Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2012 Dec 26;7(12):e52767. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052767

The Right Time to Happen: Play Developmental Divergence in the Two Pan Species

Elisabetta Palagi 1,2,*, Giada Cordoni 1
Editor: Patrick Callaerts3
PMCID: PMC3530486  PMID: 23300765

Abstract

Bonobos, compared to chimpanzees, are highly motivated to play as adults. Therefore, it is interesting to compare the two species at earlier developmental stages to determine how and when these differences arise. We measured and compared some play parameters between the two species including frequency, number of partners (solitary, dyadic, and polyadic play), session length, and escalation into overt aggression. Since solitary play has a role in developing cognitive and physical skills, it is not surprising that chimpanzees and bonobos share similar developmental trajectories in the motivation to engage in this activity. The striking divergence in play developmental pathways emerged for social play. Infants of the two species showed comparable social play levels, which began to diverge during the juvenile period, a ‘timing hotspot’ for play development. Compared to chimpanzees, social play sessions in juvenile bonobos escalated less frequently into overt aggression, lasted longer, and frequently involved more than two partners concurrently (polyadic play). In this view, play fighting in juvenile bonobos seems to maintain a cooperative mood, whereas in juvenile chimpanzees it acquires more competitive elements. The retention of juvenile traits into adulthood typical of bonobos can be due to a developmental delay in social inhibition. Our findings show that the divergence of play ontogenetic pathways between the two Pan species and the relative emergence of play neotenic traits in bonobos can be detected before individuals reach sexual maturity. The high play motivation showed by adult bonobos compared to chimpanzees is probably the result of a long developmental process, rooted in the delicate transitional phase, which leads subjects from infancy to juvenility.

Introduction

Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus), the humans' closest living primate relatives, differ in multiple aspects of social behavior including aggression [1], conflict management [2], [3], sex [4], cooperation [5], and play [6]. Such differences have been attributed to different neurobiological systems [7], ecological pressures [8], and heterochronic mechanisms [9], which are changes in time of development respective to the ancestral ontogenies [10]. Parker and McKinney [10] pointed out that heterochronic mechanisms can produce both an overdevelopment and an underdevelopment. The underdevelopment, historically defined as pedomorphosis, can be reached via three different timing processes: progenesis (an early termination of development), postdisplacement (a late starting of development), and neoteny (a slowing down in the developmental rate) [11], [10]. Neoteny works throughout all phases of ontogeny and can produce an underdeveloped organism if onset and offset time are unchanged respect to the ancestral organism. The difference of some behavioral and morphological traits between the two Pan species are ascribed by many authors to neotenic processes [6], [12][16]. One of the main behaviors revealing the neotenic nature of a species is play, an activity strictly linked to the immature phase in most mammals [17][20]. Palagi [6] demonstrated that bonobos, compared to chimpanzees, maintain higher levels of playful motivation as adults. Given the evidence for developmental slowing down in bonobo play, it is interesting to compare the two Pan species at earlier developmental stages to determine how and when these differences in adult play arise in ontogeny. We hypothesize that the ontogenetic divergence of the two Pan species in play behavior occurs before animals reach sexual maturity and, more specifically, from infancy to juvenility. Moreover, if the age-related play divergence between immature bonobos and chimpanzees is due to the higher social tolerance levels showed by the former [15], we expect that this divergence involves more social than solitary play. Furthermore, within social play the divergence between infants and juveniles of the two species is mainly expected in the tolerance propensity during play fighting (or Rough&Tumble, which is characterized by strong physical contact) and polyadic play (when individuals have to manage a higher number of partners all interacting within a single session).

Results

Solitary play

A solitary play session started when an individual performed the first play behavioral pattern (see Table 1). If the bout started again after a delay of 10-s it was counted as a new play session. We distinguished object (the animal shakes, dangles, throws, an object of its environment in solitary way) and acrobatic sessions (the animal performs patterns such as pirouetting, somersaulting, headshaking, jumping, and running) for solitary play.

Table 1. Play behavioral patterns recorded during the observation sessions of both chimpanzee and bonobo groups.

Locomotor-Rotational play Definition
Acrobatic Play An animal climbs, jumps, and dangles from supports in its environment (e.g., branches, ropes) in solitary or social way (animals climb, jump, and dangle together and concurrently often on the same support).
Pirouetting An animal performs rolling over either on the ground or on vertical supports in solitary or social way (animals roll in contact hanging on the same vertical support)
Play recovering a thing An animal chases playmate and attempts to grab object carried by it
Play running An animal runs alone (solitary play) or chases play partner (social play)
Somersault An animal flips over either on the ground or on vertical supports in solitary or social way (animals flip in contact)
Play jumping An animal can solitarily jump on the substrate (ground, platforms, rocks, trunks)
Play sliding down An animal slides down. The sliding down pattern can be done on a slippery surface or on an inclined plane.
Headshaking An animal shakes its head laterally. Head shaking can be performed also when the animal is upside down
Play fighting
Play biting An animal gently bites the playmate
Play brusque rushing An animal jumps with its four limbs on playmate
Play pushing An animal pushes playmate either with its hands or feet
Play pulling An animal pulls a playmate with its hand
Play retrieving An animal holds playmate to prevent its flight
Play slapping An animal slaps any part of playmate's body
Play stamping An animal jumps on a playmate with its feet
Other play patterns
Full play face Playful facial display: mouth is opened with upper and lower teeth exposed. It can be performed both during solitary and social play sessions
Object play manipulation An animal shakes, dangles, throws, an object of its environment in solitary or social way (when the action is directed to a playmate; the pattern does not imply any kind of contact between the two animals)
Play face Playful facial display: mouth is opened with only lower teeth exposed. It can be performed both during solitary and social play sessions
Tickle An animal contacts the partner's body with its mouth or hands

For both species, we found a negative correlation between the acrobatic play levels and the subjects' ages (chimpanzees: Spearman rs = −0.767, N = 36, p = 0.0001, bonobo rs = −0.778, N = 34, p = 0.0001). Infant bonobos and chimpanzees did not differ in their acrobatic play rates (Mann-Whitney U = 19, nchimp = 8, nbon = 7, n.s.); while juvenile and adult bonobos showed higher levels of acrobatic play than chimpanzees (juveniles: U = 3.00, nchimp = 7, nbon = 6, p = 0.007; adults: U = 109.00, nchimp = 21, nbon = 21, p = 0.004) (Figure 1a).

Figure 1. Distribution of solitary play.

Figure 1

Hourly frequency of solitary acrobatic play (a) and solitary object play (b) performed by infants, juveniles, and adults of the two Pan species. Solid horizontal lines indicate medians; length of the boxes corresponds to inter-quartile range; thin horizontal lines indicate range of observed values. Only the significant differences are reported.

For both species, we found a negative correlation between the object play frequency and the subjects' ages (chimpanzees: rs = −0.720, n = 36, p = 0.0001; bonobo rs = −0.711, n = 34, p = 0.0001). Infants, juveniles, and adults of the two species did not differ in their object play rates (infants, U = 27, nchimp = 8, nbon = 7, n.s.; juveniles, U = 18.00, nchimp = 7, nbon = 6, n.s.; adults: U = 158.50, nchimp = 21, nbon = 21, n.s.) (Figure 1b).

Social play

A social play session started when an individual directed any playful pattern towards a fellow and ended when the playmates stopped their activities or one of them moved away [21]. For each play session we recorded: i) the identity and the number of playmates, ii) the play patterns performed and their chronological sequence iii) the context in which play took place (e.g. feeding, sexual). Within social play, we distinguished between Locomotor-Rotational play (including play recovering a thing, play run, pirouetting, sliding down, see Table 1), when a session was characterized by the absence of any kind of physical contact between the playmates [22], [23], and play fighting (including biting, pushing, pulling, slapping, stamping, retrieving, brusque rushing; see Table 1), when the participants exhibited physical contact.

Play frequency

The Locomotor-Rotational play levels negatively correlated with the playmates' ages for either chimpanzees (rs = −0.779, n = 36, p = 0.000001) or bonobos (rs = −0.832, n = 34, p = 0.000001). The play fighting rates negatively correlated with the chimpanzees' ages (rs = −0.842, n = 36, p = 0.000001) but not with the bonobos' ages (rs = -0.394, n = 34, n.s.).

Infant Locomotor-Rotational play and play fighting levels did not differ between the two species (LR-play: U = 16.00; nchimp = 8; nbon = 7; n.s.; play fighting: U = 24.00; nchimp = 8; nbon = 7; n.s.). Conversely, juvenile and adult Locomotor-Rotational play and play fighting rates were higher in bonobos than in chimpanzees (juveniles: LR-play, U = 1.50, nchimp = 7, nbon = 6, p = 0.003; play fighting: U = 4.50, nchimp = 7, nbon = 6, p = 0.015; adults: LR-play, U = 15.0, nchimp = 21, nbon = 21, p = 0.00000021; play fighting: U = 106.0, nchimp = 21, nbon = 21, p = 0.0039) (Figure 2a and 2b).

Figure 2. Distribution of social play.

Figure 2

Hourly frequency of locomotor-rotational play (a) and play fighting (b) performed by infants, juveniles, and adults of the two Pan species. Solid horizontal lines indicate medians; length of the boxes corresponds to inter-quartile range; thin horizontal lines indicate range of observed values. Only the significant differences are reported.

The intra-specific analysis on play initiation as a function of the playmates' ages revealed interesting differences between the two species. In chimpanzees, there was a significant difference in play initiation frequency among the three age-classes considered (Kruskall-Wallis' χ2  = 23.112, NI = 8, NJ = 7, NA = 21, d.f. = 2, p = 0.00001). Adults initiated play bouts less frequently than juveniles and infants (Dunn's post-hoc test; NI = 8, NA = 21, Q = 4.34, p = 0.0001; NJ = 7, NA = 21, Q = 3.47, p = 0.0002), whereas no difference was found between infant and juvenile subjects (Dunn post-hoc test: NI = 8, NJ = 7, Q = 0.298, n.s.) (Figure 3a). Also in bonobos, the analysis showed a significant difference in play initiation rates across the three age classes (Kruskall-Wallis' χ2  = 15.789, NI = 7, NJ = 6, NA = 21, d.f. = 2, p = 0.00001). Juveniles initiated play bouts more frequently than adults (Dunn post-hoc test NJ = 6, NA = 21, Q = 4.14, p = 0.0001); whereas, no difference was found between infants and juveniles (Dunn post-hoc test: NI = 7, NJ = 6, Q = 1.95, n.s.) and between infants and adults (Dunn post-hoc test: NI = 7, NA = 21, Q = 1.55, n.s.) (Figure 3b).

Figure 3. Play invitations in bonobos and chimpanzees.

Figure 3

Hourly frequency of play invitations performed by the three age classes of chimpanzees (a) and bonobos (b). Solid horizontal lines indicate medians; length of the boxes corresponds to inter-quartile range; thin horizontal lines indicate range of observed values. Only the significant differences are reported.

Polyadic play frequency

For each play session, the number of playmates was also recorded, thus permitting the distinction between dyadic (two players involved) and polyadic (more than two players involved) play sessions (as described by Hayaki [24]). The individual rates of polyadic sessions were defined as the number of polyadic sessions divided by the total number of play sessions performed. The polyadic session frequency negatively correlated with the ages of chimpanzees (rs = −0.705, n = 36, p = 0.000001) but not with those of bonobos (rs = 0.121, n = 34, p = 0.496). In chimpanzees, the analysis revealed a significant difference in polyadic play levels across the three age classes (Kruskall-Wallis' χ2  = 23.33, NI = 8, NJ = 7, NA = 21, d.f. = 2, p = 0.00001). Infant and juvenile chimpanzees did not differ in their polyadic play session rates (Dunn post-hoc test: NI = 8, NJ = 7, Q = 0.54, n.s.); adult chimpanzees performed significantly less polyadic sessions than infants (Dunn post-hoc test NI = 8, NA = 21, Q = 3.79, p<0.001) and juveniles (Dunn post-hoc test NJ = 7, NA = 21, Q = 2.96, p<0.01) (Figure 4a).

Figure 4. Polyadic play interactions of bonobos and chimpanzees.

Figure 4

Rates of the polyadic play sessions performed by the three age classes of chimpanzees (a) and bonobos (b). Solid horizontal lines indicate medians; length of the boxes corresponds to inter-quartile range; thin horizontal lines indicate range of observed values. Only the significant differences are reported.

In bonobos there was a significant difference in polyadic play rates across the diverse ages (Kruskall-Wallis' χ2  = 8.774, NI = 7, NJ = 6, NA = 21, d.f. = 2, p = 0.012). Juvenile bonobos performed polyadic sessions more frequently than infants (Dunn post-hoc test NI = 7, NJ = 6, Q = 2.96, p = 0.005). Polyadic session rates of adult bonobos were comparable to those of infants (Dunn post-hoc test NI = 7, NA = 21, Q = 1.61, n.s.) and juveniles (Dunn post-hoc test NJ = 6, NA = 21, Q = 2.03, n.s.) (Figure 4b).

Play session length

We measured the length (seconds) of each play session per each dyad. To estimate the individual session length, we calculated the median of the medians of the length of the sessions in which that subject was involved.

Both in chimpanzees and bonobos we did not find any correlation between the play length of each session and the subjects' ages (chimpanzees: rs = −0.134, n = 36, n.s.; bonobos: rs = −0.380, n = 34, n.s.). The duration of each play session did not differ between bonobo and chimpanzee infants (U = 8.00, nchimp = 8, nbon = 7, n.s.; mean duration of each session, bonobos = 4.91±0.55 SE; chimpanzees = 3.25±0.19 SE) and between bonobo and chimpanzee adults (U = 182.00, nchimp = 21, nbon = 21, n.s.; mean duration of each session, bonobos = 2.93±0.18 SE; chimpanzees = 3.71±0.39 SE). As for juveniles, bonobos showed a longer duration of each session compared to chimpanzees (U = 3.00, nchimp = 7, nbon = 6, p = 0.007; mean duration of each session, bonobos = 5.40±0.68 SE; chimpanzees = 3.06±0.24 SE).

Escalation of play into overt aggression

Play sessions can sometimes escalate into overt aggressions. In the two Pan species, there are some vocalizations and facial displays that express fear such as screaming and bared-teeth [25]. We classified as “escalated” those play sessions that ended with screaming and/or bared-teeth by one of the players and/or ended with an aggressive interaction (e.g., chase/flee) between them. We measured the individual frequency of escalation as the number of escalated play sessions on the number of the total play sessions performed by each subject.

Bonobo and chimpanzee infants did not differ in the rates of escalated play sessions (Mann-Whitney's U = 14.00, nchimp = 8, nbon = 7, p = 0.103). A similar result was also found when we compared the adults of the two species (U = 210.0, nchimp = 21, nbon = 21, p = 1.0). On the other hand, juvenile chimpanzees, compared to juvenile bonobos, showed higher levels of play escalation into overt aggression (U = 4.50, nchimp = 7, nbon = 6, p = 0.012) (Figure 5).

Figure 5. Escalation of social play into aggressive encounters.

Figure 5

Rates of the escalated play sessions performed by infants, juveniles, and adults of the two Pan species. Solid horizontal lines indicate medians; length of the boxes corresponds to inter-quartile range; thin horizontal lines indicate range of observed values. Only the significant differences are reported.

Discussion

The divergence of play developmental timing between bonobos and chimpanzees seems to occur during the transitional phase from infancy to juvenility. This divergence emerges particularly in social play, which shows greater variation in the two Pan species.

Many authors agree that solitary play, in its object and acrobatic version, has similar adaptive functions in social animals [20], [26][28]. For this reason, it is not surprising that the two Pan species share similar developmental pathways in the motivation to engage in this activity. Many scholars affirm that object and acrobatic play, independently of different cultures [29], [30], helps individuals in developing cognitive [31] and physical skills [32], which are relevant to subsistence activities such as prey catching, agonistic behavior, and tool use [26], [33]. For example, human and non-human acrobatic play, with its balance-disturbing actions (e.g., somersaults, pirouettes, body-rotation), provides an important vestibular stimulation, which favors motor development [34]. The higher frequency of acrobatic play recorded for juvenile bonobos compared to chimpanzees can be related to the delay in the body size development of skeleton features [10], [35] and locomotion habits of Pan paniscus [36]. In fact, with their varied locomotion involving arboreal quadrupedal and bipedal activities, bonobos are considered the most suspensory of the African apes [37].

The most striking divergence in the developmental pathways of bonobos and chimpanzees are evident in a particular type of social play: play fighting. Chimpanzees engaged in less play fighting sessions as their age increased, in contrast with bonobos, who maintained constant levels of play throughout infant, juvenile, and adult periods. The hotspot for play fighting timing divergence is juvenility; in fact, infant bonobos and chimpanzees showed similar levels of this practice, which began to follow a divergence trend at the onset of the juvenile phase. Play fighting is one of the most complex interactions used by human and non-human animals to gather information on the potential of conspecifics as competitors or social partners [19]. This competitive/cooperative interaction serves to test a partner's willingness to invest in a relationship and, simultaneously, to demonstrate one's own willingness to accept vulnerability [6].

Compared to chimpanzees, social play sessions in juvenile bonobos escalated less frequently into overt aggression, lasted longer, and frequently involved more than two partners concurrently (polyadic play). All these findings suggest that social play can undergo a functional shift from infancy to juvenility. In juvenile bonobos, play fighting seems to maintain a cooperative mood [21], [23], whereas in juvenile chimpanzees [38] and in human adolescents [20], [39] it acquires more competitive elements. A further, but not alternative, explanation for the divergence trend in social play of the two Pan species could be the low degree of bonobo social inhibitory control, which is essential to make this practice efficient [19]. Bonobos, compared to chimpanzees, show a developmental delay in social inhibition that can be responsible for the retention of juvenile traits into adulthood [15]. Our data on social play go further by indicating that in bonobos such delay can be responsible for the retention of infant traits into the juvenile period. Play ontogenetic pathways of immature bonobos seem to show similarities with play ontogenetic pathways of children. In fact, even though ethological data are scarce, a human cross-cultural analysis of social play revealed some stylistic variations but a common distribution in frequency according to the different age phases [40].

Both infants and adults of the two Pan species showed a similar duration of a single play session, which, on the contrary, differed between the juveniles of the two species, with chimpanzees performing shorter sessions than bonobos. This finding, together with the low preference for chimpanzees to engage in polyadic play, indicates that juveniles of this species are less able than bonobos to manage a playful session in relation to time and number of playmates. This is probably due to the higher competitive nature of chimpanzee playful interactions [39] and to their lower social tolerance degree, which become evident in the juvenile phase [41].

In conclusion, our findings show that the divergence of play ontogenetic pathways between the two Pan species and the relative emergence of play neotenic traits in bonobos can be detected before individuals reach sexual maturity. The high play motivation showed by adult bonobos [16] compare to chimpanzees [6] is probably the result of a long developmental process which is rooted in the delicate transitional phase, which leads subjects from infancy to juvenility.

Methods

Ethics statement

This study was approved by University of Pisa (Animal Care and Use board). Since the study was purely observational the committee waived the need for a permit. The study was conducted with no manipulation of animals. The parks gave the permission to collect data on the animals.

The study colonies, data collection, and analysis

The study involved eight infant, seven juvenile and 21 adult chimpanzees (2001–2002, ZooParc de Beauval, France; 2004, Dierenpark Amersfoort, the Netherlands) and seven infant, six juvenile, and 21 adult bonobos (2000–2003, 2009, Apenheul Primate Park, the Netherlands; 2006, FrankfurtZoo, Germany; 2009–2010, WilhelmaZoo, Germany) (Table 2). Since the chimpanzee and bonobo ages did not differ, we could compare the different Pan species groups (infants: Mann-Whitney U = 27; nchimp = 8; nbon = 7, n.s.; juveniles: U = 13.5; nchimp  = 7; nbon  = 6; n.s.; adults: U = 154; nchimp = 21; nbon = 21; n.s.).

Table 2. Composition of the chimpanzee and bonobo groups.

Subjects Sex Class Age in years Mother-Offspring Relationship Residence
CHIMPANZEE GROUPS
Ituri Female 0.5 Amersfoort
Kumi Male 2.0 Amersfoort
Karibuna Male 2.5 Amersfoort
Ghafula Female 3.5 Amersfoort
Chura Female 6.0 Amersfoort
Bibi Female 7.0 Amersfoort
Willy Female 13.0 Amersfoort
Sanne Female 15.0 Amersfoort
Cees Male 25.0 Amersfoort
Belle Female 27.0 Amersfoort
Silvia Female 30.0 Sanne's mother Amersfoort
Jet Female 34.0 Amersfoort
Sjimmie Female 37.0 Amersfoort
Kokkie Female 38.0 Cees'mother Amersfoort
Sjors Female 38.0 Ghafula and Ituri's mother Amersfoort
Mike Male 39.0 Amersfoort
Sonja Female 40.0 Amersfoort
Rachel Female 1.0 Beauval
Bazou Male 2.0 Beauval
Makury Male 2.5 Beauval
Melie Female 3.5 Beauval
Leo Male 4.0 Beauval
Isabel Female 5.5 Beauval
Benji Male 6.0 Beauval
Christmas Female 6.5 Beauval
Tsavo Male 7.0 Beauval
Gamin Male 13.0 Beauval
Domi Female 13.0 Rachel's mother Beauval
Gypso Female 15.0 Melie's mother Beauval
Joseph Male 19.0 Beauval
Bonobo Female 20.0 Benji and Makuri's mother Beauval
Julie Female 20.0 Christmas and Leo's mother Beauval
Baraka Female 23.0 Tsavo and Bazou's mother Beauval
Micheline Female 24.0 Beauval
Charlotte Female 29.0 Domi and Isabel's mother Beauval
La Vieille Female 43.0 Beauval
BONOBO GROUPS
Jasiri Female 0.5 Lomela's daughter Apenheul
Kumbuka Female 1.5 Molaso's daughter Apenheul
Tarishi Male 2.5 Jill's son Apenheul
Liboso Female 2.5 Zuani's daughter Apenheul
Hongo Male 3.5 Hortense's son Apenheul
Nayembi Female 3.5 Liboso's daughter Apenheul
Lingala Female 6.0 Jill's daughter Apenheul
Lomela Female 9.0 Jasiri's mother Apenheul
Zuani Female 10.0 Liboso's mother Apenheul
Hani Male 11.0 Apenheul
Rosie Female 11.0 Apenheul
Zamba Male 11.0 Hortense's son Apenheul
Molaso Female 15.0 Kumbuka's mother Apenheul
Mwindu Male 15.0 Apenheul
Jill Female 15.0 Tarishi and Lingala's mother Apenheul
Mobikisi Male 20.0 Apenheul
Hortense Female 31.0 Zamba and Hongo's mother Apenheul
Kelele Male 2.0 FrankfurtZoo
Heri Male 5.0 Natalie's son FrankfurtZoo
Haiba Female 5.0 Ukela's daughter FrankfurtZoo
Kutu Female 8.0 FrankfurtZoo
Zomi Female 8.0 FrankfurtZoo
Kamiti Female 19.0 FrankfurtZoo
Ukela Female 21.0 Natalie's daughter FrankfurtZoo
Ludwig Male 22.0 FrankfurtZoo
Natalie Female 40.0 Ukela and Heri's mother FrankfurtZoo
Margrit Female 54.0 FrankfurtZoo
Kianga Female 5.0 Kombote's daughter WilhelmaZoo
Kasai Male 5.5 Chipita's son WilhelmaZoo
Banbo Female 7.0 WilhelmaZoo
Mixi Female 8.0 Chipita's daughter WilhelmaZoo
Chimba Female 14.0 WilhelmaZoo
Chipita Female 18.0 Mixi and Kasai's mother WilhelmaZoo
Kombote Female 43.0 Kianga's mother WilhelmaZoo

All the study subjects were reared and breastfed by their natural mothers and all were in good health. All the chimpanzee and bonobo groups were housed in enclosures made of both indoor and outdoor facilities equipped with trunks, lianas, ropes, stones, and platforms so the animals could move freely in all three dimensions. The visibility conditions were excellent for each of the study groups considered. Both chimpanzees and bonobos were fed three times a day with vegetables, fresh fruits, nuts, grains, eggs, pellets, and yogurt that were scattered on the ground or concealed under trunks or stones. No animals performed any sign of distress or stereotypic behaviors.

Before systematic data collection, the eight observers underwent a training period to become skilled in play patterns and animals' identification. For each observer involved in this study, the training was carried out by the first author. Each training session was performed on the colony the observers would follow for data collection. During the training process, after recording data on the same play session the observers compared and contrasted the collected items. This procedure permitted a measure of inter-observer reliability by calculating the Cohen's kappa value, which was calculated at least three times during the entire observational period for each colony. Cohen's kappa values were never less than 0.70. Only Elisabetta Palagi looked at all the groups involved in the study, whereas each other observer looked at only one group. Observations took place over a 6-hour period, 6 days per week (also covering the feeding-times). Via focal sampling (individual mean hours 31±1.40 SE for chimpanzees; 40±0.92 SE for bonobos) we collected data on solitary and social play.

Due to the non-normality of the data and the small sample sizes, nonparametric statistical tests were used for the analyses at the individual level [42]. We made use of exact tests according to the threshold values suggested by Mundry & Fischer [43]. For all the two-tailed analyses, we adjusted the significance level via the Bonferroni correction (α/3 = 0.017). The analyses were performed by using SPSS 12.0.

Acknowledgments

We thank the six students from the University of Pisa and Turin for helping to collect data; the directors and keepers of the European parks; Roscoe Stanyon and Matthew Campbell for the critical review of the manuscript and for their accurate linguistic revision. Finally, the authors thank all the participants in the NIMBioS (http://www.nimbios.org/workinggroups/WG_play) Working Group on “Play, Evolution, and Sociality” for their stimulating input on one of the most controversial and wonderful behaviors an ethologist can come across.

Funding Statement

The authors have no support or funding to report.

References

  • 1.Wrangham R, Peterson D (1996) Demonic males. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
  • 2.Palagi E, Cordoni G (2007) The art of peace-keeping. Different tactics in the great apes. In: Zelick PR, editor. Issues in the Psychology of Motivation. New York: Nova Science Publishers. 157–171.
  • 3. Furuichi T (2011) Female contributions to the peaceful nature of bonobo society. Evol Anthropol 20: 131–142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. de Waal FBM (1987) Tension regulation and nonreproductive functions of sex in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus). Nat Geo Res 3: 318–335. [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Hare B, Melis AP, Woods V, Hastings S, Wrangham RW (2007) Tolerance allows bonobos to outperform chimpanzees on a cooperative task. Curr Biol 17: 619–623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Palagi E (2006) Social play in bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): implications for natural social systems and interindividual relationships. Am J Phys Anthropol 129: 418–426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Rilling JK, Scholz J, Preuss TM, Glasser M, Errangi B, et al.. (2011) Differences between chimpanzees and bonobos in neural systems supporting social cognition. SCAN (DOI 10.1093/scan/nsr017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 8. Rosati AG, Stevens JR, Hare B, Hauser MD (2007) The evolutionary origins of human patience: temporal preferences in chimpanzees, bonobos, and adult humans. Curr Biol 17: 1663–1668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Brosnan S (2010) Behavioral development: timing is everything. Curr Biol 20: 98–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Parker ST, McKinney ML (1999) Origins of intelligence: the evolution of cognitive development in monkeys, apes, and humans. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press.
  • 11. McKinney ML (1998) The juvenilized ape myth: our “overdeveloped” brain. Bioscience 48: 109–116. [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Tratz E, Heck H (1954) Der afrikanische Anthropoide “Bonobo”. eine neue Menschenaffengattung. Saugetierk. Mitteilungen 2: 97–101. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.de Waal FBM (1989) Peacemaking among primates. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.
  • 14. Lieberman DE, Carlo J, de Leon M, Zollikofer CP (2007) A geometric morphometric analysis of heterochrony in the cranium of chimpanzees and bonobos. J Hum Evol 52: 647–662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Wobber V, Wrangham R, Hare B (2010) Application of the heterochrony framework to the study of behavior and cognition. Curr Biol 20: 226–230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Enomoto T (1990) Social play and sexual behavior of the bonobo (Pan paniscus) with special reference to flexibility. Primates 31: 469–480. [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Coppinger R, Glendinning J, Torop E, Matthay C, Sutherland M, et al. (1987) Degree of behavioral neoteny differentiates canid polymorphs. Ethology 75: 89–108. [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Morey DF (1994) The early evolution of the domestic dog. Am Sci 82: 336–347. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Pellis SM, Pellis VC (2009) The playful brain: venturing to the limits of neuroscience. Oxford: Oneworld Publications.
  • 20.Pellegrini AD (2009) The role of play in human development. New York: Oxford University Press. 278 p.
  • 21. Palagi E (2008) Sharing the motivation to play: the use of signals in adult bonobos. Anim Behav 75: 887–896. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Burghardt GM (2005) The Genesis of Animal Play: Testing the Limits. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. 500 p.
  • 23. Palagi E, Paoli T (2007) Play in adult bonobos (Pan paniscus): modality and potential meaning. Am J Phys Anthropol 134: 219–225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Hayaki H (1985) Social play of juvenile and adolescent chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates 26: 343–360. [Google Scholar]
  • 25. de Waal FBM (1988) The communicative repertoire of captive bonobos (Pan paniscus), compared to that of chimpanzees. Behavior 106: 183–251. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Smith PK (2010) Understanding children's worlds. Children and Play. Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.
  • 27. Kahlenberg SM, Wrangham RW (2010) Sex differences in chimpanzees' use of sticks as play objects resemble those of children. Curr Biol 20: R1067–R1068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Palagi E (in press) Playing Alone and with Others – A Lesson from Animals. In: Coplan RJ & Bowker J, editors. A handbook of solitude: psychological perspectives on social isolation, social withdrawal, and being alone. Hoboken (New Jersey, USA): John Wiley & Sons.
  • 29.Gosso Y, Otta E, Morais M, Ribeiro F, Bussabb V (2005) Play in hunter-gatherer society. In: Pellegrini AD, Smith PK, editors. The nature of play: Great apes and humans. New York: Guilford. 213–254.
  • 30.Ramsey J, McGrew WC (2005) Object play in great apes: studies in nature and captivity. In: Pellegrini AD, Smith PK, editors. The nature of play: Great apes and humans. New York: Guilford. 89–112.
  • 31.Power TG (2000) Play and Exploration in Children and Animals. Mahwah, New Jersey: L. Erlbaum. 497 p.
  • 32. Martin P, Caro TM (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behavioral development. Adv Stud Behav 15: 59–103. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Gruber T, Muller MN, Reynolds V, Wrangham R, Zuberbühler K (2011) Community-specific evaluation of tool affordances in wild chimpanzees. Sci Rep 1(128). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 34.Fagen RM (2011) Play and development. In: Pellegrini A, editor. The Oxford handbook of the development of play. New York: Oxford University Press. 83–100.
  • 35.Wrangham R, Pilbeam D (2002) African apes as time machines. In: Galdikas B, Briggs N, Sheeran L, Shapiro G, Goodall J, editors. All apes great and small. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. 5–18.
  • 36.Fleagle JG (1999) Primate adaptation and evolution, 2nd ed. San Diego: Academic Press. 596 p.
  • 37.Doran DM, Hunt KD (1994) Comparative locomotor behavior of chimpanzees and bonobos. In: Wrangham RW, McGrew WC, de Waal FBM, Heltne PG, editors. Chimpanzee Cultures. Chicago: Chicago Academy of Science. 93–109.
  • 38. Paquette D (1994) Fighting and playfighting in captive adolescent chimpanzees. Aggr Behav 20: 49–65. [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Cordoni G, Palagi E (2011) Ontogenetic trajectories of chimpanzee social play: similarities with humans. PlosOne 6: e27344 (DOI 10.1371/journal.pone.0027344).. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Fry D (2005) Rough-and-tumble social play in humans. In: Pellegrini AD, Smith PK, editors. The nature of play: Great apes and humans. New York: Guilford. 54–85.
  • 41. Wrangham RW (1993) The evolution of sexuality in chimpanzees and bonobos. Hum Nat 4: 47–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Siegel S, Castellan NJ (1988) Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill. 399 p.
  • 43. Mundry R, Fischer J (1998) Use of statistical programs for nonparametric tests of small samples often lead to incorrect P values: examples from animal behavior. Anim Behav 56: 256–259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES