Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012 Aug 29;128(1-2):83–89. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.08.006

Neighborhood Characteristics and the Initiation of Marijuana Use and Binge Drinking

Joan S Tucker, Michael S Pollard, Kayla de la Haye, David P Kennedy, Harold D Green Jr a
PMCID: PMC3521064  NIHMSID: NIHMS404312  PMID: 22938829

Abstract

BACKGROUND

This study examines whether residential neighborhood characteristics influence the initiation of marijuana use and binge drinking, and if these neighborhood factors heighten or dampen peer influences on substance use.

METHODS

Predictors of marijuana (N = 6,516) and binge drinking (N = 6,630) initiation over a one-year period were identified using data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. Participants were ages 12–19 years at baseline. The main predictor variables were neighborhood characteristics, using both objective (proportion of households below the poverty line and female-headed, unemployment rate, residential stability) and subjective (perceived cohesion and safety) measures. Binge drinking was defined as 5 or more drinks in a row.

RESULTS

Initiation occurred for 12.9% of adolescents in the case of marijuana and 16.4% for binge drinking. Marijuana initiation was more likely among adolescents who lived in neighborhoods with a higher unemployment rate, and binge drinking initiation was more likely among those who perceived greater safety in their neighborhood, after adjusting for other neighborhood characteristics, demographics, friend characteristics, and behavioral and family risk factors. There was no evidence that neighborhood context moderates the associations of peer factors on initiation.

CONCLUSIONS

Select neighborhood characteristics appear relevant to the initiation of marijuana use and binge drinking, although the mechanisms appear to be distinct for each substance. If these results are found to be robust, future research will help better understand how neighborhood context influences the initiation of adolescent substance use in order to inform prevention efforts.

Keywords: Adolescent, neighborhood, peer, marijuana, binge drinking

1. INTRODUCTION

1.1. The potential influence of neighborhoods on adolescent substance use

Alcohol and drug use are largely recognized as being influenced by multiple social contexts and processes during adolescence, including neighborhoods, family and peers. However, research has focused more heavily on family and peer influences than more distal contextual influences such as neighborhoods. Greater attention to neighborhood context is warranted given that adolescence is a time of increasing independence from family and more time spent in new and broader environments. Much of the research on neighborhoods is informed by social disorganization theory (Shaw and McKay, 1942), which posits that neighborhood features such as low socioeconomic status and residential instability influence individual behavior through their impact on neighborhood-level social processes such as increased exposure to deviant individuals and activities, environmentally-induced stress, and fewer forms of social control and monitoring.

Studies assess neighborhood context using either objective or subjective measures, although rarely examining both types simultaneously. Objective neighborhood measures are most often based on aggregations of individual data (e.g., socioeconomic status) within a geographic area (e.g., census tract) derived from census data. The few studies examining their associations with adolescent drinking or marijuana use have yielded mixed results. A study of 114 9th–10th grade students in the northeastern U.S., for example, reported no association between neighborhood disadvantage and a composite measure of adolescent substance use (e.g., Allison et al., 1999). However, a study in Ontario, Canada found greater adolescent alcohol and drug use in areas with the lowest SES characteristics (Smart et al., 1994) and another of over 4,000 students in Chicago found a positive association between area deprivation and alcohol use among African American students (but not Hispanics; Tobler et al., 2011). A recent study examining the growth of neighborhood disorder found that young adults residing in deteriorating neighborhoods in Baltimore were 30% more likely to use marijuana two years after high school compared to those living in always-good neighborhoods (Furr-Holden et al., 2011). Still other work has yielded results contrary to what might be expected from social disorganization theory. In a sample of 2,006 at-risk high school students in Seattle, those residing in more disadvantaged neighborhoods reported lower rates of alcohol and marijuana use (Snedker et al., 2009).

Studies using subjective neighborhood measures, which are based on residents' perceptions of their neighborhood such as disorder, cohesion and safety, have tended to yield more consistent associations between adolescents' reports of their neighborhood and their involvement in alcohol or drug use. In one of the few studies to specifically examine initiation, 95 6th graders from a school in the Midwest were more likely to initiate substance use by 8th grade if they had witnessed neighborhood events such as drinking on the streets, robbery, drug use, arrests, and fighting (Burlew et al., 2009; see similar cross-sectional findings by Wilson et al., 2005, Winstanley et al., 2008). Other studies of middle school students (Choi et al., 2006) and young adults (Theall et al., 2009) have found greater substance use among youth who report feeling less safe and more fearful in their neighborhoods.

Although the existing literature suggests that neighborhood context may in some way be influential in adolescent alcohol and marijuana use, it has often been based on studies using cross-sectional designs, composite substance use measures (sometimes aggregated with other risk behaviors), and small regional samples. In particular, there is a lack of information on whether subjective and objective neighborhood characteristics play a role in the initiation of marijuana use and binge drinking during adolescence.

1.2. The Role of Peers

Based on social disorganization theory and its focus on neighborhood-level social processes, one might expect that the negative impact of a disorganized neighborhood on substance use may be accounted for, to some extent, by exposure to deviant or substance using peers in these neighborhoods. Longitudinal studies have provided some support for this idea, with peer characteristics found to mediate associations of neighborhood disadvantage with adolescent substance use using both subjective (Brook et al., 1989) and objective (Chuang et al., 2005) neighborhood measures. Less studied is whether the neighborhood context may serve as an accelerating or de-accelerating agent, as some have suggested (Snedker et al., 2009), that allows peer factors to have a greater impact in some contexts than in others. To our knowledge, studies have not examined whether neighborhood disadvantage moderates the risk of marijuana and binge drinking initiation that is associated with exposure to deviant peers.

1.3. The Present Study

This study examines whether census-based indicators of neighborhood disorganization, as well as adolescent's subjective assessments of their neighborhood, predict their initiation of marijuana use and binge drinking over a one-year period. In general, we hypothesized that the initiation of both types of substance use would be more likely among adolescents residing in neighborhoods that: (a) were more economically disadvantaged (e.g., higher unemployment rate, more female-headed households, more households with incomes below the poverty line); (b) had greater residential instability; (c) were perceived to be less cohesive in terms of neighbors looking out for one another; and (d) and were perceived to be unsafe. This study also explores the role of peers in the initiation of marijuana use and binge drinking, particularly whether residing in a neighborhood with these characteristics might amplify the well-established risk of substance use that is associated with adolescents' exposure to deviant peers.

Our analytic approach addresses a concern that has been raised with regards to analyses of neighborhood-level effects on risk behavior (Haynie et al., 2006): associations may be due to compositional differences among individuals rather than neighborhood characteristics, especially when neighborhood variables (e.g., disadvantage) are constructed based on aggregations of individual traits (e.g., household socioeconomic status). Selection into neighborhoods is a particular concern as well; for example, more conscientious parents (an unmeasured trait) may tend to choose to reside in neighborhoods that are less disordered (and thus observed neighborhood effects would reflect the decision making of conscientious parents rather than a true neighborhood effect). We use the approach adopted by Haynie et al. (2006) to examine whether associations of neighborhood characteristics with substance use initiation change after adjusting for composition and selection variables.

2. METHODS

2.1 Data

Analyses are based on data from Waves I-II of the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health, a nationally representative study of adolescents in grades 7–12 in the U.S. in 1995 who have been followed with multiple interview waves. The sampling frame included all high schools in the U.S. Over 90,000 participants from 145 schools were given a basic in-school interview. Data from this in-school interview were used to generate a baseline sample of 20,745 adolescents aged 12–19 to complete in-home interviews between April-December 1995 (Wave I) and April-August 1996 (Wave II). In addition, 17,670 parents of Add Health respondents were interviewed at Wave I. 14,738 Add Health respondents were re-interviewed at Wave II (87.6% response rate among eligible Wave I respondents; adolescents in grade 12 at Wave I were not interviewed at Wave II by design). See Harris et al. (2009) for more details on the Add Health design and longitudinal data.

Adolescents were excluded from the analyses due to: a) missing the in-school, Wave II in-home, or parent interview (excluding n=11,348 of the Wave I in-home sample); b) reporting any (or missing) lifetime use of marijuana or any (or missing) lifetime binge drinking at Wave I, or missing network measures of substance use (excluding n=2,763 for marijuana and 2,611 for binge drinking); c) missing information on use of the substance at Wave II (excluding n=24 for marijuana and 60 for binge drinking); or (d) missing information on perceived safety, selected neighborhood, race/ethnicity, closeness to mother, or availability of drugs or alcohol in the home (excluding n=94 for marijuana and 96 for binge drinking; all other predictor variables were mean imputed). This resulted in a final analytic sample of N=6,516 for the marijuana analyses and N=6,630 for the binge drinking analyses, with a 76% overlap in these two samples. Table 1 provides unweighted descriptive statistics for the study variables.

Table 1.

Description of Study Variables (Unweighted)

Variable Marijuana Sample Mean (SD) or Proportion Binge Sample Mean (SD) or Proportion
Neighborhood Characteristics
 Proportion Below Poverty Line 0.14 (0.13) 0.14 (0.13)
 Proportion Households Female-Headed 0.07 (0.07) 0.07 (0.07)
 Unemployment Rate (Percent) 7.46 (5.62) 7.56 (5.69)
 Residential Instability 0.57 (0.15) 0.56 (0.15)
 Neighborhood Cohesion 0.76 (0.43) 0.75 (0.43)
 Perceived Safety 0.90 (0.29) 0.90 (0.31)
 Selected Neighborhood 0.82 (0.38) 0.81 (0.39)
 Moved Between Waves 0.05 (0.22) 0.05 (0.21)
Personal Demographics
 Male 0.46 (0.50) 0.46 (0.50)
 Age (in years) 15.02 (1.59) 15.01 (1.57)
 Black 0.20 (0.40) 0.23 (0.42)
 Hispanic 0.15 (0.36) 0.15 (0.36)
 Other Race (reference category = white) 0.10 (0.29) 0.10 (0.30)
 Household Income (in thousands) 46.94 (50.35) 45.88 (43.64)
 Mother's Education 4.12 (1.67) 4.13 (1.67)
 Both Parents In Home 0.72 (0.45) 0.71 (0.46)
Friends
 Proportion School Friends Using Marijuana 0.06 (0.18) 0.06 (0.19)
 Proportion School Friends Binge Drinking 0.14 (0.29) 0.13 (0.27)
 Only Outside-of-School Friends 0.17 (0.38) 0.18 (0.38)
 Best Frierd a “Good Influence” 0.65 (0.48) 0.64 (0.48)
Behavioral and Family Risk Factors
 Respondent Marijuana Use at Wave 1 ---- 0.05 (0.22)
 Respondent Binge Drinking at Wave 1 0.12 (0.33) ----
 Trouble at School 3.68 (2.65) 3.76 (2.70)
 Delinquency 2.97 (3.59) 3.16 (3.86)
 Low School Participation 0.13 (0.34) 0.14 (0.35)
 Low Parental Control 4.92 (1.56) 4.92 (1.56)
 Closeness to Mother 4.48 (1.05) 4.46 (1.06)
 Drugs Available In Home 0.02 (0.13) 0.02 (0.14)
Alcohol Available In Home 0.28 (0.45) 0.28 (0.45)

2.2 Key measures

2.2.1 Marijuana use and binge drinking

Adolescents were asked how many times in their life they used marijuana, and how many days in the past 12 months they drank five or more drinks in a row (information on lifetime binge drinking is not available). They were considered to have not initiated marijuana if they reported never trying it, and to have not initiated binge drinking if they reported no days in the past 12 months. At Wave II, we derived dichotomous measures of any past year marijuana use and binge drinking to determine whether initiation had occurred since Wave I. This was the only Wave II information used.

2.2.2 Residential neighborhood characteristics

Objective neighborhood characteristics were assessed using 1990 U.S. Census data: proportion with income below the poverty line; proportion of family households that are female-headed with children under 18 years old; the unemployment rate; and the proportion of individuals aged 5 or older who lived in a different household 5 years earlier (an indicator of residential instability). These characteristics were assessed at the block group level. Two subjective neighborhood characteristics were based on adolescent report: neighborhood cohesion (People in this neighborhood look out for each other; 0=false, 1=true); and perceived safety (Do you usually feel safe in your neighborhood; 0=no, 1=yes). Following Haynie et al. (2006), we addressed possible selection effects by controlling for the most important reason provided by parents for living in their neighborhood (out of 10 options, this variable is coded as 1 if it is due to better schools, to be near family/friends, or because of low crime in the neighborhood and coded as 0 for all other reasons). An indicator of whether respondents changed neighborhoods between waves is included to control for the reduction in exposure to the Wave I neighborhood factors among those who moved.

2.2.3 Friend characteristics

Information on the proportion of school friends who were marijuana users and binge drinkers comes from the in-school survey. All respondents were asked to nominate up to five male and five female friends. Nominated friends who belong to Add Health schools are linked to their own in-home survey responses to assess each friend's self-reported substance use. The proportions of a respondent's friends that reported any marijuana use in the past 30 days and any binge drinking in the past year were used to calculate the proportion of total number of nominated school friends engaging in that behavior (note that information on past year friend marijuana use is not available). Additionally, we examined two friend variables that might be expected to increase the likelihood of initiation. Whether the parent perceived the adolescent's best friend to be a good influence (0=no, 1=yes) was included to capture friend deviancy beyond their substance use behavior. Whether the adolescent only had outside-of-school friends (0=no, 1=yes, based on the in-school survey which identified whether nominated friends did not go to the adolescent's school) was included given its association with other forms of problem behavior (Haynie and Payne, 2006).

2.2.4 Model covariates: Personal demographics and behavioral and family risk factors

Demographics included gender, age, race/ethnicity, parent-reported household income, mother's education (1=8thgrade or less to 7=professional training beyond a 4-year college or university), and whether the adolescent lives with both parents (0=no, 1=yes). Behavioral risk factors are assessed with five variables. Initial binge drinking during the past year (0=no, 1=yes) is included in the model predicting to marijuana initiation, and initial marijuana use during the past 30 days (0=no, 1=yes) is included in the model predicting binge drinking initiation. Trouble at school is measured as the sum of four items asking how often they have trouble getting along with peers, paying attention, getting homework done, and getting along with other students (0=never, 4=everyday; α=.86). Delinquency is assessed using 14 items from the Add Health delinquency scale (one item on drug selling was excluded), which asks how often the adolescent engaged in various behaviors in the 12 months (0=never to 3=5 or more times; α=.83). Low school participation is a dichotomous indicator of whether they reported not participating in any school clubs, organizations or teams (0=any, 1=none). Family risk factors are assessed with four variables. Low parental control is measured as the sum of seven items asking whether their parents let them make their own decisions about curfew, who they hang around with, and so forth (0=no, 1=yes; α=.59). Closeness to mother is assessed by the question “How close do you feel to your mother?” (0=not at all to 5=very much). Separate items asked adolescents whether alcohol and illegal drugs were “easily available” to them in their home (0=no, 1=yes).

2.3 Analytic approach

We examined bivariate associations among the predictor variables to ensure that there were no multicollinearity issues. A series of four nested logistic regression models predicting initiation were then estimated separately for marijuana and binge drinking. The baseline model includes the objective and subjective neighborhood characteristics. A second model adds the personal demographics covariates to the neighborhood characteristics. This is followed by models that add friend information and, finally, the behavioral and family risk factor covariates. Additional models tested interactions of each neighborhood characteristic with each of the four friend characteristics. Regression analyses were corrected for complex sample design effects using strata, cluster, and weight variables (Chantala and Tabor, 1999).

3. RESULTS

3.1. Marijuana initiation

Marijuana initiation occurred for 12.1% of adolescents during the one-year follow-up. Table 2 shows results of the regression models for marijuana initiation. Of the neighborhood characteristics we examined, residing in a neighborhood with a higher unemployment rate was the most consistent risk factor for adolescents initiating marijuana use. Although the bivariate association was only marginally significant, residing in a neighborhood with a higher unemployment rate became a statistically significant risk factor when controlling for other neighborhood characteristics and the reason why parents reported moving into the neighborhood. The association between living in a neighborhood with a higher unemployment rate and the initiation of marijuana use was not affected by including personal demographics (e.g., gender, age, race/ethnicity, household income, mother's education, whether the family was intact), exposure to deviant peers, behavioral problems within or outside of school, and family risk factors (e.g., low parental control, the availability of drugs in the home) in the model. None of the other objective or subjective neighborhood characteristics that we examined was significantly associated with the initiation of marijuana use.

Table 2.

Odds Ratios and 95% Confidence Intervals from Logit Models Predicting Marijuana Use Inititation

Bivariate [1] [2] [3] [4]
Neighborhood Characteristics
 Proportion Below Poverty Line 1.16 (0.43, 3.14) 0.43 (0.15, 1.27) 0.39 (0.13, 1.15) 0.38 (0.13, 1.12) 0.51 (0.15, 1.69)
 Proportion Households Female-Headed 2.19 (0.21, 22.52) 1.25 (0.10, 15.47) 1.23 (0.09, 17.19) 1.20 (0.09, 16.81) 2.16 (0.14, 34.60)
 Unemployment Rate (Percent) 1.02 (1.00, 1.05) 1.04 (1.01, 1.06)** 1.04 (1.01, 1.06)** 1.03 (1.01, 1.06)** 1.03 (1.01, 1.06)*
 Residential Instability 1.18 (0.50, 2.79) 1.18 (0.49, 2.84) 1.30 (0.52, 3.25) 1.32 (0.51, 3.41) 1.69 (0.61, 4.66)
 Neighborhood Cohesion 0.84 (0.67, 1.05) 0.83 (0.64, 1.08) 0.86 (0.66, 1.12) 0.85 (0.65, 1.11) 0.95 (0.71, 1.25)
 Perceived Safety 1.15 (0.68, 1.93) 1.26 (0.76, 2.09) 1.29 (0.77, 2.15) 1.26 (0.77, 2.08) 1.58 (0.95, 2.63)
 Selected Neighborhood 1.28 (0.94, 1.74) 1.31 (0.96, 1.79) 1.30 (0.95, 1.77) 1.31 (0.96, 1.79) 1.31 (0.94, 1.83)
 Moved Between Waves 1.51 (0.97, 2.36) 1.50 (0.98, 2.30) 1.41 (0.91, 2.19) 1.37 (0.88, 2.12) 1.37 (0.87, 2.16)
Personal Demographics
 Male 0.93 (0.74, 1.16) 0.95 (0.76, 1.19) 0.98 (0.78, 1.22) 0.80 (0.63, 1.02)
 Age 0.98 (0.90, 1.06) 0.98 (0.90, 1.06) 0.95 (0.88, 1.03) 0.91 (0.84, 0.99)*
 Black 0.91 (0.69, 1.20) 0.81 (0.61, 1.07) 0.88 (0.66, 1.18) 0.91 (0.66, 1.26)
 Hispanic 1.09 (0.76, 1.57) 1.04 (0.70, 1.56) 1.07 (0.72, 1.59) 1.08 (0.73, 1.60)
 Other Race (reference category = white) 1.11 (0.75, 1.63) 1.08 (0.74, 1.59) 1.09 (0.75, 1.57) 1.10 (0.75, 1.62)
 Household Income 1.00 (1.00, 1.00) 1.00 (1.00, 1.00) 1.00 (1.00, 1.00) 1.00 (1.00, 1.00)
 Mother's Education 0.95 (0.88, 1.02) 0.96 (0.89, 1.04) 0.96 (0.89, 1.04) 0.97 (0.90, 1.05)
 Both Patents In Home 0.70 (0.55, 0.89)** 0.69 (0.53, 0.90)** 0.71 (0.55, 0.92)** 0.76 (0.58, 1.00)*
Friends
 Proportion School Friends Using Marijuana 2.55 (1.67, 3.88)*** 1.60 (0.98, 2.63) 1.53 (0.85, 2.73)
 Proportion School Friends Binge Drinking 2.15 (1.65, 2.81)*** 2.13 (1.57, 2.88)*** 1.77 (1.25, 2.49)***
 Only Outside-of-School Friends 1.24 (0.94, 1.64) 1.52 (1.13, 2.05)** 1.39 (1.02, 1.88)*
 Best Friend a “Good Influence” 0.89 (0.74, 1.06) 0.93 (0.77, 1.12) 1.01 (0.83, 1.23)
Behavioral and Family Risk Factors
 Respondent Binge Drinking at Wave 1 2.74 (2.07, 3.63)*** 1.91 (1.39, 2.63)***
 Trouble at School 1.15 (1.11, 1.19)*** 1.10 (1.05, 1.14)***
 Delinquency 1.14 (1.12, 1.17)*** 1.10 (1.08, 1.12)***
 Low School Participation 1.22 (0.89, 1.69) 1.22 (0.91, 1.63)
 Low Parental Control 1.10 (1.00, 1.21) 1.11 (1.01, 1.22)*
 Closeness to Mother 0.85 (0.79, 0.92)*** 0.93 (0.85, 1.01)
 Drugs Available In Home 1.33 (0.64, 2.77) 0.95 (0.42, 2.15)
 Alcohol Available In Home 1.22 (0.95, 1.59) 1.08 (0.82, 1.42)

p<.10

*

p<.05,

**

p<.01,

***

p<.001,

In addition, several of the baseline covariates were associated with subsequent marijuana initiation in the fully adjusted multivariate model. Demographic risk factors for initiation include not having an intact nuclear family and younger age; peer risk factors include having a higher proportion of in-school friends who binge drink and having only outside-of-school friends; behavioral risk factors include being a binge drinker at baseline, getting into trouble at school, and engaging in more delinquent behaviors; and family risk factors included low parental control.

3.2. Binge drinking initiation

Binge drinking initiation was reported by 16.3% of adolescents (note that these were generally not the same youth who initiated marijuana use; only 14.6% of all initiators began using both substances during the follow-up period). As shown in Table 3, the neighborhood characteristic most consistently associated with the initiation of binge drinking is perceived safety: adolescents are more likely to initiate binge drinking if they report that they usually feel safe in their neighborhood. This association remained after controlling for other neighborhood characteristics and the reason why parents reported moving into the neighborhood. Although the effect diminished somewhat and was no longer statistically significant when personal demographics and friend characteristics were added to the model, it emerged as statistically significant in the fully adjusted model that also included behavioral and family risk factors. The initiation of binge drinking was unrelated to the unemployment rate of the adolescents' residential neighborhood (notable given its association with marijuana initiation) and the other objective and subjective neighborhood characteristics that we examined.

Table 3.

Odds Ratios and 95% Confidence Intervals from Logit Models Predicting Binge Drinking Inititation

Bivariate [1] [2] [3] [4]
Neighborhood Characteristics
 Proportion Below Poverty line 0.36 (0.11, 1.12) 0.51 (0.12, 2.18) 0.73 (0.17, 3.18) 0.71 (0.16, 3.13) 1.02 (0.24, 4.40)
 Proportion Households Female-Headed 0.18 (0.01, 2.43) 0.60 (0.05, 7.81) 1.77 (0.14, 22.40) 2.06 (0.19, 22.58) 2.10 (0.18, 24.08)
 Unemployment Rate (Percent) 0.98 (0.96, 1.01) 1.00 (0.97, 1.02) 1.00 (0.98, 1.02) 0.99 (0.97, 1.02) 1.00 (0.98, 1.02)
 Residential Instability 1.17 (0.56, 2.42) 1.04 (0.50, 2.16) 1.18 (0.59, 2.40) 1.15 (0.56, 2.35) 1.29 (0.62, 2.67)
 Neighborhood Cohesion 1.04 (0.87, 1.23) 0.96 (0.80, 1.13) 0.99 (0.83, 1.19) 0.99 (0.82, 1.18) 1.10 (0.91, 1.34)
 Perceived Safety 1.55 (1.07, 2.24)* 1.45 (1.02, 2.06)* 1.42 (0.97, 2.06) 1.35 (0.93, 1.96) 1.52 (1.02, 2.27)*
 Selected Neighborhood 1.01 (0.81, 1.26) 0.94 (0.76, 1.16) 0.91 (0.74, 1.12) 0.91 (0.74, 1.12) 0.95 (0.77, 1.17)
 Moved Between Waves 0.79 (0.52, 1.21) 0.83 (0.54, 1.27) 0.76 (0.49, 1.19) 0.75 (0.48, 1.17) 0.83 (0.53, 1.30)
Personal Demographics
 Male 1.06 (0.89, 1.27) 1.02 (0.85, 1.23) 1.05 (0.88, 1.27) 0.93 (0.77, 1.13)
 Age 1.22 (1.14, 1.29)*** 1.24 (1.17, 1.32)*** 1.21 (1.14, 1.28)*** 1.19 (1.12, 1.27)***
 Black 0.38 (0.27, 0.53)*** 0.33 (0.21, 0.51)*** 0.36 (0.23, 0.55)*** 0.31 (0.21, 0.47)***
 Hispanic 1.17 (0.87, 1.58) 0.97 (0.70, 1.33) 0.99 (0.72,1.35) 1.04 (0.76,1.42)
 Other Race (reference category = white) 0.86 (0.60, 1.23) 0.74 (0.50, 1.09) 0.75 (0.51, 1.09) 0.71 (0.49,1.03)
 Household Income 1.00 (1.00, 1.00) 1.00 (1.00, 1.00) 1.00 (1.00, 1.00) 1.00 (1.00, 1.00)
 Mother's Education 0.97 (0.92, 1.02) 0.96 (0.91, 1.01) 0.96 (0.91, 1.01) 0.94 (0.89, 1.00)*
 Both Patents In Home 1.06 (0.85, 1.32) 0.86 (0.69, 1.07) 0.89 (0.71, 1.11) 1.00 (0.30, 1.26)
Friends
 Proportion School Friends Using Marijuana 2.76 (2.02, 3.78)*** 1.47 (0.99, 2.17) 1.14 (0.74, 1.75)
 Proportion School Friends Binge Drinking 2.81 (2.10, 3.76)*** 2.19 (1.58, 3.03)*** 2.18 (1.59, 3.00)***
 Only Outside-of-School Friends 1.05 (0.81, 1.35) 1.21 (0.94, 1.55) 1.09 (0.85, 1.39)
 Best Friend a “Good Influence” 0.91 (0.76, 1.07) 0.93 (0.78, 1.10) 1.01 (0.84, 1.22)
Behavioral and Family Risk Factors
 Respondent Marijuana Use at Wave 1 3.33 (2.55, 4.35)*** 2.06 (1.51, 2.82)***
 Trouble at School 1.11 (1.07, 1.14)*** 1.07 (1.03, 1.11)***
 Delinquency 1.10 (1.07, 1.12)*** 1.08 (1.05, 1.11)***
 Low School Participation 1.13 (0.85, 1.51) 0.95 (0.71, 1.27)
 Low Parental Control 1.17 (1.10, 1.25)*** 1.09 (1.03, 1.16)**
 Closeness to Mother 0.88 (0.82, 0.94)*** 0.94 (0.87, 1.02)
 Drugs Available In Home 1.95 (1.17, 3.24)* 1.12 (0.63, 1.97)
 Alcohol Available In Home 1.66 (1.37, 2.02)*** 1.43 (1.17, 1.74)***

p<.10

*

p<.05,

**

p<.01,

***

p<.001

As is the case for marijuana initiation, several of the baseline covariates are significant risk factors for the initiation of binge drinking. Adolescents are more likely to initiate binge drinking if they are older, white (vs. African American), and have a less highly educated mother. The only peer factor associated with initiation is having more friends who are binge drinkers at baseline. Finally, behavioral and family risk factors associated with binge drinking initiation include being a marijuana user at baseline, getting into trouble at school, delinquent behavior, low parental control, and having alcohol easily available in the home.

3.3. Moderating Effect of Friend Characteristics

Additional models considering the moderating effects of neighborhood characteristics on each of the friend characteristics were tested (results not shown). Each of the six objective and subjective neighborhood factors was interacted with each of the four friend characteristics. Interaction terms were added individually to the nested models in Tables 2 and 3. None of the interaction terms reached marginal significance (p < .10) for either outcome.

4. DISCUSSION

Results from this study indicate that select neighborhood characteristics appear relevant to the initiation of marijuana use and binge drinking, although the mechanisms appear to be distinct for each substance. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to find that residing in an area with higher unemployment may be more important than other aspects of neighborhood context in increasing an adolescent's risk of initiating marijuana use. This effect was found after adjusting for a range of other neighborhood, adolescent, friend and family characteristics, bolstering confidence that it actually reflects the influence of residing in a neighborhood with higher unemployment on adolescent risk behavior. Given that unemployed adults are more likely to initiate or escalate drug use compared to those who are employed (Green et al., 2010; Merline et al., 2004), residing in a neighborhood with high unemployment may increase adolescents' exposure to adults who use drugs. This greater exposure may weaken adolescents' beliefs about the potential harm of marijuana use and strengthen their positive expectancies for use (see Lambert et al., 2004), as well as provide more opportunities for experimentation. Adolescents may also be at higher risk for involvement in drug selling and related activities if they foresee few employment opportunities for themselves and have less hope for the future (Wilson et al., 2005); if so, this would help explain the stronger association of neighborhood unemployment with drug use relative to binge drinking. This finding of an association between neighborhood unemployment and marijuana initiation may also help shed light on the increase in youth marijuana use in the U.S. during the past four years (Johnston et al., 2011), which coincided with the economic recession and relatively high unemployment rates. Greater access to evidence-based drug prevention programs, such as mandated school-based programs or voluntary after-school programs (D'Amico et al., 2012), may be particularly needed in communities with high unemployment.

Our results also indicate that adolescents who usually felt safe in their neighborhood were more likely to initiate binge drinking compared to those who felt less safe, adjusting for a range of other neighborhood, adolescent, friend and family factors. Although this association was not necessarily expected, and perceived safety was assessed in a limited fashion by a single item, it adds to a small literature suggesting that certain aspects of neighborhood advantage may increase the likelihood of adolescent alcohol use (e.g., Snedker et al., 2009; Trim and Chassin, 2008). Although admittedly speculative, it may be the case that the general indicator of perceived safety used in this study was assessing feelings of safety based on factors other than environmental threats, such as low parental supervision in the neighborhood from permissive or absent parents, which might both foster a sense of neighborhood safety and increase the likelihood of binge drinking (e.g., van der Zwaluw et al., 2008). In contrast, studies that ask youth about their perceptions of neighborhood safety in the context of specific negative events – whether witnessed, experienced, or feared – tend to find the expected associations with higher substance use (Choi et al., 2006; Theall et al., 2009). This type of specific, event-based measure of neighborhood perceptions may also be more strongly correlated with objective measures of neighborhood disorganization and, when these indicators of neighborhood context are examined together, exhibit a more similar pattern of associations with adolescent substance use than was found in the present study.

Adolescents were more likely to initiate marijuana use and binge drinking if a higher proportion of their school-based friends binge drank, consistent with the larger literature on homophily in adolescent substance use (Connell et al., 2010; Creemers et al., 2010; Duan et al., 2009). Peer binge drinking may have a stronger effect than peer marijuana use on initiation due to its greater prevalence and thus observability. It was also the case that adolescents were more likely to initiate marijuana use in particular if all of their friend nominations consisted of peers outside of their school. Having exclusively outside-of-school friendships may be a marker of greater deviancy, as suggested by Add Health analyses that have linked it with a tendency towards perpetrating serious violence (Haynie and Payne, 2006), which might be one reason why it was more strongly associated with the initiation of illicit drug use than binge drinking. However, the associations of neighborhood characteristics with initiation could not be explained by adolescents' greater exposure to substance-using friends or only having outside-of-school friends, contradicting previous findings that some neighborhood effects on substance use can be accounted for by peer influence processes. We further hypothesized that `risky' neighborhood characteristics would heighten the negative influence of deviant peers on substance use, but found no evidence to support this idea for either marijuana use or binge drinking. These findings may suggest that peer effects on substance use are pervasive, and are not strongly impacted by neighborhood features that provide more or less opportunities for substance use. Perhaps these peer processes largely play out in school settings that are structured fairly similarly across different neighborhoods. It may also be the case that the peer effects observed in this study are not due to friends' shared opportunities for substance use (that are likely to differ based on neighborhood characteristics), but rather are the result of interpersonal processes such as behavior modeling and imitation that are less impacted by context.

In interpreting results from this study, it is important to keep in mind that some of the measures had relatively low reliabilities or were single items, and the absence of a lifetime measure of binge drinking may have resulted in some adolescents being misclassified as non-initiates at baseline. Although not a limitation per se, it is also important to emphasize that most of the associations that we examined between neighborhood characteristics and initiation were not significant. These issues, combined with the general lack of longitudinal research examining how neighborhood characteristics are related to the initiation of marijuana use and binge drinking during adolescence, highlights the importance of replicating these findings.

Results point to three important directions for future research on neighborhood context and adolescent substance use. First, there is a need for additional psychometric work on commonly used measures of perceived neighborhood context to better understand what they are measuring and how they relate to objective neighborhood conditions. Second, there is a need to better understand the mechanisms through which neighborhood context influences the initiation of substance use, including substance-specific mechanisms, in order to inform prevention efforts. Existing studies have often focused on a single substance or used a composite substance use measure. Examining multiple substances within the same study (and, in the case of the present study, essentially within the same group of adolescents given the substantial overlap in our two samples), allows for the documentation of how certain neighborhood characteristics are differentially associated with the initiation of different types of substance use. Finally, although this study found little evidence for interactive effects between neighborhood and friend characteristics, further research in this area is warranted given that so little attention has focused on the potential moderating effects of neighborhood context on established risk factors for initiation.

Acknowledgements

This research uses data from Add Health, a program project designed by J. Richard Udry, Peter S. Bearman, and Kathleen Mullan Harris, and funded by a grant P01-HD31921 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, with Cooperative funding from 17 other agencies. Special acknowledgment is due Ronald R. Rindfuss and Barbara Entwisle for assistance in the original design. Persons interested in obtaining data files from Add Health should contact Add Health, Carolina Population Center, 123 W. Franklin Street, Chapel Hill, NC 27516-2524 (addhealth@unc.edu)

Role of Funding Sources Funding for these analyses was provided by grant R01DA030380 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (PI: Joan Tucker).

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Contributors Drs. Tucker and Pollard were primarily responsible for the design of the study. Dr. Pollard conducted the data analysis and Dr. Tucker wrote the first draft of the manuscript. Drs. De la Haye, Kennedy and Green assisted with the design of the study and provided feedback on drafts of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

REFERENCES

  1. Allison KW, Crawford I, Leone PE, Trickett E, Perez-Febles A, Burton LM, Le Blanc R. Adolescent substance use: preliminary examinations of school and neighborhood context. Am. J. Clin. Pathol. 1999;27:111–141. doi: 10.1023/A:1022879500217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Brook JS, Nomura C, Cohen P. A network of influences on adolescent drug involvement: neighborhood, school, peer, and family. Genet. Soc. Gen. Psychol. Monogr. 1989;115:125–145. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Burlew AK, Johnson CS, Flowers AM, Peteet BJ, Griffith-Henry KD, Buchanan ND. Neighborhood risk, parental supervision and the onset of substance use among African American adolescents. J. Fam. Stud. 2009;18:680–689. [Google Scholar]
  4. Chantala K, Tabor J. Strategies to Perform a Design-Based Analysis Using the Add Health Data. University of North Carolina; Chapel Hill: 1999. [Google Scholar]
  5. Choi Y, Harachi TW, Catalano RF. Neighborhoods, family, and substance use: comparisons of the relations across racial and ethnic groups. Soc. Serv. Rev. 2006;80:675–704. doi: 10.1086/508380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chuang Y, Ennett ST, Bauman KE, Foshee VA. Neighborhood influences on adolescent cigarette and alcohol use: mediating effects through parent and peer behaviors. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2005;46:187–204. doi: 10.1177/002214650504600205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Connell CM, Gilreath TD, Aklin WM, Brex RA. Social-ecological influences on patterns of substance use among non-metropolitan high school students. Am. J. Community Psychol. 2010;45:36–48. doi: 10.1007/s10464-009-9289-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Creemers HE, Dijkstra JK, Vollebergh WAM, Ormel J, Verhulst FC, Huizink AC. Predicting life-time and regular cannabis use during adolescence; the roles of temperament and peer substance use: the TRAILS study. Addiction. 2010;105:699–708. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02819.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. D'Amico EJ, Tucker JS, Miles JNV, Zhou A, Shih RA, Green HD., Jr. Preventing alcohol use with a voluntary after school program for middle school students: results from a randomized controlled trial of Project CHOICE. Prev. Sci. 2012;13:415–425. doi: 10.1007/s11121-011-0269-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Duan L, Chou C-P, Andreeva V, Pentz M. Trajectories of peer social influences as long-term predictors of drug use from early through late adolescence. J. Youth Adolesc. 2009;38:454–465. doi: 10.1007/s10964-008-9310-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Furr-Holden CD, Lee MH, Milam AJ, Johnson RM, Lee K-S, Ialongo NS. The growth of neighborhood disorder and marijuana use among urban adolescents: a case of policy and environmental interventions. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs. 2011;72:371–379. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2011.72.371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Green KM, Doherty EE, Reisinger HS, Chilcoat HD, Ensminger M. Social integration in young adulthood and the subsequent onset of substance abuse and disorders among a community population of urban African Americans. Addiction. 2010;105:484–493. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02787.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Harris KM, Halpern CT, Entzel P, Tabor J, Bearman PS, Udry JR. The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health: Research Design. [Accessed on August 2, 2010];2009 http://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth/design.
  14. Haynie DL, Payne DC. Race, friendship networks, and violent delinquency. Criminology. 2006;44:775–805. [Google Scholar]
  15. Haynie DL, Silver E, Teasdale B. Neighborhood characteristics, peer networks, and adolescent violence. J. Quant. Criminol. 2006;22:147–169. [Google Scholar]
  16. Johnston LD, O'Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. University of Michigan News Service; Ann Arbor, MI: 2011. [Accessed on March 16, 2012]. Marijuana Use Continues to Rise Among U.S. Teens, While Alcohol Use Hits Historic Lows. http://www.monitoringthefuture.org. [Google Scholar]
  17. Lambert SF, Brown TL, Phillips CM, Ialongo NS. The relationship between perceptions of neighborhood characteristics and substance use among urban African American adolescents. Am. J. Commun. Psychol. 2004;34:205–218. doi: 10.1007/s10464-004-7415-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Merline AC, O'Malley PM, Schulenberg JE, Bachman JG, Johnston LD. Substance use among adults 35 years of age: prevalence, adulthood predictors, and impact of adolescent substance abuse. Am. J. Public Health. 2004;94:96–102. doi: 10.2105/ajph.94.1.96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Shaw CR, McKay H. Juvenile Delinquency in Urban Areas. University of Chicago Press; Chicago: 1942. [Google Scholar]
  20. Simons RL, Johnson C, Beaman J, Conger RD, Whitbeck LB. Parents and peer group as mediators of the effect of community structure on adolescent problem behavior. Am. J. Commun. Psychol. 1996;24:145–171. doi: 10.1007/BF02511885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Smart RG, Adlaf EM, Walsh GW. Neighbourhood socio-economic factors in relation to student drug use and programs. J. Child Adolesc. Subst. 1994;3:37–46. [Google Scholar]
  22. Snedker KA, Herting JR, Walton E. Contextual effects and adolescent substance use: exploring the role of neighborhoods. Soc. Sci. Q. 2009;90:1272–1297. [Google Scholar]
  23. Theall KP, Sterk CE, Elifson KW. Perceived neighborhood fear and drug use among young adults. Am. J. Health Behav. 2009;33:353–365. doi: 10.5993/ajhb.33.4.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Tobler AL, Livingston MD, Komro KA. Racial/ethnic differences in the etiology of alcohol use among urban adolescents. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs. 2011;72:799–810. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2011.72.799. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Trim RS, Chassin L. Neighborhood socioeconomic status effects on adolescent alcohol outcomes using growth models: exploring the role of parental alcoholism. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs. 2008;69:639–648. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2008.69.639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. van der Zwaluw CS, Scholte RHJ, Vermulst AA, Buitelaar JK, Verkes RJ, Engels RC. Parental problem drinking, parenting, and adolescent alcohol use. J. Behav. Med. 2008;31:189–200. doi: 10.1007/s10865-007-9146-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Wilson N, Syme L, Boyce WT, Battistich VA, Selvin S. Adolescent alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana use: the influence of neighborhood disorder and hope. Am. J. Health Promot. 2005;20:11–19. doi: 10.4278/0890-1171-20.1.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Winstanley EL, Steinwachs DM, Ensminger ME, Latkin CA, Stitzer ML, Olsen Y. The association of self-reported neighborhood disorganization and social capital with adolescent alcohol and drug use, dependence, and access to treatment. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008;92:173–182. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.07.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES