Abstract
A large body of evidence, including longitudinal analyses of personality change, suggests that core personality traits are predominantly stable after age 30. To our knowledge, no study has demonstrated changes in personality in healthy adults after an experimentally manipulated discrete event. Intriguingly, double-blind controlled studies have shown that the classic hallucinogen psilocybin occasions personally and spiritually significant mystical experiences that predict long-term changes in behaviors, attitudes and values. In the present report we assessed the effect of psilocybin on changes in the five broad domains of personality - Neuroticism, Extroversion, Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness. Consistent with participant claims of hallucinogen-occasioned increases in aesthetic appreciation, imagination, and creativity, we found significant increases in Openness following a high-dose psilocybin session. In participants who had mystical experiences during their psilocybin session, Openness remained significantly higher than baseline more than one year after the session. The findings suggest a specific role for psilocybin and mystical-type experiences in adult personality change.
Keywords: hallucinogen, mystical experience, openness, personality, psilocybin, psychedelic
Psilocybin and other classic hallucinogens with actions mediated at the 5-HT2A receptor site (Glennon et al., 1984; Nichols, 2004) produce a unique profile of subjective effects including robust changes in perception, cognition, affect, volition, and somaesthesia (Isbell, 1959; Wolbach et al., 1962; Rosenberg et al., 1964). In early trials of hallucinogens administered under supportive conditions, 50 - 80% of participants claimed lasting beneficial changes in personality, values, attitudes and behavior (Metzner and Editors, 1963; McGlothlin and Arnold, 1971). Some of the most frequent subjective reports included greater appreciation of music, art and nature, greater tolerance of others, and increased creativity and imagination (McGlothlin et al., 1967). Consistent with these findings, Studerus et al. (2010) recently reported that nearly 40% of participants in several laboratory studies of psilocybin claimed positive long-term changes in aesthetic experience and in their relationship with the environment (i.e., nature) following their psilocybin sessions.
The long-term positive impact of hallucinogens may depend on their ability to occasion profound insights and mystical-type experiences (Pahnke, 1963; Doblin, 1991). The core features of mystical experience, as defined by Stace (1960) and Hood (2003), are feelings of unity and interconnectedness with all people and things, a sense of sacredness, feelings of peace and joy, a sense of transcending normal time and space, ineffability, and an intuitive belief that the experience is a source of objective truth about the nature of reality. Because such experiences appear to enable individuals to transcend their usual patterns of thinking, feeling, and acting, it is plausible that they could occasion changes in core dimensions of personality. In support of this, a double-blind controlled study by Griffiths et al. (Griffiths et al., 2006; Griffiths et al., 2008) demonstrated that a single psilocybin session occasioned mystical experiences associated with positive changes in behaviors, attitudes and values more than a year later. Moreover, independent ratings from participants’ romantic partners, coworkers, and friends corroborated the first-person reports. These findings suggest fundamental changes in personal concerns, goals, and identity, which are considered to be important dimensions of personality (e.g., in the framework described in McAdams, 1995). However, the impact of psilocybin-occasioned mystical experience on core personality traits is unknown.
There is general agreement that personality traits are relatively enduring styles of thinking, feeling, and acting (McCrae and Costa, 1997). The most widely accepted model of personality structure is the five-factor model, which describes five broad domains of personality each encompassing many related traits: Neuroticism, Extroversion, Openness, Agreeableness and Conscientiousness (Digman, 1990; McCrae, 2009). Many studies have confirmed that these personality factors are heritable (Bouchard et al., 1990) and reliable across cultures, suggesting a universal human personality structure that is rooted in biology (McCrae and Costa, 1997). Longitudinal studies have shown that individual personality is predominantly stable across the lifespan. Although there are mean-level personality changes after age 30, these shifts are typically gradual and subtle (1 - 2 T-score points per decade; Terracciano et al., 2005).
Despite the relatively stable nature of personality, researchers have hypothesized that significant life events could change adult personality quite dramatically. Studies investigating individual differences in personality-trait change have found that certain life experiences are associated with changes in adult personality (see review in Roberts and Mroczek, 2008). For example, personality changes have been associated with divorce (e.g., increases in Extroversion and Openness in women, Costa et al., 2000), remarriage (e.g., decreases in Neuroticism in men, Mroczek and Spiro, 2003), and career success (e.g., decreases in Conscientiousness in individuals who were fired vs. promoted, Costa et al., 2000). However, such correlational studies cannot address the causal link between particular events and subsequent personality change.
Relatively little research has investigated personality change in the laboratory, likely because events that might be expected to change personality are difficult to create under experimental (i.e., randomized or assigned) conditions. Some studies have shown changes in personality after experimental treatment interventions in patients, such as several weeks of antidepressant medication (e.g., Costa et al., 2005) and intensive outpatient counseling for substance abuse rehabilitation (Piedmont, 2001). Personality changes have also been shown in healthy adults after three months of intensive contemplative training in attention- and emotion-regulation (Sahdra et al., 2011). These personality changes, as well as socioemotional and behavioral improvements, were maintained several months after completion of training, suggesting possible long-term benefits.
To our knowledge, no study has prospectively demonstrated personality change in healthy adults after an experimentally manipulated discrete event. Although it has been speculated that treatment with classic hallucinogens could be a method for occasioning dramatic and rapid personality change (Unger, 1964), a controlled study of LSD in healthy volunteers failed to find significant changes in a large battery of empirical measures of personality, aesthetic sensitivity, and creativity six months after LSD sessions (McGlothlin et al., 1967). Nevertheless, these investigators confirmed the subjective claims of long-term change frequently reported in uncontrolled trials.
The subjective claims of hallucinogen-occasioned long-term changes (Metzner and Editors, 1963; McGlothlin et al., 1967; McGlothlin and Arnold, 1971; Studerus et al., 2010) appear to align with the personality construct of Openness, which encompasses aesthetic appreciation and sensitivity, imagination and fantasy, and broad-minded tolerance of others’ viewpoints and values. The present report combines data from two double-blind controlled studies of psilocybin (Griffiths et al., 2006; Griffiths et al., 2011) to analyze changes in Openness and the other four broad personality domains using the NEO Personality Inventory (Costa and McCrae, 1992). Personality change was assessed 1 - 2 months after a high-dose psilocybin session and again more than one year later to determine the persistence of personality change. Consistent with previous results indicating a correlation between mystical experiences during the session and long-term spiritual significance and personal meaning attribution (Griffiths et al., 2008), we hypothesized that mystical experiences during the psilocybin session would lead to enduring increases in Openness.
Methods
Participants
Recruitment methods and screening procedures were similar across the two studies. Participants were recruited from the local community by flyers announcing “a study of states of consciousness brought about by psilocybin, a naturally occurring psychoactive substance used sacramentally in some cultures”. The first phase of screening involved a scripted telephone interview, which was used to pre-screen for the major inclusion/exclusion criteria. Common reasons for not passing the telephone screen included distance from study site, extensive prior hallucinogen use, and inability to make the time commitment required for participation. Individuals who passed the telephone screen (approximately 20% of those screened) were evaluated in-person for inclusion/exclusion criteria and medical and psychiatric health, as described previously for human research with classic hallucinogens (Johnson et al., 2008). The inclusion/exclusion criteria were similar across the two studies, although participants in Study 2 were permitted to have limited prior experience with hallucinogens (Griffiths et al., 2011). The single participant in Study 2 who had prior lifetime experience with psilocybin was excluded from the present analyses. In addition, 1 participant in Study 1 (Griffiths et al., 2006) had incomplete personality data and was excluded. Thus, the present analyses included 52 hallucinogen-naïve participants (Table 1). Participants were generally well educated (54% had post-graduate degrees) and spiritually active (90% regularly participated in religious services, discussion groups, prayer, or meditation).
Table 1.
Study 1 | Study 2 | |
---|---|---|
Age at study entry (years) | 46 (24 – 64) | 46 (29 – 62) |
Sex | 14 M, 21 F | 8 M, 9 F |
Education | 4.2 (2 – 5) | 4.0 (2 – 5) |
Mysticism Scale - Lifetime | 215 (184 – 283) | 216 (124 – 282) |
NEO Personality Inventory | ||
Neuroticism | 41.8 (26.4 - 64.6) | 46.1 (19.3 - 61.3) |
Extroversion | 55.3 (31.3 - 76.4) | 52.1 (34.0 - 78.0) |
Openness | 65.4 (42.1 - 80.3) | 62.7 (45.6 - 83.8) |
Agreeableness | 55.7 (41.0 - 75.1) | 55.1 (37.2 - 72.6) |
Conscientiousness | 51.0 (22.7 - 80.6) | 51.7 (32.9 - 72.1) |
Note: The table presents mean values (with ranges in parentheses) for hallucinogen-naïve participants with complete personality data (n = 35 in Study 1; n = 17 in Study 2). Educational achievement was scored on the following scale: 1 = high school diploma; 2 = some college; 3 = college degree; 4 = some graduate study; 5 = graduate degree. Mystical experiences across the lifetime were quantified as total score on the Hood Mysticism Scale at screening (Hood et al., 2001). Personality was assessed using a computer-based version of the revised NEO Personality Inventory (Costa and McCrae, 1992). No significant study-group differences in demographic or personality variables were evident at screening (ps > .10).
Participants did not receive monetary compensation but generally reported being motivated by curiosity about the effects of psilocybin and the opportunity for extensive self-reflection. The Institutional Review Board of the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine approved the studies, and all participants gave their informed consent before participation.
Psilocybin sessions
Dose conditions
Participants completed two to five 8-hour drug sessions, with consecutive sessions separated by at least 3 weeks. Participants were informed that they would receive a “moderate or high dose” of psilocybin during one of their drug sessions, but neither the participant nor the session monitors knew when this session would occur. In Study 1 (Griffiths et al., 2006), each participant received psilocybin (30 mg/70 kg body weight) on one session and an active control drug, methylphenidate (40 mg/70 kg), on the other one or two sessions. In Study 2 (Griffiths et al., 2011), each participant received four doses of psilocybin (5, 10, 20 and 30 mg/70 kg) in ascending or descending order (50% random assignment) across four sessions, with a fifth placebo session quasi-randomly inserted in the sequence. In the present report, we analyzed mystical experiences and changes in personality associated with the high-dose (30 mg/70 kg) psilocybin session.
Session protocol
Participants received extensive preparation and met with session monitors for at least eight hours on several different occasions prior to their first drug session, in line with recommendations for hallucinogen research (Johnson et al., 2008). The drug was orally administered in capsule form with 100 mL water at the beginning of each session. During the session, participants were encouraged to lie down on a couch, use an eye mask to block external visual distraction, wear headphones through which a music program was played, and focus their attention on their inner experiences.
Measures
NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI)
Personality was assessed at screening, 1 - 2 months after each drug session, and approximately 14 months after the last drug session using a computer-based version of the revised NEO-PI (Costa and McCrae, 1992). This 240-item instrument measures the five factors of personality and the six facets that define each factor. The facets of Openness are Fantasy (e.g., “I have a very active imagination”), Aesthetics (e.g., “I am intrigued by patterns I find in art and nature”), Feelings (e.g., “I experience a wide range of emotions and feelings”), Ideas (e.g., “I often enjoy playing with theories or abstract ideas”), Values (e.g., “I consider myself broad-minded and tolerant of other people’s lifestyles”), and Actions (e.g., “I think it’s interesting to learn and develop new hobbies”). Items were answered on a 5-point scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree, and raw scores were standardized as T scores (M = 50, SD = 10) using the combined-sex norms reported in the NEO PI-R manual.
States of Consciousness Questionnaire (SOCQ)
Mystical-type phenomena during the drug sessions were assessed using 43 mystical-experience items embedded within a 100-item instrument, the States of Consciousness Questionnaire (SOCQ), which assesses phenomenological content during altered states of consciousness (Griffiths et al., 2006). The mystical-experience items have been shown to be sensitive to the effects of psilocybin (Pahnke, 1963; Doblin, 1991; Griffiths et al., 2006; Griffiths et al., 2011) and other hallucinogens (Turek et al., 1974; Richards et al., 1977). The SOCQ was administered after each session (approximately 7 hours after capsule administration). Items were answered on a 6-point scale ranging from 0 (none, not at all) to 5 (extreme, more than ever before in my life and stronger than 4). Data on each of the following six subscales were expressed as percentage of maximum possible score: unity; transcendence of time and space; ineffability and paradoxicality; sacredness; noetic quality; and positive mood. Consistent with previous research (Griffiths et al., 2006), a “complete mystical experience” was defined as having a score of 60% or higher on each subscale. A total score was also calculated as the mean across all items in the six subscales.
Mysticism Scale
The Mysticism Scale was originally developed to assess mystical experiences across the lifetime and has been validated in several studies, including cross-culturally (Hood, 1975; Hood et al., 2001; Hood, 2003). Items were answered on a 9-point scale ranging from −4 (this description is extremely not true of my own experience) to +4 (this description is extremely true of my own experience). Total score (sum across 32 items) was used to quantify lifetime mystical experiences at screening (see Table 1). Additionally, a modified version of the Mysticism Scale was used to assess mystical effects during the drug sessions. Participants completed the experience-specific version approximately 7 hours after capsule administration and answered the items in reference to their experiences since receiving the capsule that morning.
APZ
The APZ is a 72-item yes/no questionnaire designed to assess altered states of consciousness, including those produced by hallucinogens (Dittrich, 1998). The three scales on the APZ are OSE (“oceanic boundlessness”; this scale assesses phenomena such as unity, bliss, and transcendence of time and space); VUS (“visionary restructuralization”; this scale assesses visual pseudo-hallucinations, illusions, and synaesthesias); and AIA (“dread of ego dissolution”; this scale assesses dysphoric feelings such as anxiety, fear, and feeling trapped). Data on each scale were expressed as percentage of maximum possible score.
Analysis
We combined data from two double-blind psilocybin studies to achieve greater statistical power to detect personality change at the group level and to examine personality change in sub-groups of individuals who differed in mystical-type psilocybin effects. Because Study 2 involved multiple doses of psilocybin in addition to the single high dose used in Study 1, it was not possible to completely isolate the high-dose session in Study 2. However, the session protocol implemented in Study 2 was identical to that used in Study 1. Furthermore, changes in personality following the high-dose session were always assessed within 1 - 2 months of the high-dose session and before any subsequent session.
We hypothesized that mystical experiences during the psilocybin session, as measured by the SOCQ, would lead to increases in Openness. We investigated this prediction using three analysis approaches (all analyses were conducted in SPSS version 18.0). First, we used multivariate repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) to analyze changes in Openness and the other four NEO-PI factors (Neuroticism, Extroversion, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness) from screening to post-test (1 - 2 months after the high-dose psilocybin session). After confirming that Openness changed significantly following the high-dose session, we calculated Pearson’s linear correlations between changes in Openness and session-day measures (the SOCQ, the Mysticism Scale, the OSE, the VUS, and the AIA). We then confirmed via regression analysis that mystical experience, as measured by the SOCQ, was a significant predictor of changes in Openness. Specifically, we conducted a series of linear regression analyses that tested the predictive power of the SOCQ above and beyond the predictive power of the four questionnaire measures that also correlated with Openness change (the Mysticism Scale, the OSE, the VUS, and the AIA). In each of four models (one model for each questionnaire), we tested the following predictors of Openness at post-test in a step-wise manner: 1) Openness at screening, 2) an alternate questionnaire measure (e.g., total score on the Mysticism Scale), and 3) mystical score on the SOCQ. We hypothesized that mystical score on the SOCQ (Step 3) would explain significantly more variability in Openness than each of the alternate questionnaire measures (Step 2), as indicated by significant estimates of R-squared change from Step 2 to Step 3.
Finally, we compared Openness change in participants who met the criteria for having had a complete mystical experience during their high-dose session (n = 30) to participants who did not meet the criteria (n = 22). We used the “complete mystical experience” classification (60% on each subscale of the SOCQ) rather than data-driven classification (e.g., median split) for several reasons. First, median split analyses have been associated with reductions of power and effect size (MacCallum et al., 2002). Second, classification based on subscale scores takes into account the breadth of mystical-type effects, which total score does not. For example, a participant could have a high total score while not endorsing many sacredness items. Finally, designating participants who met the criteria for a complete mystical experience allowed a comparison with previous studies (Griffiths et al., 2008; Griffiths et al., 2006; Pahnke, 1963; Richards et al., 1977; Griffiths et al., 2011).
Results
Changes in personality following the psilocybin session
Consistent with our hypothesis, the repeated measures ANOVA showed that Openness increased significantly from screening to post-test (M = +2.8, F(1, 51) = 5.47, p = .023, ηp2 = .10). Moreover, because participants received the high dose on different sessions, it was possible to conduct a comparison analysis of changes in Openness following the first session in the subgroup of participants who did not receive a high dose of psilocybin on that session (n = 30 participants received inactive placebo, methylphenidate, or 5 mg/70 kg of psilocybin on their first session). There were no significant changes in Openness after the first session in this subgroup (F(1, 29) = 0.52, p = .48). Finally, with regard to changes in the other four personality factors after the high-dose session, there were no significant changes from screening to post-test in Neuroticism (M = −0.5; F(1, 51) = 0.24, p = .62), Extroversion (M = +0.8; F(1, 51) = 0.79, p = .38), Agreeableness (M = +1.7; F(1, 51) = 2.31, p = .16), or Conscientiousness (M = −0.9; F(1, 51) = 1.22, p = .28), and test-retest correlations were high across assessments (rs = .61 - .84). Taken together, these results indicate predominantly stable personality traits with specific increases in Openness following the high-dose session.
Relationship between session-day effects and changes in Openness
We hypothesized that individual differences in mystical experience during the psilocybin session, as measured by the SOCQ, would be related to individual differences in Openness change. There was considerable variability in mystical-type effects during the high-dose session, with nearly the full range of possible scores (range: 0.03 - 1.00) represented across the six subscales of mystical experience. Consistent with our hypothesis, mystical experience (mean score across all of the items in the six subscales) correlated significantly with increases in Openness (r = .42, p = .002; Figure 1). Importantly, mystical experience did not correlate significantly with screening levels of Openness (r = .12, p = .41) or its facets; the highest correlation was with Feelings (r = .24, p = .09).
Changes in Openness were also correlated with scores on the Mysticism Scale (r = .33, p = .017), the OSE (r = .44, p = .001), the VUS (r = .41, p = .003), and the AIA (r = .31, p = .024). The significant relationships shown by Pearson’s linear correlations were also shown by Spearman’s rank order correlations between change in Openness and the SOCQ (rs = .47, p < .001), the Mysticism Scale (rs = .40, p = .003), the OSE (rs = .45, p = .001), the VUS (rs = .47, p < .001), and the AIA (rs = .33, p = .018). However, a series of stepwise regression analyses showed that the SOCQ explained significantly more variability in Openness change than the Mysticism Scale (R-squared change = .046, p = .033), the OSE (R-squared change = .047, p = .031), the VUS (R-squared change = .042, p = .036), and the AIA (R-squared change = .12, p = .001).
Effect of mystical experience on changes in openness
We further examined the relationship between mystical experience and increases in Openness by comparing participants who met the criteria for having had a complete mystical experience during their high-dose session (n = 30) to participants who did not meet the criteria (n = 22). ANOVA revealed a significant interaction between time (screening vs. post-test) and group (complete mystical experience vs. no complete mystical experience) (F(1, 50) = 9.32, p = .004, ηp2 = .16). Follow-up t tests confirmed that the complete mystical experience group showed significant increases in Openness from screening to post-test (M = +5.7; t(29) = 3.44, p = .002) while the other group did not (M = −1.2; t(21) = .87, p = .39) (Figure 2). Five of the six facets of Openness showed this same pattern when examined individually: The interaction between time and group was significant for Fantasy (p = .029, ηp2 = .09), Aesthetics (p = .010, ηp2 = .13), Feelings (p = .038, ηp2 = .08), Ideas (p = .014, ηp2 = .11), and Values (p = .036, ηp2 = .09), but not Actions (p = .19). Follow-up t tests for the five facets showing significance confirmed that the complete mystical experience group increased in these facets while the other group did not (Figure 3).
Follow-up
The persistence of increases in Openness was assessed at a follow-up assessment conducted more than one year after the high-dose session (M = 16 months). In participants who had a complete mystical experience (n = 30), Openness levels at follow-up decreased slightly from post-test but did not differ significantly from Openness levels at post-test (p = .12) and remained significantly higher than at screening (M = +4.2; F(1, 29) = 4.17, p = .050, ηp2 = .13). In contrast, participants who did not have a complete mystical experience (n = 21) had nearly identical Openness levels at follow-up and screening (M = −0.3; p = .86).1, 2
Discussion
We observed significant increases in Openness after a high-dose psilocybin session that were larger in magnitude than changes in personality typically observed in healthy adults over decades of life experience (Terracciano et al., 2005). Moreover, in participants who met criteria for having had a complete mystical experience during their psilocybin session, Openness remained significantly higher than baseline more than one year after the session. This is the first study to demonstrate changes in personality in healthy adults after an experimentally manipulated discrete event. To our knowledge, the only other experimental (i.e., randomized) intervention reported to change healthy adults’ personality involved hundreds of hours of solitary meditation over the course of three months (Sahdra et al., 2011). The present findings are suggestive of lasting change in core personality traits. However, it will be important to replicate these results in larger groups of individuals with diverse baseline personality profiles.
It is informative to compare the magnitude of Openness change observed in the present study with previous findings of longitudinal personality change. During normal aging, Openness typically decreases linearly at a rate of approximately 1 T-score point per decade (see Figure 4 in Terracciano et al., 2005). In comparison, participants in the present study who had a complete mystical experience during their psilocybin session increased more than 4 T-score points from screening to follow-up. Notably, this increase is larger than increases in Openness seen in individuals treated successfully with antidepressant medication (see Figure 1 in Costa et al., 2005) and intensive outpatient counseling for substance abuse (see Table 1 in Piedmont, 2001).
Openness includes a relatively broad range of intercorrelated traits covering aesthetic appreciation and sensitivity, fantasy and imagination, awareness of feelings in self and others, and intellectual engagement. People with high levels of Openness are “permeable to new ideas and experiences” and “motivated to enlarge their experience into novel territory” (DeYoung et al., 2005). Openness is strongly associated with creativity (Silvia et al., 2009), and some of its facets (Ideas, Values) are correlated with general fluid intelligence and cognitive ability (DeYoung et al., 2005; DeYoung et al., 2009). Although the present study did not directly investigate the corollary benefits of increased Openness, significant increases in nearly all of the facet scores indicate the potential for improvements in aesthetic and cognitive domains.
Very few controlled studies have attempted to characterize the effects of classic hallucinogens on aesthetic and creative outcomes. One previous well-controlled study of LSD (McGlothlin et al., 1967) found evidence for short-term improvements on measures of artistic ability (e.g., figure drawings), aesthetic sensitivity (e.g., compatibility between participant and expert ratings of paintings), and creativity (e.g., generating alternate uses for a common object). However, McGlothin and colleagues failed to find significant improvements in creativity or aesthetic measures at a 6-month follow-up. Notably, the McGlothlin study included graduate students who were paid for participation, generally showed little interest in LSD, and received minimal preparation and support for the LSD sessions. In contrast, the present study included unpaid community volunteers who were motivated by curiosity about the effects of psilocybin and received extensive interpersonal preparation and support for the psilocybin sessions. It is likely that differences in participant motivation and preparation contributed to different long-term outcomes following hallucinogen sessions. In addition, nearly all of the participants in the present study regularly engaged in spiritual activities such as religious services, prayer, and meditation. It is possible that such individuals are particularly sensitive to the mystical-type effects of psilocybin, which were predictive of increases in Openness.
Cross-sectional methods have been used to examine differences in personality, attitudes and values between regular hallucinogen users and controls. A study in Brazil by Grob et al. (1996) found that individuals who used the hallucinogenic plant ayahuasca as a spiritual sacrament had lower trait levels of excitability, disorderliness and impulsivity, and higher trait levels of confidence, gregariousness and optimism compared to community members who did not use ayahuasca. Lerner & Lyvers (2006) found that individuals who used hallucinogens valued spirituality, concern for others, concern for the environment (i.e., nature), and creativity more highly than individuals who used other illicit drugs (e.g., marijuana, amphetamine, heroin). Although the findings are suggestive of personality changes that might be associated with hallucinogen exposure, it is not possible to isolate the effects of hallucinogens per se because of the self-selection bias that may confound results of cross-sectional studies. Longitudinal studies will be required to replicate hallucinogen-related changes in personality, attitudes, and values.
The NEO-PI is one of the most widely used measures of personality in modern psychology. However, it is useful to consider alternate methods of classifying personality. For example, the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) measures personality as a combination of temperament or automatic responses to emotional stimuli and character or self-aware concepts that are associated with voluntary intentions and attitudes (see review in Cloninger, 1994). The TCI scales are considered distinct from the factors in the NEO-PI. However, a factor analysis showed that TCI scales loaded on the five NEO-PI factors (McCrae et al., 2001). In particular, the Self-Transcendence scale of the TCI loaded on Openness, effectively representing “openness to spiritual experience” (McCrae, 2009: 154). Given the connection between mystical experience and increases in Openness observed in the present study, it is likely that Self-Transcendence might also increase following a high-dose psilocybin session.
In contrast to the numerous questionnaires available for assessing personality, very few questionnaire measures have been developed to assess mystical experiences. Despite the generally infrequent use of the SOCQ, it has been used effectively along with the more widely used and validated Mysticism Scale (Hood, 2003) to characterize hallucinogen effects (Griffiths et al., 2008; Griffiths et al., 2006; Griffiths et al., 2011). Although a psychometric validation of the SOCQ has not been published, data from a study in our laboratory involving several hundred respondents indicate that the mystical items in the SOCQ have a reliable factor structure and that the factor scores differentiate self-reported mystical experiences from otherwise profound positive experiences. In the present study, the SOCQ was a better predictor of changes in Openness than other questionnaire measures of mystical effects (e.g., the Mysticism Scale) and positively experienced hallucinogen effects (e.g., the OSE). Overall, these results suggest a specific role for the SOCQ in quantifying mystical experiences and predicting personality change, although it will be necessary to continue to examine the sensitivity and validity of the SOCQ in characterizing the mystical-type effects of psilocybin and other classic hallucinogens.
The findings have important implications for the application of psilocybin and classic hallucinogens in clinical psychiatry. Recent reviews have suggested that hallucinogens could have positive effects on the outcomes of clinical disorders, particularly anxiety and mood disorders (Griffiths and Grob, 2010; Vollenweider and Kometer, 2010). Clinical work along these lines was initiated in the 1950s and suggested possible improvements after hallucinogen treatment in individuals with terminal cancer (Kast, 1967; Richards et al., 1977; Grob et al., 2011) and substance dependence (Mangini, 1998; Halpern, 1996; Terrill et al., 1964). Although there is still much to learn about the mechanisms by which hallucinogens might produce positive changes, the increases in Openness observed in the present study indicate novel avenues of future research into the biological and psychological mechanisms of psilocybin treatment.
It will also be important in future treatment studies to examine the effects of psilocybin and mystical experience on changes in maladaptive personality traits. For example, although we did not observe decreases in mean Neuroticism (feeling tense, anxious, moody, or insecure), which was nearly 1 SD below normal at baseline, reductions in Neuroticism have been observed in patients undergoing treatment for depression (Costa et al., 2005) and substance abuse (Piedmont, 2001). Similarly, reductions in Neuroticism might accompany reductions in anxiety and depressed mood following psilocybin treatment in patient populations (e.g., individuals with advanced-stage cancer; Grob et al., 2011). In addition, although the present study focused on personality change following a single high-dose session, it is possible that multiple sessions would be associated with changes in additional domains besides Openness.
In conclusion, the present results indicate that a high dose of psilocybin administered under supportive conditions produced increases in Openness, consistent with participants’ subjective claims of the long-term effects of hallucinogens. Importantly, participants who had a complete mystical experience during their high-dose session, but not others, showed enduring increases in Openness, suggesting that other mystical experiences could occasion similar change. Qualitative research has documented sudden and dramatic positive changes in attitudes, values and behaviors following spontaneous mystical or spiritual experiences (Miller, 2004; Paloutzian et al., 1999). Further, correlational research has highlighted a role for spirituality in addiction recovery (Kelly et al., 2011) and in coping with life-threatening illnesses such as cancer (McCoubrie and Davies, 2006), which may account for the potential efficacy of psilocybin in the aforementioned treatment of addictions and cancer distress. Future research should continue to investigate the conditions - both pharmacological and non-pharmacological - that might occasion mystical experiences, as well as describe their potential long-term consequences.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by grants from NIDA (R01DA03889; T32DA007209), the Council on Spiritual Practices, Heffter Research Institute, and the Betsy Gordon Foundation. We thank Margaret Klinedinst, Samantha Gebhart and Ashley Bathgate for data management; as well as the authors and contributors to the two original studies upon which this analysis was based.
Funding: This research was supported by grants from NIDA (R01DA03889; T32DA007209), the Council on Spiritual Practices, the Heffter Research Institute, and the Betsy Gordon Foundation.
Footnotes
The overall pattern of results was similar between Study 1 and Study 2. In participants who had a complete mystical experience, there were no differences between the studies in Openness change (p = .11) or in Openness at screening (p = .28), post-test (p = .69) or follow-up (p = .36).
Three participants in Study 2 met the criteria for a complete mystical experience during their 20 mg/70 kg (i.e., moderately high dose) psilocybin session but not during their 30 mg/70 kg (i.e., high dose) session. Including these participants in the “complete mystical experience” group (n = 33) did not change the reported results.
Contributor Information
Katherine A. MacLean, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, 5510 Nathan Shock Drive, Baltimore MD 21224-6823, USA
Matthew W. Johnson, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, 5510 Nathan Shock Drive, Baltimore MD 21224-6823, USA
Roland R. Griffiths, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, 5510 Nathan Shock Drive, Baltimore MD 21224-6823, USA; Department of Neuroscience, 5510 Nathan Shock Drive, Baltimore MD 21224-6823, USA
References
- Bouchard TJ, Jr., Lykken DT, McGue M, et al. Sources of human psychological differences: the Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart. Science. 1990;250:223–228. doi: 10.1126/science.2218526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cloninger CR. Temperament and personality. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1994;4:266–273. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(94)90083-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa PT, Jr., Bagby RM, Herbst JH, et al. Personality self-reports are concurrently reliable and valid during acute depressive episodes. J Affect Disord. 2005;89:45–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa PT, Jr., Herbst JH, McCrae RR, et al. Personality at midlife: stability, intrinsic maturation, and response to life events. Assessment. 2000;7:365–378. doi: 10.1177/107319110000700405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa PT, Jr., McCrae RR. Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Professional Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources; Odessa, FL: 1992. [Google Scholar]
- DeYoung CG, Peterson JB, Higgins DM. Sources of openness/intellect: cognitive and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality. J Pers. 2005;73:825–858. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00330.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeYoung CG, Shamosh NA, Green AE, et al. Intellect as distinct from Openness: differences revealed by fMRI of working memory. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2009;97:883–892. doi: 10.1037/a0016615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Digman JM. Personality structure: emergence of the five-factor model. Annu Rev Psychol. 1990;41:417–440. [Google Scholar]
- Dittrich A. The standardized psychometric assessment of altered states of consciousness (ASCs) in humans. Pharmacopsychiatry. 1998;31(Suppl 2):80–84. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-979351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doblin R. Pahnke’s good friday experiment: a long-term follow-up and methodological critique. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology. 1991;23:1–28. [Google Scholar]
- Glennon RA, Titeler M, McKenney D. Evidence for the 5-HT2 involvement in the mechanism of action of hallucinogenic agents. Life Sci. 1984;35:2505–2511. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(84)90436-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths RR, Grob CS. Hallucinogens as medicine. Scientific American. 2010 Dec;:77–79. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths RR, Johnson MW, Richards WA, et al. Psilocybin occasioned mystical-type experiences: immediate and persisting dose-related effects. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2011 doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2358-5. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths RR, Richards WA, Johnson MW, et al. Mystical-type experiences occasioned by psilocybin mediate the attribution of personal meaning and spiritual significance 14 months later. J Psychopharmacol. 2008 doi: 10.1177/0269881108094300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths RR, Richards WA, McCann U, et al. Psilocybin can occasion mystical-type experiences having substantial and sustained personal meaning and spiritual significance. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;187:268–283. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0457-5. discussion 284-292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grob CS, Danforth AL, Chopra GS, et al. Pilot study of psilocybin treatment for anxiety in patients with advanced-stage cancer. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68:71–78. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grob CS, McKenna DJ, Callaway JC, et al. Human psychopharmacology of hoasca, a plant hallucinogen used in ritual context in Brazil. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1996;184:86–94. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199602000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halpern JH. The use of hallucinogens in the treatment of addiction. Addict Res. 1996;4:177–189. [Google Scholar]
- Hood RW. The construction and preliminary validation of a measure of reported mystical experience. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion. 1975;14:29–41. [Google Scholar]
- Hood RW. Conceptual and empirical consequences of the unity thesis. In: Belzen JA, Geels A, editors. Mysticism: A Variety of Psychological Perspectives. Rodopi; New York: 2003. pp. 17–54. [Google Scholar]
- Hood RW, Ghorbani N, Watson PJ, et al. Dimensions of the mysticism scale: confirming the three-factor structure in the United States and Iran. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion. 2001;40:691–705. [Google Scholar]
- Isbell H. Comparison of the reactions induced by psilocybin and LSD-25 in man. Psychopharmacologia. 1959;1:29–38. doi: 10.1007/BF00408109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson M, Richards W, Griffiths R. Human hallucinogen research: guidelines for safety. J Psychopharmacol. 2008;22:603–620. doi: 10.1177/0269881108093587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kast EC. Attenuation of anticipation: a therapeutic use of lysergic acid diethylamide. Psychiatry Quarterly. 1967;41:646–657. doi: 10.1007/BF01575629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly JF, Stout RL, Magill M, et al. Spirituality in recovery: a lagged mediational analysis of Alcoholics Anonymous’ principle theoretical mechanism of behavior change. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2011;35:454–463. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01362.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lerner M, Lyvers M. Values and beliefs of psychedelic drug users: a cross-cultural study. J Psychoactive Drugs. 2006;38:143–147. doi: 10.1080/02791072.2006.10399838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacCallum RC, Zhang S, Preacher KJ, et al. On the practice of dichotimization of quantitative variables. Psychological Methods. 2002;7:19–40. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mangini M. Treatment of alcoholism using psychedelic drugs: a review of the program of research. J Psychoactive Drugs. 1998;30:381–418. doi: 10.1080/02791072.1998.10399714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McAdams DP. What do we know when we know a person? J Pers. 1995;63:365–396. [Google Scholar]
- McCoubrie RC, Davies AN. Is there a correlation between spirituality and anxiety and depression in patients with advanced cancer? Supportive Care in Cancer. 2006;14:379–385. doi: 10.1007/s00520-005-0892-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCrae RR. The five-factor model of personality traits: consensus and controversy. In: Corr PJ, Matthews G, editors. The Cambridge Handbook of Personality Psychology. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 2009. [Google Scholar]
- McCrae RR, Costa PT., Jr. Personality trait structure as a human universal. Am Psychol. 1997;52:509–516. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.52.5.509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCrae RR, Herbst JH, Costa PT., Jr. Effects of acquiescence on personality factor structures. In: Riemann R, Ostendorf F, Spinath F, editors. Personality and Temperament: Genetics, Evolution, and Structure. Pabst Science Publishers; Berlin: 2001. pp. 217–231. [Google Scholar]
- McGlothlin WH, Arnold DO. LSD revisited: a ten-year follow-up of medical LSD use. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1971;24:35–49. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1971.01750070037005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGlothlin WH, Cohen S, McGlothlin MS. Long lasting effects of LSD on normals. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1967;17:521–532. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1967.01730290009002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Metzner R, editor. The subjective after-effects of psychedelic experiences: a summary of four recent questionnaire studies. Psychedelic Review. 1963;1:18–26. [Google Scholar]
- Miller WR. The phenomenon of quantum change. J Clin Psychol. 2004;60:453–460. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mroczek DK, Spiro AI. Modeling intraindividual change in personality traits: findings from the Normative Aging Study. Journals of Gerontology B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2003;58:153–165. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.3.p153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nichols DE. Hallucinogens. Pharmacol Ther. 2004;101:131–181. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2003.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pahnke WN. Drugs and mysticism: an analysis of the relationship between psychedelic drugs and the mystical consciousness. Harvard University; Boston: 1963. [Google Scholar]
- Paloutzian RF, Richardson JT, Rambo LR. Religious conversion and personality change. J Pers. 1999;67:1047–1079. [Google Scholar]
- Piedmont RL. Cracking the plaster cast: Big Five personality change during intensive outpatient counseling. Journal of Research in Personality. 2001;35:500–520. [Google Scholar]
- Richards WA, Rhead JC, DiLeo FB, et al. The peak experience variable in DPT-assisted psychotherapy with cancer patients. Journal of Psychedelic Drugs. 1977;9:1–10. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts BW, Mroczek DK. Personality trait change in adulthood. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2008;17:31–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00543.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenberg DE, Isbell H, Miner EJ, et al. The effect of N, N-Dimethyltryptamine in human subjects tolerant to lyseric acid diethylamide. Psychopharmacologia. 1964;5:217–227. doi: 10.1007/BF00413244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sahdra BK, MacLean KA, Shaver PR, et al. Enhanced response inhibition during intensive meditation training predicts improvements in self-reported adaptive socioemotional functioning. Emotion. 2011;11:299–312. doi: 10.1037/a0022764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silvia PJ, Nusbaum EC, Berg C, et al. Openness to experience, plasticity, and creativity: exploring lower-order, higher-order, and interactive effects. Journal of Research in Personality. 2009;43:1087–1090. [Google Scholar]
- Stace WT. Mysticism and Philosophy. Lipincott; Philadelphia: 1960. [Google Scholar]
- Studerus E, Kometer M, Hasler F, et al. Acute, subacute and long-term subjective effects of psilocybin in healthy humans: a pooled analysis of experimental studies. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2010 doi: 10.1177/0269881110382466. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terracciano A, McCrae RR, Brant LJ, et al. Hierarchical linear modeling analyses of the NEO-PI-R scales in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Psychol Aging. 2005;20:493–506. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.3.493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terrill J, Savage C, Jackson DD. LSD, transcendence, and the new beginning. In: Solomon D, editor. LSD: The Consciousness-expanding Drug. G. P. Putnam’s Sons; New York: 1964. [Google Scholar]
- Turek IS, Soskin RA, Kurland AA. Methylenedioxyamphetamine (MDA) subjective effects. Journal of Psychedelic Drugs. 1974;6:7–14. [Google Scholar]
- Unger SM. Mescaline, LSD, psilocybin and personality change. In: Solomon D, editor. LSD: The Consciousness-expanding Drug. G. P. Putnam’s Sons; New York: 1964. [Google Scholar]
- Vollenweider FX, Kometer M. The neurobiology of psychedelic drugs: implications for the treatment of mood disorders. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:642–651. doi: 10.1038/nrn2884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolbach AB, Miner EJ, Isbell H. Comparison of psilocin with psilocybin, mescaline and LSD-25. Psychopharmacologia. 1962;3:219–223. doi: 10.1007/BF00412109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]