Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Dec 18.
Published in final edited form as: J Res Read. 2011 Feb 15;34(1):77–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01488.x

Predicting bilingual Spanish–English children’s phonological awareness abilities from their preschool English and Spanish oral language

Shelley E Scarpino 1, Frank R Lawrence 2, Megan D Davison 3, Carol S Hammer 4
PMCID: PMC3524584  NIHMSID: NIHMS417324  PMID: 23258945

Abstract

This longitudinal study investigated the relationship between oral language abilities and phonological awareness in 85 typically developing, Spanish–English preschool children (average age in preschool was 3 years, 9 months). Receptive language skills in Spanish and English were assessed in the autumn and spring during the children’s 2 years in Head Start for a total of four measurement occasions. Phonological awareness was assessed during the spring of children’s kindergarten year. Results indicated that English receptive vocabulary at the end of preschool predicted English phonological awareness abilities in kindergarten, whereas Spanish vocabulary was observed to have a negative predictive relationship with children’s English phonological awareness abilities. However, after controlling for English vocabulary, Spanish vocabulary no longer had an effect on English phonological awareness. Broad receptive language abilities in English and Spanish did not predict later English phonological awareness skills.


In 2008, Latino children constituted 22% of the school-age population in the United States, with 68% speaking a language other than English at home (Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics, 2010). Children from homes in which English is not the primary language and children of low socioeconomic status have been shown to be at risk of poor reading outcomes (Snow, Burns & Griffin, 1998). Approximately 29% of Latino children in the United States live in poverty, placing them at risk of reading difficulties (Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics, 2010). Because Latino students constitute the fastest growing population in the United States (Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics, 2010), there is a critical need to understand factors that impact their reading outcomes. In particular, a better understanding of the relationship between oral language and emergent literacy skills is necessary.

Phonological awareness, which refers to the ability to reflect upon and manipulate the sound structure of language, is one of the most widely researched emergent literacy skills. Phonological awareness has been found to predict future reading abilities in monolingual English-speaking children (Anthony, Williams, McDonald & Francis, 2007; Goswami, 2001; Lonigan, Burgess, Anthony & Barker, 1998; Wagner, Torgesen & Rashotte, 1994) as well as bilingual children (Chiappe, Siegel & Gottardo, 2002; Durgunoğlu, Nagy & Hancin-Bhatt, 1993; Gottardo, 2002; Jimenez-Gonzalez, 1997). Furthermore, deficits in phonological awareness have been linked to delays in word-level reading abilities in monolingual and bilingual children (DaFontoura & Siegel, 1995; Stanovich, 1994).

Children often do not demonstrate the phonological awareness skills that are most predictive of later reading abilities until they are 4 or 5 years old. This implies that children with phonological awareness deficits may not be identified until they begin to receive early literacy instruction, leaving little time for remediation before the expectation of word-level reading. Therefore, it is important to investigate predictors of phonological awareness to facilitate earlier identification of children at risk of reading difficulties, including bilingual children from low socioeconomic backgrounds.

Contributions of oral language to phonological awareness

The role of vocabulary in the development of phonological awareness has received much attention. Theories of lexical development have implied that the underlying phonological representations of words become more segmented as the result of an expanding lexicon. Metsala and Walley (1998) proposed a lexical restructuring model to explain how vocabulary development impacts spoken word recognition. According to this model, words are first represented in the lexicon holistically, but then, as a result of vocabulary growth, the representations are restructured to represent phonological segments of words such as syllables, rimes, onsets and finally individual phonemes. According to this view, the phoneme emerges first as an implicit unit required for processing speech and then develops later as an explicit unit that can be used for reading an alphabetic orthography (Metsala & Walley, 1998). Evidence supporting the lexical restructuring model suggests a positive relationship between vocabulary size and phonological awareness (Rvachew & Grawburg, 2006; Scarborough, 1990; Wagner et al., 1994). For example, Metsala (1999) found significant correlations between phonological awareness and receptive vocabulary in monolingual English-speaking children aged 3–4 years. Similarly, Rvachew and Grawburg (2006) observed positive relationships between receptive vocabulary and phonological awareness in 47 children with speech sound disorders before and at the end of kindergarten. Furthermore, receptive vocabulary before kindergarten explained approximately 10% of the variance in phonological awareness at the end of kindergarten.

The relationship between vocabulary and phonological awareness in bilingual children has not been widely examined. Dickinson, McCabe, Anastasopoulos, Peisner-Feinberg and Poe (2003) investigated the relationship between English language, literacy and print knowledge in 533 4-year-old Spanish–English children attending Head Start. Results were similar to those of Rvachew and Grawburg (2006) and Metsala (1999) in that receptive vocabulary was moderately correlated with phonological awareness. These findings provide support for the relationship between phonological awareness and vocabulary for bilingual as well as monolingual children.

Studies of monolingual English children have demonstrated that phonological awareness is supported by other oral language abilities in addition to vocabulary (Burgess & Lonigan, 1998; Cheney, 1992; Lonigan et al., 1998; Scarborough, 2001; Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998, 2001). These findings do not contradict the proposed lexical restructuring model. Rather, they suggest that vocabulary is learned in the context of rich oral language environments, and therefore, an increase in vocabulary is likely to occur in tandem with growth in other facets of receptive and expressive language as well (i.e. phonology, morphology and syntax). Therefore, it is reasonable to predict that there will be a relationship between oral comprehension abilities and phonological awareness.

The majority of studies investigating the relationship between language skills and phonological awareness have been conducted on monolingual English speakers. It is necessary to investigate whether this relationship is evident in bilingual children. Also, because bilingual children are developing two language systems, it is important to understand the relationship between language abilities in each of the children’s languages and their later phonological awareness abilities in English.

Transfer of phonological awareness in bilingual children

To better understand bilingual children’s development, investigators have examined whether their phonological awareness abilities in one language transfer to the other and whether oral language development in one language impacts phonological awareness in the other. For example, Durgunoğlu et al. (1993) found a relationship between phonological awareness in Spanish and word reading in English. Subsequent studies have also found a relationship between Spanish phonological awareness skills and English word reading abilities (Branum-Martin et al., 2006; Gottardo, 2002; Lindsey, Manis & Bailey, 2003; Quiroga, Lemos-Britton, Mostafapour, Abbott & Berninger, 2002). These findings support the notion that the development of phonological awareness in bilingual children is not specific to one language. Rather, the metalinguistic knowledge necessary for identifying the individual phonological components of spoken words and mapping them to letters supports both Spanish and English word recognition (Durgunoğlu et al., 1993). Once children acquire the metalinguistic knowledge to attend to increasingly smaller segments of speech, that knowledge can be applied when learning to read in a second language. This view of transfer is similar to that of Cummins (1984) who posited that cognitive and academic skills share a common underlying proficiency that supports both languages of a bilingual child.

These findings suggest that children who begin school with limited English proficiency may not necessarily fall behind in English decoding abilities provided they have developed the prerequisite phonological awareness skills in Spanish. However, not all bilingual children begin school with adequate phonological awareness skills. Therefore, studies investigating the contributions of each of a bilingual child’s languages to English phonological awareness are particularly important for identifying those who may be at risk of poor literacy outcomes.

The few studies that investigated the relationship between early oral language and phonological awareness skills in bilingual Spanish–English-speaking children have had disparate results. For example, Durgunoğlu et al. (1993) studied the language and phonological awareness skills of 27 Spanish-speaking children with limited English proficiency (mean age 7 years, 1 month). Spanish and English language skills were assessed using the prelanguage assessment scales (Duncan & De Avila, 1986) in Spanish and English. These tests consist of six subtests that assess listening comprehension, expressive vocabulary, repeating and completing phrases and story retell. Spanish phonological awareness was assessed using segmenting, blending and matching tasks. No significant correlations were found between oral language measures in either language and Spanish phonological awareness.

SanFrancisco, Carlo, August and Snow (2006) examined the roles of language of instruction and vocabulary on English phonological awareness in 102 low-income kindergarten and first-grade students. All participants received instruction in letter sounds and reading at the syllable and word levels. Forty-five of the bilingual participants received literacy instruction in Spanish, 35 received literacy instruction in English and 22 participants were monolingual English speakers receiving instruction in English. Expressive vocabulary in Spanish and English (in English only for the monolingual participants) was assessed along with English phonological awareness. The English phonological awareness task required participants to segment diphthongs. Both English and Spanish vocabulary scores predicted English phonological awareness. However, there was an interaction effect, with Spanish vocabulary not having as great an impact on English phonological awareness in children who received literacy instruction in English.

Similarly, Gottardo and Mueller (2009) found moderate correlations between English receptive vocabulary ability and English PA awareness skills of phoneme detection, phoneme deletion and sound blending in their first- and second-grade participants. English receptive vocabulary was moderately correlated with Spanish rhyming and phoneme detection tasks of phonological awareness indicating a transfer of skills.

These findings stand in contrast to those of Durgunoğlu et al. (1993) who found no relationship between oral language and phonological awareness. It is possible that differences in oral language predictive measures accounted for some of the disparity in results across the studies. Durgonoğlu and colleagues used a broad measure of language ability of which vocabulary constituted only 15% of the total score, whereas SanFrancisco et al. (2006) utilised expressive vocabulary measures to predict phonological awareness scores. It may be that broad measures of language are not as predictive of phonological awareness as vocabulary measures alone, lending further support to the lexical restructuring model (Metsala & Walley, 1998). Outcome measures also differed among the studies. Durgonoğlu and colleagues used Spanish phonological awareness as their outcome measure, whereas SanFrancisco et al. (2006) used English phonological awareness, and Gottardo and Mueller (2009) used both Spanish and English. Therefore, it is difficult to draw direct comparisons among these studies, because predictors and outcome measures were not similar.

It is also important to note that the previous studies examined the relationship between oral language skills and phonological awareness in school-aged children. SanFrancisco and colleagues suggested that future studies investigate the role of vocabulary in bilingual children before they receive any reading instruction, given that language of instruction interacted with vocabulary abilities in predicting phonological awareness outcomes in their study.

Several studies have, in fact, investigated the relationship between oral language abilities and phonological awareness in bilingual children. Similar to the studies of school-age children, results of the studies involving preschoolers did not indicate a consistent relationship between oral language and phonological awareness. López and Greenfield (2004) assessed the relationship between oral language and phonological awareness in a group of 4–5-year-old Spanish–English bilingual children. Phonological awareness was measured using an investigator-developed phonological sensitivity test that consisted of rhyme matching, alliteration matching and sentence segmentation tasks. Although López and Greenfield used the same, but later version, of the broad language measure used in the Durgunoğlu et al. (1993) study, findings were contrary to those of Durgonoğlu and colleagues. López and Greenfield found moderate correlations between oral language and phonological awareness measures within each language. Additionally, using stepwise regressions, it was found that English oral language accounted for 27% of the variance in English phonological awareness. Spanish oral language skills accounted for an additional 3% of the variance in English phonological awareness scores, indicating cross-linguistic support for phonological awareness. The fact that these studies differed primarily in the age of participants lends further support to the notion that oral language skills are most predictive of phonological awareness before formal literacy instruction. However, because López and Greenfield utilised a broad language measure in their study, it is not possible to examine the independent contribution of vocabulary as opposed to broad language measures in predicting phonological awareness.

In another study of preschool children, Dickinson, McCabe, Clark-Chiarelli and Wolf (2004) studied 123 4-year-old bilingual children enrolled in Head Start. They sought to determine the effects of emergent literacy, receptive vocabulary and prior levels of phonological awareness in the opposing language on English and Spanish phonological awareness. Phonological awareness tasks consisted of phoneme deletion detection and rhyme recognition. Language dominance, as reported by teachers and parents, was also a factor in analyses. The best predictors of English phonological awareness were English vocabulary and Spanish phonological awareness, whereas the best predictors of Spanish phonological awareness were Spanish vocabulary and English phonological awareness. The effect of Spanish receptive vocabulary, however, was greater for Spanish dominant speakers. Cross-linguistic vocabulary effects on phonological awareness were not significant. This study suggests that receptive vocabulary supports phonological awareness in the same language, but not in the other. These results stand in contrast to López and Greenfield (2004) who found a cross-linguistic effect for oral language predicting phonological awareness.

The combined results of these studies indicate that further research is needed to examine the relationship between oral language abilities developed during the preschool years and later phonological awareness in Spanish–English bilingual children. Of particular importance is an understanding of how oral language skills in each language may impact phonological awareness in English, because most bilingual children in the United States receive literacy instruction in English. Therefore, it is of particular interest to examine the predictive relationship between their oral language abilities in each language at the end of their Head Start experience and English phonological awareness at the end of kindergarten, before the commencement of formal literacy instruction. Also, given that it is unclear whether overall receptive language or receptive vocabulary abilities in particular are the best predictors of phonological awareness, we propose studying the effect of receptive vocabulary and overall receptive language abilities in each language at the end of Head Start and their independent contributions to English phonological awareness.

Purpose of the study

The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between receptive language skills of bilingual children during preschool and their English phonological awareness abilities in kindergarten. The following questions were posed:

  1. Do bilingual children’s English and Spanish receptive vocabulary and overall receptive language abilities during Head Start predict English phonological awareness at the end of kindergarten?

  2. To what extent do receptive language abilities in Spanish and English uniquely contribute to English phonological awareness?

Based on the lexical restructuring model (Metsala & Walley, 1998), we hypothesised that English receptive vocabulary abilities in Head Start would predict English phonological awareness abilities in kindergarten. We also hypothesised that overall receptive language abilities during Head Start would predict English phonological awareness. We hypothesised this relationship between overall receptive language and phonological awareness because vocabulary develops within the context of other language domains and not in isolation. We hypothesised similar patterns would be observed between Spanish receptive vocabulary and overall receptive language abilities and English phonological awareness. This hypothesis stemmed from the notion that Spanish language skills support development of phonological awareness as a metalinguistic skill that is available for use in either language.

Method

Participants

The participants included 85 preschool-aged bilingual children who were part of a longitudinal study of the language and English literacy development of bilingual children attending Head Start centres in urban communities of Central Pennsylvania. The children were of Puerto Rican descent and lived in families that financially qualified for 2 years of Head Start services indicating that they were from low socioeconomic backgrounds. Children were typically developing as determined by (a) no parental or teacher concerns about their development, (b) no history of developmental, neurological or physiological deficits by parent and teacher report and (c) passage of the Denver Developmental Screening Test-II (Frankenburg, Dodds, Shapiro & Bresnick, 1992). All children passed a hearing screening. The average age of the children at the initial time of data collection was 3 years, 9 months (SD = 4 months).

All children were reported by their mothers as speaking Spanish at the beginning of the study. Additional familial background information is displayed in Table 1. Children attended English Immersion Head Start classrooms in which English was the primary language of instruction. Development of children’s language and literacy abilities in English was the goal of their educational programmes. Informal observations indicated that Spanish was spoken infrequently to children in the classrooms. In addition, English was the primary language of instruction when children entered kindergarten.

Table 1.

Child and family background descriptive characteristics.

N children in Head Start 85
Male 36
Female 49
N children in kindergarten 48
Male 20
Female 28
Mean (SD) in years
Children’s age
HS autumn year 1 3.6 (.33)
HS spring year 1 4.0 (.32)
HS autumn year 2 4.5 (.32)
HS spring year 2 5.1 (.35)
Spring kindergarten 6.1 (.34)
Mothers’ age 26.5 (4.1)
Mothers’ education 11.7 (2.2)

Procedures

Children’s Spanish and English language development was assessed in the autumn and spring of their first and second years in Head Start for a total of four measurement occasions. Children’s English phonological awareness was assessed at the end of kindergarten. All tests were administered individually by trained bilingual data collectors. All data collectors were fluent in English and Spanish and lived in the children’s communities.

Measures

Peabody picture vocabulary test-III (PPVT-III)

The PPVT-III (Dunn & Dunn, 1997) was used to assess the children’s receptive vocabulary in English. The PPVT-III was designed for use with individuals from 2½ to 90 years of age. The test consists of 204 items with each item worth one point. During the administration, children were instructed to point to the picture, from a choice of four, which corresponded to the targeted word. The median internal reliability coefficient for the PPVT-III is .95.

Test of early language development-3 (TELD-3)

The auditory comprehension subtest of the TELD-3 (Hresko, Reid & Hammill, 1999) was used to assess English oral comprehension. The TELD-3 was designed for use with children between 2 years and 7 years, 11 months. This auditory comprehension subtest consists of 35 items that assess children’s comprehension of vocabulary, concepts (qualitative, quantitative, spatial and time), morphology and syntax and inferencing. Each of the 35 items was worth one point. Items required the child to point to pictures, provide a grammaticality judgement response or provide a verbal response. The median internal reliability coefficient for the TELD-3 is .91.

Test de vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody (TVIP)

The TVIP (Dunn, Lugo, Padilla & Dunn, 1986) was used to document the children’s receptive vocabulary in Spanish. The test, which consists of 125 items worth one point each, was developed for use with children who range in age from 2 years, 6 months to 17 years, 11 months. Similar to the PPVT-III, children were asked to point to the picture that was named when given a choice of four pictures. The median internal reliability coefficient is .93.

Preschool language scale-3 (PLS-3) Spanish edition

The receptive language subtest of the Spanish version of the PLS-3 (Zimmerman, Steiner & Pond, 1992) was used to assess children’s oral comprehension in Spanish. The PLS-3 is designed for use with children who range in age from birth to 6 years, 11 months. This receptive language subtest includes 83 items that assess children’s comprehension of basic vocabulary, quantitative qualitative and spatial concepts, morphological and syntactic structures, inferencing and categorising. Items required the child to point to pictures or manipulate objects. The median internal consistency reliability coefficient is .86.

Comprehensive test of phonological processing (CTOPP)

English phonological awareness abilities were tested at the end of kindergarten using the phonological awareness composite score of the CTOPP (Wagner, Torgesen & Rashotte, 1999). The phonological awareness composite score for 5- and 6-year-olds comprises the following subtests: elision, blending words and sound matching. For the elision subtest, children were required to segment spoken words into smaller units (say the word toothbrush without tooth). The blending words subtest required children to combine individually presented segments into whole words. Additionally, the sound matching subtest had children match a pictured word to another picture based on the same initial or final sound of the word. For the elision, sound matching and blending words subtests phonological awareness composite of the CTOPP, the reliability coefficients are .88, .83 and .88, respectively. The reliability coefficient for the phonological awareness composite score is .79. Spanish phonological awareness was not assessed because a standardised test of Spanish phonological awareness did not exist at the time of the study.

Data analysis

A distal outcome growth model, displayed in Figure 1, was used to study the relationship between bilingual children’s language development during preschool and kindergarten phonological awareness in a two-stage developmental modelling process (cf. Hancock & Lawrence, 2006; Lawrence & Hancock, 1998; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). Stage 1 consisted of modelling bilingual children’s language development during Head Start using latent variable growth curves (Hammer, Lawrence & Miccio, 2007). Time was centred (Singer & Willet, 2003) at the last measurement occasion in Head Start; hence, the intercept estimated the children’s true scores at the end of Head Start. The growth models were constructed using the raw scores of each of the four language measures (TELD-3, PPVT-III, PLS-3 and TVIP). Linear growth models may have parameters representing two fixed effects and up to three random effects. The fixed effects are estimates for the initial expected value (intercept), where time is zero, and a linear rate of change (slope). Two of the three random effects are the between-subject variation in both the intercept and slope. The third random effect is the covariance between the intercept and slope. All five growth parameters may be tested to determine if they are significantly different from zero. With regard to random effects, failure to reject the null hypothesis (α = .1) indicates that the parameter should be fixed to zero. Similarly, fixed effects may be tested to determine if there is sufficient evidence (α = .05) that they are not zero in the population. The alpha levels between fixed and random effects differ due to the fact that fixed effects are tested with a nondirectional hypothesis and thus, the area for rejection is divided equally between the positive and negative tails. Because variance is always positive for random effects, an adjustment must be made to the Type I error rate in a mixed model in order to maintain the same tolerance for Type I error across fixed and random effects (cf. Snijders & Bosker, 1999, p. 90).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Growth model.

For this study, significance was determined by computing confidence intervals around parameter estimates instead of using p values. All confidence intervals reported are 95% confidence intervals (α = .05). An estimate was considered significant if the corresponding confidence interval did not contain zero, which indicated that the true value was unlikely to support the null hypothesis.

Stage 2 incorporated significant random growth parameters estimated from stage 1 into a regression model designed to predict English phonological awareness at the end of kindergarten. The models were constructed using the phonological awareness composite measure as the criterion variable and each growth model’s significant growth parameters as predictor variables. Stage 2 modelling was conducted within the context of Bayesian modelling (e.g. Bolstad, 2004; Congdon, 2003; Gelman, Carlin, Stern & Rubin, 2004; Gilks, Richardson & Spiegelhalter, 1996; Gill, 2002) with uninformative priors and relying on Markov Chain Monte Carlo techniques to derive parameter estimates. The confidence region is calculated in such a way that if a set of measurements were repeated multiple times and a confidence region calculated in the same way on each set of measurements, then a certain percentage of the time, on average (e.g. 95%), the confidence region would include the point representing the ‘true’ values of the set of variables being estimated. However, it does not mean, when one confidence region has been calculated, that there is a 95% probability that the ‘true’ values fall within theregion, because a particular probability distribution of the ‘true’ values is not assumed and there may or may not be other information about where they are likely to fall. All models were estimated using the R-software 2.11.1 (R Core Development Team, 2010).

Results

Table 2 displays the descriptive statistics for the English and Spanish language raw scores and the English phonological awareness composite standard score. In general, children’s raw scores increased in each of the language measures across the 2 years in Head Start. With regard to English phonological awareness abilities, children were performing within one standard deviation of the levels expected for monolingual English-speaking children of the same age.

Table 2.

Descriptive statistics for English and Spanish receptive language raw scores and phonological awareness standard score.

Mean (SD)
PPVT-III
HS autumn year 1 19.56 (12.90)
HS spring year 1 24.34 (15.53)
HS autumn year 2 34.53 (15.13)
HS spring year 2 47.03 (15.20)
Receptive language subtest of TELD-3
HS autumn year 1 12.35 (5.43)
HS spring year 1 15.62 (6.87)
HS autumn year 2 19.94 (5.96)
HS spring year 2 22.75 (5.30)
TVIP
HS autumn year 1 4.91(5.15)
HS spring year 1 6.82 (7.41)
HS autumn year 2 10.06 (9.89)
HS spring year 2 11.31 (11.97)
Auditory language subtest of PLS-3
HS autumn year 1 23.12 (7.34)
HS spring year 1 26.77 (9.58)
HS autumn year 2 32.29 (9.24)
HS spring year 2 32.91 (8.67)
Phonological awareness composite standard score of CTOPP
Kindergarten spring 98.68 (11.15)

Note: PPVT-III = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III; TELD-3 = Test of Early Language Development-3; TVIP = Test de vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody; PLS-3 = Preschool Language Scale-3; CTOPP = Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing.

Relationships between children’s language abilities and English phonological awareness

In order to determine the relationship between children’s language abilities and English phonological awareness, all growth models of English and Spanish receptive vocabulary and overall receptive language were set as parallel models. Growth models initially considered random slopes and random intercepts; however, variation in the slopes of each language measure was not significant. Therefore, only random intercepts were used in stage 2 modelling. Table 3 displays the significant growth model parameter estimates from stage 1 modelling.

Table 3.

Growth model parameter estimates from stage 1 modelling.

Parameter estimate Standard error t
PPVT-III
Intercept 45.78 1.58 28.90
Time 9.24 0.44 21.60
Receptive language subtest of TELD-3
Intercept 22.90 0.64 35.80
Time 3.52 0.21 17.10
TVIP
Intercept 11.23 0.94 12.00
Time 2.12 0.30 7.19
Auditory language subtest of PLS-3
Intercept 34.14 0.93 36.70
Time 3.51 0.36 9.80

Note: PPVT-III = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III; TELD-3 = Test of Early Language Development-3; TVIP = Test de vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody; PLS-3 = Preschool Language Scale-3.

It is important to note that there was approximately 43% missing data at the last measurement occasion involving the English phonological awareness composite score in the spring of children’s kindergarten year. Therefore, full-information maximum likelihood was used to address missing data in the analyses. Missing data is a common phenomenon in longitudinal studies. Past research has shown that inappropriate treatment of missing data may lead to bias in parameter estimates, standard errors and test statistics (Jones, 1993) unless it can be assumed that the missing data were missing at random. The Bayesian approach compensates for missing data by assuming that missing data are missing at random which implies that the missing data may be associated with known characteristics or variables of the study but it is in no way dependent on the value of the criterion variable.

Bayesian parameter estimates from stage 2 modelling are displayed in Table 4 where intercept represents the fixed effect intercept, estimated intercept represents the stage 1 regression path and residual variance represents random error. The 95% confidence regions computed for the estimates indicated that the coefficients for English and Spanish receptive vocabulary, as measured by the PPVT-III and TVIP, significantly predicted English phonological awareness abilities in kindergarten. In addition, it was determined that children’s English receptive vocabulary accounted for approximately 9% of the variance while children’s Spanish receptive vocabulary accounted for approximately 4% of the variance in their English phonological awareness abilities. The relationship between Spanish receptive vocabulary and English phonological awareness was negative. No significant relationship was observed between children’s overall receptive language abilities in either language as measured by the TELD and PLS-4 at the end of Head Start and their English phonological awareness abilities at the end of kindergarten.

Table 4.

Bayesian parameter estimates for English phonological awareness regressed on stage 1 random intercepts.

2.5% 50% 97.5%
Model 1: Receptive language subtest of TELD-3
Intercept 63.17 80.22 100.61
Stage 1 estimated intercept −0.01 0.79 1.45
Residual variation 85.88 121.36 175.60
Model 2: PPVT-III
Intercept 72.88 84.44 98.25
Stage 1 estimated intercept* 0.05 0.31 0.52
Residual variation 83.63 118.18 171.00
Model 3: Auditory language subtest of PLS-3
Intercept 96.06 116.89 141.79
Stage 1 estimated intercept −1.20 −0.52 0.01
Residual variation 87.80 124.08 179.53
Model 4: TVIP
Intercept 97.36 103.27 110.33
Stage 1 estimated intercept* −0.81 −0.37 −0.05
Residual variation 87.69 123.92 179.31

Note: TELD-3 = Test of Early Language Development-3; PPVT-III = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III; PLS-3 = Preschool Language Scale-3; TVIP = Test de vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody. An R2 value was determined at .09 for the PPVT-III; an R2 value was determined at .04 for the TVIP.

*

denotes significance (α = .05).

An additional model was computed using only the significant language outcomes (i.e. English and Spanish receptive vocabulary). Table 5 displays the parameter estimates for a Bayesian model where children’s phonological awareness scores were regressed on the random intercepts from both the PPVT-III and the TVIP. Results indicated that the unique contribution of children’s English receptive vocabulary levels at the end of Head Start significantly predicted English phonological awareness in kindergarten when controlling for children’s Spanish receptive vocabulary levels. However, when controlling for English receptive vocabulary levels at the end of Head Start, children’s Spanish receptive vocabulary at that same point in time did not predict English phonological awareness at the end of kindergarten.

Table 5.

Bayesian parameter estimates for English phonological awareness regressed on Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III (PPVT-III) and Test de vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody (TVIP) random intercept.

2.5% 50% 97.5%
Intercept 75.91 89.91 102.90
Random intercept for PPVT-III* 0.02 0.29 0.56
Random intercept for TVIP −0.74 −0.34 0.01
Residual variation 83.34 114.33 165.20
*

denotes significance (α = .05).

Discussion

This investigation examined the relationship between bilingual preschoolers’ receptive language development during 2 years in Head Start and their English phonological awareness skills at the end of kindergarten. This study serves to extend prior research on the transfer of linguistic precursors to phonological awareness. The relationship between vocabulary and phonological awareness in monolingual children has been established (Metsala & Walley, 1998; Rvachew & Grawburg, 2006; Scarborough, 1990). More recent work has begun to examine this relationship in bilingual children (Dickinson et al., 2003; SanFrancisco et al., 2006). Additionally, previous studies examining phonological awareness abilities in bilingual school-age children suggest there is transfer between their language skills and phonological awareness. The purpose of this investigation was to build upon the current knowledge of the relationship between receptive vocabulary and overall receptive language during the preschool years and phonological awareness in kindergarten.

Results of this study indicated that both English and Spanish vocabulary levels at the end of Head Start explained a significant proportion of variance in English phonological awareness abilities at the end of kindergarten. However, after controlling for Spanish vocabulary levels at the end of Head Start, only English vocabulary levels at the end of Head Start predicted a significant amount of variance in English phonological awareness at the end of kindergarten. Conversely, end of Head Start levels of overall receptive language abilities in Spanish and English did not predict English phonological awareness abilities at the end of kindergarten.

Results of this study lend support to the lexical restructuring model proposed by Metsala and Walley (1998) as children’s English vocabulary scores at the end of Head Start predicted their phonological awareness skills at the end of kindergarten. These results are similar to those of Rvachew (2006) who found that receptive vocabulary before kindergarten explained approximately 10% of the variance in phonological awareness at the end of kindergarten, controlling for prekindergarten phonological awareness abilities. However, in the current study, it was hypothesised that Spanish vocabulary levels would also predict phonological awareness in English, assuming that vocabulary development in Spanish would facilitate metalinguistic awareness in Spanish that would then transfer to English. In contrast, when considered separately from English, Spanish vocabulary levels were observed to have a negative relationship with English phonological awareness. These results might suggest that children with higher levels of Spanish vocabulary have lower levels of English phonological awareness at the end of kindergarten. However, after controlling for English vocabulary levels, Spanish vocabulary was no longer a predictive factor indicating that most of the variance attributed to Spanish vocabulary was shared with English vocabulary. In other words, the negative relationship between Spanish vocabulary and English phonological awareness was a spurious effect and was due to the confounding effect of English vocabulary. This suggests that English vocabulary at the end of Head Start is the dominant predictive factor of kindergarten English phonological awareness. These results are similar to those of Dickinson et al. (2004) who also found that preschoolers’ English vocabulary and not Spanish vocabulary predicted their concurrent English phonological awareness abilities. The results do not support the notion that vocabulary development in either language supports metalinguistic awareness that transfers to the other language.

It was also hypothesised that levels of English and Spanish overall receptive language at the end of Head Start would predict phonological awareness in kindergarten. This was not the case, however. Overall receptive language levels in both languages were not predictive of later phonological awareness abilities. This finding further supports the lexical restructuring theory in that vocabulary abilities specifically, and not language abilities in general, support phonological awareness.

These results differ from those of López and Greenfield (2004) who found that English phonological awareness was related to English overall language abilities in 4-year-old Spanish–English-speaking Head Start preschoolers. Our study differs from that of López and Greenfield in several ways. López and Greenfield’s language assessment instrument consisted of expressive and receptive language subtests whereas only receptive language measures were used in the current study. It could be that expressive language tasks are more indicative of language competence in bilingual children and therefore would be a better predictor of phonological awareness skills in this population. In addition, López and Greenfield’s phonological awareness tasks required segmentation of larger linguistic units (i.e. sentence segmentation and rhyming), whereas the phonological awareness tasks used in the current study measured phoneme-level abilities such as sound matching and sound blending. Therefore, general language abilities may be better suited to investigating the relationships between language and larger unit phonological awareness, such as sentence segmentation ability, whereas vocabulary ability may be more predictive of later phoneme-level awareness.

It was anticipated that this group of bilingual children would exhibit individual variability in growth due, not only to individual ability levels, but also to the variety of language experiences in their homes and communities. Surprisingly, the children’s rates of growth were not significantly different from one another to the extent that random slopes could be used to explain the changes in their language abilities across the four measurement occasions. This result could be due to several factors, two of which are related to the assessment instruments used in this study. The instruments used were standardised on monolingual speakers of English and Spanish. Therefore, they may not adequately measure the language abilities of bilingual children. In addition, the instruments may not be sensitive enough to capture individual differences in rates of growth, particularly in this population. It also may be necessary to assess children more frequently in order to adequately capture the growth in language skills in bilingual children, particularly to capture individual variations in growth rate.

Attrition, which in turn affected the sample size, may have also played a role in the lack of significant differences in individual language and vocabulary growth rates. Attrition is practically unavoidable in any longitudinal undertaking, and it is especially likely when studying Spanish–English bilingual children whose families may have high mobility rates. Longitudinal studies which set out to evaluate differences in language skills over time in bilingual children should consider recruiting larger samples at the outset to allow for high rates of attrition.

Future directions

This study examined phonological awareness outcomes in English only, because English was the language of instruction in preschool and kindergarten. Future studies should examine the role of receptive vocabulary, in both English and Spanish, on the development of Spanish phonological awareness. Continued research into the unique contributions of the various components of language to phonological awareness is needed. For example, this study found that receptive vocabulary levels predicted English phonological awareness outcomes in this group of bilingual children. Future studies should investigate whether or not other language component skills such as phonology and morphosyntax in Spanish and English support phonological awareness abilities in kindergarten. Furthermore, the present study only investigated the relationship between receptive language skills and later phonological awareness skills. It would be important to examine the contributions of children’s expressive language to their phonological awareness abilities. Lastly, the current study used a composite phonological awareness score as the outcome measure. Future work in this area of research should examine the relationship between children’s language abilities and specific phonological awareness component skills such as rhyming, elision and blending tasks. Understanding the factors that most impact emergent literacy skills is essential for facilitating positive literacy outcomes in bilingual children.

Biography

Shelley E. Scarpino, MS, CCC-SLP, is an Assistant Professor in the Department of Speech Pathology and Audiology at the Richard Stockton College of New Jersey. Her research focuses on speech sound development and disorders in monolingual and bilingual children, the relationships among speech, language and early literacy skills, and clinical service delivery issues pertaining to culturally and linguistically diverse populations.

Frank R. Lawrence, PhD, is a statistical consultant. Dr Lawrence previously worked as a Research Professor at the University of Alabama, an Assistant Professor in the College of Health and Human Development at the Pennsylvania State University, and the Senior Data Modeller at the Defense Manpower Data Center.

Megan Dunn Davison, PhD, CCC-SLP, is a Clinical Assistant Professor in the Department of Communication Disorders at Temple University. Her research interests include language and literacy development in children from diverse backgrounds. In addition, Dr Davison is interested in examining the language and literacy development, including written language, of children at risk of language and reading disorders.

Carol Scheffner Hammer, PhD, CCC-SLP, is Professor of Communication Sciences and Disorders at Temple University. Her research focuses on environmental and cultural influences on children’s language and literacy development and on the efficacy of school readiness interventions. Her current work is funded by NIH-NICHD, IES, ACF and OSEP.

Contributor Information

Shelley E. Scarpino, The Richard Stockton College of New Jersey, USA

Frank R. Lawrence, The Pennsylvania State University, USA

Megan D. Davison, Temple University, USA

Carol S. Hammer, Temple University, USA

References

  1. Anthony J, Williams J, McDonald R, Francis D. Phonological processing and emergent literacy in younger and older preschool children. Annals of Dyslexia. 2007;57(2):113–137. doi: 10.1007/s11881-007-0008-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bolstad WM. Introduction to Bayesian statistics. John Wiley & Sons; Hoboken, NJ: 2004. [Google Scholar]
  3. Branum-Martin L, Mehta PD, Fletcher JM, Carlson CD, Ortiz A, Carlo M, et al. Bilingual phonological awareness: Multilevel construct validation among Spanish-speaking kindergarteners in transitional bilingual education classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2006;98(1):170–181. [Google Scholar]
  4. Burgess S, Lonigan C. Bidirectional relations of phonological sensitivity and prereading abilities: Evidence from a preschool sample. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1998;70(2):117–141. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1998.2450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Cheney C. Language development, metalinguistic skills, and print awareness in 3-year-old children. Applied Psycholinguistics. 1992;13:485–514. [Google Scholar]
  6. Chiappe P, Siegel L, Gottardo A. Reading-related skills of kindergartners from diverse linguistic backgrounds. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2002;23(1):95–116. [Google Scholar]
  7. Congdon P. Applied Bayesian modeling. John Wiley & Sons; West Sussex: 2003. [Google Scholar]
  8. Cummins J. Bilingualism and special education: Issues in assessment and pedagogy. College-Hill Press; San Diego, CA: 1984. [Google Scholar]
  9. DaFontoura HA, Siegel LS. Reading, syntactic, and working memory skills of bilingual Portuguese–English Canadian children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal. 1995;7(1):139–153. [Google Scholar]
  10. Dickinson D, McCabe A, Anastasopoulos L, Peisner-Feinberg E, Poe M. The comprehensive language approach to early literacy: The interrelationships among vocabulary, phonological sensitivity, and print knowledge among preschool-aged children. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2003;95(3):465–481. [Google Scholar]
  11. Dickinson DK, McCabe A, Clark-Chiarelli N, Wolf A. Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness in low-income Spanish and English bilingual preschool children. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2004;25(3):323–348. [Google Scholar]
  12. Duncan SE, De Avila EA. Scoring and interpretation manual for the pre-LAS Español. 2nd edn CTB/McGraw-Hill; Monterey, CA: 1986. [Google Scholar]
  13. Dunn LM, Dunn LM. Peabody picture vocabulary test-III. American Guidance Service; Circle Pines, MN: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  14. Dunn LM, Lugo DE, Padilla ER, Dunn LM. Test de vocabulario en imágenes Peabody. American Guidance Service; Circle Pines, MN: 1986. [Google Scholar]
  15. Durgunoğlu A, Nagy W, Hancin-Bhatt B. Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1993;85(3):453–465. [Google Scholar]
  16. Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics . America’s children in brief: Key national indicators of well-being, 2010. US Government Printing Office; Washington, DC: 2010. [Google Scholar]
  17. Frankenburg WK, Dodds J, Shapiro H, Bresnick B. DENVER II. Denver Developmental Materials Inc.; Denver, CO: 1992. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gelman A, Carlin JB, Stern HS, Rubin DB. Bayesian data analysis. 2nd edn CRC Press; Boca Raton, FL: 2004. [Google Scholar]
  19. Gilks WR, Richardson, Spiegelhalter DJ. Markov chain Monte Carlo in practice. CRC Press; Boca Raton, FL: 1996. [Google Scholar]
  20. Gill J. Bayesian methods: A social and behavioral sciences approach. CRC Press; Boca Raton, FL: 2002. [Google Scholar]
  21. Goswami U. Early phonological development and the acquisition of literacy. In: Neuman SB, Dickinson DK, editors. Handbook of early literacy research. Guilford Press; New York: 2001. pp. 111–125. [Google Scholar]
  22. Gottardo A. The relationship between language and reading skills in bilingual Spanish–English speakers. Topics in Language Disorders. 2002;22(5):46–70. [Google Scholar]
  23. Gottardo A, Mueller J. Are first- and second-language factors related in predicting second-language reading comprehension? A study of Spanish-speaking children acquiring English as a second language from first to second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2009;101(2):330–344. [Google Scholar]
  24. Hammer CS, Lawrence FR, Miccio AW. Bilingual children’s language abilities and early reading outcomes in Head Start and kindergarten. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 2007;38(3):237–248. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2007/025). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hancock GR, Lawrence FR. Using latent growth models to evaluate longitudinal change. In: Hancock GR, Mueller RO, editors. Structural equation modeling: A second course. Information Age Publishing Inc.; Greenwood, CT: 2006. [Google Scholar]
  26. Hresko W, Reid DK, Hammill D. Test of early language development – 3. ProEd Publishers; Austin, TX: 1999. [Google Scholar]
  27. Jimenez-Gonzalez JE. A reading level match study of phonemic processes underlying reading disabilities in a transparent orthography. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal. 1997;9(1):23–40. [Google Scholar]
  28. Jones RH. Longitudinal data with serial correlation: A state-space approach. Chapman & Hall; London: 1993. [Google Scholar]
  29. Lawrence FR, Hancock GR. Assessing change over time using latent growth modeling. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development. 1998;30(1):211–224. [Google Scholar]
  30. Lindsey K, Manis F, Bailey C. Prediction of first-grade reading in Spanish-speaking English-language learners. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2003;95(3):482–494. [Google Scholar]
  31. Lonigan CJ, Burgess SR, Anthony JL, Barker TA. Development of phonological sensitivity in two- to five-year-old children. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1998;90(2):294–311. [Google Scholar]
  32. López L, Greenfield D. The cross-language transfer of phonological skills of Hispanic Head Start children. Bilingual Research Journal. 2004;28(1):1–18. [Google Scholar]
  33. Metsala JL. Young children’s phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1999;91(1):3–19. [Google Scholar]
  34. Metsala JL, Walley AC. Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In: Metsala JL, Ehri LC, editors. Word recognition in beginning literacy. Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 1998. pp. 89–120. [Google Scholar]
  35. Quiroga T, Lemos-Britton Z, Mostafapour E, Abbott R, Berninger V. Phonological awareness and beginning reading in Spanish-speaking ESL first graders: Research into practice. Journal of School Psychology. 2002;40(1):85–111. [Google Scholar]
  36. Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS. Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. Sage; Newbury Park, CA: 2002. [Google Scholar]
  37. R Core Development Team . R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Version 2.11.1 R Foundation for Statistical Computing; Vienna: 2010. [Google Scholar]
  38. Rvachew S. Longitudinal predictors of implicit phonological awareness skills. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2006;15:165–176. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2006/016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Rvachew S, Grawburg M. Correlates of phonological awareness in preschoolers with speech-sound disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2006;49(1):74–87. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. SanFrancisco A, Carlo M, August D, Snow C. The role of language of instruction and vocabulary in the English phonological awareness of Spanish–English bilingual children. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2006;27(2):229–246. [Google Scholar]
  41. Scarborough HS. Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Development. 1990;61(6):1728–1743. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Scarborough HS. Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In: Neuman SB, Dickinson DK, editors. Handbook of early literacy research. Guilford Press; New York: 2001. pp. 97–110. [Google Scholar]
  43. Singer JD, Willet JB. Applied longitudinal data analysis. Oxford University Press; New York: 2003. [Google Scholar]
  44. Snijders TAB, Bosker RJ. Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. Sage Publishers; London: 1999. [Google Scholar]
  45. Snow CE, Burns MS, Griffin P, editors. Preventing reading difficulties in young children. National Academies Press; Washington, DC: 1998. [Google Scholar]
  46. Stanovich KE. Does dyslexia exist? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1994;35(4):579–595. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01208.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA. The development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bi-directional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology. 1994;30(1):73–87. [Google Scholar]
  48. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA. Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Pro-Ed; Austin, TX: 1999. [Google Scholar]
  49. Whitehurst GJ, Lonigan CJ. Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development. 1998;69(3):848–872. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Whitehurst GJ, Lonigan CJ. Emergent literacy: Development from prereaders to readers. In: Neuman S, Dickinson DK, editors. Handbook of early literacy research. Guilford; New York: 2001. pp. 11–29. [Google Scholar]
  51. Zimmerman I, Steiner V, Pond R. Preschool language scale-3. Spanish edn Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1992. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES