]
- Booth RJ, Petrie KJ, et al. Changes in circulating lymphocyte numbers following emotional disclosure: Evidence of buffering? Stress Medicine. 1996;43:293–306. [Google Scholar]
- Bower JE, Kemeny ME, et al. Finding positive meaning and its association with natural killer cell cytotoxicity among participants in a bereavement-related disclosure intervention. Ann Behav Med. 2003;25(2):146–155. doi: 10.1207/S15324796ABM2502_11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brambilla F, Bellodi L, et al. Plasma concentrations of interleukin-1beta, interleukin-6 and tumor necrosis factor-alpha in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1998;23(5):439–447. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(98)00027-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brennan FX, Charnetski CJ. Stress and immune system function in a newspaper's newsroom. Psychological Reports. 2000;87(1):218–222. doi: 10.2466/pr0.2000.87.1.218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlson LE, Speca M, et al. Mindfulness-based stress reduction in relation to quality of life, mood, symptoms of stress, and immune parameters in breast and prostate cancer outpatients. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2003;65(4):571–581. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000074003.35911.41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrico AW, Antoni MH, et al. Cognitive behavioral stress management effects on mood, social support, and a marker of antiviral immunity are maintained up to 1 year in HIV-infected gay men. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2005;12(4):218–226. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm1204_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castes M, Hagel I, et al. Immunological changes associated with clinical improvement of asthmatic children subjected to psychosocial intervention. Brain, Behavior and Immunity. 1999;13(1):1–13. doi: 10.1006/brbi.1999.0551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christensen AJ, Edwards DL, et al. Effect of verbal self-disclosure on natural killer cell activity: moderating influence of cynical hostility. Psychosom Med. 1996;58(2):150–155. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199603000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Claesson M, Birgander LS, et al. Cognitive-behavioural stress management does not improve biological cardiovascular risk indicators in women with ischaemic heart disease: a randomized-controlled trial. Journal of Internal Medicine. 2006;260(4):320–331. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2006.01691.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coates TJ, McKusick L, et al. Stress reduction training changed number of sexual partners but not immune function in men with HIV. American Journal of Public Health. 1989;79(7):885–887. doi: 10.2105/ajph.79.7.885. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cruess S, Antoni M, et al. Reductions in herpes simplex virus type 2 antibody titers after cognitive behavioral stress management and relationships with neuroendocrine function, relaxation skills, and social support in HIV-positive men. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2000;62(6):828–837. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200011000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson RJ, Kabat-Zinn J, et al. Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2003;65(4):564–570. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000077505.67574.e3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doering LV, Cross R, et al. Infection, depression, and immunity in women after coronary artery bypass: a pilot study of cognitive behavioral therapy. Altern Ther Health Med. 2007;13(3):18–21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donaldson VW. A clinical study of visualization on depressed white blood cell count in medical patients. Applied Psychophysiology & Biofeedback. 2000;25(2):117–128. doi: 10.1023/a:1009518925859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eller LS. Effects of two cognitive-behavioral interventions on immunity and symptoms in persons with HIV. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 1995;17(4):339–348. doi: 10.1007/BF02888599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elsenbruch S, Langhorst J, et al. Effects of mind-body therapy on quality of life and neuroendocrine and cellular immune functions in patients with ulcerative colitis. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 2005;74(5):277–287. doi: 10.1159/000086318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elsesser K, Van Berkel M, et al. The effects of anxiety management training on psychological variables and immune parameters in cancer patients: A pilot study. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy. 1994;22(1):13–23. [Google Scholar]
- Esterling BA, Antoni MH, et al. Psychosocial modulation of antibody to Epstein-Barr viral capsid antigen and human herpesvirus type-6 in HIV-1-infected and at-risk gay men. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1992;54(3):354–371. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199205000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fawzy FI, Kemeny ME, et al. A structured psychiatric intervention for cancer patients. II. Changes over time in immunological measures. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47(8):729–735. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810200037005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox PA, Henderson DC, et al. Immunological markers of frequently recurrent genital herpes simplex virus and their response to hypnotherapy: A pilot study. International Journal of STD & AIDS. 1999;10(11):730–734. doi: 10.1258/0956462991913420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fry L, Mason SS, et al. Effect of hypnosis on allergic skin responses in asthma and hay-fever. BMJ. 1964;1:1145–1148. doi: 10.1136/bmj.1.5391.1145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodkin K, Feaster DJ, et al. A bereavement support group intervention is longitudinally associated with salutary effects on the CD4 cell count and number of physician visits. Clin Diagn Lab Immunol. 1998;5(3):382–391. doi: 10.1128/cdli.5.3.382-391.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grape C, Sandgren M, et al. Does singing promote well-being? An empirical study of professional and amateur singers during a singing lesson. Integrative Physiological & Behavioral Science. 2003;38(1):65–74. doi: 10.1007/BF02734261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green ML, Green RG, et al. Daily relaxation modifies serum and salivary immunoglobulins and psychophysiologic symptom severity. Biofeedback & Self Regulation. 1988;13(3):187–199. doi: 10.1007/BF00999169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green RG, Green ML. Relaxation increases salivary immunoglobulin A. Psychological Reports. 1987;61(2):623–629. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1987.61.2.623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gregerson MB, Roberts IM, et al. Absorption and imagery locate immune responses in the body. Biofeedback & Self Regulation. 1996;21(2):149–165. doi: 10.1007/BF02284693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gruber BL, Hall NR, et al. Immune system and psychological changes in metastatic cancer patients using relaxation and guided imagery: A pilot study. Scandinavian Journal of Behaviour Therapy. 1988;17(1):25–46. [Google Scholar]
- Gruber BL, Hersh SP, et al. Immunological responses of breast cancer patients to behavioral interventions. Biofeedback & Self Regulation. 1993;18(1):1–22. doi: 10.1007/BF00999510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gruzelier J, Smith F, et al. Cellular and humoral immunity, mood and exam stress: the influences of self-hypnosis and personality predictors. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2001;42(1):55–71. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(01)00136-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall H, Papas A, et al. Directional changes in neutrophil adherence following passive resting versus active imagery. International Journal of Neuroscience. 1996;85(3–4):185–194. doi: 10.3109/00207459608986682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall HR, Minnes L, et al. Voluntary modulation of neutrophil adhesiveness using a cyberphysiologic strategy. Int J Neurosci. 1992;63(3–4):287–297. doi: 10.3109/00207459208987203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasson D, Anderberg UM, et al. Psychophysiological effects of a web-based stress management system: a prospective, randomized controlled intervention study of IT and media workers [ISRCTN54254861] BMC Public Health. 2005;5:78. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-5-78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hewson-Bower B, Drummond PD. Secretory immunoglobulin A increases during relaxation in children with and without recurrent upper respiratory tract infections. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 1996;17(5):311–316. doi: 10.1097/00004703-199610000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hewson-Bower B, Drummond PD. Psychological treatment for recurrent symptoms of colds and flu in children. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2001;51(1):369–377. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(01)00212-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hidderley M, Holt M. A pilot randomized trial assessing the effects of autogenic training in early stage cancer patients in relation to psychological status and immune system responses. European Journal of Oncology Nursing. 2004;8(1):61–65. doi: 10.1016/j.ejon.2003.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jasnoski ML, Kugler J. Relaxation, imagery, and neuroimmunomodulation. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1987;496:722–730. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1987.tb35835.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson VC, Walker LG, et al. Can relaxation training and hypnotherapy modify the immune response to stress, and is hypnotizability relevant? Contemporary Hypnosis. 1996;13(2):100–108. [Google Scholar]
- Kamei T, Toriumi Y, et al. Correlation between alpha rhythms and natural killer cell activity during yogic respiratory exercise. Stress and Health: Journal of the International Society for the Investigation of Stress. 2001;17(3):141–145. [Google Scholar]
- Kern-Buell CL, McGrady AV, et al. Asthma severity, psychophysiological indicators of arousal, and immune function in asthma patients undergoing biofeedback-assisted relaxation. Applied Psychophysiology & Biofeedback. 2000;25(2):79–91. doi: 10.1023/a:1009562708112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiecolt-Glaser JK, et al. Psychosocial enhancement of immunocompetence in a geriatric population. Health Psychology. 1985;4(1):25–41. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.4.1.25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Glaser R, et al. Modulation of cellular immunity in medical students. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1986;9(1):5–21. doi: 10.1007/BF00844640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Marucha PT, et al. Hypnosis as a modulator of cellular immune dysregulation during acute stress. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology. 2001;69(4):674–682. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.69.4.674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim S, Kim H. Effects of a relaxation breathing exercise on anxiety, depression, and leukocyte in hemopoietic stem cell transplantation patients. Cancer Nursing. 2005;28(1):79–83. doi: 10.1097/00002820-200501000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimata H. Increase in dermcidin-derived peptides in sweat of patients with atopic eczema caused by a humorous video. J Psychosom Res. 2007;62(1):57–59. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimura H, Nagao F, et al. Beneficial Effects of the Nishino Breathing Method on Immune Activity and Stress Level. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine. 2005;11(2):285–291. doi: 10.1089/acm.2005.11.285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koh KB, Lee Y. Reduced anxiety level by therapeutic interventions and cell-mediated immunity in panic disorder patients. Psychotherapy & Psychosomatics. 2004;73(5):286–292. doi: 10.1159/000078845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kugler J, Kruse B, et al. Psychoneuroimmunological effects of coping training in patients with multiple sclerosis. Psychologische Beitrage. 2000;42(1):50–59. [Google Scholar]
- Laidlaw TM, Kerstein R, et al. Hypnotizability and immunological response to psychological intervention in HIV. Contemporary Hypnosis. 2004;21(3):126–135. [Google Scholar]
- Laidlaw TM, Naito A, et al. The influence of 10 min of the Johrei healing method on laboratory stress. Complementary Therapies in Medicine. 2006;14(2):127–132. doi: 10.1016/j.ctim.2005.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lekander M, Furst CJ, et al. Immune effects of relaxation during chemotherapy for ovarian cancer. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 1997;66(4):185–191. doi: 10.1159/000289133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine MI, Geer JH, et al. Hypnotic suggestion and the histamine wheal. J Allergy. 1966;37(4):246–250. doi: 10.1016/0021-8707(66)90119-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lloyd AR, Hickie I, et al. Immunologic and psychologic therapy for patients with chronic fatigue syndrome: a double-blind, placebo-controlled trial.[see comment] American Journal of Medicine. 1993;94(2):197–203. doi: 10.1016/0002-9343(93)90183-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Locke SE, Ransil BJ, et al. Failure of hypnotic suggestion to alter immune response to delayed-type hypersensitivity antigens. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1987;496:745–749. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1987.tb35841.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Locke SE, Ransil BJ, et al. Effect of hypnotic suggestion on the delayed-type hypersensitivity response. JAMA. 1994;272(1):47–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lowe G, Bland R, et al. Progressive muscle relaxation and secretory immunoglobulin A. Psychological Reports. 2001;88(3):912–914. doi: 10.2466/pr0.2001.88.3.912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lutgendorf SK, Antoni MH, et al. Cognitive-behavioral stress management decreases dysphoric mood and herpes simplex virus-type 2 antibody titers in symptomatic HIV-seropositive gay men. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology. 1997;65(1):31–43. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.65.1.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lutgendorf SK, Antoni MH, et al. Changes in cognitive coping strategies predict EBV-antibody titre change following a stressor disclosure induction. J Psychosom Res. 1994;38(1):63–78. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90009-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lutgendorf SK, Logan H, et al. Effects of acute stress, relaxation, and a neurogenic inflammatory stimulus on interleukin-6 in humans. Brain, Behavior, & Immunity. 2004;18(1):55–64. doi: 10.1016/s0889-1591(03)00090-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCain NL, Zeller JM, et al. The influence of stress management training in HIV disease. Nursing Research. 1996;45(4):246–253. doi: 10.1097/00006199-199607000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGrady A, Conran P, et al. The effects of biofeedback-assisted relaxation on cell-mediated immunity, cortisol, and white blood cell count in healthy adult subjects. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1992;15(4):343–354. doi: 10.1007/BF00844727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGregor BA, Antoni MH, et al. Cognitive-behavioral stress management increases benefit finding and immune function among women with early-stage breast cancer. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2004;56(1):1–8. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00036-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mohr DC, Goodkin DE, et al. Treatment of depression is associated with suppression of nonspecific and antigen-specific T(H)1 responses in multiple sclerosis. Archives of Neurology. 2001;58(7):1081–1086. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.7.1081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mulder CL, Antoni MH, et al. Psychosocial group intervention and the rate of decline of immunological parameters in asymptomatic HIV-infected homosexual men. Psychotherapy & Psychosomatics. 1995;63(3–4):185–192. doi: 10.1159/000288958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naito A, Laidlaw TM, et al. The impact of self-hypnosis and Johrei on lymphocyte subpopulations at exam time: a controlled study. Brain Research Bulletin. 2003;62(3):241–253. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2003.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicholas PK, Webster A. A behavioral medicine intervention in persons with HIV. Clinical Nursing Research. 1996;5(4):391–406. doi: 10.1177/105477389600500403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Connor ME, Schmidt W, et al. Relaxation training and breast milk secretory IgA.[see comment] Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. 1998;152(11):1065–1070. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.152.11.1065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Leary A, Shoor S, et al. A cognitive-behavioral treatment for rheumatoid arthritis. Health Psychol. 1988;7(6):527–544. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.7.6.527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olness K, Culbert T, et al. Self-regulation of salivary immunoglobulin A by children. Pediatrics. 1989;83(1):66–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pawlow LA, Jones GE. The impact of abbreviated progressive muscle relaxation on salivary cortisol and salivary immunoglobulin a (sIgA) Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback. 2005;30(4):375–387. doi: 10.1007/s10484-005-8423-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peavey BS, Lawlis G, et al. Biofeedback-assisted relaxation: Effects on phagocytic capacity. Biofeedback & Self Regulation. 1985;10(1):33–47. doi: 10.1007/BF00998676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennebaker JW, Kiecolt-Glaser JK, et al. Disclosure of traumas and immune function: health implications for psychotherapy. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988;56(2):239–245. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.56.2.239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrie KJ, Booth RJ, et al. Disclosure of trauma and immune response to a hepatitis B vaccination program. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1995;63(5):787–792. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.63.5.787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reid MR, Drummond PD, et al. The effect of moderate aerobic exercise and relaxation on secretory immunoglobulin A. International Journal of Sports Medicine. 2001a;22(2):132–137. doi: 10.1055/s-2001-11347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reid MR, Mackinnon LT, et al. The effects of stress management on symptoms of upper respiratory tract infection, secretory immunoglobulin A, and mood in young adults. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2001b;51(6):721–728. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(01)00234-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richardson MA, Post-White J, et al. Coping, life attitudes, and immune responses to imagery and group support after breast cancer treatment. Alternative Therapies in Health & Medicine. 1997;3(5):62–70. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rider MS, Achterberg J. Effect of music-assisted imagery on neutrophils and lymphocytes. Biofeedback Self Regul. 1989;14(3):247–257. doi: 10.1007/BF01000097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rider MS, Achterberg J, et al. Effect of immune system imagery on secretory IgA. Biofeedback Self Regul. 1990;15(4):317–333. doi: 10.1007/BF01000026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson FP, Mathews HL, et al. Psycho-endocrine-immune response to mindfulness-based stress reduction in individuals infected with the human immunodeficiency virus: a quasiexperimental study.[see comment] Journal of Alternative & Complementary Medicine. 2003;9(5):683–694. doi: 10.1089/107555303322524535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenberg HJ, Rosenberg SD, et al. Expressive disclosure and health outcomes in a prostate cancer population. Int J Psychiatry Med. 2002;32(1):37–53. doi: 10.2190/AGPF-VB1G-U82E-AE8C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruzyla-Smith P, Barabasz A, et al. Effects of hypnosis on the immune response: B-cells, T-cells, helper and suppressor cells. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis. 1995;38(2):71–79. doi: 10.1080/00029157.1995.10403185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savard J, Simard S, et al. Randomized clinical trial on cognitive therapy for depression in women with metastatic breast cancer: psychological and immunological effects. Palliative & Supportive Care. 2006;4(3):219–237. doi: 10.1017/s1478951506060305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savard J, Simard S, et al. Randomized study on the efficacy of cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia secondary to breast cancer, part II: Immunologic effects. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 2005;23(25):6097–6106. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2005.12.513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharpe L, Sensky T, et al. A blind, randomized, controlled trial of cognitive-behavioural intervention for patients with recent onset rheumatoid arthritis: preventing psychological and physical morbidity. Pain. 2001;89(2–3):275–283. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(00)00379-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherman JJ, Carlson CR, et al. Effects of stretch-based progressive relaxation training on the secretion of salivary immunoglobulin A in orofacial pain patients. Journal of Orofacial Pain. 1997;11(2):115–124. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simoni JM, Pantalone DW, et al. A randomized controlled trial of a peer support intervention targeting antiretroviral medication adherence and depressive symptomatology in HIV-positive men and women. Health Psychol. 2007;26(4):488–495. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.26.4.488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith GR, Conger C, et al. Psychological modulation of the delayed type hypersensitivity skin test. Psychosomatics. 1992;33(4):444–451. doi: 10.1016/S0033-3182(92)71949-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solberg EE, Halvorsen R, et al. Meditation: a modulator of the immune response to physical stress? A brief report. British Journal of Sports Medicine. 1995;29(4):255–257. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.29.4.255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor DN. Effects of a behavioral stress-management program on anxiety, mood, self-esteem, and T-cell count in HIV positive men. Psychological Reports. 1995;76(2):451–457. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1995.76.2.451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Urakawa K, Yokoyama K. Can relaxation programs with music enhance human immune function? Journal of Alternative & Complementary Medicine. 2004;10(4):605–606. doi: 10.1089/1075553041829334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Pompe G, Duivenvoorden HJ, et al. Effectiveness of a short-term group psychotherapy program on endocrine and immune function in breast cancer patients: an exploratory study. J Psychosom Res. 1997;42(5):453–466. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(96)00393-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vedhara K, Bennett PD, et al. Enhancement of Antibody Responses to Influenza Vaccination in the Elderly following a Cognitive-Behavioural Stress Management Intervention. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 2003;72(5):245–252. doi: 10.1159/000071895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weber C, Arck P, et al. Impact of a relaxation training on psychometric and immunologic parameters in tinnitus sufferers. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2002;52(1):29–33. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(01)00281-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitehouse WG, Dinges DF, et al. Psychosocial and immune effects of self-hypnosis training for stress management throughout the first semester of medical school. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1996;58(3):249–263. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199605000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zachariae R, Bjerring P. The effect of hypnotically induced analgesia on flare reaction of the cutaneous histamine prick test. Arch Dermatol Res. 1990;282(8):539–543. doi: 10.1007/BF00371950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zachariae R, Bjerring P. Increase and decrease of delayed cutaneous reactions obtained by hypnotic suggestions during sensitization. Studies on dinitrochlorobenzene and diphenylcyclopropenone. Allergy. 1993;48(1):6–11. doi: 10.1111/j.1398-9995.1993.tb02168.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zachariae R, Bjerring P, et al. Modulation of type I immediate and type IV delayed immunoreactivity using direct suggestion and guided imagery during hypnosis. Allergy. 1989;44(8):537–542. doi: 10.1111/j.1398-9995.1989.tb04198.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zachariae R, Hansen JB, et al. Changes in cellular immune function after immune specific guided imagery and relaxation in high and low hypnotizable healthy subjects. Psychotherapy & Psychosomatics. 1994;61(1–2):74–92. doi: 10.1159/000288872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zachariae R, Jorgensen MM, et al. Effects of relaxation on the delayed-type hypersensitivity (DTH) reaction to diphenylcyclopropenone (DCP) Allergy. 1997;52(7):760–764. doi: 10.1111/j.1398-9995.1997.tb01235.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zachariae R, Kristensen J, et al. Effect of psychological intervention in the form of relaxation and guided imagery on cellular immune function in normal healthy subjects: An overview. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 1990;54(1):32–39. doi: 10.1159/000288374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zachariae R, Oster H, et al. Effects of hypnotic suggestions on ultraviolet B radiation-induced erythema and skin blood flow. Photodermatology, Photoimmunology & Photomedicine. 1994;10(4):154–160. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.