Abstract
Personality traits are basic dimensions of behavioural variation, and twin, family, and adoption studies show that around 30% of the between-individual variation is due to genetic variation. There is rapidly-growing interest in understanding the evolutionary basis of this genetic variation. Several evolutionary mechanisms could explain how genetic variation is maintained in traits, and each of these makes predictions in terms of the relative contribution of rare and common genetic variants to personality variation, the magnitude of nonadditive genetic influences, and whether personality is affected by inbreeding. Using genome-wide SNP data from >8,000 individuals, we estimated that little variation in the Cloninger personality dimensions (7.2% on average) is due to the combined effect of common, additive genetic variants across the genome, suggesting that most heritable variation in personality is due to rare variant effects and/or a combination of dominance and epistasis. Furthermore, higher levels of inbreeding were associated with less socially-desirable personality trait levels in three of the four personality dimensions. These findings are consistent with genetic variation in personality traits having been maintained by mutation-selection balance.
Keywords: balancing selection, mutation-selection balance, antagonistic pleiotropy, correlational selection, neutral, trade-offs, personality, temperament, mutation, evolution, behavioural syndromes
Introduction
Personality traits are basic dimensions of behavioural variation, comprising various more specific characteristics that tend to correlate together. In humans, much of the behavioural variation between individuals is thought to be accounted for by between three and seven roughly independent personality dimensions (Eysenck and Eysenck 1976; Cloninger 1987; Digman 1990; Almagor et al. 1995), and more than 50 years of twin, family, and adoption studies indicate that around 30% or more of the personality variation between individuals can be accounted for by genetic variation (see Johnson et al. 2008 for a recent review). In other animals, personality traits (or `behavioural syndromes') have been the subject of fewer genetic studies, but there is ample evidence in several species that inter-individual variation in behavioural tendencies is also due substantially to genetic variation (Bakker 1986; Drent et al. 2003; Sinn et al. 2006). The proportion of total trait variation that is accounted for by genetic variation is called broad-sense heritability. This consists of the additive component of heritability (due to the accumulation of the average allelic effects) and may also include nonadditive genetic variation (due to interaction of alleles within (dominance) or between (epistasis) loci). Although it is statistically difficult to distinguish nonadditive from additive genetic variation, there is evidence in humans suggesting that both contribute to personality variation (Eaves et al. 1998; Lake et al. 2000; Keller et al. 2005).
Recently, there has been a rapidly growing interest in understanding the evolutionary basis of heritable personality variation, both in humans (Bouchard and Loehlin 2001; Nettle 2005, 2006; Penke et al. 2007; Alvergne et al. 2010; Gangestad 2010; Nettle and Penke 2010; Verweij et al. 2010; Buss and Hawley 2011; Lukaszewski and Roney 2011; Del Giudice 2012) and in other animals (Dingemanse et al. 2004; Cote et al. 2008; Bergmuller and Taborsky 2010; Dingemanse and Wolf 2010; Dochtermann and Roff 2010; van Oers and Mueller 2010; Wolf and Weissing 2010). Indeed, the broader line of inquiry is one of the major outstanding questions in evolutionary biology (Mitchell-Olds et al. 2007): how is genetic variation maintained in traits where there is selection for only the most advantageous genotypic trait values?
Broadly, there are three main possibilities for explaining the maintenance of genetic variation in personality. The first, selective neutrality, is that genetic variants underlying personality traits do not affect individuals' fitness and so are free to randomly drift in frequency without being affected by selection. Under selective neutrality, individual genetic variants will be lost due to drift, but in the meantime new mutations will also arise and maintain genetic variation in the population (i.e. a mutation-drift balance). An argument against selective neutrality in humans is that personality traits are associated with traits that are presumably related to fitness such as mental and physical health (Lahey 2009; Kotov et al. 2010), mortality (Shipley et al. 2007; Mosing et al. 2012), attractiveness (Lukaszewski and Roney 2011), mating behaviour (Zietsch et al. 2010), and number of offspring (Eaves et al. 1990; Jokela et al. 2009; Alvergne et al. 2010; Jokela et al. 2010). However, positive correlations with one fitness component can be counterbalanced by negative correlations with other fitness components (e.g. Nettle 2005; Alvergne et al. 2010), which could potentially result in a zero net effect on fitness (Roff and Fairbairn 2007). In this vein, MacDonald (1995) proposed that human personality dimensions each represent a continuum of alternative strategies for maximising fitness, so that average fitness would be approximately uniform (selectively neutral) across the normal personality range. Expanding on this view, Nettle (2006) proposed concrete cost-benefit trade-offs associated with five of the major dimensions of personality variation in humans. For example, he proposed that high extraversion conferred the benefits of greater mating and social success, which were balanced by increased risk of accident and injury due to greater novelty seeking behaviour. In line with this type of view, recent theoretical work has emphasised that genetic variants affecting multiple traits can be invisible to selection when multivariate genetic constraints result in little or no variation in fitness effects; this can occur even when the individual traits correlate with fitness and have substantial genetic variation (Walsh and Blows 2009).
A second possibility for explaining the maintenance of genetic variation in personality traits is mutation-selection balance (Lande 1975; Zhang and Hill 2005; Keller and Miller 2006). In this view, deviations from an optimal personality trait level (averaged across environments) are selected against, eliminating alleles that do not predispose to this optimum, and thus reducing genetic variation. In the meantime though, new mutations affecting the trait arise in the population. The vast majority of mutations that affect fitness are deleterious (Eyre-Walker and Keightley 2007), since they randomly disrupt finely-tuned systems. Mutations with strong and dominant effects are purged quickly by selection; mutations with recessive and/or weak effects, which are less visible to selection, may persist for many generations before being eliminated, but are unlikely to become common in the population because of the selection against them (Eyre-Walker 2010). As a result of this and the constant influx of new mutations, individuals each carry an accumulated `mutation load' consisting of alleles that tend to be rare, (partially) recessive, and mildly deleterious. Individuals' mutation loads can vary in many ways, such as their numerousness, recessiveness, and which trait(s) they affect. Traits that are affected by a large number of loci and that therefore have a large “mutational target size” will tend to be disrupted to a larger extent by mutations (Houle 1998). Given that over half the genome is expressed in the brain (Sandberg et al. 2000), it is possible that personality traits have a large mutational target size and that much of their genetic variation is mutational.
The third possibility for explaining the maintenance of genetic variation in personality traits is balancing selection. Under balancing selection, genetic variation is maintained rather than depleted by selection; for example by selection pressures that fluctuate over time and space (environmental heterogeneity), that differ between the sexes (sex-dependent selection), or that favour rarer trait values (negative frequency-dependent selection) or heterozygotes (overdominance). Investigating the relationship of exploratory personality with survival and reproduction rates in Great Tits, Dingemanse et al. (2004) found that selection pressures were opposite in males and females and fluctuated from year to year depending on food and space availability. They argued that this variation in selection was likely to maintain the substantial heritable component of exploratory behaviour in these birds. Similarly, Penke et al (2007) noted the varied and changing physical and social environments that humans have experienced and created for themselves in their evolutionary history, and argued that genetic variation in personality traits is most likely to be actively maintained by balancing selection by environmental heterogeneity, often mediated by negative frequency-dependent selection on life-history strategies. Another perspective (Tooby and Cosmides 1990) is that genetic variation in personality is a side effect of pathogen-driven balancing selection, whereby rare alleles are of higher fitness because pathogens are usually poorly adapted to attacking the rarest host genotypes (Garrigan and Hedrick 2003) – this would be an example of `pleiotropic balancing selection' (Turelli and Barton 2004).
Evaluating these possibilities has proved difficult. In humans, quantifying total fitness and relating it to personality traits is challenging even in contemporary societies, and it is harder still to infer relationships between total fitness and personality traits in the varied environments of our evolutionary history. However, using recently-developed methodologies in statistical genetics, it is possible to test competing predictions from the three evolutionary models. In the present investigation, we attempt to gain insight into several properties of alleles underlying human personality—their number, their effect sizes, their commonness in the population (i.e. minor allele frequency; MAF), and their degree and direction of recessiveness –to gain traction on the mechanisms most likely influencing their genetic variation (Keller et al. 2011a).
Predictions from different mechanisms of maintaining genetic variation
Selective neutrality predicts that the distribution of the additive genetic variance explained as a function of MAF is uniform (Eyre-Walker 2010; Visscher et al. 2011). For example, loci with MAF between 0 and 0.01 should account for 2% of the additive genetic variation, and loci with MAF between 0.01 and 0.50 should account for the other 98%. Furthermore, the proportion of genetic variation that is non-additive should be lower in neutral traits than in traits under directional or stabilising selection because these forms of selection erode additive genetic variation (Fisher 1930; Merila and Sheldon 1999; Stirling et al. 2002; Penke et al. 2007). There should also be no systematic tendency for recessive alleles to influence a personality trait in any particular direction if it is selectively neutral (Lynch and Walsh 1998; DeRose and Roff 1999). Inbreeding depression, which only occurs in the presence of directional recessiveness (Lynch and Walsh 1998), is therefore not expected to affect personality traits if they have been selectively neutral.
Predictions regarding the genetic architecture of traits under mutation-selection balance differ from those of selective neutrality above. If personality traits have been under mutation-selection balance, alleles underlying personality traits should be rarer than expected under selective neutrality (Eyre-Walker 2010). Second, the depletion of additive alleles should result in a substantial nonadditive component to the genetic variation underlying personality (Crnokrak and Roff 1995; Merila and Sheldon 1999; Stirling et al. 2002). Third, inbreeding should affect personality trait levels by pushing them in the opposite direction to that in which selection is acting; the exception is if the population mean is already at the optimum (i.e. stabilising selection), in which case inbreeding depression would not be expected because recessive allele effects pushing the trait away from its mean in each direction would cancel each other out on average.
Evolutionary genetic modelling on all forms of balancing selection reveals that it only maintains polymorphisms at high frequencies (i.e. both alleles are common), because at low allele frequencies the balancing mechanisms become unstable and the rare allele is lost (Mani et al. 1990; Curtsinger et al. 1994; Turelli and Barton 2004; Kopp and Hermisson 2006; Penke et al. 2007). Thus, alleles responsible for personality trait variation should be at a higher frequency than expected under neutrality if they have been maintained by balancing selection (Johnson and Barton 2005). Most models in which balancing selection acts directly on a trait (e.g. negative frequency-dependent selection, sex-dependent selection, overdominance resulting from antagonistic pleiotropy) make the additional prediction that variation can only be maintained at a small number of genetic loci per trait (Curtsinger et al. 1994; Burger 2000; Barton and Keightley 2002; Turelli and Barton 2004; Kopp and Hermisson 2006). However, despite statistical power to detect SNPs of even very small effect size (~0.5% of trait variance), large genome-wide association studies on personality have failed to find strong evidence of association with any SNPs (de Moor et al. 2010; Verweij et al. 2010), suggesting a highly polygenic basis to personality. Nevertheless, modelling suggests that some forms of balancing selection - namely, spatial and temporal environmental heterogeneity, and pleiotropic selection as a side effect of balancing selection on another trait - can maintain variation at a large number of genetic loci, although the requisite conditions are quite restrictive (Burger and Gimelfarb 2002; Turelli and Barton 2004). As such, it remains possible that either of these balancing selection mechanisms could have maintained polymorphisms at many genetic loci underlying personality variation; this would lead to the prediction that the genetic architecture of personality traits consists largely of genetic variants of high frequency. Nonadditive genetic variation in the trait of interest (as opposed to fitness itself) is not a requirement of these latter forms of balancing selection (Turelli and Barton 2004), and since additive genetic variation is maintained (rather than depleted) by balancing selection, a high proportion of nonadditive variation is not expected. Furthermore, there would be no reason to expect inbreeding to affect the trait since that requires directional dominance with respect to the trait (Charlesworth and Charlesworth 1987; Roff 2005).
While they make different sets of predictions, these three main mechanisms for maintaining genetic variation are not mutually exclusive possibilities. For example, if genetic variation in personality traits is under very weak selection (e.g. because trade-offs reduce its fitness consequences), many mutations with small effects will be governed largely by drift and the overall genetic architecture will look like that of selective neutrality with only a slight bias in the frequency distribution of alleles (Eyre-Walker 2010). It is also important to note that current genetic architecture reveals past evolutionary processes, so implications regarding selective pressures on personality traits may not be reflected in today's environment. With these issues in mind, predictions from the different evolutionary mechanisms are summarised in Table 1. (Hill et al. 2008),
Table 1.
Model | No. of causal variantsa | % VA due to common variants (MAF>.01) | VNA/VGb | h2(SNPs)/H2 | Inbreeding affects trait | Useful references |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Selective neutrality | No prediction | 98% | Low | High | No | (Eyre-Walker 2010) |
Mutation-selection balance | Many | << 98% | Higher | Low | Possiblyc | (Eyre-Walker 2010) |
Balancing selection | ||||||
pleiotropic balancing selection | No prediction | > 98% | Low | High | No | (Turelli and Barton 2004) |
environmental heterogeneity | No prediction | > 98% | Low | High | No | (Turelli and Barton 2004) |
negative frequency-dependent selection | Few | > 98% | Low | High | No | (Mani et al. 1990; Kopp and Hermisson 2006) |
sex-dependent selection | Up to two | > 98% | Low | High | No | (Turelli and Barton 2004) |
overdominance for fitness, resulting from antagonistic pleiotropyd | Few | > 98% | Higher | Low | Possiblye | (Curtsinger et al. 1994; Hedrick 1999; Burger 2000) |
VA additive genetic variation;
VNA: nonadditive genetic variation;
MAF: minor allele frequency
Previous research strongly suggests a highly polygenic basis to personality (Verweij et al. 2010)
VNA/VG is expected to be fairly low under neutrality (Hill et al. 2008), though no specific level can be predicted; `higher' predicted levels are in comparison this baseline
Yes under directional selection, no under stabilising selection
These predictions appear to approximately generalise to overdominance in general (Burger 2000)
Inbreeding is expected to decrease fitness, but does not necessarily affect the trait in question
In this study we test the strongest competing predictions of selective neutrality, mutation-selection balance, and balancing selection: (a) the extent to which all common genetic variants contribute to variation in personality; and (b) whether or not inbreeding affects personality traits. To do this, we use genotypic and phenotypic data from four community-based samples from Australia and Finland (total N>8,000) who were assessed on Cloninger's Harm Avoidance, Novelty Seeking, Reward Dependence, and Persistence dimensions. To test (a) above, we use recently-developed methodology (Visscher et al. 2010; Yang et al. 2010; Yang et al. 2011b) to estimate the proportion of variation in these personality traits that can be accounted for by ~270,000 SNPs taken together. This method captures the vast majority of the combined effect of common variants, but much less of the combined effect of rare (MAF<0.01) variants (Yang et al. 2010), since the rarer a variant is the less it can possibly be correlated with a common SNP in a sample of unrelated people (Wray 2005; Wray et al. 2011)1. To test (b), we examine the association between personality traits and the level of inbreeding in the ancestry of each individual as indexed by the extent to which their genome is in `runs of homozygosity' (i.e. homozygous stretches of DNA that can be observed in the offspring of even distant relatives (Keller et al. 2011b)).
Methods
Participants
This study incorporates data from one Australian and three Finnish subsamples. Table 2 provides an overview of available individuals with both phenotype and genotype data.
Table 2.
Sample | Country | N | N males | N females | Age (M±SD) | Year collected | Questionnaire | Genotyping platform |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
YFS | Finland | 1,382 | 634 | 748 | 32.5 (±5.1) | 2001 | TCI, 240 rating scale items | Illumina 670K Custom BeadChip |
HBCS | Finland | 1,441 | 578 | 863 | 63.4 (±2.9) | 2004 | TPQ, 98 dichotomous items | Illumina 610K Quad Chip |
NFBC | Finland | 4,506 | 2,013 | 2,493 | 31† | 1997 | TPQ, 107 dichotomous items | Illumina 370duo Chip |
QIMR | Australia | 5,530 | 2,006 | 3,524 | 36.7 (±12.3) | 1988–1990 | TPQ, 54 dichotomous items | Illumina 317K Illumina HumanCNV370-Quadv3 |
Illumina Human610-Quad | ||||||||
Total | 12,859 | 5,231 | 7,628 |
all participants in the NFBC sample were 31 years old
The Young Finns Study (YFS) subsample derives from longitudinal data collection from five Finnish university cities and surrounding areas (Akerblom et al. 1991; Raitakari et al. 2008). The Helsinki Birth Cohort Study (HBCS) is a birth cohort sample of individuals born at Helsinki University Central Hospital between 1934 and 1944 (Barker et al. 2005; Eriksson et al. 2006; Raikkonen et al. 2008). The Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort (NFBC) is a population based birth cohort comprising 12,058 individuals born in 1966 in the northernmost provinces (Rantakallio 1969).The Queensland Institute of Medical Research (QIMR) subsample includes two population based cohorts of Australian twins and their families. The first cohort was assessed in 1988 and the second in 1990. The total QIMR subsample is 5530 individuals from 2791 independent families. More details about the phenotypic and genotypic data collection at QIMR can be found elsewhere (Keller et al. 2005; Verweij et al. 2010). Note that the core analyses required unrelated individuals; discarding related individuals (using different levels of relatedness as cut-offs for different analyses) reduced the subsamples.
Ethical constraints preclude us from making the phenotypic and genotypic data publically available because participants, who took part in the studies on the condition that their data would remain confidential, could potentially be identified from their DNA.
Personality measures
The different subsamples used different versions of Cloninger's personality scales (see Table 2 - Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire (TPQ short version, see Cloninger et al. 1991; Heath et al. 1994) and Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI, Cloninger et al. 1993). To get homogenous phenotypes, in this study we only included the 54 items of the revised short version of the TPQ (as used in the QIMR sample); all these items were also incorporated in the other questionnaires. This yielded 18 Harm Avoidance, 19 Novelty Seeking, 12 Reward Dependence, and 5 Persistence items. Internal consistency of the scales of this short version of the TPQ were acceptable and comparable with those reported for the full TPQ scales and the short-term test-retest reliability of the scales was good (see Table 1 in Keller et al. 2005 for these statistics on the QIMR subsample). These items and scales are the same as used in Keller et al (2005) to estimate genetic and environmental variance components from twin-family data, except that they analysed one item as contributing to the Reward Dependence scale while we assigned it to the Novelty Seeking scale in accordance with the scales' revision (Cloninger 1994).
The following data cleaning procedure was performed separately for each subsample. The personality scale scores were calculated by summing the relevant item scores, reverse scoring where necessary. (Note that, for consistency, the rating scale used in the YFS study was converted to a 0–1 measure by converting the item scores as follows: 1=0, 2=0.25, 3=0.5, 4=0.75, and 5=1.0.) Missing items were imputed with the sample mean score on the item. Personality scale scores for individuals with more than 25% missing values on that scale were assigned as missing. To minimize departures from normality the scale scores were then angular transformed (Freeman and Tukey 1950; Eaves et al. 1989), as was also done in Keller et al (2005). Last, scale scores were corrected (by regression) for sex, age, age2, sex*age, and sex*age2 effects and each scale was standardised separately per sex. Note that because all individuals in the NFBC sample were 31 years old we only corrected for sex effects in that cohort.
Genotyping and quality control
The genotype data from each subsample first underwent separate standard quality control (QC) procedures (not reported here), before undergoing two additional, more stringent rounds of QC for this project (see Supplementary Table 1). In each subsample, we removed SNPs with a MAF <0.01, with a Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) test p-value<.001, and a call rate <95% (i.e. missing genotype calls > 5%). We further removed individuals with an overall call rate <95%. Note that the QIMR subsample consisted of data from three genotype platforms - SNP and individual call rates were checked separately for data from each platform prior to this study. After combining the data from all subsamples we performed another round of QC on the total sample, again checking for Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium and SNP call rate. Our final sample included 12,859 individuals and 269,616 SNPs that were genotyped in at least 95% of individuals in the sample.
Estimating the proportion of personality trait variation accounted for by all autosomal SNPs
The method used here does not estimate the effect of each individual SNP as is the case in (genome-wide) association studies (Manolio 2010) and genetic prediction studies (Wray et al. 2007) – in those methodologies, summing the estimates of SNP effects also sums the error component of those estimates and thus does not yield an unbiased estimate of the variance explained by the aggregate of all SNP effects. Instead, we computed one unbiased estimate of the aggregate effect of all SNPs. Conceptually, this is achieved by determining to what extent genetic similarity (at the SNPs) between all individuals corresponds to their phenotypic similarity. Technically, the SNP effects are treated as random effects in a mixed linear model and the total trait variance explained by all the SNPs is estimated by restricted maximum likelihood (REML) analysis, as implemented in the freely available Genome-wide Complex Trait Analysis (GCTA) program (Yang et al. 2011b; see http://gump.qimr.edu.au/gcta/). Technical details of the method are described in Yang et al (2010) and Yang et al (2011b), and a plainer language explanation of the method and common misunderstandings is provided by Visscher et al (2010).
We estimated the genetic similarity matrix between all individuals using the 269,616 autosomal SNPs that passed quality control and were common to at least 95% of individuals in the combined sample. We excluded one of each pair of individuals with an estimated genetic similarity of >0.05 (approximately closer than second cousins), in order to reduce the possibility that the phenotypic resemblance between close relatives could be caused by shared environmental effects and/or causal variants not correlated with SNPs but captured by pedigree (Visscher et al. 2010; Yang et al. 2010). This led to an exclusion of 4,197 individuals, resulting in a retained dataset of 8,662 individuals. To check if shared environmental effects and/or causal variants captured by pedigree were still biasing our estimate, we also tested a more stringent cut-off by excluding one of each pair of individuals with an estimated genetic relationship of >0.025 (~ closer than third or fourth cousins). This led to an exclusion of 7,957 individuals, resulting in a retained dataset of 4,902 individuals. Population structure (i.e. differences in allele frequencies between subpopulations which might also differ in personality) can inflate the genetic variance estimates, so to control for this we included the first 20 principal components (eigenvectors of the genetic relatedness matrix) and cohort status (i.e. which subsample they belong to) as covariates in the analysis. We checked to what extent population structure would have affected the results by comparing results from analyses with and without the 20 principal components as covariates.
While we have dense SNP coverage across the genome, the SNPs may not be in complete linkage disequilibrium (LD) (i.e. perfectly correlated) with all common causal variants. We therefore adjusted the variance estimates explained by our SNPs for incomplete LD with causal variants, under the assumption that the causal variants have the same allelic spectrum as the genotyped SNPs. This adjustment procedure is based on a formula empirically established by Yang et al. (2010) and is described in detail in their paper. The adjustment is implemented in the GCTA program. In this way we tested to what extent the variance explained by the SNPs captured the variance explained by all common variants (including common structural variants e.g. copy number variants). Additionally, we tested whether including more SNPs in our analyses (all SNPs that were genotyped for at least a third of our sample, N=532,030 SNPs) would affect the variance accounted for.
Because there is some evidence that partly different genetic factors influence males and females for Harm Avoidance and Reward Dependence (Keller et al. 2005), for these scales we performed separate analyses by sex in addition to the main analyses with the sexes pooled.
Testing the effect of inbreeding on personality traits
To test whether inbreeding influenced the personality traits we obtained an index of the level of inbreeding in each individual's ancestry based on their SNP data, and then tested if this coefficient was correlated with the personality scale scores.
Using PLINK software (Purcell et al. 2007), we quantified individuals' level of inbreeding by estimating the proportion of their genome that is in runs of homozygosity (ROH), by summing the total length of all their autosomal ROHs divided by the total SNP-mappable autosomal genome length (2.77*109). ROHs are homozygous stretches of DNA that can be observed in the offspring of even distant relatives (Howrigan et al. 2011; Keller et al. 2011b)). The Runs of Homozygosity program (PLINK; Purcell et al. 2007) slides a moving window of a specified number of SNPs across the genome in order to detect long runs of homozygous genotypes. Runs are flexibly definable in terms of the required number of homozygous SNPs spanning a certain distance.
We define ROHs following recommendations in Howrigan et al. (2011), in which simulations were used to determine the ROH definitions that yield the most power to detect distant inbreeding (i.e. within the last 50 generations). Accordingly, we define ROHs as stretches of at least 65 continuously homozygous SNPs, using lightly pruned SNP data (i.e. removing SNPs with a MAF<.05 and with a variance inflation factor [VIF] > 10 using PLINK (Purcell et al. 2007) (see Supplementary Table 2)). To minimise underestimation of the number of runs, three (~5%) missing genotypes within an otherwise unbroken homozygous segment were allowed in a run. Further details of the parameters used - based on recommendations from Howrigan et al. (2011) - can be found in Supplementary Information Table 3.
Table 3.
Personality Scale | H2 | N | h2SNPs (SE) | p-value | h2SNPs/H2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Harm Avoidance | .36 | 8613 | .066 (.037) | .04 | .18 |
Novelty Seeking | .34 | 8620 | .099 (.036) | .003 | .28 |
Reward Dependence | .30 | 8606 | .042 (.036) | .12 | .14 |
Persistence | .28 | 8618 | .081 (.037) | .01 | .29 |
Includes SNPs genotyped for at least 95% of the sample, excludes one of each pair of individuals with an estimated genetic relatedness > .05.
H2 = heritability estimate of the trait from AE models of twin-siblings (from Keller et al. (2005)) – p <.001 for each trait.
N refers to the size of sample that h2SNPs is estimated from
h2SNPs = proportion of variance accounted for by all autosomal SNPs;
SE=standard error of estimate.
p-values denote whether the variance accounted for by SNPs is significantly different from zero.
Additionally, we examined the relative importance of close versus distant inbreeding by comparing the effect on personality traits of short (<5Mb) versus long (>5Mb) ROHs. ROHs with a length of 5Mb or less should originate from a common ancestor ten or more generations ago, whereas longer ROHs should originate from a common ancestor less than ten generations ago.
To test the robustness of our results to different types of inbreeding measures, we also calculated a different type of inbreeding coefficient based on the correlation between uniting gametes, as implemented in GCTA (i.e. (Yang et al. 2011b); termed Falt in Keller et al 2011b); no pruning was used). We chose this coefficient over the other two inbreeding coefficients implemented in GCTA because it is independent of the MAF and therefore less biased and is predicted to have lower error (Yang et al. 2011b).
For these analyses there was no need for a stringent genetic relatedness cut-off as in the heritability estimation described earlier, but we did exclude one of each pair of individuals with a genetic relatedness larger than 0.3 so that twin and sibling pairs did not bias the p-values. This resulted in a sample of 10,247 individuals. Population structure (first 20 principle components) was corrected for before analysis.
Results
Descriptive statistics of the four personality scales in the four subsamples are in Supplementary Table 4, and correlations between the personality scales are in Supplementary Table 5.
Table 4.
Runs of homozygosity | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Personality scale | N | Proportion of genome in ROH | Close inbreeding: proportion of genome in ROH > 5Mb | Distant inbreeding: proportion of genome in ROH < 5Mb | |||
r | Beta (SE) | r | Beta (SE) | r | Beta (SE) | ||
Harm Avoidance | 10,197 | .058** | 7.65 (1.31) | .047** | 9.11 (1.91) | .051** | 13.12 (2.57) |
Novelty Seeking | 10,202 | −.052** | −6.81 (1.30) | −.042** | −8.08 (1.90) | −.045** | −11.75 (2.56) |
Reward Dependence | 10,185 | −.038** | −4.92 (1.30) | −.030** | −5.83 (1.90) | −.033** | −8.52 (2.55) |
Persistence | 10,202 | −.006 | −0.76 (1.30) | −.005 | −1.02 (1.90) | −.004 | −1.10 (2.56) |
p<.01
Variance explained by all autosomal SNPs
Common SNPs explained between 4.2% and 9.9% of the total variation in the four personality traits, at an average of 7.2% (Table 3). Due to the large sample size, the standard errors of these estimates were small (~3.7%), and estimates for Harm Avoidance, Novelty Seeking and Persistence were significantly different from zero (p<.05). Correcting for incomplete LD between the SNPs and causal variants, or almost doubling the number of SNPs used, had a negligible effect on the estimates (see Supplementary Table 6), indicating that our estimates captured the vast majority of all common additive genetic variant effects. Re-running the tests without the 20 principal components as covariates increased the estimates only a little (see Supplementary Table 6), and re-running analyses with a more stringent cut-off for relatedness (0.025) somewhat lowered the estimates for Harm Avoidance, Novelty Seeking and Persistence (see Supplementary Table 6), suggesting that our main estimates (Table 3) could be slightly inflated due to causal variants not correlated with SNPs but captured by pedigree. As such, the estimates in Table 3 are best considered upper limits, reinforcing that common additive genetic variant effects play only a minor role in personality variation.
Based on previous findings suggestive of sex-limitation, we also re-ran the analyses separately for males and females for Harm Avoidance and Reward Dependence. These estimates (see Supplementary Table 6) did not differ substantially from those in Table 3. We also tested for heterogeneity of the estimates from the different subsamples - the estimates from individual subsamples were very imprecise and varied widely, but a formal test revealed no significant heterogeneity (p>0.1 for all scales).
Total heritability estimates for the different scales range from 0.28 to 0.36, as obtained from an additive genetic + residual (AE) model based on a large twin-sibling study (N~13,000; Keller et al. 2005). These are essentially estimates of broad-sense heritability and include any nonadditive genetic variance (separate unbiased estimates of additive and nonadditive genetic influences are not available), whereas the GCTA heritability estimates from the SNPs (h2SNPs) do not include nonadditive genetic variance – this is considered further in the Discussion section.
Overall, these results suggest that common additive genetic variants account for a small percentage (~20%) of the total genetic variation in all four personality traits, consistent with mutation-selection balance but not consistent with selective neutrality or balancing selection models for highly polygenic traits. The rest of the genetic variation is likely to comprise of rare variant effects and/or some combination of dominance and epistasis.
The effect of inbreeding on personality traits
We tested for a correlation between personality traits and an index of inbreeding in individuals' ancestry – i.e. the proportion of the genome in runs of homozygosity (Table 4). Descriptives of the number of runs and the total proportion of the genome in homozygous runs for the overall sample and each subsample are shown in Supplementary Table 7. As shown in Table 4, proportion of genome in runs of homozygosity correlated significantly and positively with Harm Avoidance, and significantly and negatively with Novelty Seeking and Reward Dependence. The alternative inbreeding coefficient based on uniting gametes () (Yang et al. 2011b) gave very similar results, the only difference being that Persistence was also significantly correlated with inbreeding (negatively, p=.02, see Supplementary Table 8). Multiple regression (data not shown) indicated the significant effects were at least partly unique to each trait, rather than a result of their intercorrelation. Furthermore, results were almost identical whether or not inbreeding coefficients were winsorised (i.e. extreme values set at 3 standard deviations from the mean; see Supplementary Table 8), suggesting that the results are not driven by outliers resulting from close inbreeding. Finally, significant effects could be observed within separate subsamples (though not consistently, due to reduced power), reinforcing that the effect is not due to population stratification (see Supplementary Table 8).
Table 4 also shows that both short (<5Mb) and long (>5Mb) ROHs affected personality traits highly significantly, and in the same directions to very similar degrees. ROH (short) and ROH (long) did not correlate very highly with each other (r=0.40), and both predicted the traits when entered together in multiple regression (data not shown), implying very similar and somewhat independent effects of distant and close inbreeding, respectively.
Overall, these results provide strong evidence that inbreeding affects some personality traits, consistent with being influenced by a load of mutations that tend to be rare, recessive, and deleterious, as predicted under mutation-selection balance. These inbreeding effects are not consistent with selective neutrality or balancing selection models for highly polygenic traits, as these provide no reason to expect bias in the direction of dominance across many loci.
Discussion
Using ~270,000 SNPs, we created a genetic similarity matrix of over 8,000 unrelated individuals. By determining to what extent individuals' genetic similarity corresponded to their similarity in personality traits, we estimated the proportion of total personality trait variance that could be explained by the additive genetic effects of common causal variants that are associated with these SNPs. The variation explained by SNPs (4.2–9.9%) was statistically significant in three of the four traits, but for all four traits it represented a small proportion (~20%) of the total genetic variation previously estimated by various designs (twin, family, and adoption studies). The heritability estimated using the 270,000 SNPs captured the effects of the vast majority of common (MAF>0.01) causal variants, due to linkage disequilibrium between the SNPs and other common variants, but only a small portion of the genetic variation due to rare causal variants. As such, these results suggest that common additive genetic variants account for little of the variation in Cloninger's personality traits, and therefore rare genetic variants and/or some combination of dominance and epistasis are likely to account for most of the variation. This is consistent with the hypothesis that genetic variation in human personality traits has been maintained by mutation-selection balance, but is less consistent with it being selectively neutral or maintained by pleiotropic balancing selection or balancing selection via environmental heterogeneity. Overdominance might also be consistent with these results because it predicts high levels of dominance variation, but it also predicts that genetic variation is due to common alleles at a relatively small number of loci per trait (Curtsinger et al. 1994; Burger 2000), which is inconsistent with previous research on these and other personality scales (de Moor et al. 2010; Verweij et al. 2010). The contribution of common additive genetic variants to genetic variation in personality traits is less than that of some other traits that have been subject to the same analysis - for example, the proportion of the genetic variation that can be explained by common SNPs is around half for height (Yang et al. 2010) and intelligence (Davies et al. 2011), one-third for risk of schizophrenia (Lee et al. under review), and one-quarter for body mass index (Yang et al. 2011a).
We also investigated whether inbreeding affects personality by testing for correlation of personality traits with runs of homozygosity, which are homozygous stretches of DNA that indicate distant as well as close inbreeding. We found that inbreeding correlated significantly and positively with Harm Avoidance, and negatively with both Reward Dependence and Novelty Seeking, but did not correlate significantly with Persistence. The absolute values of the correlations were very small, but this was to be expected given the modest effects of inbreeding depression reported in the literature (Roff 1997; Charlesworth and Willis 2009) and the small variation in inbreeding in outbred populations (Keller et al. 2011b). An effect of inbreeding on personality traits is consistent with mutation-selection balance, but is not expected under selective neutrality, balancing selection via environmental heterogeneity, or pleiotropic balancing selection (Charlesworth and Charlesworth 1987; Turelli and Barton 2004; Roff 2005). Consistent with inbreeding pushing traits towards their low fitness ends, high Novelty Seeking, high Reward Dependence, and low Harm Avoidance are all associated with the socially desirable (and supposed high-fitness) end of the so-called `general factor of personality' (Rushton and Irwing 2008; Rushton et al. 2009). The lack of a significant inbreeding effect on Persistence might suggest that the population mean is close to the optimum (i.e. under stabilising rather than directional selection) or might be due to lack of power to detect a true inbreeding effect. If our inbreeding results reflect the influence of a load of pleiotropic deleterious mutations, the three personality traits should be genetically intercorrelated in line with the direction of the inbreeding effects – i.e. high Harm Avoidance with low Novelty Seeking and low Reward Dependence. This is indeed what has been found in previous research (Gillespie et al. 2003).
Our findings have important implications for how personality is positioned in an evolutionary framework. Results consistent with most of the genetic variation being due to rare variants and/or nonadditive genetic effects suggest that personality traits have been under selection, and results consistent with inbreeding depression suggest that three of the personality traits have been under directional selection. Directional selection does not necessarily mean that extremely high or low values are favoured, just that the mean trait level in the population deviates from the optimum. Several possibilities exist for why the means of personality traits are not at the evolutionarily optimal levels. One is that personality traits are condition-dependent; for example, Lukaszewski and Roney (2011) have argued that high extraversion (closely related to Novelty Seeking) is usually displayed by physically attractive individuals (through facultative calibration) because it is a more beneficial strategy for them than for less extraverted individuals. Under this model, the heritable variation in extraversion is a side effect of the heritable variation in physical attractiveness (which is presumably condition-dependent and under mutation-selection balance). Similarly, low (optimal) levels of Harm Avoidance might only be adaptive in high-fitness individuals that are able to successfully avoid the dangers associated with risk taking behaviours.
There are several limitations to the current research that warrant caution regarding the conclusions we have drawn. First, the Cloninger scales may not represent a comprehensive assessment of personality, and it remains to be seen to what extent the results generalise to other personality traits, such as the Big Five. However, results from an international consortium show that SNP-based heritability estimates for two of the Big Five traits, Extraversion and Neuroticism, very closely accord with our results for the related traits Novelty Seeking and Harm Avoidance, respectively (Vinkhuyzen et al. in press). Second, we had to rely on previous twin-sibling studies for the heritability of Cloninger's scales due to all genetic variants. Twin-sibling studies provide fairly robust estimates of broad-sense heritability (i.e. H2 in Table 3), but they do not allow separate unbiased estimates of additive and nonadditive genetic variation (Keller and Coventry 2005; Keller et al. 2010). Extended twin-family designs (which can make good estimates of these parameters) are only available for Neuroticism (closely related to Cloninger's Harm Avoidance), for which a very large (N=45,850) study including parents, aunts/uncles, and spouses estimated additive and nonadditive genetic influences in females at 34% and 13% respectively, and in males at 31% and 10%.
While it is unfortunate not to have good estimates of separate additive and nonadditive genetic variance components for Cloninger's scales, it should be remembered that a greater proportion of a trait's genetic variation is expected to be nonadditive if it is maintained by mutation-selection balance than if it is maintained by selective neutrality, pleiotropic balancing selection, or environmental heterogeneity. As such, our conclusion that genetic variation in personality traits is best explained by mutation-selection balance would hold regardless of the extent to which the gap between h2(SNPs) and H2 is due to rare variants or genetic nonadditivity.
A third limitation is that we may have overestimated the variance accounted for by common genetic variants. One reason is that population stratification can potentially inflate the variance accounted for by SNPs even after controlling for population structure (Browning and Browning 2011), though probably very little (Goddard et al. 2011). Another reason is that common genotyped SNPs that trace distant relatedness will to some extent reflect the relatedness at old causal mutations that have been co-inherited with the SNPs, so the effects of these rare variants may be partially captured. As such, our estimates are best considered as an upper limit of the additive variance that can be due to common genetic variants, but this only strengthens our conclusions regarding the small role they play in personality traits and the evolutionary implications of this.
A fourth limitation is that we cannot rule out the possibility that certain personality traits cause greater inbreeding, rather than (or as well as) the other way around. For example, those with greater Novelty Seeking may tend to choose a mate further from their birthplace (and, possibly, less related to themselves) - resulting offspring may inherit greater Novelty Seeking and also have a lower inbreeding coefficient.
Notwithstanding these limitations, this study provides empirical findings that bolster our understanding of the evolutionary genetics of personality, suggesting that genetic variation is maintained primarily by a balance between an influx of deleterious mutations and selection against them. While this study focuses on human personality, the results may help guide theory and empirical research in other species and other traits; indeed, the methodology used here can in principle be used to investigate maintenance of variation in any trait in any species, providing sufficiently large samples can be obtained. Furthermore, methodological developments in the near future (e.g. low-cost genome sequencing) may allow more direct assessment of the effect of mutation load on personality and other traits, opening rich new avenues for exploration.
Acknowledgements
We thank Naomi Wray and Anna Vinkhuyzen for valuable comments on a draft of the manuscript.
NFBC: NFBC1966 received financial support from the Academy of Finland (project grants 104781, 120315, 129269, 1114194, Center of Excellence in Complex Disease Genetics and SALVE), University Hospital Oulu, Biocenter, University of Oulu, Finland (75617), the European Commission (EURO-BLCS, Framework 5 award QLG1-CT-2000-01643), NHLBI grant 5R01HL087679-02 through the STAMPEED program (1RL1MH083268-01), NIH/NIMH (5R01MH63706:02), ENGAGE project and grant agreement HEALTH-F4-2007-201413, the Medical Research Council, UK (G0500539, G0600705, PrevMetSyn/SALVE), roadmap grants for the Consortium for Neuropsychiatric Phenomics, UL1-DE019580 and RL1MH083268, for EC; and NIH/NINDS training grant T32 NS048004 for EC. The DNA extractions, sample quality controls, biobank up-keeping and aliquotting was performed in the National Public Health Institute, Biomedicum Helsinki, Finland and supported financially by the Academy of Finland and Biocentrum Helsinki. We thank Professor Paula Rantakallio (launch of NFBC1966 and 1986), Ms Outi Tornwall and Ms Minttu Jussila (DNA biobanking).
YFS: The Young Finns Study has been financially supported by the Academy of Finland: grants 126925, 121584, 124282, 129378 (Salve), 117787 (Gendi), and 41071 (Skidi), the Social Insurance Institution of Finland, Kuopio, Tampere and Turku University Hospital Medical Funds (grant 9M048 for T.L.), Juho Vainio Foundation, Paavo Nurmi Foundation, Finnish Foundation of Cardiovascular Research and Finnish Cultural Foundation, Tampere Tuberculosis Foundation, Emil Aaltonen Foundation (T.L. & M.H.), and Ella and Georg Ehrnrooth Foundation (MH). The expert technical assistance in data management by Irina Lisinen and Ville Aalto are gratefully acknowledged.
HBCS: We acknowledge support from grants from the Academy of Finland, the Finnish Diabetes Research Society, Folkhälsan Research Foundation, Novo Nordisk Foundation, Finska Läkaresällskapet, Signe and Ane Gyllenberg Foundation, University of Helsinki, European Science Foundation (EUROSTRESS), Ministry of Education, Ahokas Foundation, Emil Aaltonen Foundation
QIMR: We thank Dixie Statham (sample collection); Lisa Bowdler, Steven Crooks (DNA processing); David Smyth, Harry Beeby, and Daniel Park (IT support). Funding was provided by the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (241944, 339462, 389927, 389875, 389891, 389892, 389938, 442915, 442981, 496739, 552485, 552498, 613608), the FP-5 GenomEUtwin Project (QLG2-CT-2002-01254), and the U.S. National Institutes of Health (NIH grants AA07535, AA10248, AA13320, AA13321, AA13326, AA14041, DA012854, MH66206). A portion of the genotyping was carried out at the Center for Inherited Disease Research, Baltimore (CIDR), through an access award to our late colleague Dr. Richard Todd (Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis). K.J.H.V. is supported by an ANZ Trustees PhD scholarship in Medical Research. B.P.Z is supported by a Discovery Early Career Researcher Award from the Australian Research Council.
Footnotes
Common genotyped SNPs that trace distant relatedness will to some extent reflect the relatedness at distant causal mutations that have been co-inherited with the SNPs, so some of the combined effect of distant rare variants may be captured, but not the effect of relatively recent mutations.
References
- Akerblom HK, Uhari M, Pesonen E, Dahl M, Kaprio EA, Nuutinen EM, Pietikainen M, Salo MK, Aromaa A, Kannas L, Keltikangasjarvinen L, Kuusela V, Rasanen L, Ronnemaa T, Knip M, Telama R, Valimaki I, Pyorala K, Viikari J. Cardiovascular risk in young Finns. Annals of Medicine. 1991;23:35–39. doi: 10.3109/07853899109147928. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Almagor M, Tellegen A, Waller NG. The Big 7 Model - A cross-cultural replication and further exploration of the basic dimensions of natural-language trait descriptors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995;69:300–307. [Google Scholar]
- Alvergne A, Jokela M, Lummaa V. Personality and reproductive success in a high-fertility human population. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010;107:11745–11750. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1001752107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bakker TCM. Aggressiveness in sticklebacks (Gasterosteus Aculeatus): A behavior-genetic study. Behaviour. 1986;98:1–144. [Google Scholar]
- Barker DJP, Osmond C, Forsen TJ, Kajantie E, Eriksson JG. Trajectories of growth among children who have coronary events as adults. New England Journal of Medicine. 2005;353:1802–1809. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa044160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barton NH, Keightley PD. Understanding quantitative genetic variation. Nature Reviews Genetics. 2002;3:11–21. doi: 10.1038/nrg700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bergmuller R, Taborsky M. Animal personality due to social niche specialisation. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2010;25:504–511. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2010.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouchard TJ, Loehlin JC. Genes, evolution, and personality. Behavior Genetics. 2001;31:243–273. doi: 10.1023/a:1012294324713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Browning SR, Browning BL. Population structure can inflate SNP-based heritability estimates. American Journal of Human Genetics. 2011;89:191–193. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2011.05.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burger R. The mathematical theory of selection, recombination, and mutation. Wiley; Chichester, UK: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Burger R, Gimelfarb A. Fluctuating environments and the role of mutation in maintaining quantitative genetic variation. Genetical Research. 2002;80:31–46. doi: 10.1017/s0016672302005682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buss DM, Hawley PH, editors. The evolution of personality and individual differences. Oxford University Press; New York: 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Charlesworth D, Charlesworth B. Inbreeding depression and its evolutionary consequences. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 1987;18:237–268. [Google Scholar]
- Charlesworth D, Willis JH. The genetics of inbreeding depression. Nature Reviews Genetics. 2009;10:783–796. doi: 10.1038/nrg2664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cloninger CR. A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants: A proposal. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1987;44:573–588. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800180093014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cloninger CR. The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A guide to its development and use. Washington University; St Louis, Missouri: 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Cloninger CR, Przybeck TR, Svrakic DM. The Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire:United States normative data. Psychological Reports. 1991;69:1047–1057. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1991.69.3.1047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cloninger CR, Svrakic NM, Przybeck TR. A psychobiological model of temperament and character. General Archives of Psychiatry. 1993;50:975–990. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820240059008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cote J, Dreiss A, Clobert J. Social personality trait and fitness. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences. 2008;275:2851–2858. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.0783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crnokrak P, Roff DA. Dominance variance: Associations with selection and fitness. Heredity. 1995;75:530–540. [Google Scholar]
- Curtsinger JW, Service PM, Prout T. Antagonistic pleiotropy, reversal of dominance, and genetic polymorphism. American Naturalist. 1994;144:210–228. [Google Scholar]
- Davies G, Tenesa A, Payton A, Yang J, Harris SE, Liewald D, Ke X, Le Hellard S, Christoforou A, Luciano M, McGhee K, Lopez L, Gow AJ, Corley J, Redmond P, Fox HC, Haggarty P, Whalley LJ, McNeill G, Goddard ME, Espeseth T, Lundervold AJ, Reinvang I, Pickles A, Steen VM, Ollier W, Porteous DJ, Horan M, Starr JM, Pendleton N, Visscher PM, Deary IJ. Genome-wide association studies establish that human intelligence is highly heritable and polygenic. Mol Psychiatry. 2011 doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Moor MHM, Costa PT, Terracciano A, Krueger RF, de Geus EJC, Toshiko T, Penninx BWJH, Esko T, Madden PAF, Derringer J, Amin N, Willemsen G, Hottenga JJ, Distel MA, Uda M, Sanna S, Spinhoven P, Hartman CA, Sullivan P, Realo A, Allik J, Heath AC, Pergadia ML, Agrawal A, Lin P, Grucza R, Nutile T, Ciullo M, Rujescu D, Giegling I, Konte B, Widen E, Cousminer DL, Eriksson JG, Palotie A, Peltonen L, Luciano M, Tenesa A, Davies G, Lopez LM, Hansell NK, Medland SE, Ferrucci L, Schlessinger D, Montgomery GW, Wright MJ, Aulchenko YS, Janssens ACJW, Oostra BA, Metspalu A, Abecasis GR, Deary IJ, Raikkonen K, Bierut LJ, Martin NG, van Duijn CM, Boomsma DI. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for personality. Mol Psychiatry. 2010 doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Del Giudice M. Sex ratio dynamics and fluctuating selection on personality. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 2012;297:48–60. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2011.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeRose MA, Roff DA. A comparison of inbreeding depression in life-history and morphological traits in animals. Evolution. 1999;53:1288–1292. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1999.tb04541.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Digman JM. Personality structure: Emergence of the 5-factor model. Annual Review of Psychology. 1990;41:417–440. [Google Scholar]
- Dingemanse NJ, Both C, Drent PJ, Tinbergen JM. Fitness consequences of avian personalities in a fluctuating environment. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences. 2004;271:847–852. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2680. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dingemanse NJ, Wolf M. Recent models for adaptive personality differences: a review. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences. 2010;365:3947–3958. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dochtermann NA, Roff DA. Applying a quantitative genetics framework to behavioural syndrome research. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2010;365:4013–4020. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drent PJ, van Oers K, van Noordwijk AJ. Realized heritability of personalities in the great tit (Parus major) Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences. 2003;270:45–51. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eaves LJ, Eysenck HJ, Martin JM. Genes, culture and personality: an empirical approach. Academic Press; London: 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Eaves LJ, Heath AC, Neale MC, Hewitt JK, Martin NG. Sex differences and non-additivity in the effects of genes on personality. Twin Research. 1998;1:131–137. doi: 10.1375/136905298320566267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eaves LJ, Martin NG, Heath AC, Hewitt JK, Neale MC. Personality and reproductive fitness. Behavior Genetics. 1990;20:563–568. doi: 10.1007/BF01065872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eriksson JG, Osmond C, Kajantie E, Forsen TJ, Barker DJP. Patterns of growth among children who later develop type 2 diabetes or its risk factors. Diabetologia. 2006;49:2853–2858. doi: 10.1007/s00125-006-0459-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eyre-Walker A. Genetic architecture of a complex trait and its implications for fitness and genome-wide association studies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010;107:1752–1756. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0906182107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eyre-Walker A, Keightley PD. The distribution of fitness effects of new mutations. Nat Rev Genet. 2007;8:610–618. doi: 10.1038/nrg2146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eysenck HJ, Eysenck SBG. Psychoticism as a dimension of personality. Hodder and Stoughton; London: 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Fisher RA. The genetical theory of natural selection. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 1930. [Google Scholar]
- Freeman MF, Tukey JW. Transformations related to the angular and the square root. Annals of Mathematical Statistics. 1950;21:607–611. [Google Scholar]
- Gangestad SW. Evolutionary biology looks at behavior genetics. Personality and Individual Differences. 2010;49:289–295. [Google Scholar]
- Garrigan D, Hedrick PW. Detecting adaptive molecular polymorphism: Lessons from the MHC. Evolution. 2003;57:1707–1722. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2003.tb00580.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gillespie NA, Cloninger CR, Heath AC, Martin NG. The genetic and environmental relationship between Cloninger's dimensions of temperament and character. Personality and Individual Differences. 2003;35:1931–1946. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00042-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goddard ME, Lee H, Yang J, Wray NR, Visscher PM. Population structure can inflate SNP-based heritability estimates response. American Journal of Human Genetics. 2011;89:193–195. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2011.05.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heath AC, Cloninger CR, Martin NG. Testing a model for the genetic-structure of personality - a comparison of the personality systems of Cloninger and Eysenck. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994;66:762–775. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.66.4.762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hedrick PW. Antagonistic pleiotropy and genetic polymorphism: a perspective. Heredity. 1999;82:126–133. [Google Scholar]
- Hill WG, Goddard ME, Visscher PM. Data and theory point to mainly additive genetic variance for complex traits. Plos Genetics. 2008;4 doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1000008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houle D. How should we explain variation in the genetic variance of traits? Genetica. 1998;102–103:241–253. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howrigan DP, Simonson MA, Keller MC. Detecting autozygosity using runs of homozygosity: A comparison of three autozygosity detection algorithms. BMC Genomics. 2011 doi: 10.1186/1471-2164-12-460. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson AM, Vernon PA, Feiler AR. Behavioral genetic studies of personality: An introduction and review of the results of 50+ years of research. In: Boyle G, Matthews G, Saklofske D, editors. Handbook of personality theory and assessment. Sage; London: 2008. pp. 145–173. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson T, Barton N. Theoretical models of selection and mutation on quantitative traits. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences. 2005;360:1411–1425. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jokela M, Hintsa T, Hintsanen M, Keltikangas-Jarvinen L. Adult temperament and childbearing over the life course. European Journal of Personality. 2010;24:151–166. [Google Scholar]
- Jokela M, Kivimaki M, Elovainio M, Keltikangas-Jarvinen L. Personality and having children: A two-way relationship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2009;96:218–230. doi: 10.1037/a0014058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller MC, Coventry WL. Quantifying and addressing parameter indeterminacy in the classical twin design. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2005;8:201–213. doi: 10.1375/1832427054253068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller MC, Coventry WL, Heath AC, Martin NG. Widespread evidence for non-additive genetic variation in Cloninger's and Eysenck's personality dimensions using a twin plus sibling design. Behavior Genetics. 2005;35:707–721. doi: 10.1007/s10519-005-6041-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller MC, Howrigan DP, Simonson MA. Theory and methods in evolutionary behavioral genetics. In: Buss DM, Hawley PH, editors. The evolution of personality and individual differences. Oxford University Press; New York: 2011a. [Google Scholar]
- Keller MC, Medland SE, Duncan LE. Are extended twin family designs worth the trouble? A comparison of the bias, precision, and accuracy of parameters estimated in four twin family models. Behavior Genetics. 2010;40:377–393. doi: 10.1007/s10519-009-9320-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller MC, Miller G. Resolving the paradox of common, harmful, heritable mental disorders: Which evolutionary genetic models work best? Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2006;29:385–404. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X06009095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller MC, Visscher PM, Goddard ME. Quantification of inbreeding due to distant ancestors and its detection using dense single nucleotide polymorphism data. Genetics. 2011b;189:237–U920. doi: 10.1534/genetics.111.130922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kopp M, Hermisson J. The evolution of genetic architecture under frequency-dependent disruptive selection. Evolution. 2006;60:1537–1550. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D. Linking “Big” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2010;136:768–821. doi: 10.1037/a0020327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lahey BB. Public health significance of Neuroticism. American Psychologist. 2009;64:241–256. doi: 10.1037/a0015309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lake RIE, Eaves LJ, Maes HHM, Heath AC, Martin NG. Further evidence against the environmental transmission of individual differences in neuroticism from a collaborative study of 45,850 twins and relatives on two continents. Behavior Genetics. 2000;30:223–233. doi: 10.1023/a:1001918408984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lande R. The maintenance of genetic variability by mutation in a polygenic character with linked loci. Genetical Research. 1975;26:221–235. doi: 10.1017/s0016672300016037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee SH, DeCandia T, Ripke S, Yang J, The PGC-SCZ Consortium. The International Schizophrenia Consortium. The MGS Consortium. Sullivan PF, Goddard ME, Keller MC, Visscher PM, Wray NR. One quarter of variation in liability to schizophrenia is tagged by SNPs and partly reflects common causal variants. under review. [Google Scholar]
- Lukaszewski AW, Roney JR. The origins of extraversion: Joint effects of facultative calibration and genetic polymorphism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2011;37:409–421. doi: 10.1177/0146167210397209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch M, Walsh B. Genetics and analysis of quantitative traits. Sinauer; Sunderland, MA: 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Macdonald K. Evolution, the 5-factor model, and levels of personality. Journal of Personality. 1995;63:525–567. [Google Scholar]
- Mani GS, Clarke BC, Shelton PR. A model of quantitative traits under frequency-dependent balancing selection. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences. 1990;240:15–28. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1990.0024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manolio TA. Genomewide association studies and assessment of the risk of disease. New England Journal of Medicine. 2010;363:166–176. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra0905980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merila J, Sheldon BC. Genetic architecture of fitness and nonfitness traits: empirical patterns and development of ideas. Heredity. 1999;83:103–109. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2540.1999.00585.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell-Olds T, Willis JH, Goldstein DB. Which evolutionary processes influence natural genetic variation for phenotypic traits? Nature Reviews Genetics. 2007;8:845–856. doi: 10.1038/nrg2207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mosing MA, Medland SE, McRae A, Landers JG, Wright MJ, Martin NG. Genetic Influences on Life Span and Its Relationship to Personality: A 16-Year Follow-Up Study of a Sample of Aging Twins. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2012;74:16–22. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182385784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nettle D. An evolutionary approach to the extraversion continuum. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2005;26:363–373. [Google Scholar]
- Nettle D. The evolution of personality variation in humans and other animals. American Psychologist. 2006;61:622–631. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.6.622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nettle D, Penke L. Personality: Bridging the literatures from human psychology and behavioural ecology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2010;365:4043–4050. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Penke L, Denissen J, Miller G. The evolutionary genetics of personality. European Journal of Personality. 2007;21:549–587. [Google Scholar]
- Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, Thomas L, Ferreira MAR, Bender D, Maller J, Sklar P, de Bakker PIW, Daly MJ, Sham PC. PLINK: A tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. American Journal of Human Genetics. 2007;81:559–575. doi: 10.1086/519795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raikkonen K, Pesonen AK, Heinonen K, Lahti J, Kajantie E, Forsen T, Osmond C, Barker DJP, Eriksson JG. Infant growth and hostility in adult life. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2008;70:306–313. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181651638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raitakari OT, Juonala M, Ronnemaa T, Keltikangas-Jarvinen L, Rasanen L, Pietikainen M, Hutri-Kahonen N, Taittonen L, Jokinen E, Marniemi J, Jula A, Telama R, Kahonen M, Lehtimaki T, Akerblom HK, Viikari JSA. Cohort Profile: The Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2008;37:1220–1226. doi: 10.1093/ije/dym225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rantakallio P. Groups at risk in low birth weight infants and perinatal mortality. Paediatr Scand Suppl. 1969;193:43. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roff DA. Evolutionary quantitative genetics. Chapman & Hall; New York: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Roff DA. Variation and life-history evolution. In: Hallgrimsson B, Hall BK, editors. Variation: A central concept in biology. Elsevier Academic Press; New York: 2005. pp. 333–355. [Google Scholar]
- Roff DA, Fairbairn DJ. The evolution of trade-offs: where are we? Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2007;20:433–447. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01255.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rushton JP, Bons TA, Ando J, Hur YM, Irwing P, Vernon PA, Petrides KV, Barbaranelli C. A General Factor of Personality From Multitrait-Multimethod Data and Cross-National Twins. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2009;12:356–365. doi: 10.1375/twin.12.4.356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rushton JP, Irwing P. A General Factor of Personality (GFP) from two meta-analyses of the Big Five: Digman (1997) and Mount, Barrick, Scullen, and Rounds (2005) Personality and Individual Differences. 2008;45:679–683. [Google Scholar]
- Sandberg R, Yasuda R, Pankratz DG, Carter TA, Del Rio JA, Wodicka L, Mayford M, Lockhart DJ, Barlow C. Regional and strain-specific gene expression mapping in the adult mouse brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2000;97:11038–11043. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.20.11038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shipley BA, Weiss A, Der G, Taylor MD, Deary IJ. Neuroticism, extraversion, and mortality in the UK Health and Lifestyle Survey: A 21-year prospective cohort study. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2007;69:923–931. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815abf83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinn DL, Apiolaza LA, Moltschaniwskyj NA. Heritability and fitness-related consequences of squid personality traits. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2006;19:1437–1447. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01136.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stirling DG, Reale D, Roff DA. Selection, structure and the heritability of behaviour. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2002;15:277–289. [Google Scholar]
- Tooby J, Cosmides L. On the universality of human-nature and the uniqueness of the individual - The role of genetics and adaptation. Journal of Personality. 1990;58:17–67. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1990.tb00907.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turelli M, Barton NH. Polygenic variation maintained by balancing selection: Pleiotropy, sex-dependent allelic effects and GxE interactions. Genetics. 2004;166:1053–1079. doi: 10.1534/genetics.166.2.1053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Oers K, Mueller JC. Evolutionary genomics of animal personality. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences. 2010;365:3991–4000. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verweij KJH, Zietsch BP, Medland SE, Gordon SD, Benyamin B, Nyholt DR, McEvoy BP, Sullivan PF, Heath AC, Madden PA, Henders AK, Montgomery GW, Martin NG, Wray NR. A genome-wide association study of Cloninger's Temperament scales: Implications for the evolutionary genetics of personality. Biological Psychology. 2010;85:306–317. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.07.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vinkhuyzen AE, Pedersen NL, Yang J, Lee SH, Magnussen PKE, Iacono WG, McGue M, Madden PAF, Heath AC, Luciano M, Payton A, Horan M, Ollier W, Pendleton N, Deary IJ, Montgomery GW, Martin NG, Visscher PM, Wray NR. Common SNPs explain a significant proportion of the `missing heritability' in the personality dimensions of neuroticism and extraversion. Translational Psychiatry. doi: 10.1038/tp.2012.27. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Visscher PM, Goddard ME, Derks EM, Wray NR. Evidence-based psychiatric genetics, AKA the false dichotomy between common and rare variant hypothesis. Molecular Psychiatry electronic publication ahead of print. 2011 doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Visscher PM, Yang JA, Goddard ME. A commentary on `Common SNPs explain a large proportion of the heritability for human height' by Yang et al. (2010) Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2010;13:517–524. doi: 10.1375/twin.13.6.517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh B, Blows MW. Abundant genetic variation plus strong selection = multivariate genetic constraints: A geometric view of adaptation. Annual Review of Ecology Evolution and Systematics. 2009;40:41–59. [Google Scholar]
- Wolf M, Weissing FJ. An explanatory framework for adaptive personality differences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences. 2010;365:3959–3968. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wray NR. Allele frequencies and the r2 measure of linkage disequilibrium, impact on design and interpretation of association studies. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2005;8:87–94. doi: 10.1375/1832427053738827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wray NR, Goddard ME, Visscher PM. Prediction of individual genetic risk to disease from genome-wide association studies. Genome Research. 2007;17:1520–1528. doi: 10.1101/gr.6665407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wray NR, Purcell SM, Visscher PM. Synthetic associations created by rare variants do not explain most GWAS results. Plos Biology. 2011;9 doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang J, Manolio TA, Pasquale LR, Boerwinkle E, Caporaso N, Cunningham JM, de Andrade M, Feenstra B, Feingold E, Hayes MG, Hill WG, Landi MT, Alonso A, Lettre G, Lin P, Ling H, Lowe W, Mathias RA, Melbye M, Pugh E, Cornelis MC, Weir BS, Goddard ME, Visscher PM. Genome partitioning of genetic variation for complex traits using common SNPs. Nature Genetics. 2011a;43:519–U544. doi: 10.1038/ng.823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang JA, Benyamin B, McEvoy BP, Gordon S, Henders AK, Nyholt DR, Madden PA, Heath AC, Martin NG, Montgomery GW, Goddard ME, Visscher PM. Common SNPs explain a large proportion of the heritability for human height. Nature Genetics. 2010;42:565–U131. doi: 10.1038/ng.608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang JA, Lee SH, Goddard ME, Visscher PM. GCTA: A tool for genome-wide complex trait analysis. American Journal of Human Genetics. 2011b;88:76–82. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.11.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang XS, Hill WG. Genetic variability under mutation selection balance. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2005;20:468–470. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zietsch BP, Verweij KJH, Bailey JM, Wright MJ, Martin NG. Genetic and environmental influences on risky sexual behaviour and its relationship with personality. Behavior Genetics. 2010;40:12–21. doi: 10.1007/s10519-009-9300-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]