Abstract
Purpose
The primary objectives of this study were to determine the prevalence of late language emergence (LLE) and to investigate the predictive status of maternal, family, and child variables.
Method
This is a prospective cohort study of 1766 epidemiologically ascertained twenty-four-month singleton children. The framework was an ecological model of child development, encompassing a wide range of maternal, family, and child variables. Data were obtained using postal questionnaire. Item analyses of the 6-item Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ) Communication Scale yielded a composite score encompassing comprehension as well as production items. One standard deviation below the mean yielded good separation of affected from unaffected children. Analyses of bivariate relationships with maternal, family, and child variables were carried out, followed by multivariate logistic regression to predict LLE group membership.
Results
13.4% of the sample showed late language emergence via the ASQ criterion; 19.1% using a single item “combining words.” Risk for LLE at 24 months was not associated with particular strata of parental educational levels, socioeconomic resources, parental mental health, parenting practices or family functioning. Significant predictors included familial history of late language emergence, male gender and early neurobiological growth. Covariates included psychosocial indicators.
Conclusion
Results are congruent with models of language emergence and impairment that posit a strong role for neurobiological and genetic mechanisms of onset that operate across a wide variation in maternal and family characteristics.
Children’s language comprehension and production emerge between 12 and 24 months of age. Some otherwise healthy children require more time to begin talking, a condition described here as “late language emergence” (LLE). The reasons for such variation at the toddler stage of development are relatively unexplored. Variations in family or maternal resources are thought to play a role, although actual outcomes are mixed. More recently, genetic studies have focused on possible inherited risk for LLE (cf., Dale et al, 1998; Spinath, Price, Dale & Plomin, 2004). LLE is widely assumed to be the first diagnostic symptom of children with language impairments. Tager-Flusberg and Cooper (1999) called for studies of early identification of SLI, “with particular emphasis on predicting which late talkers develop SLI.” (p. 1277).
A handful of studies have documented the phenomenon (Fenson et al., 1994; Paul, 1996; Rescorla, 1989; Thal & Katich, 1996; Whitehurst & Fischel, 1994) and provided valuable descriptive and interpretive information. At the same time, with few exceptions, the studies were limited by small sample sizes and/or convenience sampling procedures and a small number of independent variables. In addition, much of the literature relies on relatively extensive parental questionnaires to document children’s lexical development. These instruments are often infeasible for large scale investigations of a wide range of possible predictors and covariates of late language emergence. A few alternatives have been developed, although not yet used to evaluate predictors of LLE in single born children in a study with a large number of participants and independent variables. What remains unknown are the following: 1) The prevalence of LLE in the general population of 24-month-old children; and 2) The extent to which a wide range of maternal, family, and child variables are predictive of late talking. These issues are addressed in this investigation of an epidemiologically ascertained sample of 1766 24-month singleton children who were participants in a large scale investigation of health outcomes and whose participants provided information on a wide range of targeted maternal, family, and child variables.
Ecological Model of Child Development
The participants in this study were recruited at birth (1995/1996) into an on-going longitudinal study of children’s health and developmental outcomes, RASCALS (Randomly Ascertained Sample of Children born in Australia’s Largest State), based in Perth, Australia. According to current census data, Western Australia (WA) is demographically similar to some states in the Midwestern USA. For example, the population of the state of Kansas is 2.7 million; WA, 1.8 million, and in each state most of the population is in urban areas. The states are predominately Caucasian (86% for Kansas; 96% for WA) and native speakers of English, well-educated (86% high school completion in each state), and family-oriented (in Kansas, 55% of all families are couple families with children and 9% are sole parent families; in WA, 49% and 15%, respectively). On a wide variety of behavioural and biological assessments of children and adults, distributional outcomes conform to normative expectations for instruments normed in the USA or the United Kingdom.
This health outcomes study was guided by an ecological model of child development (Bronfenbrenner, 1979). This model views child development as a complex interplay between a child’s biogenetic endowment and the proximal (i.e., maternal and family) and distal (i.e., societal) resources available to the child. The model recognizes that the proximal and distal resources available to the child will vary over the life course due to changes in circumstances for better or worse. While this framework has not been used in previous studies of late talkers, the independent variables linked to late talking can all be placed in this model and categorized as relating to the child (neurobiological and genetic mechanisms) or the maternal or family environment. Consistent with the ecological framework, information was collected on a wide range of variables to describe maternal and family attributes and socioeconomic factors, concurrent with extensive documentation of children’s perinatal status, developmental and health outcomes. Findings are summarized in a series of reports commissioned for public policy application (Silburn et al., 1996; Zubrick et al., 1995; Zubrick et al., 1997).
Candidate Predictors of the Emergence of Language
Maternal and family variables, in particular socioeconomic indictors, have been linked with the onset of language in young children. Mother’s education level and family SES are thought to be proxy measures of environmental support for language learning. Mother’s education is reported to be associated with the amount of talking to children (cf. Hart & Risley, 1995; Hoff-Ginsberg, 1994; Wells, 1985), which in turn is predictive of vocabulary development in singletons (Dollaghan et al., 1999; Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer, & Lyons, 1991) and twins (Lytton, Conway, & Sauve, 1977) and positively associated with a number of language indices in the first three years, including verb tenses (Hart & Risley, 1995) and utterance length (Dollaghan et al., 1999). Furthermore, maternal and paternal education is reported to be a predictor of language impairment (Tomblin, 1996). LaBenz and LaBenz (1980) document language outcomes of a national sample of 20,137 children, followed from birth to 8 years of age, and report that mother’s education predicted failure at age 8 on language comprehension testing. The outcomes of these studies, and the conclusions of Entwisle and Astone (1994), that mother’s education is the preferred index of “human capital” in the home when considering environmental contributions to young children’s development, support consideration of levels of mother’s education as a general risk index for children’s language acquisition.
Recent studies, however, yield mixed evidence with regard to the chain of predictive effects sketched above. In an investigation of 108 low-income toddlers Pan, Rowe, Singer & Snow (2005) found that maternal talkativeness was not related to growth in children’s vocabulary production in the 24-36-month period observed. Instead, maternal language and literacy (which was collinear with maternal education) was a significant predictor of growth; mothers with lower vocabularies/lower reading levels had children with lower levels of vocabulary production in spontaneous samples. Pan et al. (2005) note that the outcomes are compatible with either a genetic view of shared linguistic aptitude between mothers and children or an environmental input view that mothers with higher language and literature skills interact with their children differently than mothers with lower language and literacy skills. They conclude that the maternal language and literacy effect, in their data, is not entirely mediated by maternal input, suggesting a need to consider influences beyond input. Pan et al. (2005) also investigated maternal depression as a predictor of growth, and found it to have direct effects that increased with time. They note earlier reports that depressed mothers talk less to their children than healthy mothers (Breznitz & Sherman, 1987), and caution that in their study the outcome measure was children’s vocabulary use in interactions with their mothers. Further investigation of maternal depression as a possible predictor is warranted.
Family SES levels are also implicated as risk factors. Although SES and maternal education are highly associated in the general population, there is strong reason to consider them as separate variables when evaluating children’s development (cf. Entwisle & Astone, 1994). For example, in the Western Australia Child Health Survey (Zubrick et al., 1997)there was a stronger association between the education and employment status of caregivers and children’s academic competence than income and family structure and academic outcomes. This finding would have been obscured if a composite measure had been used. With regard to SES, in general, as noted by Hoff-Ginsberg (1998), high SES mothers are reported to have higher levels of child-related adjustments that are thought to enhance children’s language acquisition, although as she notes in her study of 63 high and mid-level SES children ages 18 to 29 months, the effects of maternal differences and child differences may be less detectable in the mid- to high-levels of SES. Total vocabulary size in young children is significantly related to SES, although at very low levels of association and/or localised in the low end of the distribution (Fenson et al., 1994; Rescorla & Alley, 2001; Rice, Spitz, & O’Brien, 1999; Wells, 1985). Pan et al. (2005) reported no effect for family income in their study of low income toddlers. Overall, there is reason to regard mother’s education and SES as risk factors for language emergence in toddlers, although the associations may not be strong, and the available evidence yields mixed outcomes.
Late Language Emergence: Measurement, Prevalence, Candidate Predictors, and Covariates
Parent report measures of children’s vocabulary and early word combinations are used to determine LLE status, described as “Late Talking,” typically benchmarked to the 24-month age level. Two widely used criteria are 50 words in reported vocabulary and presence of 2-3 word combinations. Rescorla (1989) developed the Language Development Survey (LDS) as a parental report measure, comprised of a 310-word vocabulary checklist and a question about the presence and frequency of children’s early word combinations. Prevalence estimates from this instrument, using a criterion of fewer than 50 words or no word combinations yield estimates of 10 -20% of children as Late Talkers (Klee et al., 1998; Rescorla, 1989; Rescorla & Achenbach, 2002; Rescorla & Alley, 2001; Rescorla, Hadick-Wiley, & Escarce, 1993). Paul (1996) also used this criterion to identify a group of Late Talkers for longitudinal assessment. The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: Words and Sentences (CDI/WS) (Fenson et al., 1993) is a widely used parent report measure that utilizes a 680-word checklist as well as questions about early word combinations. According to the normative data provided by Fenson et al. (1993), at 24 months the bottom 5% of the distribution varies according to gender: for boys, it is an expressive vocabulary of 70 words; for girls, 48 words. Estimates of word combinations are available for children 16-30 months. Within this age range, for children who were reported to produce 50 words or less, 21% were reported to combine words “sometimes” and 1.3% were reported to do so “often.” Children with an expressive vocabulary of fewer than 50 words or not combining words by 24 months represented the bottom 10% of the CDI norming sample (Fenson et al., 1993). Thal, Tobias and Morrison (1991) used the CDI/WS criterion, at or below the 10th percentile to identify late talkers in a follow-up study. Dale et al. (2003) used the 10th percentile; Feldman et al. (2005) reported five levels to define delays, i.e., 5th, 10th, 15th, 20th, and 25th percentiles.
Candidate predictors for LLE in children younger than 3 years have been identified from studies that have differentiated late talkers and controls and compared them on a select number of child, maternal and family variables that have been linked theoretically and/or empirically to language development and language impairment. The child characteristics that have been examined include gender, gestational age, perinatal and obstetric risks, child behaviour and motor development. The maternal characteristics include mother’s education and mother-child interaction. The family influences include SES, birth order, family size and family history of late talking (cf. Olswang, Rodriguez, & Timler, 1998; Whitehurst & Fischel, 1994 for reviews).
Among the candidate predictors, there is a strong, replicated gender risk for late talking. In prevalence studies the proportion of boys who are late to talk is much higher than girls (Horwitz et al., 2003; Klee et al., 1998; Rescorla, 1989; Rescorla & Achenbach, 2002; Rescorla & Alley, 2001; Rescorla et al., 1993), as well as in late talker cohort studies (Ellis Weismer, Murray Branch, & Miller, 1994; Paul, 2000; Rescorla, 2002; Whitehurst, Fischel, Arnold, & Lonigan, 1992). This strong gender effect seems to be a phenomenon of the lower tail of the distribution of children; it is not apparent across the full distribution where the gender effects in favor of girls are significant but small (Feldman et al., 2000; Fenson et al., 1994; Huttenlocher et al., 1991; Rescorla & Achenbach, 2002) or not evident at all (Bornstein, Tamis-LeMonda, & Maurice Haynes, 1999; Pan et al., 2005; Wells, 1985). Normal distributions which differ only modestly in their means can have very large relative differences at the extremes.
Children’s birth history and perinatal status do not appear to be viable risk indicators for LLE. Late talkers do not have elevated rates of fetal and birth complications compared to controls (Paul, 1991; Rescorla et al., 1993; Whitehurst et al., 1992). In the most recent epidemiological study of SLI in kindergarten age children, adverse intrauterine and birth events were not risk exposures for SLI (Tomblin, Smith, & Zhang, 1997). Similarly, in a recent twin study, prenatal, perinatal and obstetric risks were not associated with lower levels of language performance in twins compared to singletons at 20 and 36 months (Rutter, Thorpe, Greenwood, Northstone, & Golding, 2003). Maschik et al. (in press) reported that children (N = 15) who scored below the 10th percentile on an Austrian adaptation of the CDI/WS at 18 months had lower Apgar Scores than controls; and that five late talkers (and none of the controls) required neonatal intensive care. Interestingly, eight of the fifteen late talkers scored within the normal range on the CDI/WS at 24 months.
Delayed motor development has been reported in several studies of late talkers. Rescorla and Alley (2001), Klee et al. (1998) and Carson et al. (1998) conducted direct assessment of motor abilities using standardised tests and reported that late talkers had lower levels of motor development than controls. None of the children in these studies had developmental disorders or syndromes associated with delayed motor development.
Information about the influence of SES, parental education and occupation on Late Talkers is very limited, in part because of the predominately convenience sampling methods which draw heavily from middle class families (Rescorla, 2002). Using the CDI, Thal et al. (1997) reports at 16-25 months a slight SES advantage for early talkers and a slight disadvantage for late talkers, although this trend was not confirmed by post hoc testing and was not present when the children were 21-31 months. Using the MCDI-Short Form, Horwitz, et al. (2003) found SES and maternal education effects at 24-months, although their study also reported that living in a bilingual household was a strong predictor, thereby confounding the interpretation of LLE risk with bilingualism.
There is evidence that LLE influences the dynamics of parent-child interaction. Whitehurst and colleagues (1988) compared parental interactions for groups of late talkers, age-matched and language-matched controls. They reported differences for late talkers compared to age-matched controls and similarities between late-talkers and language-matched controls. They concluded that the differences in parent-interaction between late talkers and age-matched normally developing children reflected parental adaptation to the language abilities of the children. Paul and Elwood (1991) reported similar results.
Feldman et al (2005) call for investigation of the role of a positive family history of language disorders or delays as a potential predictor of outcomes. Hadley and Holt (2006) investigated maternal education and positive family history as predictors of growth in tense marking abilities in two-year-old children with low levels of language development. Positive family history was related to differences in tense marking growth trajectories whereas maternal education was not a predictor. The finding contrasted with Hart and Risley’s (1995) finding that maternal education was associated with children’s production of verb tenses. Hadley and Holt studied children in the low range of language abilities whereas Hart and Risley studied children across the full range of the distribution of language abilities. This suggests that the influence of maternal education on performance is modulated by child characteristics. Hadley and Holt’s study was the first to carry out growth curve analyses including positive familial history as a predictive variable for children’s late talking. This extends the findings from previous studies that have reported higher levels of familial risk in late talkers compared to controls (Ellis Weismer et al., 1994; Paul, 1991; Rescorla & Schwartz, 1990).
Psychosocial development has been linked to late talking. The temperament and behaviour characteristics of small numbers of late talkers have been investigated in several studies. Caufield, Fischel, DeBaryshe and Whitehurst (1989) studied 34 late talkers and 34 controls (24-32 months); Carson, Klee, Perry, Muskina and Donaghy (1998) 11 late talkers and 53 controls (24–26 months); Irwin, Carter and Briggs-Gowan (2002), 14 late talkers and 14 controls (21-32 months); Paul and James (1990), 34 late talkers and 33 controls (24 months). Higher rates of problems were reported for late talkers compared to controls in these studies. In contrast to these studies, Rescorla and Achenbach (2002) did not find an association between language delay and behaviour problems in a general population sample of 278 children 18-35 months.
Interpretation of the relation between psychosocial development and LLE is not straightforward. There are measurement confounds, in that many parent report measures of psychosocial ability include items such as “talks with other children,” that are confounded with language ability. Further, psychosocial differences could be consequences of limited language ability (cf. Redmond & Rice, 2002; Redmond & Rice, 1998). Thus, psychosocial development does not carry the straightforward predictor status as other variables, such as gender and maternal education.
The outcome of LLE is of considerable interest. Late onset of language is a hallmark characteristic of children with language impairments. In the case of children with Specific Language Impairment, who do not demonstrate other developmental limitations, late talking can be the first diagnostic symptom. Available studies report that 17% (Rescorla, 2002) to 26% (Paul, 1996) of the late talkers have persistent SLI at 4-6 years, although the actual estimates are complicated by the criteria used for diagnosis. These outcomes should be considered preliminary, given very small sample sizes. Rescorla (2002) followed 34 children; Paul (1996), 31; Whitehurst and Fischel (1994), 37; Thal et al. (1991), 10 children. Not surprisingly, outcomes of late talking include social and academic risk (i.e., reading ability), in tandem with the likelihood of immature language competences (Paul, 1996; Rescorla, 2002; Whitehurst & Fischel, 1994).
Limitations of current evidence
The samples of children studied have been small in size (with the exception of the twin study of Dale et al., 2003) and drawn from predominately middle- and upper-middle-class families. Furthermore, some of the samples have included an admixture of monolingual and bilingual children or households in which multiple languages are spoken (Fenson et al., 1994; Klee et al., 1998; Rescorla, 1989; Rescorla et al., 1993), providing a possible confounding effect of undetermined significance. Under such sampling constraints it is not possible to estimate the extent to which the outcomes generalize to the general population of 24-month-old children or to interpret risk for LLE independent of risk associated with English acquired simultaneously with one or more other languages. The examination of potential predictors of late talking has been limited to a few variables and further constrained by the small sample sizes and sampling confounds.
Multivariate models of risk have also been constrained by the available empirical evidence. Our review of the literature yielded a single model (Olswang, et al. 1998). The model posits the child’s speech, language and social development prior to age 3 years as predictors of subsequent language growth, combined with the following risk factors: Positive family history, prolonged periods of otitis media, and the family’s SES levels. The proposed risk factors were not subjected to formal analyses.
What is needed is information on a relatively wide range of maternal, family, and child variables, as possible predictors of late talking in the same population of late talkers and controls, with formal multivariate analyses for risk assessment. The questions addressed in this study are: 1) What is the prevalence of LLE in an epidemiologically ascertained sample of 24-month-old children? 2) Which maternal, family, and child variables are predictive of LLE?
Methods
Participants
Data for this analysis came from the RASCALS (Randomly Ascertained Sample of Children born in Australia’s Largest State) study in Western Australia. RASCALS is an on-going longitudinal postal study of a sample of children born in Western Australia in 1995 and 1996. The sample design is an epidemiological prospective observational study of infants randomly ascertained from a total population frame of birth notifications for the state of Western Australia and followed annually from birth. These designs are sometimes called cohort studies. In cohort studies, the relationship between exposure and the incidence of an outcome is examined by following the entire cohort and measuring the rate of occurrence of new cases in different exposure groups. The prospective follow-up allows the investigator to identify subjects with and without the outcomes of interest. In a case-control study the individuals who develop the outcome condition (the cases) are identified by some other mechanism than follow up, and a group of subjects (the controls) is used to represent the subjects who do not develop the outcome condition. As findings in this report focus on the 24 month follow-up of the RASCALS cohort only, the data analyses has been approached from the standpoint of a case-control study nested in a cohort study (i.e., nested case control study) (Clayton and Hills, 1996).
The original sample was randomly drawn from a total population sample frame comprising statutory notifications of birth (Stanley, Read, Kurinczuk, Croft, & Bower, 1997). The RASCALS study was designed to survey health-related behaviours (Kurinczuk, Parsons, Dawes, & Burton, 1999), to identify and evaluate health promotion opportunities from birth to eight years, and to investigate early causal pathways of mental health problems in childhood. Four thousand and seven mothers responded to the initial questionnaire sent at three months post-partum. A comparison with data available about all births in Western Australia (Stanley et al., 1997) showed that the 4007 respondents were representative of all women with live births in that period, with the exception of a slight under-representation of women with low birth weight babies (5.3% overall versus 4.7% in the sample) and mothers aged less than twenty years (6% overall versus 3.6% in the sample). Because metropolitan Aboriginal mothers were participating in a similar but more culturally appropriate study they were not recruited into the RASCALS study.
Following the three-month post-partum response, the study was converted to a longitudinal study and for resource reasons just less than a 70% random sample of mothers of singletons was drawn from the initial 4007 respondents. However, to ensure ‘hard-to-reach’ groups remained in the RASCALS study in sufficiently informative proportions we also included all mothers who were either unmarried or not co-habiting, those who had an annual household income of $A 16,000 or less, and those women whose partner was absent from the household; One hundred mothers were added to the sample on this basis. Thus, a total sample of 2,837 mothers and their singleton infants were selected for longitudinal follow-up, of whom 2,224 (78%) agreed, when their infant was one year old, to participate. Of the 2,224 women who agreed to participate, 1,880 (85%) returned a completed questionnaire when their child was 2 years old. These are the children that are the focus of this report (see Figure 1).
Of potential concern is the representativeness of the study sample to the population from which it was drawn. Our assessment of this suggests that the two year old follow-up sample is a reasonably representative sample of two year-old non-Aboriginal Western Australian singleton children. The potential effects of sample attrition were examined by comparing a range of early life characteristics present at three months of age for the respondents at two years of age compared with the respondents at three months. Small but significant variations were noted. Participants at two years of age were more likely to be from families earning more than $A25,000 per year (74.5% vs 70.7%, Chi square = 10.4, df = 2, p <.01), married households (79.3% vs 75.6%, Chi square = 11.7, df = 4, p <.03), with a higher level of maternal education (27.8% vs 23.3%, Chi square = 23.6, df = 4, p <.001). Aside from this no significant differences were observed for mother’s place of birth, number of children or adults in the household, father absence, or receipt of government benefits.
Exclusions
As the principal focus of this study is on the phenomenology of late talking in children where English is their only language, prior to undertaking analysis the 1880 subjects were assessed for eligibility and possible exclusion. Thirty-three subjects were excluded because they did not speak English, another 66 children who spoke both English and another language were excluded and 7 children with known medical conditions or syndromes. A further 8 subjects lacked sufficient data from which to determine their possible eligibility and hence they were excluded. In total, 114 subjects were excluded leaving a final subject pool of 1766 (Figure 1).
Data collection
Data from the RASCALS participants were collected by postal questionnaire. On, or within a month of, the study child’s first and subsequent birthdays to age eight, the parents were mailed a questionnaire for self-completion with a reply paid envelope. Mothers (who represented the majority of the respondents) provided all details on themselves, partners and the study child. Non-respondents received a reminder letter and were subsequently contacted directly by the study research assistant to confirm receipt of the questionnaire and ascertain reasons for non-response. Only data collected at ages one and two were used in the analysis reported here.
Measures
In line with our ecological model, the variable space is drawn from three broad domains of potential developmental influence: Characteristics related to the mother, to the family and to the child. The measures have been used successfully in other population based child health surveys in Western Australia (Garton, Zubrick, & Silburn, 1995; Silburn et al., 1996; Zubrick et al., 1995; Zubrick et al., 1997). The high completion rate (85%) and low level of missing data (2.2%) in this study provides further support for the suitability of the measures for diverse population samples.
Maternal characteristics
Respondents were asked their age in years, current educational level, place of birth, whether they were currently employed and, if so, the numbers of hours per week that they were in paid employment, and whether they smoked during their pregnancy with the index child, and whether they were a current smoker.
Each mother completed the 42-item Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS). The DASS items assesses symptoms of depression, anxiety and stress in adults. Each item (ie “I feel sad and depressed”) is rated on a four point Likert scale. Items are summed to generate a score for each of the three domains. The scale has high reliability for the Depression (alpha = .91), Anxiety (alpha = .81) and Stress (alpha = .89) scales, and good discriminant and concurrent validity (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995a; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995b). Higher scores are associated with higher levels of distress.
Mothers were also asked to complete the Parenting Scale (PS). This 30-item questionnaire measures three dysfunctional discipline styles: Laxness (permissive discipline); Over-reactivity (authoritarian discipline, displays of anger, meanness and irritability); and Verbosity (overly long reprimands or reliance on talking). The PS Total score (range = 1–7) increases with increasingly dysfunctional parenting, has good internal consistency (alpha = .84), good test-retest reliability (r = .84), and reliably discriminates between parents of clinical and non-clinical children where scores in excess of 3.1 denote “clinical” levels of dysfunctional parenting (Arnold, O’Leary, Wolff, & Acher, 1993). It has been used extensively in research and been shown to be responsive to parenting interventions.
Family Characteristics
Respondents were also asked to provide details on the number of individuals usually resident with the child and their biological and non-biological relationship to one another. This allowed a basic description of family structure (ie original, step/blended, sole parent or other). While birth order was not gathered, the number of children in the household under the age of 18 was recorded. Total family income before tax was gathered as well as receipt of government benefits. The residential address was linked to census track data permitting each child’s record to be populated with three small-area indices – the Socioeconomic Indicators for Areas (SEIFA) (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 1998).
The SEIFA indicators used in this report measure Disadvantage, Resources and Occupation/education within the census collection district of the index household. These indexes were developed by the Australian Bureau of Statistics. Each index summarizes a different aspect of the socio-economic conditions of the Australian population using a combination of variables, in this case, from the 1996 Population and Housing Census. The Index of Relative Socio-Economic Disadvantage is derived from variables that reflect or measure relative disadvantage. Variables used to calculate the index of relative socio-economic disadvantage include low income, low educational attainment, high unemployment and people with low skilled occupations. Lower scores are associated with greater disadvantage. The Index of Economic Resources summarizes the income and expenditure of families, such as income and rent living in the census district. Additionally, variables which reflect wealth, such as dwelling size, are also included. Lower scores reflect lower area economic resources. The Index of Education and Occupation is designed to reflect the educational and occupational structure of communities. The education variables in this index show either the level of qualification achieved or whether further education is being undertaken. The occupation variables classify the workforce into the major groups of the Australian Standard Classification of Occupations (ASCO) and the unemployed. This index does not include any income variables. Lower scores are associated with lower levels of education and lower levels of job skill. Each index is standardized to have a mean of 1000 and a standard deviation of 100.
Mothers completed the 12-item General Factor scale from the McMaster Family Assessment Device (FAD). The 12-item General Factor scale measures overall family functioning across six areas of family functioning: problem-solving, communication, affective involvement, affective responsiveness, roles, and behavior control. It has adequate test-retest reliability, low correlations with social desirability, and shows evidence of both concurrent and discriminative validity (Miller, Epstein, Bishop, & Keitner, 1985). Chronbach alphas on the general factor scale are on the order of .86 (Epstein, Bishop, Ryan, Miller, & Keitner, 1993; Miller et al., 1985; Zubrick et al., 1997). Higher scores are associated with higher levels of dysfunction.
Finally, mothers were asked if there was a family history of late talking (i.e. “Has anyone in your family been slow in learning to talk”). Although this is a minimal estimate of family risk, there is evidence in support of validity. Rice, Haney & Wexler (1998) investigated 19 families ascertained via a child with SLI versus 41 control families. This question yielded 39% of the SLI families with a positive history versus 10% of the control families, a statistically significant difference.
Characteristics of the child: birth status
The population database from which the RASCALS sample was drawn contains each child’s gender, birth date, race (Caucasian, Aboriginal and Other), birthweight in grams, low birthweight status (<2500 grams), time to spontaneous respiration in minutes and gestational age in weeks (Stanley et al., 1997). These data are collected by statute on all live-births, stillbirths and neonatal deaths in the state of Western Australia. An additional measure, the proportion of optimal birth weight (POBW), is also derived from these data.
POBW is a measure of the appropriateness of intrauterine growth and is routinely calculated from the birth records of all children born in Western Australia. Because birth weight is the end result of growth over the period of gestation it is therefore determined both by the length of gestation and the rate of intra-uterine growth. The rate of intrauterine growth is determined by many factors both pathological (maternal, fetal or environmental) and non-pathological (genetic endowment, particularly fetal gender, and maternal environment). Thus it is appropriate that fetal growth rate should vary between individuals, since the non-pathological factors determining growth rate varies between individuals: female newborns appropriately weigh less than male newborns of the same gestation, babies of small women weigh less than babies of tall women and a woman’s first birth tends to weigh less than her subsequent births. We define the optimal fetal growth rate for any particular fetus as the median birth weight achieved by fetuses with the same values for the non-pathological determinants of fetal growth and duration of gestation1, in the absence of any pathological determinants of fetal growth. This median is expressed as the ‘optimal birth weight’ once the values of the non-pathological determinants of growth have been specified.
The non-pathological determinants considered in our statistical models were fetal gender, maternal age, height and parity. Exclusion of pathological factors was achieved by limiting the sample from which optimal birth weights were identified to singleton, live births without congenital abnormalities born to non-smoking mothers following pregnancies without any complications known to affect intrauterine growth (Blair, 1996). The median value of POBW is 100 and values less than this signify infants that are under grown while values greater than this represent growth in excess of optimal growth. Infants whose POBW is less than 85% are classified as being growth restricted at birth.
POBW is an important index of the child’s developmental status and is associated with increased risks for developmental and academic failure (Zubrick et al., 2000). The advantage of this measure of appropriateness of growth, over birth weight, is that it is both individualised and takes into account the duration of gestation. The advantage over the commonly used percentile measures (sometimes termed ‘small for gestational age’) is that it is more accurate and generalizable at the extremes, and being a parametric ratio quantity, is more amenable to statistical manipulation. Where POBW can be calculated it is generally preferable to more traditional measures such as gestational age and birthweight (Blair, Liu, de Klerk, & Lawrence, 2005).
Characteristics of the child: developmental status
Mothers were asked to complete the Infant/Child Monitoring Questionnaires (now called the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ)) (Bricker & Squires, 1999; Squires & Bricker, 1993; Squires, Bricker, & Potter, 1997; Squires et al., 1999) for children aged 24-months. The ASQ requires the mother to observe her child and answer six questions about her child’s development in each of five principal areas: (1) Communication, (2) Gross Motor skills, (3) Fine Motor skills, (4) Adaptive problem solving , and (5) Personal-Social skills, for a total of 30 items. A final set of questions enquire about the child’s overall development and are not used here. The authors of the ASQ have undertaken revision of the instrument and as a result there is some slight variation in the original items used in the RASCALS cohort, which are those in the original Infant/Child Monitoring Questionnaires, and those now currently used in the ASQ (see Squires, Potter, Bricker, 1995, p. 141).
The manual reports that at 24 months of age the internal reliability for each of the ASQ scales is 0.75 for Communication, 0.80 for Gross Motor skills, 0.58 for Fine Motor skills, 0.57 for Adaptive problem solving, and 0.58 for Personal social skills. Two week test-retest reliability, measured as a percentage of agreement between parent-completed questionnaires was 94% and inter-observer reliability is similarly high. Extensive analyses of the validity of the ASQ , including receiver operator characteristics (ROC) analysis and assessments of concurrent validity using the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale (Thorndike, Hagen, & Sattler, 1985), the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (Bayley, 1969)and the Revised Gesell and Armatruda Developmental and Neurological Examination (Knobloch, Stevens, & Malone, 1980), show the ASQ to be valid for the identification of children at risk for developmental difficulties and in need of additional examination (Squires et al., 1999). Using the RASCALS data, composite scores for each of the five areas were calculated using the method outlined in the administration manual and cut-offs applied to differentiate normal and abnormal performances (Squires & Bricker, 1993). Further detail about the Communication Scale is provided below.
Each mother completed a Child Behavior Checklist on the child (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1991). The CBCL is a checklist of 99 specific behavior problems (e.g., ‘nightmares’, ‘too shy or timid’, ‘wakes up often at night’, ‘aches or pains (without medical cause)’, ‘gets in many fights’, ‘destroys his/her own things’). Respondents are asked to identify if the item is ‘Not true’, Somewhat true’ or ‘Very true’. The number of problems are converted to normalized T scores (Mean = 50, SD = 10). The higher the T score, the higher the problem behaviors. T Scores can be computed for Total Problems, Internal and Externalizing problems, and seven syndrome scales (Anxiety/Depression, Withdrawal, Sleep Problems, Somatic problems, Aggressive behavior, Destructive behavior and Other Behavioral Problems). We report T scores and Abnormal T scores for Total Problems and Internalizing and Externalizing problems. The CBCL was chosen on the basis of its extensive use in Australia and in numerous other settings and cultures (Hensley, 1988; Sawyer, Arney et al., 2000; Sawyer et al., 2001; Sawyer, Clark, & Baghurst, 1993; Sawyer, Kosky et al., 2000; Sawyer, Sarris, Baghurst, Cornish, & Kalucy, 1990; Verhulst et al., 2003; Zubrick et al., 1995). Test-retest reliability is reported to be on the order of 0.87 over one-week and 0.75 over periods of twelve months (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1991). The CBCL has been demonstrated to be acceptable, relatively quick to administer, and to supply adequate coverage of the phenomenology of child behavior problems. Australian studies have found eight week test-retest reliability of the parent CBCL to be 0.87 and six month test-retest reliability 0.75 (Garton et al., 1995; Zubrick et al., 1997). The CBCL has also been used in previous studies of late talkers (Carson et al., 1998; Rescorla & Achenbach, 2002).
The Revised Dimensions of Temperament Survey (DOTS-R) (Windle, 1992; Windle & Lerner, 1986) was completed by the mother of each study child. In pre-school children this 54-item scale measures nine characteristics of temperament: (1) activity level – general (high scores signify high levels of energy and motor activity), (2) activity level – sleep (high scores signify high motor activity during sleep e.g. tossing and turning), (3) approach-withdrawal (low scorers tend to withdraw or move away from persons, objects, situations), (4) flexibility-rigidity (low scorers respond inflexibly to changes in the environment) (5) mood (low scores are characterized by negative affect), (6) rhythmicity – sleep (low scorers signify an irregular timing of daily sleep-wake cycle), (7) rhythmicity – eating (low scores characterize irregularity of eating habits pertinent to appetite and quantity consumed), (8) rhythmicity – daily habits (low scores characterize irregularity of diurnal activities such as toileting, peak periods of vigor, taking a rest), and (9) task orientation (low scores lack concentration and lack perceptual focus in the presence of extraneous stimuli and do not tend to stay with, or continue with an activity for relatively long periods of time). Alpha coefficients of internal consistency for each of the characteristics range from .62 to .89 and six week test-retest correlations range from .59 to .75 (Windle, 1992). We conducted a preliminary factor analysis to assess the suitability of Windle’s model for use with Australian children. Using Pearson product moment correlations as input a principal components analysis using the RASCALS data revealed good factorability (KMO = .88), communalities ranging from .306 to .839 with nine factors accounting for 52.0% of the common factor variance. The number of non-trivial factors was determined by using Cattel’s scree plot in association with those Eigen values greater than or equal to 1.0. Following a varimax rotation the final factor structure revealed a satisfactory correspondence with only four of the fifty-four variables loading on factors different from those reported by Windle (Windle, 1992; Windle & Lerner, 1986). In keeping with Windle’s recommendations each of the dimension scores was coded into a dichotomous variable with a score of 1 indicative of dimension scores below the 30th percentile for all but activity-level sleep and general activity level – these being coded 1 if above the 70th percentile (Windle, 1992).
Finally, the mother was asked the child’s day care status at the time of the interview, and the hours per week that the child attended or received day care.
Results
All data were screened and distributions inspected for outliers and incorrect values. Missing data were present, to some degree, in all modelled variables. The average amount of missing data among the 1766 subjects was 2.2% and ranged from zero (mother’s place of birth, child gender and age) to 7.6% (Dimensions of Temperament, rhythmicity-sleep). To address this problem we carried out data imputation via a multiple imputation procedure using SAS PROC MI (SAS Institute, 2004). Five complete data sets were generated; each subsequent analysis was performed on each of the data sets and results were then combined. This imputation approach is preferable to single imputation which substitutes a single number for each missing value in that the multiple imputation approach accounts for the variability in plausible replacement values (Rubin, 1987). Using a Markov Chain Monte Carlo procedure, all data were imputed at the item level before computing the scale values.
Following imputation, characteristics of the mother, the family and the child were summarised (Table 1). Mothers were predominately between the ages of 24 and 34 years at the time of the birth of the child. Australia mandates 10 years of compulsory education. Years (ie Grades) 11 and 12 are principally used for college entry preparation. The majority of mothers completed 10 years of education and the distribution of maternal education was bimodal with about a quarter of mothers having less than 12 years of education, 19.3% completed 12 years of schooling, 13.1% completed a trade certificate, 13.7% some study towards a post-school qualification, while another 29.2% had completed a post-school technical qualification or university degree. Three quarters of the mothers were born in Australia, and forty percent of them were in paid employment working an average of 22 hours per week. Mean maternal DASS scores for depression, anxiety and stress are comparable to those of the normative sample (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995b). The mean PS Score reported by the RASCALS mothers was also commensurate with the means reported originally by Arnold et al (1993) for their clinical and non-clinical groups and is comparable to population means reported by Zubrick et al. (2005).
Table 1.
N 1766a |
||
---|---|---|
| ||
Maternal characteristics | ||
Mean | SD | |
Age at child’s birth (years) | 29.5 | 4.9 |
% | ||
⇐ 19 years | 2.5 | |
20–34 years | 82.1 | |
35+ years | 15.4 | |
| ||
Education | % | |
< 12 years | 24.6 | |
12 years | 19.3 | |
Trade certificate | 13.1 | |
Post-school study | 13.7 | |
Completed post-school qualification | 29.2 | |
| ||
Mother’s place of birth | % | |
Australia | 74.7 | |
United Kingdom | 14.8 | |
New Zealand | 3.9 | |
Asia and India | 2.4 | |
Europe | 1.8 | |
North America | 1.5 | |
Africa | 1.0 | |
| ||
Currently in paid employment | % | |
Yes | 40.1 | |
Mean | SD | |
Hours in paid employment per week | 22.0 | 12.7 |
| ||
Cigarette use | % | |
Smoked before or during pregnancy | 28.2 | |
Current smoker | 20.0 | |
| ||
DASS scoresb | Mean | SD |
Depression | 3.7 | 5.3 |
Anxiety | 1.9 | 3.3 |
Stress | 7.4 | 6.8 |
Clinical ranges | % | |
Depression | ||
Normal | 90.1 | |
Mild | 4.1 | |
Moderate – severe | 5.7 | |
Anxiety | ||
Normal | 94.3 | |
Mild | 1.9 | |
Moderate to Severe | 3.8 | |
Stress | ||
Normal | 86.7 | |
Mild | 7.1 | |
Moderate – severe | 6.1 | |
Mean | SD | |
| ||
Parenting Scorec | 2.8 | 0.57 |
Parenting score in clinical range | % | |
Yes | 28.8 | |
Family characteristics | ||
Family structure | % | |
Original | 84.6 | |
Step/blended | 5.1 | |
Sole parent | 7.9 | |
Other | 2.4 | |
| ||
Family size | Mean | SD |
Number of children | 2.06 | 0.95 |
% | ||
1 | 29.8 | |
2 or more | 70.2 | |
| ||
Income | % | |
$1 to $154 wk / $1-8000 yr | 0.7 | |
$155 to $308 wk / $8001-16000 yr | 5.5 | |
$309 to $481 wk / $16001-25000 yr | 11.5 | |
$482 to $769 wk / $25001-40000 yr | 25.9 | |
$770 to $961 wk / $40001-50000 yr | 17.0 | |
$962 or more wk / $50001 or more yr | 35.4 | |
Not stated | 3.8 | |
| ||
Area SES indicators | Mean | SD |
Disadvantage | 1007.9 | 59.6 |
Resources | 1028.6 | 63.3 |
Occupation/education | 987.3 | 76.2 |
% | ||
Disadvantage –1.0sd | 3.4 | |
Resources –1.0sd | 1.7 | |
Occupation/education –1.0sd | 7.0 | |
| ||
Family function | % | |
Abnormal family function | 9.4 | |
| ||
Child characteristics | ||
Gender (male) | % 50.7 |
|
| ||
Mean | SD | |
Age (yrs) | 2.1 | 0.13 |
| ||
Race e | % | |
Caucasian | 96.6 | |
Aboriginal | 0.8 | |
Other | 2.4 | |
| ||
Neonatal period | % | |
Gestational age < 37 weeks | 6.2 | |
< 85% POBW | 9.1 | |
Low birth weight (<2500 gms) | 3.7 | |
Mean | SD | |
Mean percent expected birth weight | 100.4 | 12.3 |
Mean birth weight - grams | 3439.7 | 515.7 |
Time to spontaneous respiration (mins) | 1.26 | 0.9 |
Gestational age (weeks) | 38.9 | 1.7 |
| ||
Family history of late talking | % | |
Yes | 13.5 | |
| ||
Ages and Stages Questionnaire
(% abnormal) |
% | |
Communication score | 2.3 | |
Gross motor score | 2.0 | |
Fine motor score | 3.7 | |
Adaptive score | 8.3 | |
Personal social score | 1.6 | |
| ||
Child Behaviour Checklist | Mean | SD |
CBCL Total T Score | 48.1 | 8.4 |
CBCL Internalising T | 46.3 | 8.4 |
CBCL Externalising T | 49.5 | 8.3 |
% | ||
Total T Score Abnormal | 10.4 | |
Total Internalizing T Score Abnormal | 7.2 | |
Total Externalising T Score Abnormal | 16.3 | |
| ||
Child characteristics | ||
| ||
Dimensions of temperament | % | |
Higher general activity level | 37.1 | |
Higher sleep activity level | 38.9 | |
Withdrawal orientation | 23.8 | |
Inflexible behavioural style | 26.6 | |
Negative mood quality | 24.7 | |
Irregularity in sleeping pattern | 26.3 | |
Irregularity in eating behavior | 23.1 | |
Irregular daily habits | 25.5 | |
Low persistence and high distractibility | 26.3 | |
| ||
Day care status at interview | % | |
In day care | 37.4 | |
Mean | SD | |
Hours per week in day care | 15.5 | 12.9 |
Missing data were present in 44 of the 47 variables in the table. The average amount of missing data was 2.2% and ranged from 0% to 7.6%. Data imputation methods have been used (see methods).
Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995a; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995b).
Parenting Scale (Arnold et al., 1993).
* = p< .05; ** = p<.01; *** = p<.001
Racial status recorded on Midwive’s Notification Form
Families were predominately two-parent original families with 13% of the remaining families being either step/blended or sole parent families. The average number of children per family was two. Assessment of family income revealed a small proportion of families (5.5%) earning $A16,000 or less per annum. Area SEIFA indicators for disadvantage, resources, and occupation/education were well within population averages for these measures. About 9% of families were classified as having abnormal family function using the FAD. This compares well to the population proportion of Western Australia families reporting abnormal family function (10%) (Silburn et al., 1996). A family history of late talking was reported in 13.5% of the families.
Children in the study were an average age of 2.1 (sd 0.13) years and were nearly all Caucasian (96.6%). With respect to neonatal characteristics, fewer low birthweight infants were in the study sample (3.7%) relative to the Western Australian population proportion (6.4%) but otherwise the neonatal characteristics of the study sample were unremarkable with mean birthweight, mean gestational age and time to spontaneous respiration being comparable to Western Australian population averages (Gee, 1996).
With respect to normative development, study sample mean CBCL T scores were at the approximate 50th percentile and about 10% of the study children had a CBCL Total T-score in the clinical range. These are the first Australian data to be gathered on children as young as two years; however, the proportion of children scoring in the clinical range is comparable to Western Australian population studies of 4-11 year old children using the appropriate-for-age CBCL parent reported measure (Zubrick et al., 1995). The ASQ developmental measures ranged from 1.6% (Personal Social Score) to 8.3% (Adaptive Score) in the abnormal range with 2.3% of the sample having ASQ Communication Scores in the abnormal range. A little over one-third of the children were receiving day care with the mean number of hours being 16 per week.
Determination of late language emergence (LLE) and prevalence
The scale of the study required an assessment of late language emergence with minimal effort loading on the part of the parent respondents. The instrument used is the ASQ Communication Scale, comprised of a short list of language milestones drawn from the normative literature by Bricker & Squires (1999). The Communication Scale is part of an instrument developed as a parent report measure to screen for developmental impairments. Recently, Luinge, Post, Wit, & Goorhuis-Brouwer (2006) followed the same milestone method to develop a brief language screening instrument intended for public health assessments.
The ASQ Communication Scale uses six items to assess aspects of the child’s developing skills in speech production and comprehension. Mothers were asked to report whether their child could (1) point to pictures on request, (2) use two- or three-word phrases, (3) carry out simple directions on request, (4) name simple objects, (5) point to body parts on request, and (6) use personal pronouns such as “me”, “I” and “you”. The response categories for each item were (1) “Not Yet”, (2) “Sometimes”, and (3) “Yes”.
In our sample the Communication Scale had a Cronbach’s alpha of .71, essentially replicating the estimate provided in the test manual. Because the manual does not report validity estimates for the ASQ Communication subscale (only for the full instrument), we carried out analyses of criterion and concurrent validity for our ASQ outcome measure based on item response theory (ASQ IRT, described further below). Criterion validity is hampered by the lack of an external “gold standard” measure of late language emergence (McCardle, Cooper, & Freund, 2005; Tager-Flusberg & Cooper, 1999). We were, however, in a position to assess some aspects of concurrent validity against another measure of speech and language collected via parent-report at the time of the survey. For half of the cohort (N=902), the LDS had been sought from the parent at the time the questionnaire was completed. Of the children with LDS data, 888 also had ASQ data. This permitted estimating concurrent validity for our measure against the LDS. Both the ASQ IRT score and the LDS score are continuous variables and were moderately correlated (.675; p<.001). Additionally, for those children for whom we had a parent-completed LDS, we were able to calculate mean LDS scores for children differentiated by LLE status on the ASQ. Children defined with LLE on the ASQ measure had significantly lower mean LDS scores than those children classified in the normal range on the ASQ measure (MLLE = 62.5, sd 52.5 vs MNormal = 196.2, sd 70.7; df = 198.1, t = 24.7, p <.001).
We also assessed the correspondence between the LDS item, “Does your child combine 2 or more words into phrases…?” and the ASQ item, “Does your child say 2 or 3 words together…?”. Complete data were available on both of these items for 896 of the children. Frequency distributions were obtained on both the LDS and ASQ items. Ninety percent of children were reported on the LDS to be combining two or more words into phrases and 89% of children were reported on the ASQ to be saying two or three words together. Cross tabulation of these items indicated complete correspondence of these items for 860 of these cases (Chi square = 547.9, df=1, p <.001; Kappa=.78). As initial reports of the validity of the ASQ Communication Scale, these findings suggest an acceptable level of concurrent validity with another measure frequently used to assess early language emergence.
The Graded Response Model
To assess the suitability of the ASQ Communication scale to identify children with late language emergence, we undertook an item response analysis using a type of polytomous item response theory (IRT) model known as the Graded Response Model (GRM) (Samejima, 1969). The GRM is a polytomous IRT model which models each of the three response categories simultaneously, creating a scaled value representing a person’s overall ability on the test. In general, Likert-type scales with fewer than five response choices and a small number of items are difficult to summarize with a single “scale” score that has a quantifiable standard error of measurement. The GRM is well suited to the ASQ analyses because it generates an ordering of persons on the ability scale where the responses for the scale are essentially ordered categorical responses. The GRM assumes: 1) that the relationship between ability level and the probability of endorsing a particular item response category (or a higher category) is monotonic, 2) that the items are unidimensional and have only one common factor and 3) that ability is distributed normally with a mean of zero and a standard deviation of one, even if the items do not measure the entire range of the distribution. This third assumption is not a necessary assumption of the model but is merely an identification condition to set the scale of ability and may be modified if desired. The major advantages of the IRT approach over other methods of scaling include: 1) use of all items rather than a reliance on a single item; 2) differential adjustment for item difficulty; 3) provisions for appropriate handling of missing data by determining estimates of ability that are based on all of the items answered and do not impute the individual’s mean score for missing items; and 4) use of a continuous estimate of (in this case) communication/verbal ability which is on a scale that is not sample dependent.
We commenced our assessment of the ASQ Communication scale by testing the tenability of the dimensionality assumption. To do this we used a principal components analysis. In addition to this traditional analysis, we also used the DETECT algorithm (Stout, 1987) which is confirmatory in nature. The results of both of these analyses indicated that the six items represent only one dimension.
Item characteristic curves (ICCs, also known as item response functions) for each of the six items were then evaluated. For economy of space, an example of one of the items is shown in Figure 2. The lines show the probability of endorsing a certain response at a given level of ability. These figures show that with increasing ability the probability of a ‘Not yet’ response decreases while the probability of a ‘Yes’ response increases. At the upper end of the ability scale there is very little difference in the probability of a yes response. Thus, for Item 2, measuring the use of two or three word phrases (Figure 2), a child with an ability of one standard deviation above the mean would have about the same probability of a ‘yes’ response as an individual with an ability of 3 standard deviations above the mean. The graph in the right panel, the item information curve, represents how well the item can distinguish or discriminate between different levels of ability. We can see that item 2 is best at discriminating individuals with ability near −1.5 standard deviations. This is where the item is most informative and where measurement error is the lowest. Our assessment of each of the item characteristic curves showed that the ASQ Communication scale measured the low end of ability quite well.
Having determined the item parameters from the child’s response on each of the six items, they were then used to create an estimate of each child’s ability. This estimate gives the child’s most “likely” ability level that explains the child’s responses. As shown in the test information curve in Figure 3, we can see that the six item scale provides increasing discrimination and lower measurement error in the range from −1.0 to −1.5 standard deviations below the mean. The IRT/GRM models do not generate an exact cut-off point for creating a dichotomous variable for LLE, but the choice of the cut-off point is guided, in part, by the range of scores within which the scale is more precise in discriminating between different ability levels and also by the researcher’s judgment based on previous research and clinical factors.
For reasons of clinical benchmarking and to avoid missing children with LLE we chose – 1.0 S.D. as the cut-off to demark those children with and without LLE (c.f., Feldman, et al, 2005). Of the 1766 children a total of 238 (13.4%) were classified as having LLE (Table 2). The 13.4% estimate from the IRT composite can be compared to an alternative estimate. Following precedents in the literature, the ability to combine words at 24 months was used as a criterion for grouping children. Of the sample, 10.7% of the children were reported to not combine words; 8.4% “sometimes” and 80.9% “yes,” yielding an overall estimate of 19.1% of the sample who were not routinely combining words in utterances.
Table 2.
Language Emergence | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Normal N = 1528 |
Late N = 238 |
X2 (t)d | |||
Maternal characteristics | |||||
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||
Age at child’s birth (years) | 29.5 | 5.0 | 29.2 | 4.7 | (0.79) |
% | % | ||||
⇐ 19 years | 2.5 | 2.6 | .99 | ||
20 –34 years | 81.8 | 84.2 | |||
35+ years | 15.7 | 13.2 | |||
| |||||
Education | % | % | |||
< 12 years | 23.9 | 29.6 | 6.82 | ||
12 years | 19.3 | 18.5 | |||
Trade certificate | 13.3 | 12.4 | |||
Post-school study | 13.4 | 15.9 | |||
Completed post-school qualification | 30.1 | 23.6 | |||
| |||||
Mother’s place of birth | % | % | |||
Australia | 74.5 | 76.5 | 4.26 | ||
United Kingdom | 15.0 | 13.2 | |||
New Zealand | 4.0 | 3.4 | |||
Asia and India | 2.2 | 3.8 | |||
Europe | 1.7 | 1.3 | |||
North America | 1.6 | 1.3 | |||
Africa | 1.1 | 0.4 | |||
| |||||
Currently in paid employment | % | % | |||
Yes | 40.7 | 36.7 | 1.37 | ||
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||
Hours per week in paid employment |
22.0 | 12.7 | 21.7 | 12.9 | (0.37) |
| |||||
Cigarette use | % | % | |||
Smoked before/during pregnancy |
28.0 | 30.0 | 0.41 | ||
Current smoker | 19.6 | 23.0 | 1.48 | ||
Depression | 3.6 | 5.3 | 4.0 | 5.5 | (−1.22) |
Anxiety | 1.9 | 3.3 | 2.1 | 3.6 | (−0.99) |
Stress | 7.4 | 6.8 | 7.6 | 6.5 | (−0.34) |
Clinical ranges | % | % | |||
Depression | 0.47 | ||||
Normal | 90.3 | 89.1 | |||
Mild | 4.1 | 4.2 | |||
Moderate - severe | 5.6 | 6.7 | |||
Anxiety | 0.51 | ||||
Normal | 94.5 | 93.3 | |||
Mild | 1.9 | 2.1 | |||
Moderate to Severe | 3.7 | 4.6 | |||
Stress | 2.98 | ||||
Normal | 87.1 | 84.2 | |||
Mild | 6.7 | 9.8 | |||
Moderate - severe | 6.2 | 6.1 | |||
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||
| |||||
Parenting Score | 2.8 | 0.6 | 2.9 | 0.6 | (−3.04)** |
In clinical range | % | % | |||
27.6 | 36.9 | 8.67** | |||
Family characteristics | |||||
Family structure | % | % | |||
Original | 84.9 | 82.8 | 0.73 | ||
Step/blended | 5.0 | 5.6 | |||
Sole parent | 7.7 | 9.0 | |||
Other | 2.4 | 2.6 | |||
| |||||
Family size | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |
Number of children | 2.0 | 0.9 | 2.2 | 1.0 | (−2.85)** |
% | % | ||||
1 | 31.4 | 20.1 | 16.6*** | ||
2 | 68.6 | 79.9 | |||
| |||||
Income | % | % | |||
$1 to $154 wk / $1-8000 yr | 0.6 | 0.9 | 9.41 | ||
$155 to $308 wk / $8001- 16000 yr |
5.5 | 5.6 | |||
$309 to $481 wk / $16001- 25000 yr |
10.4 | 16.7 | |||
$482 to $769 wk / $25001- 40000 yr |
26.0 | 26.1 | |||
$770 to $961 wk / $40001- 50000 yr |
17.3 | 16.7 | |||
$962 or more wk / $50001 or more yr |
36.4 | 31.2 | |||
Not stated | 3.7 | 3.0 | |||
| |||||
Area SES indicators | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |
Disadvantage | 1008.6 | 59.6 | 1004.0 | 59.3 | (1.11) |
Resources | 1028.6 | 63.3 | 1028.4 | 63.3 | (0.05) |
Occupation/education | 988.3 | 76.2 | 981.0 | 75.8 | (1.36) |
% | % | ||||
Disadvantage -1.0sd | 3.4 | 3.3 | 0.01 | ||
Resources -1.0sd | 1.7 | 1.4 | 0.11 | ||
Occupation/education -1.0sd | 6.8 | 8.5 | 0.91 | ||
| |||||
Family function e | % | % | |||
Abnormal family function | 9.1 | 11.8 | 1.75 | ||
| |||||
Family history of late talking | % | % | |||
Yes | 12.1 | 22.2 | 18.2*** | ||
| |||||
Child characteristics | |||||
Gender (male) | 47.6 | 70.8 | 44.3*** | ||
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||
| |||||
Age (yrs) | 2.11 | 0.135 | 2.08 | 0.104 | (3.64)*** |
| |||||
Race | % | % | |||
Caucasian | 96.6 | 96.4 | 0.34 | ||
Aboriginal | 0.8 | 0.7 | |||
Other | 2.3 | 2.9 | |||
| |||||
Neonatal period | % | % | |||
< 85% POBW | |||||
8.2 | 14.7 | 10.5** | |||
Low birth weight (<2500 gms) | 3.4 | 5.9 | 3.56 | ||
<37 weeks gestation | 5.5 | 10.7 | 9.6** | ||
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||
Mean POBW | 100.6 | 12.0 | 99.5 | 14.0 | (1.28) |
Mean birth weight - grams | 3446.4 | 498.0 | 3396.9 | 615.7 | (1.35) |
Time to spontaneous respiration (minutes) |
1.3 | 0.9 | 1.3 | 1.0 | (−0.83) |
Gestational age (weeks) | 39.0 | 1.6 | 38.8 | 2.1 | (1.86) |
| |||||
Ages and Stages | |||||
Questionnaire (% abnormal) | % | % | |||
Communication Score | 0.0 | 16.9 | 264.3*** | ||
Gross Motor Score | 1.4 | 5.9 | 21.2*** | ||
Fine Motor Score | 2.7 | 8.1 | 18.1*** | ||
Adaptive Score | 6.3 | 21.1 | 59.3*** | ||
Personal Social Score | 0.6 | 8.1 | 73.1*** | ||
| |||||
CBCL Total T Score | 47.8 | 8.3 | 49.8 | 8.8 | (−3.42)*** |
CBCL Internalising T | 46.0 | 8.3 | 48.0 | 8.8 | (−3.46)*** |
CBCL Externalising T | 49.2 | 8.2 | 51.1 | 9.0 | (−3.16)** |
% | % | ||||
Total T Score Abnormal | 9.6 | 15.6 | 7.94** | ||
Total Internalizing T Score Abnormal |
6.7 | 11.0 | 5.64* | ||
Total Externalising T Score Abnormal |
15.1 | 23.8 | 11.43*** | ||
| |||||
Dimensions of temperament | % | % | |||
Higher general activity level | 36.9 | 38.2 | 0.15 | ||
Higher sleep activity level | 38.0 | 44.1 | 3.22 | ||
Withdrawal orientation | 23.6 | 25.0 | 0.22 | ||
Inflexible behavioural style | 26.3 | 28.4 | 0.46 | ||
Negative mood quality | 23.7 | 31.3 | 6.39* | ||
Irregularity in sleeping pattern | 25.6 | 31.3 | 3.44 | ||
Irregularity in eating behavior | 23.2 | 22.6 | 0.04 | ||
Irregular daily habits | 24.9 | 29.2 | 2.00 | ||
Low persistence and high distractibility |
25.7 | 30.3 | 2.25 | ||
| |||||
Day care status at interview | % | % | |||
In day care | 37.8 | 34.6 | 0.90 | ||
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||
Mean hrs in day care | 15.2 | 12.7 | 17.3 | 14.4 | (−1.38) |
49 subjects had missing data on the outcome variable (Late talker status).
Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995a; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995b).
Parenting Scale (Arnold et al., 1993).
* = p< .05; ** = p<.01; *** = p<.001
General Factor of the McMaster Family Assessment Device (Byles, Byrne, Boyle, & Offord, 1988)
Late language emergence – bivariate relationships with maternal, family and child characteristics
Comparisons of maternal, family and child characteristics were made for children differentiated by LLE (Table 2). Alpha levels were not adjusted for family-wise or study-wise error in order to detect any possible differences between the groups. As it turned out, when differences were evident they almost all were at conventional levels of adjustment, i.e., < .01 or .001. With respect to maternal characteristics, no significant differences for children with and without LLE were observed with regard to maternal age at the birth of the child, levels of maternal education, mother’s place of birth, maternal uptake of paid employment and cigarette use. There were no significant differences between these groups in their mean maternal DASS scores nor in the proportions of mothers reporting varying levels of clinical depression, anxiety, and stress. The only statistically significant difference observed with regards to maternal characteristics was in the Parenting Score – the mothers of children with LLE reported higher mean PS scores (M = 2.9, sd 0.6 vs M = 2.8, sd 0.6, p < .01) with a correspondingly higher proportion falling within the clinical range (36.9% vs 27.6%, Chi2 = 8.67, df = 1, p < .01) denoting a higher level of dysfunctional parenting.
Within families, LLE was associated with a family history of late talking (22.2% vs 12.1%, Chi2 = 18.2, df = 1, p < .001) and with larger family size as measured by the number of children in the family. When compared with children who did not have LLE, children with LLE were less likely to be the only child (20.1% vs 31.4%, Chi2 = 16.6, df = 3, p < .001). Otherwise there were no significant differences in the family characteristics of children with and without LLE in terms of family type (i.e., two parent, sole parent), income, area level indicators of socioeconomic status, and family function.
With respect to characteristics of the child, there were several significant differences between children with and without LLE. Children with LLE were significantly more likely to be male (70.8% vs 47.6%, Chi2 = 44.3, df = 1, p < .001). While comparisons of their mean ages showed children with LLE to be significantly younger (M = 2.08 years, sd 0.104 vs M = 2.11, sd 0.135, p < .001) this equates to a mean difference of ten days in age between these groups. In practical terms 99.8% of the children were between the ages of 23 and 24 months of age.
There was no significant difference between LLE groups on neonatal measures of birth weight, low birth weight status, and time to spontaneous respiration. However, children with LLE were significantly more likely to be born weighing less than 85% of their optimal birth weight (14.7% vs 8.2%, Chi2 = 10.5, df = 1, p < .01) and less than 37 weeks gestation. Because gestational age, and prematurity specifically, is frequently cited as a confounding factor for LLE, separate investigation of this as a possible threat to the validity of the findings is reported below.
With regard to development, significantly higher proportions of children with LLE were in the abnormal range on the ASQ Gross Motor, Fine Motor, Adaptive, and Personal Social scores. Results on the ASQ Communication Score, which is calculated from the six variables used to define LLE status, revealed all children with LLE to fall in the abnormal range of the Communication Score. In terms of behavioral and emotional adjustment, significantly higher proportions of children with LLE were in the abnormal range on the parent-reported CBCL Total Score (15.6% vs 9.6%, Chi2 = 7.94, df = 1, p < .001) with corresponding and statistically significant elevations in CBCL Internalising problems (11.0% vs 6.7%, Chi2 = 5.64, df = 1, p < .001) and Externalizing problems (23.8% vs 15.1%, Ch2 = 11.4, df =1, p < .01).
The only temperament difference between those children with and without LLE was in negative mood quality. Relative to children without LLE, a significantly greater proportion of children with LLE were reported by their mothers to have negative mood quality (31.3% vs 23.7%, Chi2 = 3.44. df = 1, p <.05).
Finally, there was no difference in the proportion of children with and without LLE who were enrolled in day care nor in the amount of day care they received as measured by mean number of hours.
Late language emergence – multivariate relationships with maternal, family and child characteristics
The numerous relationships of maternal, family and child characteristics with LLE (Table 2) were further investigated using multivariate logistic regression. Logistic regression allows the prediction of a discrete, binary outcome (in this case LLE) from a set of predictor variables (Hosmer & Lemeshow, 1989). The predictor variables may be continuous, dichotomous, discrete or a mix of these types. Estimated effects of the predictor variables are multivariately adjusted for the effects of the other predictors. In this study, the association between the outcome variable (LLE) and the candidate predictor variables were expressed as odds ratios. An odds ratio is the ratio of the probability of the occurrence of an event to the probability of the non-occurrence of the event. In this study, the ‘event’ is LLE and because LLE is an adverse outcome, the predictor variables are ‘risk’ variables. Where predictors are categorical these odds ratios are calculated with reference to a specific base or “reference” category.
The candidate predictor variables were selected from Table 2. In fitting the logistic model, virtually all variables were used and, following Hosmer and Lemwshow (1989), most were coded to be categorical, rather than continuous. Two exceptions were made. First, the country of birth of the mother was not entered in the model. The distribution of this variable reflects differential bias in the exclusion of cases owing to English language requirements. Second, the child’s age in months at the time of the interview was entered as a continuous variable. All other variables were coded as categorical variables (see Table 2).
To account for data imputation procedures (described above) we undertook logistic regression using SAS 9.1 (PROC LOGISTIC and PROC MIANALYZE) (SAS Institute Inc., 2004). Instead of filling in a single value for each missing value, these procedures combine the results of the analyses of imputations and generate valid statistical inferences by replacing each missing value with a set of plausible values that represent the uncertainty about the right value to impute (Rubin, 1976; Rubin, 1987).
All variables were entered into the model in a single step with LLE as the response variable. For each of the predictor variables, parameter estimates (Betas), their standard errors, 95% confidence intervals, degrees of freedom, t values, and their probabilities along with the odds ratios and their 95% confidence intervals are shown in Table 3.
Table 3.
Parameter | Estimate | S.E. | 95% CI min |
95% CI max |
df | t | p | Odds ratio | 95% CI min |
95% CI max |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
intercept | 3.7298 | 2.0435 | −0.2755 | 7.7351 | 26164 | 1.83 | 0.068 | |||
Maternal factors | ||||||||||
Educational level | ||||||||||
<12 Years | −0.1064 | 0.2371 | −0.5713 | 0.3586 | 2147.5 | −0.45 | 0.654 | 0.899 | 0.565 | 1.431 |
12 Years | 0.0258 | 0.2539 | −0.4722 | 0.5238 | 1876.4 | 0.1 | 0.919 | 1.026 | 0.624 | 1.688 |
Trade certificate | −0.3990 | 0.2321 | −0.8543 | 0.0563 | 1358.5 | −1.72 | 0.086 | 0.671 | 0.426 | 1.058 |
Post-school study | −0.1079 | 0.2769 | −0.6510 | 0.4353 | 1274.9 | −0.39 | 0.697 | 0.898 | 0.522 | 1.545 |
Completed post-school qualification | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
Age at birth of child | ||||||||||
⇐19 Years | −0.1813 | 0.5941 | −1.3482 | 0.9855 | 580.43 | −0.31 | 0.760 | 0.834 | 0.260 | 2.679 |
20-34 Years | −0.4144 | 0.6277 | −1.6463 | 0.8176 | 865.16 | −0.66 | 0.509 | 0.661 | 0.193 | 2.265 |
35+ Years | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
Employment status | ||||||||||
No paid employment | 0.1608 | 0.1780 | −0.1882 | 0.5098 | 5108.8 | 0.9 | 0.366 | 1.174 | 0.828 | 1.665 |
In paid employment | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
Cigarette use | ||||||||||
Non-smoker during pregnancy | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
Smoked during pregnancy | −0.2403 | 0.2707 | −0.7725 | 0.2920 | 372.93 | −0.89 | 0.375 | 0.786 | 0.462 | 1.339 |
Current non-smoker | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
Current smoker | −0.3242 | 0.2970 | −0.9084 | 0.2600 | 343.89 | −1.09 | 0.276 | 0.723 | 0.403 | 1.297 |
Depression-Anxiety-Stress Scale | ||||||||||
DASS - Depress - Normal | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
DASS - Depress - Mild | −0.2274 | 0.4140 | −1.0394 | 0.5846 | 1970.3 | −0.55 | 0.583 | 0.797 | 0.354 | 1.794 |
DASS - Depress - Severe | −0.1514 | 0.4810 | −1.0946 | 0.7918 | 2569.6 | −0.31 | 0.753 | 0.859 | 0.335 | 2.207 |
DASS - Anxious - Normal | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
DASS - Anxious - Mild | 0.2318 | 0.6171 | −0.9777 | 1.4414 | 9287.8 | 0.38 | 0.707 | 1.261 | 0.376 | 4.227 |
DASS - Anxious - Severe | −0.2373 | 0.5379 | −1.2918 | 0.8171 | 5106.2 | −0.44 | 0.659 | 0.789 | 0.275 | 2.264 |
DASS - Stress - Normal | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
DASS - Stress - Mild | 0.2232 | 0.3000 | −0.3650 | 0.8115 | 2055.9 | 0.74 | 0.457 | 1.250 | 0.694 | 2.251 |
DASS - Stress - Severe | −0.2487 | 0.4774 | −1.1847 | 0.6874 | 3775 | −0.52 | 0.603 | 0.780 | 0.306 | 1.988 |
Parenting Scale | ||||||||||
Parenting Scale - Non-clinical | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
Parenting Scale - Clinical | 0.3284 | 0.2010 | −07580.0758 | 0.7326 | 47.432 | 1.63 | 0.109 | 1.389 | 0.927 | 2.081 |
Family factors | ||||||||||
Family type | ||||||||||
Family type - Original | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
Family type - Step/blended | −0.0459 | 0.3585 | −0.7487 | 0.6569 | 11048 | −0.13 | 0.898 | 0.955 | 0.473 | 1.929 |
Family type - Sole parent | 0.1704 | 0.3552 | −0.5260 | 0.8667 | 5459.5 | 0.48 | 0.632 | 1.186 | 0.591 | 2.379 |
Family type - Other | −0.3894 | 0.5275 | −1.4239 | 0.6451 | 2027.4 | −0.74 | 0.461 | 0.677 | 0.241 | 1.906 |
Number of children in family | ||||||||||
Number of children - 1 | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
Number of children – 2 or more | 0.7315 | 0.2037 | 0.3322 | 1.1309 | 5836.8 | 3.59 | 0.000 | 2.078 | 1.394 | 3.099 |
Family income | ||||||||||
Income <8K | 0.1298 | 1.0090 | −1.8481 | 2.1077 | 10423 | 0.13 | 0.898 | 1.139 | 0.158 | 8.229 |
Income $8-16K | 0.5797 | 0.9896 | −1.3600 | 2.5193 | 23772 | 0.59 | 0.558 | 1.785 | 0.257 | 12.420 |
Income $16-25K | −0.0232 | 0.9946 | −1.9727 | 1.9262 | 282892 | −0.02 | 0.981 | 0.977 | 0.139 | 6.864 |
Income $25-40K | 0.1267 | 1.0046 | −1.8423 | 2.0958 | 94872 | 0.13 | 0.900 | 1.135 | 0.158 | 8.132 |
Income $40-50K | 0.0980 | 1.0015 | −86501.8650 | 2.0609 | 72746 | 0.1 | 0.922 | 1.103 | 0.155 | 7.853 |
Income $50K or more | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
Socioeconomic indicators for area | ||||||||||
SEIFA disadvantage - No | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
SEIFA disadvantage - Yes | −1.2546 | 0.6877 | −2.6158 | 0.1066 | 123.52 | −1.82 | 0.071 | 0.285 | 0.073 | 1.113 |
SEIFA low econ resource - No | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
SEIFA low econ resource - Yes | 0.5416 | 0.7679 | −0.9694 | 2.0526 | 303.35 | 0.71 | 0.481 | 1.719 | 0.379 | 7.788 |
SEIFA low educ occ - No | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
SEIFA low educ occ - Yes | 0.4475 | 0.3568 | −0.2541 | 1.1492 | 377.35 | 1.25 | 0.211 | 1.564 | 0.776 | 3.156 |
Family function | ||||||||||
Family function - Normal | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
Family function - Poor | 0.2721 | 0.2657 | −0.2491 | 0.7932 | 2162.1 | 1.02 | 0.306 | 1.313 | 0.780 | 2.210 |
Family history of late talking | ||||||||||
Family history late talking -No | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
Family history late talking -Yes | 0.7480 | 0.2103 | 0.3348 | 1.1612 | 511.21 | 3.56 | 0.000 | 2.113 | 1.398 | 3.194 |
Currently in day care | ||||||||||
Current day care - No | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
Current day care - Yes | −0.1239 | 0.1790 | −0.4748 | 0.2270 | 19077 | −0.69 | 0.489 | 0.883 | 0.622 | 1.255 |
Child factors | ||||||||||
Gender | ||||||||||
Gender – Female | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
Gender – Male | 1.0087 | 0.1714 | 0.6726 | 1.3449 | 2754.1 | 5.88 | <.0001 | 2.742 | 1.959 | 3.838 |
Proportion of optimal birth weight | ||||||||||
POBW >85% | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
POBW <85% | 0.6370 | 0.2383 | 0.1699 | 1.1040 | 58026 | 2.67 | 0.008 | 1.891 | 1.185 | 3.016 |
Ages and Stages Questionnaire results | ||||||||||
ASQ Gross Motor – normal | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
ASQ Gross Motor – abnormal | 1.1381 | 0.4484 | 0.2590 | 2.0171 | 5400.3 | 2.54 | 0.011 | 3.121 | 1.296 | 7.517 |
ASQ Fine Motor – normal | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
ASQ Fine Motor – abnormal | 0.8717 | 0.3529 | 0.1794 | 1.5641 | 1081.3 | 2.47 | 0.014 | 2.391 | 1.196 | 4.778 |
ASQ Adaptive score – normal | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
ASQ Adaptive score – abnormal | 0.9730 | 0.2372 | 0.5074 | 1.4386 | 769.19 | 4.1 | <.0001 | 2.646 | 1.661 | 4.215 |
ASQ Personal social – normal | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
ASQ Personal social – abnormal | 1.7099 | 0.5036 | 0.7208 | 2.6991 | 575.61 | 3.4 | 0.001 | 5.529 | 2.056 | 14.867 |
Child Behaviour Checklist | ||||||||||
CBCL Total T Score – Normal | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
CBCL Total T Score – Abnormal | 0.1111 | 0.3407 | −0.5567 | 0.7790 | 34510 | 0.33 | 0.744 | 1.118 | 0.573 | 2.179 |
CBCL Internalising – Normal | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
CBCL Internalising – Abnormal | 0.5158 | 0.3395 | −0.1524 | 1.1839 | 288.85 | 1.52 | 0.130 | 1.675 | 0.859 | 3.267 |
CBCL Externalising – Normal | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
CBCL Externalising – Abnormal | 0.2529 | 0.2730 | −0.2847 | 0.7904 | 270.79 | 0.93 | 0.355 | 1.288 | 0.752 | 2.204 |
Dimension of Temperament Scale | ||||||||||
DOTS - High general activity - no | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
DOTS - High general activity - yes | −0.0704 | 0.1819 | −0.4275 | 0.2868 | 583.11 | −0.39 | 0.699 | 0.932 | 0.652 | 1.332 |
DOTS - High sleep activity - no | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
DOTS - High sleep activity - yes | 0.2410 | 0.1735 | −0.1004 | 0.5825 | 283.57 | 1.39 | 0.166 | 1.273 | 0.904 | 1.790 |
DOTS - Withdrawal orientation - no | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
DOTS - Withdrawal orientation - yes | −0.0695 | 0.2070 | −0.4774 | 0.3384 | 224.95 | −0.34 | 0.737 | 0.933 | 0.620 | 1.403 |
DOTS - Inflexible style - no | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
DOTS - Inflexible style - yes | −0.0442 | 0.1960 | −0.4298 | 0.3414 | 323.49 | −0.23 | 0.822 | 0.957 | 0.651 | 1.407 |
DOTS - Negative mood qual - no | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
DOTS - Negative mood qual - yes | 0.2970 | 0.1879 | −0.0718 | 0.6658 | 940.91 | 1.58 | 0.114 | 1.346 | 0.931 | 1.946 |
DOTS - Irregular sleep pattern - no | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
DOTS - Irregular sleep pattern - yes | 0.1396 | 0.2214 | −0.3058 | 0.5849 | 47.243 | 0.63 | 0.532 | 1.150 | 0.737 | 1.795 |
DOTS - Irregular eat patter - no | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
DOTS - Irregular eat patter - yes | −0.2039 | 0.2114 | −0.6193 | 0.2114 | 516.33 | −0.96 | 0.335 | 0.816 | 0.538 | 1.235 |
DOTS - Irregular daily habits - no | ref | 1.000 | 1.000 | 1.000 | ||||||
DOTS - Irregular daily habits - yes | 0.1341 | 0.1859 | −0.2303 | 0.4984 | 52886 | 0.72 | 0.471 | 1.143 | 0.794 | 1.646 |
DOTS - Low persist high distract - no | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
DOTS - Low persist high distract - yes | 0.2992 | 0.1856 | −0.0650 | 0.6633 | 1006.1 | 1.61 | 0.107 | 1.349 | 0.937 | 1.941 |
Time to spontaneous respiration | ||||||||||
Time to spontaneous respiration - <2min | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
Time to spontaneous respiration - >2min | 0.0939 | 0.3028 | −0.4996 | 0.6874 | 91758 | 0.31 | 0.756 | 1.098 | 0.607 | 1.989 |
Premature birth | ||||||||||
Premature - no >36 weeks | ref | 1.000 | ||||||||
Premature - yes ⇐36 weeks | 0.6107 | 0.2898 | 0.0426 | 1.1788 | 4349.3 | 2.11 | 0.035 | 1.842 | 1.044 | 3.250 |
Age months | −0.0094 | 0.0023 | −0.0140 | −0.0049 | 6988.8 | −4.06 | <.0001 | 0.991 | 0.986 | 0.995 |
There were no statistically significant associations between the various maternal characteristics and LLE. No significant associations between LLE and maternal education, age, smoking, psychological state, or parenting style were observed.
In the variables characterising the family, LLE was significantly associated with the number of children in the family. Relative to singleton children, those children with LLE were significantly more likely to have one or more siblings (OR 2.07, 95% ci 1.39 – 3.09). Relative to families without a history of late talking, children with LLE were significantly more likely to be born to families in which a parent has a history of late talking (OR 2.11, ci 1.39 – 3.19). All other statistical associations between LLE and the set of family variables were non-significant. This included family type, income, local area disadvantage, low economic resources, and low education and occupational status, family function and day care use.
Several characteristics of the child were associated with LLE status. Relative to females, males were significantly more likely to have LLE (OR 2.74, 95% ci 1.96 – 3.83). LLE children were more likely to be born at 32 weeks or less gestation (OR 1.84, 95% ci 1.04 – 3.25) and weigh 85% or less of their optimal birth weight (OR 1.89, 95% ci 1.18 – 3.01). All ASQ variables were significantly associated with LLE. Relative to children in each of the respective normal categories, children with LLE were more likely to fall in the abnormal range of the ASQ on measures of Gross Motor Score (OR 3.12, 95% ci 1.29-7.51), Fine Motor Score (OR 2.39, 95% ci 1.19-4.77), Adaptive Score (OR 2.64, 95% ci 1.66 – 4.21) and Personal Social Score (OR 5.52, 95% ci 2.05 – 14.86).
Potential threats to validity
These findings are based upon a well defined and described sample of children aged 2 years. Exclusions from this sample included non-English background and medical conditions or syndromes known at the time of the 2 year observation. To what extent might “covert” disability – i.e. conditions not known at the time of the 2 year assessment but associated with late language emergence – impart bias to these findings? Although the focus of these findings is on the phenomenology of late language emergence at 2 years, the study children were followed until the ages of 8 years.
Subsequent examination revealed that 19 additional children developed syndromal conditions that potentially were related to late language emergence. These children were assessed on the ASQ Communication Scale at age 2 and 37% were in the normal range while 63% were classified as having LLE. Of the 19 children a total of 10 were subsequently found to have intellectual disabilities, 4 were diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorders, and the remaining 5 with developmental syndromal conditions. The multivariate analysis (Table 3) was repeated without these children. Only one change occurred in the estimates: prematurity was no longer a significant predictor of LLE status.
Further inspection of the data revealed an additional 7 children had been born less than 31 weeks of gestation. Six of these children had ASQ Communication Scale scores. Fifty percent of these children were measured at age 2 to have LLE. All 7 of these children were subsequently removed from the multivariate analysis along with the 19 children found later to have syndromal conditions. Aside from the non-significance of gestational age, results revealed no substantive changes to those reported in Table 3.
Discussion
In this study of a large number (1766) of epidemiologically ascertained 24-month-old children, early language acquisition was assessed via a 6-item parent report scale that combined comprehension and production benchmarks. Item response analyses found the composite to measure the low end of ability quite well, providing acceptable levels of discrimination and measurement error. With a criterion of −1 standard deviation from the mean, 13.4% of the sample was identified as showing Late Language Emergence (LLE). Using the criterion of no or only occasional word combinations, 19.1% of the sample were identified.
Bivariate relationships with maternal, family and child characteristics found the following maternal variables to be nonsignificant: age at child’s birth, education, birthplace, paid employment, cigarette use, depression levels, anxiety, and stress. A parenting instrument found that mothers of LLE children were more likely to use dysfunctional parenting practices. Children with LLE were more likely to have a positive family history of late talking, and were less likely to be only children. Non predictors included: family type (e.g., two parent vs sole parent), income, socioeconomic status, family function, day care enrolment or amount of time in day care. At the child level, children with LLE were more likely to be male and younger (by a mean of 10 days’ difference). Neonatal non predictors included: birth weight, low birth weight status, and time to spontaneous respiration; significant predictors were: percentage of optimal birth weight and less than 37 weeks’ gestation. Concurrent predictors at 24 months included: gross and fine motor development, adaptive, personal social scores, psychosocial development, and temperament (i.e., negative mood quality).
Multivariate analyses yielded the following significant predictors, listed in order of odds ratio, from highest to lowest: Personal social levels, gross motor skills, gender, adaptive motor skills, fine motor skills, family history, number of children, proportion of optimal birth weight, prematurity, and age.
Prevalence
Our estimate of 13.4% LLE in the general population falls in the same range as previous estimates that have varied between 10% and 20% (Fenson et al., 1994; Horwitz et al., 2003; Klee et al., 1998; Rescorla, 1989; Rescorla & Achenbach, 2002; Rescorla et al., 1993). In this sample, the prevalence estimate of 13.4% using a composite index of receptive and expressive language was more conservative than 19.1% using the expressive language criterion, ‘combining words’. Our overall estimate of 19.1% of the sample who were not routinely combining words at 24 months was comparable to the 19% estimate for the CDI sample at 25 months (Bates, Dale, & Thal, 1996) and the ALSPAC sample at 25 months (Roulstone, Loader, Northstone, Beveridge, & the ALSPAC team, 2002). With specific regards to family history, 23.0% of those children in families reporting a family history of late talking were found to have LLE as measured in this study, versus 12.0% of those children in families who reported no such history.
Predictors
The comprehensive framework provided by the Bronfenbrenner model established a set of potential predictors unprecedented in the literature for multivariate evaluation of the child’s biogenetic endowment, and proximal (maternal and family) and distal (societal) resources available to the child. The large number of null findings are noteworthy outcomes. Although the literature suggests positive predictor status for maternal education, maternal depression, family SES, and parental occupation, none of these variables predicted LLE, either in bivariate comparisons or in multivariate analyses. Simply put, in this large and diverse sample of children and families, risk for late language emergence at 24 months was not associated with particular strata of parental educational levels, socioeconomic resources, parental mental health, parenting practices or family functioning. Put another way, children with lower levels of proximal and distal resources are as likely as children with higher levels of these resources to be beyond the LLE category at 24 months.
The only environmental risk for LLE that we identified was the presence of siblings. There was a twofold increase in the risk for LLE for children with siblings, relative to only children. While we did not examine birth order effects directly, first born children are temporary ‘only’ children, so our outcome is consistent with studies that report advantages in language development for first born children that are attributed to the quantity and quality of maternal speech (Fenson et al., 1994; Hoff-Ginsberg, 1998). According to the resource dilution model, the addition of even one sibling would halve home resources for language acquisition (Downey, 2001). In this study, the risk conferred by siblings was independent of other home resources. It is possible that the number of children in the family may be a more sensitive proxy measure of home resources for language acquisition in the low performance range than measures such as maternal education and SES.
On the other hand, evidence of possible neurobiological and genetic contribution to LLE was more abundant. The first of these risk indicators were present at birth and related to male gender and suboptimal fetal growth. A disproportionate number of males had delayed language development, a finding that aligns strongly with previous studies. Males were at almost three times the risk for LLE compared to females. In contrast to the strong disadvantage for males in the low performance range, there is only a modest advantage for females across the full range of performance (Fenson et al., 1994; Huttenlocher et al., 1991; Wells, 1985). Children who were less than 85% of their optimum birth weight or born earlier than 37 weeks gestation were at almost twice the risk for LLE. As developed by Blair et al, (2005), the proportion of optimal birth weight is a population-based estimate of fetal growth that is a more differentiated measure of fetal growth than absolute birth weight. Our findings suggest that it is more sensitive to LLE than birth weight that is not associated with risk for language delay (Paul, 1991; Rescorla et al., 1993; Whitehurst et al., 1992) or SLI (Tomblin et al., 1997). At the same time, there was no difference in the physical condition of the children at birth, referenced to the time the children took to breathe independently. Prenatal, perinatal and obstetric risks have not been implicated previously in the etiology of LLE, although empirical data are scarce (Paul, 1991; Rescorla et al., 1993; Whitehurst et al., 1992). The results of this large epidemiological study that had access to medical information collected at the time of delivery showed that some risks for LLE were present at the moment of birth.
Our finding that lower levels of motor, adaptive and personal-social performance were predictors of LLE extends the findings of previous smaller scale studies that compared late talkers and controls and reported lower levels of performance on concurrent measures of general development at 24 months (Carson et al., 1998; Klee et al., 1998; Rescorla & Alley, 2001). The toddlers in previous studies, and the toddlers in our sample, did not have developmental conditions that might account for the group differences reported in previous studies or the significant prediction in our study. Our study cannot address the extent to which lower levels of motor performance or personal-social development, for example, are etiological or phenotypic.
A further complication is that although we treated personal-social skills as a predictor in the analyses, the domain of personal-social skills is difficult to interpret. Measurement confounds are an issue. The item that was most discriminating was a linguistic item, i.e., “Does your child call himself/herself “I’ or “me” more often than his/her own name?” Confounding of language emergence and personal/social skills in this age range is a difficult one to avoid in early assessments, but requires caution in interpretation. Further, children with LLE may find it more difficult to establish social interactions because of their language limitations. It will be difficult to sort out predictive status for this variable.
Yet, in general, our results indicate that children with LLE lag behind children with NLE in multiple dimensions of development and this maturational lag features in either the etiology or the phenotype of LLE. While temperament was not a risk factor for LLE, negative mood quality is biologically regulated and provides additional support for the role of maturational lag in the etiology of late language onset.
As suggested by Feldman, et al (2005), a positive family history of late talking is predictive of LLE. Children with positive histories had double the risk for LLE compared with children in families with no family history, suggesting a genetic liability for LLE. Family aggregation data have been reported for four late talker cohorts (Ellis Weismer et al., 1994; Paul, 1991; Rescorla & Schwartz, 1990; Whitehurst et al., 1991) and all but Whitehurst and colleagues reported an elevated rate of affectedness for family members of late talkers.
Negative mood quality, abnormal child behavior and dysfunctional parenting did not contribute to the risk for late language onset but were more frequent in children with LLE compared with children with NLE. Differences in temperament, behavior and parenting of children with and without early language delay at 24 months have been reported previously (Carson et al., 1998; Carson, Perry, Diefenderfer, & Klee, 1999; Irwin et al., 2002; Paul, Looney, & Dahm, 1991). Plomin and colleagues (2002) reported modest genetic associations between behavior problems and verbal and nonverbal abilities in two-year-old twins. This provides provisional support for the view that common biogenetic mechanisms influence problematic temperament, abnormal behavior and language delay in children. An alternative view is that language delay mediates child temperament and behavior. The conclusion we can draw from our data is that problematic child temperament, abnormal child behavior and dysfunctional parenting are more likely to be part of the psychosocial profile of late talkers than children with normal language development. The caveat is that the results here are inherently ambiguous; the direction of influence is undetermined and the full interpretation is likely to be quite complex.
Overall, the results of this study are congruent with models of language emergence and impairment that posit a strong role for neurobiological and genetic mechanisms of onset that operate across a wide variation in maternal and family characteristics. This study points toward familial history of late language emergence, male gender and early neurobiological growth as concomitant indicators of risk at 24 months.
Directions for future research
The import of LLE can be viewed in terms of recent growth model studies of children with SLI (cf. Hadley & Holt, 2006; Rice, Redmond & Hoffman, 2006; Rice, Wexler & Hershberger, 1998; Rice, Wexler, Marquis, & Hershberger, 2000; Rice, Tomblin, Hoffman, Richman, & Marquis, 2004). A consistent finding is that the affected group differs from unaffected children in the intercept but not the trajectory of change over time, pointing to delayed onset of language as an important part of the phenotype of language impairment (cf. Rice, in press a, b; Rice & Smolik, in press; Rice, Warren & Betz, 2005 for more complete discussion). There is an important empirical gap, however, in fleshing out the connection between LLE and later SLI. LLE status at 24 months is a limited predictor of later language impairment (cf. Dale et al., 2003), because an indeterminate number of children “outgrow” their delayed onset of language. Two kinds of evidence are needed to help sort this out. One is an accurate estimate of the proportion of children with LLE who at later ages are SLI. The second is identification of predictors of subsequent language impairment.
It would be premature to use the predictors evaluated here for screening purposes (i.e., identifying two-year-olds at risk for subsequent SLI), because this use requires measures and data collected at time points beyond the 24 month period of the current panel of data on which this paper is based. Longitudinal data are needed to investigate whether predictors of LLE at 24 months also predict language impairment later on, or whether the early predictors are modulated or supplanted later on by other variables that did not predict LLE. That is, it is conceivable that predictive relationships change over time. For example, maternal or family variables may come into play as a child’s vocabulary accelerates or as clausal structures emerge. It will be important to evaluate the extent to which predictive relationships are the same across all language dimensions. Further, long-term outcome data can help sort out possible differences in predictive relationships between children in the low performance range versus children in the normative range. Evidence of this sort will provide a foundation for the development of comprehensive etiological models of LLE and SLI.
Acknowledgements
The RASCALS study was originally created by Dr Jennifer Kurinczuk who provided details of the original sampling plan. Thanks to Kaye Moore, Deborah Parsons, and Meryl Biggs for help in maintaining the cohort and collection of data. We appreciate statistical consultation by Janet Marquis and Jonathan Templin. This work was made possible by grants from the Health Promotion Foundation of Western Australia and from the National Institutes of Health (RO1DC05226, P30DC005803, P30HD002528). We especially thank the children and families who participated in the RASCALS study.
Footnotes
Duration of gestation may be curtailed or prolonged, and this is usually the result of pathological factors, hence abnormal duration of gestation may be considered to reflect pathological factors. However, since delivery must follow the period of intrauterine growth, duration of gestation is not a determinant of growth and hence cannot be a pathological determinant of growth, though it is the primary determinant of birth weight.
References
- Achenbach TM, Edelbrock CS. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/2-3 and 1992 Profile. University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; Burlington, VT: 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Arnold DS, O’Leary SG, Wolff LS, Acher MM. The Parenting Scale: A measure of dysfunctional parenting in discipline situations. Psychological Assessment. 1993;5:137–144. [Google Scholar]
- Australian Bureau of Statistics . 1996 Census of Population and Housing. Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas (Catalogue no. 2039.0) Australian Bureau of Statistics; Canberra, Australia: 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Bates E, Dale PS, Thal D. Individual differences and their implications for theories of language development. In: Fletcher P, MacWhinney B, editors. The handbook of child language. Blackwell; Cambridge, MA: 1996. pp. 96–151. [Google Scholar]
- Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1969. [Google Scholar]
- Blair E. Why do Aboriginal neonates weigh less? II. Determinants of birth weight for gestation. Journal or Pediatrics and Child Health. 1996;32:498–503. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.1996.tb00962.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair EM, Liu Y, de Klerk NH, Lawrence DM. Optimal fetal growth for the Caucasian singleton and assessment of appropriateness of fetal growth: analysis of a total population perinatal database. BMC Pediatrics. 2005;5(13):1471–2431. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-5-13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bornstein MH, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Maurice Haynes O. First words in the second year: continuity, stability and models of concurrent and predictive correspondence in vocabulary and verbal responsiveness across age and context. Infant Behavior & Development. 1999;22(1):65–85. [Google Scholar]
- Breznitz Z, Sherman T. Speech patterning of natural discourse of well and depressed mothers and their young children. Child Development. 1987;58:395–400. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bricker D, Squires J. Ages and Stages Questionnaires: A parent completed, child-monitoring system. 2 ed Brookes; Baltimore: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Bronfenbrenner U. The Ecology of Human Development. Harvard University Press; Cambridge, MA: 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Carson DK, Klee T, Perry CK, Muskina G, Donaghy T. Comparisons of children with delayed and normal language at 24 months of age on measures of behavioral difficulties, social and cognitive development. Infant Mental Health Journal. 1998;19(1):59–75. [Google Scholar]
- Carson DK, Perry CK, Diefenderfer A, Klee T. Differences in family characteristics and parenting behavior in families with language-delayed and language-normal toddlers. Infant-Toddler Intervention: The Transdisciplinary Journal. 1999;9(3):259–279. [Google Scholar]
- Caulfield MB, Fischel J, DeBaryshe BD, Whitehurst GJ. Behavioral correlates of developmental expressive language disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1989;17:187–201. doi: 10.1007/BF00913793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clayton D, Hills M. Statistical models in epidemiology. Oxford University Press; London: 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Dale PS, Price TS, Bishop DVM, Plomin R. Outcomes of early language delay: I. Predicting persistent and transient delay at 3 and 4 years. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 2003;46:544–560. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2003/044). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dale PS, Simonoff E, Bishop DVM, Eley TC, Oliver B, Price TS, et al. Genetic influence on language delay in two-year-old children. Nature Neuroscience. 1998;1(4):324–328. doi: 10.1038/1142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dollaghan CA, Campbell TF, Paradise JL, Feldman HM, Janosky JE, Pitcairn DN, et al. Maternal education and measures of early speech and language. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research. 1999;42(6):1432–1443. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4206.1432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Downey DB. Number of siblings and intellectual development: The resource dilution explanation. American Psychologist. 2001;56(6/7):497–504. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.56.6-7.497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edwards S, Fletcher P, Garman M, Hughes A, Letts C. Reynell Developmental Language Scales III (RDLS-III) NFER-Nelson; Windsor: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Ellis Weismer S, Murray Branch J, Miller JF. A prospective longitudinal study of language development in late talkers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1994;37:852–867. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3704.852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Entwisle DR, Astone NM. Some practical guidelines for measuring youths’ race/ethnicity and socioeconomic status. Child Development. 1994;65(6):1521–1540. [Google Scholar]
- Epstein NB, Bishop DS, Ryan C, Miller IW, Keitner G. The McMaster model view of healthy family functioning. In: Walsh F, editor. Normal family processes. The Guilford Press; New York: 1993. pp. 138–160. [Google Scholar]
- Feldman HM, Dale PS, Campbell TF, Colborn DK, Kurs-Lasky M, Rockette HE, et al. Concurrent and predictive validity of parent reports of child language at ages 2 and 3 years. Child Development. 2005;76(4):856–868. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00882.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman HM, Dollaghan CA, Campbell TF, Kurs-Lasky M, Janosky JE, Paradise JL. Measurement properties of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories at ages one and two years. Child Development. 2000;71:310–322. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenson L, Dale PS, Reznick S, Thal D, Bates E, Hartung J, et al. MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: Users guide and technical manual. Singular; San Diego: 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Fenson L, S. DP, Reznick JS, Bates E, Thal D, Pethick SJ. Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1994;59(5):1–173. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garton AF, Zubrick SR, Silburn SR. The Western Australian child health survey: A pilot study. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 1995;29:48–57. doi: 10.3109/00048679509075891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gee V. Perinatal statistics in Western Australia: Thirteenth Annual Report of the Western Australian Midwives’ Notification System 1995. Health Department of Western Australia; Perth: 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Hadley PA, Holt JK. Individual differences in the onset of tense marking: A growth-curve analysis. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 2006;49(5):984–1000. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/071). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart B, Risely T. Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Paul H Brookes; Baltimore, MD: 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Hensley VR. Australian normative study of the Achenbach child behaviour checklist. Australian Psychologist. 1988;23:371–382. [Google Scholar]
- Hoff-Ginsberg E. Influences of mother and child on maternal talkativeness. Discourse Processes. 1994;18:105–117. [Google Scholar]
- Hoff-Ginsberg E. The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children’s language experience and language development. Applied Psycholinguistics. 1998;19:603–629. [Google Scholar]
- Horwitz SM, Irwin JR, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Heenan JMB, Mendoza J, Carter AS. Language delay in a community cohort of young children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42(8):932–937. doi: 10.1097/01.CHI.0000046889.27264.5E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hosmer DW, Lemeshow S. Applied logistic regression. Wiley Interscience; New York: 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Hresko WR, Kim Reid D, Hammill DD. Test of Early Language Development (TELD-3) Pro-Ed; Austin, TX: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Huttenlocher J, Haight W, Bryk A, Seltzer M, Lyons T. Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology. 1991;27(2):236–248. [Google Scholar]
- Irwin JR, Carter AS, Briggs-Gowan MJ. The social-emotional development of “late-talking” toddlers. Journal Of The American Academy Of Child And Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002;41(11):1324–1332. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200211000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klee T, Carson D, Gavin W, Hall L, Kent A, Reece S. Concurrent and predictive validity of an early language screening program. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 1998;41:627–641. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4103.627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knobloch H, Stevens F, Malone AF. Manual of developmental diagnosis: The administration and interpretation of the Revised Gesell and Armatruda Developmental and Neurological Examination. Harper & Row; New York: 1980. [Google Scholar]
- Kurinczuk JJ, Parsons DE, Dawes V, Burton PR. The relationship between asthma and smoking during pregnancy. Women and Health. 1999;29:31–47. doi: 10.1300/J013v29n03_03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaBenz PJ, LaBenz ES. Early correlates of speech, language and hearing. PSG Publishing Company; Littleton, MA: 1980. [Google Scholar]
- Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH. The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behavior Research and Therapy. 1995a;33:335–343. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lovibond SH, Lovibond PF. Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales. 2nd ed Psychology Foundation Monograph; Sydney, Australia: 1995b. [Google Scholar]
- Luinge MR, Post WJ, Wit HP, Goorhuis-Brouwer SM. The ordering of milestones in language development for children from 1 to 6 years of age. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 2006;49(5):923–940. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/067). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lytton H, Conway D, Sauve R. The impact of twinship on parent-child interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1977;35(2):97–107. [Google Scholar]
- Marschik PB, Einspieler C, Garzarolli B, Prechtl HFR. Events at early development: Are they associated with early word production and neurodevelopmental abilities at the preschool age? Early Human Development. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2006.05.009. (in press) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCardle P, Cooper J, Freund L. Language and genetics: Needs and opportunities. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2005;26:129–135. [Google Scholar]
- Miller IW, Epstein NB, Bishop DS, Keitner GI. The McMaster family assessment device: Reliability and validity. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy. 1985;11:345–356. [Google Scholar]
- Olswang LB, Rodriguez B, Timler G. Recommending intervention for toddlers with specific language learning difficulites: We may not have all the answers, but we know a lot. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 1998;7(1):23–32. [Google Scholar]
- Pan BA, Rowe ML, Singer JD, Snow CE. Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development. 2005;76(4):763–782. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00876.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paul R. Profiles of toddlers with slow expressive language development. Topics in Language Disorders. 1991;11(4):1–13. [Google Scholar]
- Paul R. Clinical implications of the natural history of slow expressive language development. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 1996;5:5–21. [Google Scholar]
- Paul R. Predicting outcomes of early expressive delay: Ethical implications. In: Bishop DVM, Leonard LB, editors. Speech and language impairments in children: Causes, characteristics, intervention and outcome. Psychology Press; Hove, East Sussex: 2000. pp. 195–209. [Google Scholar]
- Paul R, Elwood T. Maternal linguistic input to toddlers with slow expressive language development. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1991;34:982–988. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3405.982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paul R, James DF. Language delay and parental perceptions. Journal of the American Acadamy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1990;29(4):669–670. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199007000-00030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paul R, Looney SS, Dahm PS. Communication and socialization skills at ages 2 and 3 in “late-talking” young children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1991;34:858–865. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3404.858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plomin R, Price TS, Eley TC, Dale PS, Stevenson J. Associations between behaviour problems and verbal and nonverbal cognitive abilities and disabilities in early childhood. Journal Of Child Psychology And Psychiatry, And Allied Disciplines. 2002;43(5):619–633. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla L. The Language Development Survey: A screening tool for delayed language in toddlers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1989;54:587–599. doi: 10.1044/jshd.5404.587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla L. Language and reading outcomes to age 9 in late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2002;45(2):360–371. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/028). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla L, Achenbach TM. Use of the Language Development Survey (LDS) in a national probability sample of children 18 to 35 months old. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 2002;45(4):733–743. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/059). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla L, Alley A. Validation of the language development survey (LDS): a parent report tool for identifying language delay in toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 2001;44:434–445. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/035). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla L, Hadick-Wiley M, Escarce E. Epidemiological investigation of expressive language delay at age two. First Language. 1993;13:5–22. [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla L, Schwartz E. Outcome of toddlers with specific expressive language delay. Applied Psycholinguistics. 1990;11:393–407. [Google Scholar]
- Rice ML. How different is disordered language? In: Colombo J, McCardle P, Freund L, editors. Infant Pathways to Language: Methods, Models, and Research Directions. Lawrence Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: (in press) [Google Scholar]
- Rice ML, Smolik F. Gaskell G, editor. Genetics of language disorders: Clinical conditions, phenotypes, and genes. Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics. (in press) [Google Scholar]
- Rice ML. Children with Specific Language Impairment: Bridging the Genetic and Developmental Perspectives. In: Hoff E, Shatz M, editors. Handbook of Language Development. Blackwell Publishers; (in press) [Google Scholar]
- Rice ML, Redmond SM, Hoffman L. Mean length of utterance in children with specific language impairment and in younger control children shows concurrent validity and stable and parallel growth trajectories. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2006;49:793–808. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/056). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice ML, Tomblin JB, Hoffman L, Richman WA, Marquis J. Grammatical tense deficits in children with SLI and non-specific language impairment: Relationships with nonverbal IQ over time. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2004;47:816–834. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/061). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice ML, Warren SF, Betz SK. Language symptoms of developmental language disorders: An overview of autism, Down syndrome, fragile X, specific language impairment, and Williams syndrome. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2005;26:7–28. [Google Scholar]
- Rice ML, Wexler K, Hershberger S. Tense over time: The longitudinal course of tense acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1998;41:1412–1431. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4106.1412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roulstone S, Loader S, Northstone K, Beveridge M, the ALSPAC team The speech and language of children aged 25 months: Descriptive data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Early Childhood Development and Care. 2002;172(3):259–268. [Google Scholar]
- Rubin DB. Inference and Missing Data. Biometrika. 1976;63:581–592. [Google Scholar]
- Rubin DB. Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. John Wiley & Sons; New York: 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M, Thorpe K, Greenwood R, Northstone K, Golding J. Twins as a natural experiment to study the causes of mild language delay: I: Design; twin-singleton differences in language, and obstetric risks. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry. 2003;44(3):326–341. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samejima F. Estimation of latent ability using a response pattern of graded scores. Psychometrika Monograph Supplement. 1969;34(4):100–114. [Google Scholar]
- SAS Institute Inc . SAS/STAT 9.1 user’s guide. SAS Institute Inc; Cary, NC: 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Sawyer MG, Arney FM, Baghurst PA, Clark JJ, Graetz BW, Kosky RJ, et al. Child and Adolescent Component of the National Survey of Mental Health and Well Being: The Mental Health of Young People in Australia. Mental Health and Special Programs Branch, Commonwealth Department of Health and Aged Care; Canberra: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Sawyer MG, Arney FM, Baghurst PA, Clarke JJ, Graetz BW, Kosky RJ, et al. The mental health of young people in Australia: key findings from the child and adolescent component of the national survey of mental health and well-being. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;35:806–814. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1614.2001.00964.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sawyer MG, Clark JJ, Baghurst PA. Childhood emotional and behavioural problems: A comparison of children’s reports with reports from parents and teachers. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health. 1993;29:119–125. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.1993.tb00463.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sawyer MG, Kosky RJ, Graetz BW, Arney F, Zubrick SR, Baghurst P. The National Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing: the child and adolescent component. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;34:214–220. doi: 10.1080/j.1440-1614.2000.00729.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sawyer MG, Sarris A, Baghurst PA, Cornish CA, Kalucy RS. The prevalence of emotional and behavioural disorders and patterns of service utilisation in children and adolescents. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 1990;24:323–330. doi: 10.3109/00048679009077699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silburn SR, Zubrick SR, Garton A, Gurrin L, Burton P, Dalby R, et al. Western Australian Child Health Survey: Family and community health. Australian Bureau of Statistics and the TVW Telethon Institute for Child Health Research; Perth, Western Australia: 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Squires J, Bricker D. Infant/Child Monitoring Questionnaires Procedures Manual. Revised ed University of Oregon; Eugene, OR: 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Squires J, Bricker D, Potter L. Revision of a parent-completed developmental screening tool: Ages and Stages Questionnaires. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 1997;22(3):313–328. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/22.3.313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squires J, Potter L, Bricker D. The ASQ user’s guide. Second Edition Paul H Brookes; Baltimore, MD: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Stanley FJ, Read AW, Kurinczuk JJ, Croft ML, Bower C. A population maternal and child health research database for research and policy evaluation in Western Australia. Seminars in Neonatology. 1997;2:195–201. [Google Scholar]
- Stout W. A nonparametric approach for assessing latent trait unidimensionality. Psychometrika. 1987;52:589–617. [Google Scholar]
- Tager-Flusberg H, Cooper J. Present and future possibilities for defining a phenotype for specific language impairment. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research. 1999;42(5):1275–1278. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4205.1275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thal DJ, Bates E, Goodman J, Jahn-Samilo J. Continuity of language abilities: an exploratory study of late- and early-talking toddlers. Developmental Neuropsychology. 1997;13(3):239–273. [Google Scholar]
- Thal DJ, Tobias S, Morrison D. Language and gesture in late talkers: A one year follow-up. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1991;34:604–612. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3403.604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thorndike RL, Hagen EP, Sattler JM. Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale. 4 ed DLM Teaching Resources; Riverside, CA: 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Tomblin JB. Genetic and environmental contributions to the risk for specific language impairment. In: Rice ML, editor. Towards a genetics of language. Lawrence Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 1996. pp. 191–210. [Google Scholar]
- Tomblin JB, Smith E, Zhang X. Epidemiology of specific language impairment: Prenatal and perinatal risk factors. Journal of Communication Disorders. 1997;30:325–344. doi: 10.1016/s0021-9924(97)00015-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verhulst FC, Achenbach TM, van der Ende J, Erol N, Lambert MC, Leung PWL, et al. Comparisons of problems reported by youths from seven countries. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160(8):1479–1485. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.8.1479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wells G. Language development in the pre-school years. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK: 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Whitehurst GJ, Arnold DS, Smith M, Fischel J, Lonigan CJ, Valdez-Menchaca MC. Family history in developmental expressive language delay. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1991;34:1150–1157. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3405.1150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitehurst GJ, Fischel J. Early developmental language delay: What, if anything should the clinician do about it? Journal Of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1994;35:613–648. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01210.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitehurst GJ, Fischel J, Arnold DS, Lonigan CJ. Evaluating outcomes with children with expressive language delay. In: Warren SF, Reichle J, editors. Causes and effects in communication and language intervention. Vol. 1. Brookes Publishing; Baltimore, MD: 1992. pp. 227–313. [Google Scholar]
- Whitehurst GJ, Fischel JE, Lonigan CJ, Valdez-Menchaca MC, DeBaryshe BD, Caulfield MB. Verbal interaction in families of normal and expressive-language-delayed children. Developmental Psychology. 1988;24(5):690–699. [Google Scholar]
- Windle M. Temperament and social support in adolescence: interrelations with depressive symptoms and delinquent behavior. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1992;21(1):1–21. doi: 10.1007/BF01536980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Windle M, Lerner RM. Reassessing the dimensions of temperamental individuality across the lifespan: the Revised Dimensions of Temperament Survey (DOTS-R) Journal of Adolescent Research. 1986;1(2):213–230. [Google Scholar]
- Zubrick SR, Kurinczuk JJ, McDermott BM, McKelvey RS, Silburn SR, Davies LC. Fetal growth and subsequent mental health problems in children aged 4 to 13 years. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2000;42:14–20. doi: 10.1017/s0012162200000049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zubrick SR, Silburn SR, Garton A, Burton P, Dalby R, Carlton J, et al. Western Australian Child Health Survey: Developing health and well-being in the nineties. Australian Bureau of Statistics and the Institute for Child Health Research; Perth, Western Australia: 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Zubrick SR, Silburn SR, Gurrin L, Teoh HJ, Shepard C, Carlton J, et al. Western Australian Child Health Survey: Education, Health and Competence. Australian Bureau of Statistics and the TVW Telethon Institute for Child Health Research; Perth, Western Australia: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Zubrick SR, Ward KA, Silburn SR, Lawrence DM, Williams AA, Blair E, et al. Prevention of child behaviour problems via universal implementation of a group behavioural family intervention. Prevention Science. 2005;6(4):287–304. doi: 10.1007/s11121-005-0013-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]