Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: Brain Lang. 2011 Dec 7;122(3):162–170. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.10.005

A sensitive period for language in the visual cortex: Distinct patterns of plasticity in congenitally versus late blind adults

Marina Bedny 1, Alvaro Pascual-Leone 2,3, Swethasri Dravida 1, Rebecca Saxe 1
PMCID: PMC3536016  NIHMSID: NIHMS337975  PMID: 22154509

Abstract

Recent evidence suggests that blindness enables visual circuits to contribute to language processing. We examined whether this dramatic functional plasticity has a sensitive period. BOLD fMRI signal was measured in congenitally blind, late blind (blindness onset 9-years-old or later) and sighted participants while they performed a sentence comprehension task. In a control condition, participants listened to backwards speech and made match/non-match to sample judgments. In both, congenitally and late blind participants BOLD signal increased in bilateral foveal-pericalcarine cortex during response preparation, irrespective of whether the stimulus was a sentence or backwards speech. However, only in congenitally blind people left occipital areas (pericalcarine, extrastriate, fusiform and lateral) responded more to sentences than backwards speech. We conclude that age of blindness onset constrains the non-visual functions of occipital cortex: while plasticity is present in both congenitally and late blind individuals, recruitment of visual circuits for language depends on blindness during childhood.

Keywords: plasticity, development, sensitive-period, critical-period, language evolution, visual cortex, blind, sentence comprehension, foveal, pericalcarine

Introduction

Humans are believed to have specific neural adaptations that enable them, and no other species, to acquire language (Pinker, 1994). Consistent with this possibility, the neural basis of language is highly stable across languages and cultures. Languages as varied as Mandarin Chinese, English, and various sign languages, are supported by a common left-lateralized network of brain areas (Chee et al., 1999; MacSweeney, Capek, Campbell, & Woll, 2008). This network includes regions within the frontal, temporal and parietal lobes such as Broca’s area and Wernicke’s area. The capacity for language is commonly assumed to depend on intrinsic properties of these brain areas. However, recent research with blind individuals suggests that under some circumstances, brain regions thought to have evolved for vision can participate in language processing.

Blind individuals engage occipital regions during a variety of verbal tasks including Braille reading, verb generation (in response to heard nouns), covert retrieval of words from long-term memory and sentence comprehension (Amedi, Floel, Knecht, Zohary, & Cohen, 2004; Amedi, Raz, Pianka, Malach, & Zohary, 2003; Burton, Diamond, & McDermott, 2003; Burton, Snyder, Diamond, & Raichle, 2002; Sadato et al., 1996; Uhl, Franzen, Lindinger, Lang, & Deecke, 1991). Occipital activity is observed in various visual areas including the pericalcarine cortex (the anatomical location of V1 and the first stage of cortical visual processing in humans) as well as in secondary visual regions on the lateral and ventral surface of the occipital lobe.

In congenitally blind adults, occipital areas show a response profile that is similar to classic language areas: During sentence comprehension, occipital regions are sensitive to high-level linguistic information such as the meanings of words and compositional sentence structure (Bedny, Pascual-Leone, Dodell-Feder, Fedorenko, & Saxe, 2011; Burton et al., 2003; Roder, Rosler, & Neville, 2000; Roder, Stock, Bien, Neville, & Rosler, 2002). Like classic language areas, occipital regions respond most to sentences, less to lists of words and to Jabberwocky (non-word sentences), and still less to lists of nonsense words and backwards speech (Bedny, et al, 2011; Pallier, Devauchelle, & Dehaene, 2011). There is also some evidence that occipital activation is functionally relevant to language processing: rTMS to the occipital pole leads to verb-generation errors and impairs Braille reading in congenitally blind individuals (Amedi et al., 2004; Cohen et al., 1997, Kupers, et al, 2007).

These data suggest that blindness enables occipital areas to participate in language processing. A key open question is whether this remarkable plasticity results from blindness early in life, or can be caused by long periods of blindness at any age.

Early and late blindness could cause qualitatively different forms of plasticity for at least two reasons: First, there may be a sensitive period in the functional development of human visual circuits. Absence of vision early in life has distinctive effects on the neurophysiology and function of the mammalian visual system (Hubel & Wiesel, 1970, Hensch, 2004, 2005; Morishita & Hensch, 2008, Fagiolini, Jensen, & Champagne, 2009, Smirnakis et al., 2005). In animals, dark rearing impairs the development of the direction and orientation selectivity and enlarges receptive field size of cells in primary visual cortex (Crair, Gillespie, & Stryker, 1998, Li, Fitzpatrick, & White, 2006; Li, Van Hooser, Mazurek, White, & Fitzpatrick, 2008, Fagiolini, Pizzorusso, Beradi, Domenici, & Maffei, 1994; Wang, Sarnaik, & Cang, 2010). Visual deprivation in adulthood does not result in similar deficits (Hensch, 2004, 2005; Morishita & Hensch, 2008, Fagiolini, Jensen, & Champagne, 2009, Smirnakis et al., 2005). These animal studies suggest that early blindness could have more dramatic effects on the function of the human visual system.

Second, there may be a sensitive period in the plasticity of the language system itself. Once established, the neural architecture of language may not be as flexible. There is considerable evidence that the language system is more flexible during development than later in life. For example, damage to classic left hemisphere language regions during development has much less pronounced effects on language abilities than damage to the same brain regions in adulthood (Bates, 1999). Similarly, language acquisition early in life is more facile than in adulthood (Johnson & Newport, 1989). Occipital areas may only get incorporated into the language system during this key developmental period.

To assess whether early and late blindness differentially effect the visual system’s capacity to take on language functions, we compared the response of occipital cortex to language in congenitally blind and late blind individuals. If occipital responses to language emerge specifically as a result of early blindness, congenitally blind but not late blind individuals should show this language response.

Existing studies are ambiguous as to whether there is a sensitive period for language-related plasticity in occipital cortex. On the one hand, there is clear evidence of functional plasticity in occipital cortex for both congenitally and late blind adults (Sadato et al, 2002) and occipital areas are active during verbal tasks in both populations (Buchel, Price, Frackowiak, & Friston, 1998; Burton, 2003; Burton & McLaren, 2006; Burton et al., 2002b). On the other hand, it is not clear whether occipital activity in congenitally and late blind people reflects similar cognitive processes. In congenitally blind people, occipital cortex responds more during verbal than non-verbal tasks, even when the non-verbal tasks are more difficult and when no overt responses are required (Amedi et al., 2004; Amedi et al., 2003; Bedny, Pascual-Leone et al., 2011; Roder et al., 2002). Such data suggest that occipital areas respond specifically to linguistic content. By contrast, in late blind people verbal tasks have been compared to passive or less difficult non-verbal tasks (for example, verb generation compared to passive listening to non-verbal stimuli) (Burton et la, 2002a). These results leave open the possibility that occipital activity in late blind people is driven by non-verbal components of the task, such as response preparation.

Several existing pieces of evidence suggest that occipital activity might be related to different cognitive processes in congenitally blind and late blind people. First, occipital activity is much less pronounced in late blind as compared to congenitally blind participants (Burton, et al, 2002, Burton, 2003). Second, there is some evidence that responses occur in different parts of occipital cortex in these two populations. For example, during verb generation occipital activity is left-lateralized in congenitally blind participants, but bilateral or right-lateralized in late blind individuals (Buchel et al., 1998, Burton, et al, 2002a, Burton et al, 2002b, Burton, 2003). There is also some evidence that occipital activity has different functional relevance in these populations. One study found that rTMS to the occipital pole impairs Braille reading in congenitally blind and early onset blind participants, but not in late blind individuals (Cohen et al., 1999). On the other hand, in late blind participants rTMS to occipital cortex does impair performance non-verbal tasks (Merabet et al., 2009).

In light of prior data, the goal of the present study was to examine whether occipital plasticity in congenitally blind and late blind individuals is qualitatively similar. In particular, we wished to establish whether occipital areas respond to language in late blind individuals as they do in congenitally blind participants.

To explore this question, we presented late blind, congenitally blind and sighted participants with a sentence comprehension task. Participants heard short passages and answered true/false questions about them. In a control condition, participants heard the same sentences played backwards and performed a match to sample task. Backwards speech is unintelligible and lacks linguistic information, such as sentence structure, meanings of words or phonology. On the other hand, both the language and the backwards speech tasks required encoding and maintenance of information in working memory, and the auditory matching task was more difficult than the true/false task with sentences so that if we observed a larger response to sentences this could not be explained by the difficulty of the task itself. We reasoned that 1) a language-sensitive response should be higher for sentences than for backwards speech, and 2) a language-sensitive response should be present during the story portion of the trial, when no response preparation is required.

We focused our analyses on the pericalcarine visual cortex. In sighted people, pericalcarine cortex is anatomically the location of the primary visual cortex, V1, which is the earliest cortical recipient of visual information in primates (Lamme, Super, & Spekreijse, 1998). V1 is believed to have specifically evolved for visual perception and has well described anatomical properties such as an enlarged layer IV which receives inputs from the lateral geniculate nucleus (Gennari, 1782). Therefore, an important question is whether plasticity in this early visual area occurs in the same way during and after early childhood. We focused initially on left pericalcarine cortex because prior studies have found left-lateralized pericalcarine responses to language in congenitally blind people (e.g. Amedi et al, 2003, Bedny et al, 2011, Roder, et al, 2002). There is also some evidence that late blind and congenitally blind participants manifest plasticity in partially distinct areas of pericalcarine cortex along the caudal/rostral axis. In late blind people, pericalcarine activity appears to be relatively restricted to the foveal/parafoveal region (Burton, 2003). To formally test this hypothesis, we measured percent signal change (PSC) in the in foveal/parafoveal (caudal) and peripheral (rostral) pericalcarine cortex separately.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Twenty-two sighted (12 females, mean age 41 years, SD 19), 11 congenitally blind (6 females, mean age 49 years, SD 9), and 9 late blind (3 females, mean age 48, SD 9) participants took part in the study. Groups did not differ in age (P>.2). Blind participants had lost their vision due to pathology anterior to or in the optic chiasm, and had at most residual minimal light perception. None of them could see colors, shapes, or motion. Congenitally blind participants had at most minimal light perception from birth. Late blind participants lost their vision in late childhood or later (range: 9 to 29 years-of-age). The present definition of late blindness is consistent with a number of prior studies (Burton, 2002a; Burton, 2002b), but differs from some, which consider late blind only those participants who lost their vision after age 16 (Sadato, 2002). Detailed demographic information, including gender, age, age of blindness onset, cause of blindness, presence of residual light perception, and highest level of education is summarized in Table 1. None of the participants had any known neurological disorders or had ever sustained head injury. The study was approved by the institutional review board and all participants gave written informed consent.

Table 1.

ID Gender Age (y) Age totally
blind (y)
Cause of blindness Residual LP Highest Level Ed (y)
CB1 F 47 Birth Premature birth/RLF No 3 yr of college
CB2 F 61 Birth Optic chiasm malformation Left Eye Multiple MAs (22)
CB3 M 46 Birth Retinoblastoma No MA (18)
CB4 M 54 Birth Premature birth/RLF No MA (18)
CB5 F 43 Birth Premature birth/RLF No BA (16)
CB7 M 44 Birth Congenital Rubella Syndrome No BA (16)
CA8 F 57 Birth Premature birth/RLF No MA (18)
CA10 F 53 Birth Premature birth/RLF Yes MA (18)
CB11 M 39 Birth Premature birth/RLF Yes MA (18)
CB12 M 36 Birth Premature birth/RLF No MA (18)
CB14 F 61 Birth Premature birth/RLF No BA (16)
LB1 M 47 29 Retinitis pigmentosa Yes 3y post-grad (19)
LB2 M 50 Adult Retinitis pigmentosa Yes High school (12)
LB3 M 54 9 Retinoblastoma (enucleated) No MA (18)
LB4 F 50 25 Glaucoma No MA (18)
LB5 M 53 16 Glaucoma No MA (18)
LB6 M 51 11 Accident with chemicals No BA (16)
LB7 F 57 15 Retinoblastoma No MA (18)
LB8 F 26 20 Marfan syndrome Yes BA (16)
LB9 M 40 9 Congenital Cataracts No High school
Sighted 12F 41(19) - - - -

Demographic table describing participants. Abreviations: Retrolental Fibroplasia (RLF), years (y), ducation (Ed), Masters Degree (MA), Bachelors Degree (BA), Congenitally Blind (CB), Late Blind (LB), Female (F), Male (M).

Behavioral Procedures

Participants heard brief verbal passages (<12sec) and answered true or false questions about them (<6sec) in a blocked design (each block containing a single trial). Passages consisted of two to three sentences (Mean 2.43, SD .62, Mean words per sentences 13.35, SD 2.74). Words in the sentences had on average 1.40 syllables (SD .17), a Celex log frequency of 2.12 (SD .25) and an average concreteness of 445 (SD 44). True/false questions always consisted of a single declarative sentence (Number of words 9.76 (SD 2.45), syllables per word 1.42 (SD .21), Freq 2.20 (SD .38), concreteness 440 (SD 112)).

In the control condition, participants performed an auditory match to sample task with backwards speech. Backwards speech is unintelligible and lacks information about sentence structure, word meanings and English phonology. Participants first heard a long segment of backwards speech (<12sec) followed by a short segment of backwards speech (<6sec). They were asked to determine whether the short string of backward speech was a piece of the long string or a new string. On match trials, the short string was spliced from the long backwards speech string. Every trial was followed by 12 seconds of resting baseline.

Stimuli were digitally recorded by a female native English speaker at a sampling rate of 44,100 Hz to produce 32-bit digital sound files. The stories were on average 9.81 sec long, (SD 1.27), -20 RMS amplitude, and the questions were on average 3.02 sec long (SD .78), -19 RMS. Long segments of backwards-speech were created by playing the stories backwards, and short segments were created by playing the questions backwards, for non-matched trials. Long segments of backwards speech were 9.74 sec long (SD 1.27) and -20 RMS in amplitude. Short segments were 3.96 sec long (SD 1.13), -21 RMS. Stories and long backwards speech segments were not different in either duration or amplitude (P>.3). The short segments of backwards speech were longer and louder than the questions (P<.005).

The task was performed in 6 runs with 12 items per run (8 stories and 4 backward-speech). Each run was 6 minutes and 12 seconds long. There were a total of 48 stories and 24 unique segments of backwards speech; each stimulus was heard once during the experiment. Stimuli were presented in different pseudorandom orders across participants. Order of conditions was counterbalanced across participants and runs.

fMRI Data Acquisition and Analysis

MRI structural and functional data of the whole brain were collected on a 3 Tesla Siemens scanner. T1- weighted structural images were collected in 128-axial slices with 1.33 mm isotropic voxels (TR=2 ms, TE=3.39 ms). Functional, blood-oxygenation-level-dependent (BOLD) data were acquired in 3×3×4 mm voxels (TR=2 sec, TE=30 ms), in 30 near-axial slices. The first four seconds of each run were excluded to allow for steady-state magnetization. Data analyses were performed using SPM2 (SPM2 http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/) and Matlab-based in-house software. Prior to modeling, data were realigned to the first image of the first run to correct for head motion, smoothed with a 5 mm smoothing kernel, and normalized to a standard template in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space.

Left and right pericalcarine ROIs were drawn by tracing the calcarine sulcus in each congenitally blind and late blind participant using their individual normalized structural scan. Foveal/parafoveal and peripheral ROIs covered the posterior and anterior portions of the sulcus respectively. In sighted people, foveal/parafoveal pericalcarine cortex represents the foveal/parafoveal portions of the retina and the center of the visual field, whereas the peripheral pericalcarine cortex represents the peripheral portion of the retina and visual field. The peripheral ROI extended from the rostral end of the calcarine sulcus to the middle of the calcarine sulcus, whereas the foveal/parafoveal ROI extended from the midline to the edge of the occipital pole (in the rostral/caudal direction). The dorsal and ventral edges of the peripheral ROI were approximately 5mm above and below the calcarine sulcus respectively. Because the pericalcarine cortex has a larger representation of the fovea, the foveal/parafoveal ROI was 5mm away from the calcarine sulcus at its rostral edge and 12mm at its most caudal edge. In the medial/lateral plane, we traced the calcarine sulcus starting at the most medial slice and moved laterally until the calcarine could not be identified (see Figure 1 for example ROIs). ROIs were also created for a subset (N=10) of the sighted participants. ROI analyses were performed on percent signal change (PSC) from rest, extracted from each ROI. We calculated PSC for every time point after the onset of the trial for each condition [(raw signal during condition - raw signal during resting baseline)/raw signal during resting baseline]. PSC was first calculated in each voxel and then averaged over all the voxels in the ROI. We then separately averaged PSC from all the time-points of the story (4 sec to 16 sec) and question (18 sec to 22 sec).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Light gray: story portion of the trial, dark gray: question portion of the trial. Solid line: sentences, dotted line: backwards speech. Time is on the X-axis in seconds, percent signal change (PSC) is on the Y-axis. Inset displays an example foveal and peripheral pericalcarine ROI from a blind participant. Foveal region is shown in white, peripheral region in dark grey.

In whole-brain analyses, a general-linear model was used to analyze BOLD activity of each subject as a function of condition (sentences and backwards speech). We modeled each block of task as an 18 second event. Covariates of interest were convolved with a standard hemodynamic response function. Nuisance covariates included run effects, an intercept term, and global signal and data high-pass filtered (1 cycle/128 sec). BOLD signal differences between conditions and groups were evaluated through second level, random-effects analysis thresholded at α<.05 (corrected) by performing Monte-Carlo permutation tests on the data (cluster size threshold of 3) using SnPM (Hayasaka & Nichols, 2004; Nichols & Holmes, 2002).

Results

Behavioral Results

Participants were more accurate on the language task (Accuracy: Language 84%, SD=12, Backwards Speech 54%, SD=12, main effect of condition F(1,37)=86.79, P<.0001, group-by- condition interaction (F(2,37)=.56, P=.58). Although performance was poor on the backwards speech task, participants were marginally above chance (t(38)=1.93, P=.06). Responses were slightly slower for the language task (Reaction Time (RT) Language 4354 msec, SD=373; backwards speech 4416 msec, SD=431, main effect of condition F(2,36)=4.61, P=.04, group-by-condition interaction F(2,36)=.33, P=.72). Congenitally blind, late blind and sighted groups did not differ either in accuracy or reaction time (main effect of group Accuracy F(2,37)=.02, P=.98; RT F(2,36)=2.26, P=.11). Behavioral data are summarized in Table 2.

Table 2.

Accuracy % Reaction Time
(msec)
Early Blind
 Backwards Speech 55 (11) 4441 (248)
 Language 84 (14) 4372 (261)

Late Blind
 Backwards Speech 56 (11) 4259 (415)
 Language 82 (10) 4330 (257)

Sighted
 Backwards Speech 52 (13) 4413 (332)
 Language 85 (12) 4257 (279)

Average accuracy and reaction time data from neuroimaging experiment. Standard deviations of the mean are presented in parantheses.

Response in left pericalcarine cortex

We first examined the response of left pericalcarine cortex in congenitally blind, late blind and sighted individuals. To ask whether occipital areas contribute to language comprehension, we examined BOLD signal during the story portion of the trial (Figure 1). In this time-window, the congenitally blind group had a larger response to sentences than backwards speech (Effect of Condition F(1,30)=38.9, p<.0001) and a larger response in the peripheral than foveal ROI (Effect of ROI F(1,30)=4.8, p<.05). By contrast, sentences did not lead to a larger response than backwards speech in either the sighted or late blind groups, nor was there a difference in the response of the foveal/parafoveal versus peripheral ROIs in any group (sighted effect of condition F(1,28)=.85, P=.37, effect of ROI (F(1,28)=1.5, P=.23; late blind effect of condition F(1,25)=1.76, P=.20, effect of ROI F(1,25)=2.47,P=.13).

When the groups were compared directly to each other, the congenitally blind group showed a larger effect of condition (sentences>backwards speech) than the late blind group (group by condition interaction, F(1,54)=11.1, P<.005). This group difference remained significant after duration of blindness was added as a predictor in a multiple regression (group by condition interaction F(1,53)=4.90, P=.03, duration-of-blindness by condition interaction F(1,53)=.06, P=.81). The effect of condition was not different in the late blind and sighted groups (F(1,51)=.3,P=.58). These data suggest that 1) congenitally blind, but not late blind individuals show a language-sensitive response while listening to sentences and 2) the difference between congenitally and late blind people is not accounted for by differences in duration of blindness. During the question portion of the trial, congenitally blind participants still had a higher BOLD signal for sentences than backwards speech in both parts of pericalcarine cortex (condition F(1,30)=20.0, P<.0001, effect of ROI P=.60, and interaction P=.68). By contrast, in the late blind participants the effect of condition was again not reliable (F(1,24)=1.45, P=.24). However, there was a large response during the question portion of the trial both for sentences and for backwards speech in the foveal/parafoveal, but not peripheral ROI (main effect of ROI, F(1,24)=10.2,P<.005). These data suggest that a part of pericalcarine cortex is recruited during task performance in late blind participants, but this effect is not specific to language stimuli.

We then directly compared congenitally blind and late blind participants to each other in a group by condition by ROI ANOVA (during the question portion of the trial). The difference between BOLD signal in the foveal/parafoveal ROI versus the peripheral ROI was larger in the late blind group than the congenitally blind group (group by ROI interaction, (F(1,54)=5.6,P<.05). This analysis suggests that in late blind but not congenitally blind individuals, pericalcarine activity during the question portion of the trial is confined to the foveal/parafoveal region.

The analyses above suggest that late blind participants have a distinct response profile during the story and question portions of the trial. To directly test this hypothesis, we analyzed PSC in the pericalcarine ROI as a function of time in late blind participants. In this group, BOLD signal was higher during the question portion of the trial than during the story portion (main effect of time, F(1,57)=5.9,P<.05). The effect of time was more pronounced in the foveal/parafoveal ROI than in the peripheral ROI (time by ROI interaction, F(1,57)=10.5,P<.005). Furthermore, the effect of time and time by ROI interaction were more pronounced in the late blind than in the congenitally blind group (time by group interaction, F(1,165)=8.7,P<.001; time by group by ROI interaction, F(2,165)=7.0,P=.001). That is, congenitally blind participants showed activity over the whole pericalcarine region, sustained over the whole trial, but specific to linguistic stimuli; by contrast, late blind participants showed activity relatively restricted to the foveal/parafoveal region and restricted to the later phase of the trial, which generalized to both linguistic and non-linguistic stimuli.

Laterality of pericalcarine response to language

Our initial analyses revealed that in congenitally blind participants left pericalcarine cortex responds more to language than backwards speech during the story portion of the trial. We then examined whether this response to language was left-lateralized by comparing PSC in left and right pericalcarine areas. In congenitally blind participants, the sentences>backwards speech difference was larger in left pericalcarine cortex than in right pericalcarine cortex; there was no effect of laterality in the other groups (sentence PSC-backwards speech PSC outcome measure, 2-by-2 ANOVA foveal/parafoveal vs. peripheral by Left vs. Right, effect of hemisphere: congenitally blind F(1,33)=7.13, P=.01, late blind F(1,24)=2.69, P=.14, sighted F(1,27)=2.04, P=.16, Figure S1). There was no difference between foveal and parafoveal cortex in this analysis (P>.3). The laterality of the language effect was larger in congenitally blind than sighted people (group by hemisphere interaction F(1,60)=8.09, P=.006), but did not differ between the congenitally and late blind groups (F(1,57)=.68, P=.41). We also found that the overall response of pericalcarine cortex (collapsing across conditions), was more left-lateralized in congenitally as compared to late blind participants (group by hemisphere interaction (F(1,19)=5.39, P=.03).

Whole-brain analysis

The analyses above suggest that occipital cortex of congenitally blind but not late blind individuals responds specifically to linguistic information. To further test this hypothesis, we used whole-brain analyses to compare the response to sentences and backwards speech in congenitally blind, late blind, and sighted individuals.

We first compared the congenitally blind and late blind groups each to sighted participants. For sentences (relative to backwards speech) congenitally blind individuals had greater recruitment of a left lateralized group of occipital areas including pericalcarine cortex, lateral occipital and fusiform areas, than sighted participants (Figure 2 yellow; Table 3). By contrast, late blind participants did not show any elevated activity compared to the sighted group for sentences (relative to backwards speech). We then compared congenitally and late blind participants directly to each other and found a significantly larger response in congenitally blind participants for sentences in secondary visual areas: left posterior fusiform and left lateral occipital cortex (Figure 2 red, Table 3) Inspection of the time-course in these secondary visual regions revealed that as in pericalcarine cortex, PSC was higher for sentences than backwards speech both during the story and question portions of the trial in congenitally blind participants (Figure S2). Despite increased language-selectivity in occipital cortex of congenitally blind people relative to sighted and late blind participants, we found no evidence of decreased language selectivity in classic frontotemporal language areas in this group (see Supplemental Materials for details).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Group-by-condition interaction results: Congenitally blind > sighted, sentences>backwards speech (yellow), Congenitally blind > late blind, sentences > backwards speech (red), Late blind > sighted, backwards speech>rest (blue). a. The pericalcarine region is visible in sagittal slices from X=-5 to X=5 in MNI coordinates. b. axial slices Z=-22 to Z=6. Note that consistent with the ROI analyses, the response to sentences in congenitally blind participants is left-lateralized within the pericalcarine cortex and extended from the foveal/parafoveal region into peripheral pericalcarine cortex. By contrast, the response to backwards speech in late blind participants was right lateralized and only present in the foveal/parafoveal region of pericalcarine cortex.

Table 3.

Contrast
 Group
k w Pcombo Voxel
peak t
x y z Brain Area (Brodmann Area)
Sentences>Backwards Speech
 Congenitally Blind > Sighted 4680 9.52 0.000 7.98 -38 -74 -12 Left Inferior Occipital Gyrus (19)
772 6.57 0.006 7.54 -30 -60 -14 Left Cerebellum Declive
332 4.40 0.047 7.21 -44 -84 -2 Left Middle Occipital Gyrus (19)
6.26 34 -90 -6 Right Middle Occipital Gyrus (18)
4.91 44 -54 -20 Right Fusiform Gyrus (37)
5.49 48 -48 4 Right Superior Temporal Gyrus (22)
4.25 56 -48 -2 Right Middle Temporal Gyrus (22)

 Late Blind > Sighted
- - - - - - - -

 Congenitally Blind > Late Blind 1006 5.24 0.022 6.14 -36 -54 -20 Left Fusiform/Middle Occipital Gyri (37/19)
5.16 -34 -64 -14 Left Cerebellum/Fusiform Gyrus
4.90 -46 -72 -16 Left Fusiform/Inferior Occipital Gyrus (37/19/18)
Backwards Speech>Rest

 Congenitally Blind > Sighted 728 5.39 0.019 5.05 0 -80 14 Right Cuneus (18)
4.97 10 -68 6 Right Lingual Gyrus (18)
4.93 2 -86 24 Right Cuneus (18)

 Late Blind > Sighted 350 4.04 0.066 4.85 6 -94 6 Right Cuneus (18/17)
4.49 0 -88 0 Right Lingual Gyrus (18/19)
4.27 -4 -92 -8 Right Lingual Gyrus (18/19)

 Congenitally Blind > Late Blind
- - - - - - - -

Peak activations observed in whole-brain analyses (P<.05, Corrected). Data are shown in Figure 3.

While late blind participants did not have a language-sensitive response, they did have an elevated response for backwards speech relative to rest, compared to sighted participants (Figure 2 blue, peak coordinates: [6, -94, 6]; [0, -88, 0]; [-4, -92, -8]). Neuroanatomically, this effect was distinct from the language-sensitive effect observed in congenitally blind participants: it was more pronounced in the right hemisphere and was confined to the foveal/parafoveal region of pericalcarine cortex and the occipital pole (BA17/18). Congenitally and late blind individuals did not differ in the magnitude of occipital response to backwards speech, relative to rest. This analysis suggests that the response-related effect that is common to language and backwards speech is of similar magnitude irrespective of age of blindness onset.

Discussion

Although both congenitally- and late- blind adults show plasticity in occipital cortex, we find that plasticity for language is present only in congenitally blind people. In congenitally blind adults, left occipital cortex is active while listening to speech without making a response, and is more active while listening to speech than while listening to non-language sounds. Language-related activity is left lateralized and occurs in high-level visual areas as well as left pericalcarine cortex (both foveal/parafoveal and peripheral regions). We find a different, non-language related effect, in both congenitally and late blind participants. Right-lateralized occipital areas in the foveal/parafoveal region of pericalcarine cortex and the occipital pole are active when blind individuals make a response or decision, but not while simply listening to sounds. This occipital activity is of similar magnitude for linguistic and non-linguistic tasks and might reflect response preparation or attention.

Our findings of distinct occipital plasticity in late and congenitally blind individuals are consistent with several prior studies. We find that occipital activity during verbal tasks is more extensive and left-lateralized in congenitally blind people. In late blind participants, pericalcarine activity is right-lateralized and confined to the foveal/parafoveal region (Burton et al, 2002a, Burton et al, 2002b, Burton et al, 2003, Buchel et al, 1998, Cheung, Fang, He & Legge, 2009). We extend and clarify prior findings by demonstrating that only in congenitally blind people, a left-lateralized component of the occipital response is specific to language.

Based on the present data we speculate that previously observed occipital activity in late blind people during verbal tasks might reflect response preparation or attention. For example, during verb-generation (as compared to passive listening or Braille reading), occipital activity might be related to generating a verbal response (Burton et al, 2002a, Burton et al, 2002b). Similarly, one study found right pericalcarine activity when late blind participants generated semantically related words, but not when they generated rhyming words, in response to lists of heard nouns. We hypothesize that this occipital activity reflected the greater response preparation or attentional demands of the semantic task. Consistent with this possibility, sighted and blind participants alike manifested higher responses in premotor and prefrontal cortex during the semantic task (Burton et al, 2003). Like the response-associated activity found in the present study, occipital activity of late blind people was right-lateralized and concentrated in the foveal/parafoveal region of pericalcarine cortex.

Together, the available data suggest that during verbal tasks occipital activity in congenitally blind people reflects language processing and response preparation, while occipital activity in late blind people reflects processes that are independent of language. This might explain why occipital activity has distinct functional significance in these populations (Cohen et al., 1999).

How does experience enable occipital areas to participate in language processing?

The present findings suggest that blindness during development is necessary for occipital circuits to get engaged in language. In this regard, our data are consistent with a large body of animal work showing qualitatively different effects of developmental and lifelong experience on the mammalian brain (Berardi, Pizzorusso, & Maffei, 2000). Our findings are also consistent with prior research showing distinct behavioral and neural changes in late and congenitally blind individuals (Bedny, Konkle, Pelphrey, Saxe, & Pascual-Leone, 2011; Buchel et al., 1998; Cohen et al., 1997).

We argue here that the difference between the occipital response of congenitally blind and late blind individuals results from different ages of blindness onset. However, it is important to point out that there are other differences between congenitally blind and late blind populations that could in principle contribute to distinct plasticity patterns. Late blind adults generally have more residual vision than congenitally blind adults. In the present study, it is unlikely that differences in residual vision could explain the observed differences between groups since most of the late and congenitally blind participants had no residual light perception (See Table 1 for details). Late blind participants are also blind for less time than congenitally blind participants. However, in the present study, 8 of the 9 late blind participants were totally blind for 17 years or more (the one remaining participant lost her vision 6 years prior to the study). The average blindness duration, for the late blind adults, was 17 years. It seems unlikely that blindness for even longer than 17 years would qualitatively change the occipital response to language. Furthermore, congenital vs. late blindness, but not duration of blindness predicted the selectivity of left pericalcarine cortex for language in the present study. For these reasons, in the present study the most likely explanation for group differences between late and congenitally blind participants is age of blindness onset.

Our data suggest that blindness during childhood is necessary for a language-sensitive response in occipital cortex, since people who were blind from birth had a language-sensitive response in occipital areas whereas people who became blind later did not. However, the present data do not allow us to precisely delineate the time-window of this ‘sensitive period’ during childhood. Among the late blind participants in our study, the youngest to go blind was 9 years old. In future work it would be very interesting to further explore the timing of the ‘sensitive period’ for the linguistic response in occipital cortex. Does blindness from birth, blindness during early childhood (e.g. 5 years), or blindness during language acquisition, produce a linguistic response? A better understanding of the time-window of this plasticity would help determine whether recruitment of occipital areas for language is limited by plasticity of visual brain regions or by plasticity of the language system itself.

Another factor that might influence occipital reorganization is etiology of blindness (Bridge, Cowey, Ragge, & Watkins, 2009), which varies among late and congenitally blind groups (as well as between individuals within each group). For example, some late blind individuals lose their sight gradually (as in the case of Retinitis pigmentosa), while others have sudden vision loss (e.g. due to an accident). Within the congenitally blind group, etiology of blindness might effect not only postnatal but also prenatal input to the visual cortex. The presence of an intact retina and optic nerve during (specific stages of) fetal development might alter the function of occipital regions (Catalano & Shatz, 1998; Stellwagen & Shatz, 2002). Among late blind participants, disorders of the retina versus optic nerve might have different effects on occipital reorganization. These are important topics for future research.

How does linguistic information reach occipital circuits of congenitally blind participants?

A key question for understanding occipital language responses in blind individuals is how the connectivity of the brain allows linguistic information to reach occipital areas. It seems unlikely that linguistic information comes to occipital circuits from primary auditory cortex (A1) or the middle geniculate nucleus (MGN) because these auditory regions do not have specific linguistic responses. Furthermore, a recent study found language-related occipital activity in a deaf-blind individual. This participant was born deaf and became blind at age 6 due to a chemical accident. In this individual, activity was observed in a left-lateralized network of occipital areas, while he “listened” to haptic ASL (Obretenova, Halko, Plow, Pascual-Leone, & Merabet, 2010). These data suggest that occipital recruitment for language does not depend on input from auditory brain regions. Because this individual was deaf at the time of language acquisition, the results also suggest that the occipital response to language does not originate developmentally from early auditory areas.

Instead, linguistic information may reach occipital areas from language regions in the temporal lobe, prefrontal cortex or language-relevant nuclei of the thalamus (Munte & Kutas, 2008; Radanovic, Azambuja, Mansur, Porto, & Scaff, 2003; Wahl et al., 2008). Consistent with this idea, a number of studies have found enhanced resting-state correlations between left prefrontal language areas and occipital cortex in congenitally blind people (Bedny, et al, 2011, Liu, et al, 2007). One possibility suggested by the present data is that secondary visual areas in the lateral and ventral temporal cortex are a gateway that caries linguistic information to occipital circuits. Secondary visual areas, such as the lateral occipital complex (LO) and the fusiform face area (FFA), are relatively close to language regions of sighted individuals (Figure 3). The inputs that carry language-relevant information to these classic language areas could also carry linguistic information to the nearby extrastriate visual regions. Such connections might exist in all children and be typically pruned away as a result of visual experience. In blind children, these connections may remain intact. Alternatively, nearby temporal-lobe language areas could themselves relay linguistic information to extrastriate visual regions of the fusiform and lateral occipital cortex. Linguistic information might subsequently reach primary visual areas, including the pericalcarine cortex, via these extrastriate areas.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Sighted: sentences>backwards speech (blue), congenitally blind > late blind, sentences>backwards speech (red)

There may also be a more direct route to pericalcarine cortex from classic language areas either in the cortex or the thalamus (Wahl et al., 2008). For example, in non-human primates there are a small number of direct projections from polysensory areas of the lateral temporal lobe to primary visual cortex (Clavagnier, Falchier, & Kennedy, 2004; Falchier, Clavagnier, Barone, & Kennedy, 2002). Further work is needed to clarify the cortical and subcortical circuits that carry linguistic information to occipital areas.

Distinct cognitive processes supported by occipital areas of congenitally and late blind adults

A key finding of the present study is that occipital areas support partially distinct cognitive processes in congenitally and late blind adults. In congenitally blind participants a left-lateralized component of occipital activity is related to language processing. Like prior studies, we find that this response to language is not related to general working memory demands (i.e. non-language specific working memory) (Burton, Sinclair & Dixit, 2010). In the present study, both the language task and the control task (backwards speech) required encoding and maintenance of auditory information, but occipital activity was much higher during the language task. Left-lateralized occipital areas could contribute to a range of language related processes, such as encoding and maintenance of linguistic information, building of sentence structures, and/or retrieving word-meanings from long term memory (see also Bedny et al, 2011, Roder, et al, 2002, Burton et al, 2002, Amedi et al, 2003).

In contrast to congenitally blind people, we did not observe language-related activity in occipital cortex of late blind people. However, a different, non-language related response is present both in congenitally and late blind adults. Occipital activity increased when participants prepared to answer a question. Unlike the occipital responses to language, this activity was right-lateralized and restricted to foveal/parafoveal area of pericalcarine cortex. The magnitude of this response was similar for language and backwards speech. This activity might reflect response preparation, execution or attention. Factors such as task structure, attentional demands, and reward have been shown to influence activity in early visual areas of sighted and blind people (Jack, Shulman, Snyder, McAvoy, & Corbetta, 2006; Serences, 2008, Roder, Rosler, & Hennighausen, 1997; Garg, Schwartz, & Stevens, 2007). In blind people such effects might be enhanced due to absence of bottom up visual input. For example, in blind but not sighted people right occipital activity rises with increased auditory attention (Stevens et al, 2007).

Together with prior evidence, the response-related activity observed in late blind adults illustrates that cross-modal functional change can occur in adulthood (Buchel et al., 1998; Burton, 2003; Burton et al., 2003; Burton et al., 2002). Indeed, some such changes might occur very quickly - in a matter of days (Merabet et al., 2008), hours or even immediately with the changing state of the organism (Anderson, 2011).

Conclusions

We find a sensitive period in the responsiveness of occipital areas to language: the left occipital cortex responds to linguistic information in congenitally but not late blind people. Along with prior evidence, these data suggest that blindness early in life enables left occipital circuits to participate in language processing. In addition, we also observed a neuroanatomically distinct response-related effect in occipital cortex of congenitally and late blind people. Unlike the language-sensitive effect, this response-related occipital activity does not appear to have a sensitive period. In summary, we find that during verbal tasks occipital areas make distinct functional contributions in congenitally and late blind people.

Supplementary Material

01
02
03

Highlights.

Distinct occipital plasticity in congenitally and late blind people.

Growing up blind makes occipital cortex sensitive to language.

Left pericalcarine cortex and secondary visual areas respond to language in congenitally blind people.

Left occipital cortex responds more to sentences than backwards speech in congenitally, but not late blind people.

Right-foveal/parafoveal pericalcarine cortex is active during response preparation in both congenitally and late blind people.

Distinct plasticity in peripheral as opposed to foveal/parafoveal pericalcarine cortex.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank our participants and the New England blind community for making this research possible. We thank the Martinos Imaging Center at MIT for their aid in data collection. Work on this study was supported by a grant to Dr. Rebecca Saxe from the Packard Foundation and grants from the National Institutes of Health, National Eye Institute and National Center for Research Resources: Harvard Clinical and Translational Science Center (UL1 RR025758) to Dr. Alvaro Pascual-Leone.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Amedi A, Floel A, Knecht S, Zohary E, Cohen LG. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the occipital pole interferes with verbal processing in blind subjects. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7(11):1266–1270. doi: 10.1038/nn1328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amedi A, Raz N, Pianka P, Malach R, Zohary E. Early ‘visual’ cortex activation correlates with superior verbal memory performance in the blind. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6(7):758–766. doi: 10.1038/nn1072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson ML. Neural reuse: a fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behav Brain Sci. 2011;33(4):245–266. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X10000853. discussion 266-313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bates E. In The changing nervous system: Neurobehavioral consequences of early brain disorders. Oxford University Press; New York: 1999. Plasticity, Localization and Language Development; pp. 214–253. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bedny M, Konkle T, Pelphrey K, Saxe R, Pascual-Leone A. Sensitive period for a multimodal response in human visual motion area MT/MST. Curr Biol. 2011;20(21):1900–1906. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bedny M, Pascual-Leone A, Dodell-Feder D, Fedorenko E, Saxe R. Language processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(11):4429–4434. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1014818108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Berardi N, Pizzorusso T, Maffei L. Critical periods during sensory development. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2000;10(1):138–145. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(99)00047-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Buchel C, Price C, Frackowiak RS, Friston K. Different activation patterns in the visual cortex of late and congenitally blind subjects. Brain. 1998;121(Pt 3):409–419. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.3.409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bridge H, Cowey A, Ragge N, Watkins K. Imaging studies in congenital anophthalmia reveal preservation of brain architecture in ‘visualg’ cortex. Brain. 2009;132(Pt 12):3467–3480. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Burton H. Visual cortex activity in early and late blind people. J Neurosci. 2003;23(10):4005–4011. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-10-04005.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Burton H, Diamond JB, McDermott KB. Dissociating cortical regions activated by semantic and phonological tasks: a FMRI study in blind and sighted people. J Neurophysiol. 2003;90(3):1965–1982. doi: 10.1152/jn.00279.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Burton H, McLaren DG. Visual cortex activation in late-onset, Braille naive blind individuals: an fMRI study during semantic and phonological tasks with heard words. Neurosci Lett. 2006;392(1-2):38–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.09.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Burton H, Sinclair RJ, Dixit S. Working memory for vibrotactile frequencies: comparison of cortical activity in blind and sighted individuals. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31(11):1686–1701. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Burton H, Snyder AZ, Diamond JB, Raichle ME. Adaptive changes in early and late blind: a FMRI study of verb generation to heard nouns. J Neurophysiol. 2002a;88(6):3359–3371. doi: 10.1152/jn.00129.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Catalano SM, Shatz CJ. Activity-dependent cortical target selection by thalamic axons. Science. 1998;281(5376):559–562. doi: 10.1126/science.281.5376.559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Burton H, Snyder AZ, Conturo TE, Akbudak E, Ollinger JM, Raichle ME. Adaptive changes in early and late blind: a fMRI study of Braille reading. J Neurophysiol. 2002b;87(1):589–607. doi: 10.1152/jn.00285.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chee MW, Caplan D, Soon CS, Sriram N, Tan EW, Thiel T, et al. Processing of visually presented sentences in Mandarin and English studied with fMRI. Neuron. 1999;23(1):127–137. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80759-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cheung SH, Fang F, He S, Legge GE. Retinotopically specific reorganization of visual cortex for tactile pattern recognition. Current Biology. 2009;19(7):596–601. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.02.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Clavagnier S, Falchier A, Kennedy H. Long-distance feedback projections to area V1: implications for multisensory integration, spatial awareness, and visual consciousness. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2004;4(2):117–126. doi: 10.3758/cabn.4.2.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cohen LG, Celnik P, Pascual-Leone A, Corwell B, Falz L, Dambrosia J, et al. Functional relevance of cross-modal plasticity in blind humans. Nature. 1997;389(6647):180–183. doi: 10.1038/38278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cohen LG, Weeks RA, Sadato N, Celnik P, Ishii K, Hallett M. Period of susceptibility for cross-modal plasticity in the blind. Ann Neurol. 1999;45(4):451–460. doi: 10.1002/1531-8249(199904)45:4<451::aid-ana6>3.0.co;2-b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Crair MC, Gillespie DC, Stryker MP. The role of visual experience in the development of columns in cat visual cortex. Science. 1998;279(5350):566–570. doi: 10.1126/science.279.5350.566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fagiolini M, Jensen CL, Champagne FA. Epigenetic influences on brain development and plasticity. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2009;19(2):207–212. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fagiolini M, Pizzorusso T, Berardi N, Domenici L, Maffei L. Functional postnatal development of the rat primary visual cortex and the role of visual experience: dark rearing and monocular deprivation. Vision Res. 1994;34(6):709–720. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90210-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Falchier A, Clavagnier S, Barone P, Kennedy H. Anatomical evidence of multimodal integration in primate striate cortex. J Neurosci. 2002;22(13):5749–5759. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-13-05749.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Garg A, Schwartz D, Stevens AA. Orienting auditory spatial attention engages frontal eye fields and medial occipital cortex in congenitally blind humans. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(10):2307–2321. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gennari F. De Peculiari Structura Cerebri. Ex Regio Typographeo; Parma, Italy: 1782. [Google Scholar]
  28. Hayasaka S, Nichols TE. Combining voxel intensity and cluster extent with permutation test framework. Neuroimage. 2004;23(1):54–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hensch TK. Critical period regulation. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004;27:549–579. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hensch TK. Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6(11):877–888. doi: 10.1038/nrn1787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens. J Physiol. 1970;206(2):419–436. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1970.sp009022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Jack AI, Shulman GL, Snyder AZ, McAvoy M, Corbetta M. Separate modulations of human V1 associated with spatial attention and task structure. Neuron. 2006;51(1):135–147. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Johnson JS, Newport EL. Critical period effects in second language learning: the influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognit Psychol. 1989;21(1):60–99. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kupers R, Pappens M, de Noordhout AM, Schoenen J, Ptito M, Fumal A. rTMS of the occipital cortex abolishes Braille reading and repetition priming in blind subjects. Neurology. 2007;68(9):691–693. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000255958.60530.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lamme VA, Super H, Spekreijse H. Feedforward, horizontal, and feedback processing in the visual cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1998;8(4):529–535. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(98)80042-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Li Y, Fitzpatrick D, White LE. The development of direction selectivity in ferret visual cortex requires early visual experience. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(5):676–681. doi: 10.1038/nn1684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Li Y, Van Hooser SD, Mazurek M, White LE, Fitzpatrick D. Experience with moving visual stimuli drives the early development of cortical direction selectivity. Nature. 2008;456(7224):952–956. doi: 10.1038/nature07417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Liu Y, Yu C, Liang M, Li J, Tian L, Zhou Y, et al. Whole brain functional connectivity in the early blind. Brain. 2007;130(Pt 8):2085–2096. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. MacSweeney M, Capek CM, Campbell R, Woll B. The signing brain: the neurobiology of sign language. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12(11):432–440. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Merabet LB, Battelli L, Obretenova S, Maguire S, Meijer P, Pascual-Leone A. Functional recruitment of visual cortex for sound encoded object identification in the blind. Neuroreport. 2009;20(2):132–138. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32832104dc. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Merabet LB, Hamilton R, Schlaug G, Swisher JD, Kiriakopoulos ET, Pitskel NB, et al. Rapid and reversible recruitment of early visual cortex for touch. PLoS One. 2008;3(8):e3046. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Morishita H, Hensch TK. Critical period revisited: impact on vision. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2008;18(1):101–107. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Munte TF, Kutas M. Capitalizing on deep brain stimulation: thalamus as a language monitor. Neuron. 2008;59(5):677–679. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.08.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Nichols TE, Holmes AP. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Human Brain Mapping. 2002;15(1):1–25. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Obretenova S, Halko MA, Plow EB, Pascual-Leone A, Merabet LB. Neuroplasticity associated with tactile language communication in a deaf-blind subject. Front Hum Neurosci. 2010;3:60. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.060.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Pallier C, Devauchelle AD, Dehaene S. Cortical representation of the constituent structure of sentences. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(6):2522–2527. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018711108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Pinker S. The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language. William Marrow and Company; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  48. Radanovic M, Azambuja M, Mansur LL, Porto CS, Scaff M. Thalamus and language: interface with attention, memory and executive functions. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2003;61(1):34–42. doi: 10.1590/s0004-282x2003000100006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Roder B, Rosler F, Hennighausen E. Different cortical activation patterns in blind and sighted humans during encoding and transformation of haptic images. Psychophysiology. 1997;34(3):292–307. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02400.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Roder B, Rosler F, Neville HJ. Event-related potentials during auditory language processing in congenitally blind and sighted people. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(11):1482–1502. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00057-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Roder B, Stock O, Bien S, Neville H, Rosler F. Speech processing activates visual cortex in congenitally blind humans. Eur J Neurosci. 2002;16(5):930–936. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02147.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Sadato N, Pascual-Leone A, Grafman J, Ibanez V, Deiber MP, Dold G, et al. Activation of the primary visual cortex by Braille reading in blind subjects. Nature. 1996;380(6574):526–528. doi: 10.1038/380526a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Sadato N, Okada T, Honda M, Yonekura Y. Critical period for cross-modal plasticity in blind humans: a functional MRI study. Neuroimage. 2002;16(2):389–400. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Serences JT. Value-based modulations in human visual cortex. Neuron. 2008;60(6):1169–1181. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Smirnakis SM, Brewer AA, Schmid MC, Tolias AS, Schuz A, Augath M, et al. Lack of long-term cortical reorganization after macaque retinal lesions. Nature. 2005;435(7040):300–307. doi: 10.1038/nature03495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Stellwagen D, Shatz CJ. An instructive role for retinal waves in the development of retinogeniculate connectivity. Neuron. 2002;33(3):357–367. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00577-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Stevens AA, Snodgrass M, Schwartz D, Weaver K. Preparatory activity in occipital cortex in early blind humans predicts auditory perceptual performance. J Neurosci. 2007;27(40):10734–10741. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1669-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Uhl F, Franzen P, Lindinger G, Lang W, Deecke L. On the functionality of the visually deprived occipital cortex in early blind persons. Neurosci Lett. 1991;124(2):256–259. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(91)90107-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Wahl M, Marzinzik F, Friederici AD, Hahne A, Kupsch A, Schneider GH, et al. The human thalamus processes syntactic and semantic language violations. Neuron. 2008;59(5):695–707. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Wang BS, Sarnaik R, Cang J. Critical period plasticity matches binocular orientation preference in the visual cortex. Neuron. 2010;65(2):246–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

01
02
03

RESOURCES