Abstract
We evaluated the differential effects of 2 variations of a stimulus–stimulus pairing procedure on the vocalizations of 2 children with autism. For both participants, presenting 1 sound per pairing trial resulted in a higher rate of vocalizations than 3 sounds per pairing trial.
Key words: autism, stimulus–stimulus pairing, vocalizations
Stimulus–stimulus pairing (SSP) of vocalizations pairs the speech of others with the delivery of highly preferred items. The goal of this procedure is to produce a temporary increase in vocalizations, thus creating a larger variety of sounds that can subsequently be brought under appropriate stimulus control (Esch, Carr, & Grow, 2009). To date, 12 studies have evaluated SSP of vocalizations. Nine studies demonstrated a temporary increase in at least one target vocalization with 19 of the 25 participants (Carroll & Klatt, 2008; Esch et al., 2009; Miguel, Carr, & Michael, 2002; Rader et al., in press; Smith, Michael, & Sundberg, 1996; Sundberg, Michael, Partington, & Sundberg, 1996; Ward, Osnes, & Partington, 2007; Yoon & Bennett, 2000; Yoon & Feliciano, 2007). Three studies failed to demonstrate an increase in any target vocalizations for any of the participants (Esch, Carr, & Michael, 2005; Normand & Knoll, 2006; Stock, Schulze, & Mirenda, 2008).
Although the reason for these discrepant findings is unknown, one procedural variation that may produce differential outcomes is the number of presentations of the target sound per pairing trial. For example, the instructor could present the target sound once (e.g., “buh”) or multiple times (e.g., “buh, buh, buh”) during each pairing trial. Of the nine studies that demonstrated an increase in vocalizations, four presented the target sound once per pairing trial (Smith et al., 1996; Sundberg et al., 1996; Yoon & Bennett, 2000; Yoon & Feliciano, 2007). The remaining five studies presented the target sound either three (Esch et al., 2009; Rader et al., in press) or five (Carroll & Klatt, 2008; Miguel et al., 2002) times per pairing trial or did not report this information (Ward et al., 2007). The studies that did not demonstrate an increase in vocalizations presented the target sound three (Esch et al., 2005), five (Stock et al., 2008), or seven (Normand & Knoll, 2006) times per pairing trial. Stock et al. (2008) suggested that any pairing ratio of more than two presentations of the target sound per delivery of the highly preferred item would result in the target sound being paired more frequently with withholding of the highly preferred item than with its immediate presentation. The discrepant findings across studies that contained different pairing ratios warrant additional research to compare the effects of several pairing ratios on target vocalizations.
The purpose of the current study was to evaluate the differential effects of an SSP procedure that varied only in the number of presentations of the target sound per pairing trial on the rate of low-frequency vocalizations in children with autism.
METHOD
Participants and Setting
Participants were two children who had been diagnosed with autism: Mary (8 years 4 months) and Nik (6 years 6 months). Both children were selected to participate because their teachers reported that prior attempts to teach echoics and vocal mands had been unsuccessful. Results of the Behavioral Language Assessment (BLA; Sundberg & Partington, 1998) and Early Echoic Skills Assessment (EESA; Esch, 2008) for Mary indicated low-frequency vocal play and no echoic behavior (BLA Level 1–2; EESA Level 0). Assessment results for Nik indicated low-frequency vocal play (BLA Level 2), with six responses under some echoic control (EESA Level 2). During a 60-min free-play period with access to moderately preferred toys and no social interaction, the average rate of vocal play for Mary and Nik was 0.43 and 2.58 vocalizations per minute, respectively.
Sessions were conducted 3 to 5 days per week; they lasted approximately 5 to 15 min, but never more than 20 min. We conducted two sessions, one with each level of the independent variable, each day with sessions presented in a quasirandom order. A brief play period followed each session. The session room was familiar to each child and included moderately preferred toys. Closed opaque boxes contained highly preferred edible items and toys.
Response Measurement, Interobserver Agreement, and Procedural Integrity
Observers collected within-session data during each trial and during intertrial intervals on four vocalizations for each participant: two target paired (S+) vocalizations and two nontarget nonpaired (S–) vocalizations. We selected vocalizations if they occurred during 1% to 5% of 30-s intervals during the 60-min free-play period and assigned them to each condition quasirandomly (see Figure 1). Both target and nontarget vocalizations were defined as the participant's production of any vocalization that matched or was acoustically similar to the model (e.g., “uh” for “buh”). Observers scored any target or nontarget vocalization emitted by the participant that was separated by a 1-s interval without vocalizations as one response (Esch et al., 2009).
We calculated interobserver agreement on the frequency of target and nontarget vocalizations using the total frequency method (Esch et al., 2009). Mean interobserver agreement was 96% (range, 90% to 100%) for Mary and 99% (range, 97% to 100%) for Nik. Procedural integrity data were collected on the correct implementation of baseline and SSP procedures during at least 33% of sessions across conditions. We scored an instance of procedural integrity if the experimenter implemented all aspects of the procedures as indicated in the protocol. Mean procedural integrity was 97% (range, 91% to 100%) for Mary and 98% (range, 95% to 100%) for Nik.
Design and Procedure
We compared the effects of four conditions using an adapted alternating treatments design (Sindelar, Rosenberg, & Wilson, 1985). The four conditions included one presentation of the target sound per trial (1:1 S+), one presentation of the nontarget sound per trial (1:1 S–), three presentations of the target sound per trial (3:1 S+), and three presentations of the nontarget sound per trial (3:1 S–). We based our SSP procedures on the enhanced SSP procedure described by Esch et al. (2009).
Baseline
Before the first session each day, the experimenter presented six items to the participant (three toys, three foods) identified via a stimulus preference assessment (Carr, Nicolson, & Higbee, 2000). The experimenter rotated among the first three items that the participant touched during the day's sessions. Each baseline session consisted of 20 trials, 10 each of S+ and S–, quasirandomly arranged. Each trial began with a prompt to attend (e.g., “look”). The experimenter then presented the auditory model with exaggerated prosodic patterns (i.e., motherese) without pairing it with a preferred item. The trial concluded at the end of an intertrial interval of either 5 s, 10 s, 15 s, or 20 s, included to reduce temporal predictability as a confounding effect to the SSP effects (Esch et al., 2009). We conducted a separate baseline for each condition. In the 1:1 condition, the experimenter presented the auditory model for approximately 1 s (e.g., S+ “ba”; S– “dee”). In the 3:1 condition, the experimenter presented the auditory model at the rate of one sound per second for 3 s (e.g., S+ “ba, ba, ba”).
Stimulus–stimulus pairing
Pairing sessions were identical to those in baseline, with two exceptions. Presentation of S+ trials included immediate delivery of a highly preferred item just before and overlapping with the model (i.e., delay pairing). The other difference was the inclusion of a 20-s correction delay if the participant emitted the target response between the experimenter's model and delivery of the reinforcer during S+ trials. The purpose of this correction procedure was to control for adventitious reinforcement of the target response (Esch et al., 2009).
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
As seen in Figure 1, substantial and consistent increases in vocalizations occurred only in the 1:1 S+ condition for both participants. For Mary, mean increases from baseline to SSP were 0.75 (1:1 S+), 0 (1:1 S–), 0.11 (3:1 S+), and 0 (3:1 S–), and Nik's mean increases were 1.48 (1:1 S+), 0.08 (1:1 S–), 0.47 (3:1 S+), and 0.07 (3:1 S–). Although vocalizations in the 1:1 S+ condition were slightly higher and more variable than the other targets in baseline for Mary, none of the vocalizations increased during baseline in the absence of treatment. Interestingly, Nik's vocalizations increased initially and subsequently decreased in both the 1:1 S– and 3:1 S+ conditions during treatment.
This investigation is the first to compare the effects of the number of presentations of target sounds per trial during SSP. Although this study did not attempt to isolate specific variables that may be responsible for this effect, several possibilities exist. In addition to the suggestion made by Stock et al. (2008) regarding the ratio of pairing to reinforcement, the conditioned reinforcement literature also notes that the strength of a conditioned reinforcer (i.e., the rate of the sound associated with the paired targets) may depend on the frequency of the primary reinforcers correlated with it (Kelleher & Gollub, 1962). Highly preferred items were correlated less frequently with the target vocalization during the 3:1 condition than during the 1:1 condition, in which sounds were presented only once before item delivery. In addition, Esch et al. (2009) noted that a practical disadvantage of presenting a sound three times was that participants often emitted a triadic response pattern (e.g., “beh, beh, beh”). Presenting a sound only once should decrease the likelihood of triadic response patterns, eliminating the need to alter the response topography during subsequent verbal behavior training.
Several limitations are worth noting. We had difficulty identifying four targets of equivalent difficulty for each participant. Although several methods were identified initially, none were able to isolate four sounds at similar speech sound acquisition stages. Thus, we selected responses that occurred at similar frequencies during a free-play period. In addition, both participants produced the 1:1 S+ target at a slightly higher rate during baseline than the other paired and nonpaired targets, despite random assignment of targets to each condition. Finally, as in Esch et al. (2009), we used the total frequency method to calculate interobserver agreement. Because this method may overestimate agreement, future studies should use a more conservative method (e.g., exact agreement within intervals).
The results of the current study provide preliminary support for use of the 1:1 pairing procedure with children who have failed to respond to other attempts to increase vocalizations. Future research should seek to refine the SSP procedure to produce optimal results, such as the method of pairing (e.g., delay, trace, simultaneous), use of motherese modeling of the vocalizations, more frequent preference sampling to verify the value of stimuli used during pairing (Esch et al., 2009), and the use of social reinforcers (a review of SSP studies revealed that social reinforcers were associated more often with a pairing effect than tangible reinforcers; Stock et al., 2008). In addition, future research should evaluate social validity, demonstrate functional language (e.g., manding) following SSP, and assess differential effects of SSP on novel versus low-frequency targets. Finally, future SSP evaluations should seek to determine the learner profile for which this technology is most relevant.
Acknowledgments
This study is based a thesis submitted by the first author, under the supervision of the second author, to the Psychology Department at Caldwell College for the Master of Arts in Applied Behavior Analysis.We thank Garden Academy for their assistance.
Footnotes
Action Editor, Tiffany Kodak
Figure 1. .
Within-session data on the rate of target and nontarget vocalizations for Mary and Nik. The inset box shows the specific sounds for each participant for each condition presented in the International Phonetic Alphabet with sample words exemplifying the sounds. S+ = target vocalizations; S– = nontarget vocalizations.
REFERENCES
- Carr J. E, Nicolson A. C, Higbee T. S. Evaluation of a brief multiple-stimulus preference assessment in a naturalistic context. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. (2000);33:353–357. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2000.33-353. doi:10.1901/jaba.2000.33-353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carroll R. A, Klatt K. P. Using stimulus-stimulus pairing and direct reinforcement to teach vocal verbal behavior to young children with autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior. (2008);24:135–146. doi: 10.1007/BF03393062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esch B. E. Early echoic skills assessment. In: Sundberg M. L, editor. The verbal behavior milestones assessment and placement program. Concord, CA: AVB Press; (2008). 24. In. (Ed.) p. [Google Scholar]
- Esch B. E, Carr J. E, Grow L. L. Evaluation of an enhanced stimulus–stimulus pairing procedure to increase early vocalizations of children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. (2009);42:225–241. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2009.42-225. doi:10.1901/jaba.2009.42-225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esch B. E, Carr J. E, Michael J. Evaluating stimulus-stimulus pairing and direct reinforcement in the establishment of an echoic repertoire of children diagnosed with autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior. (2005);21:43–58. doi: 10.1007/BF03393009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelleher R. T, Gollub L. R. A review of positive conditioned reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. (1962);5:543–597. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1962.5-s543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miguel C. F, Carr J. E, Michael J. Effects of a stimulus-stimulus pairing procedure on the vocal behavior of children diagnosed with autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior. (2002);18:3–13. doi: 10.1007/BF03392967. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Normand M. P, Knoll M. L. The effects of a stimulus-stimulus pairing procedure on the unprompted vocalizations of a young child diagnosed with autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior. (2006);22:81–85. doi: 10.1007/BF03393028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rader L, Sidener T. M, Reeve K. F, Sidener D. W, Delmolino L, Miliotis A, et al. (in press) Stimulus-stimulus pairing of vocalizations: A systematic replication; The Analysis of Verbal Behavior. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sindelar P. T, Rosenberg M. S, Wilson R. J. An adapted alternating treatments design for instructional research. Education and Treatment of Children. (1985);8:67–76. [Google Scholar]
- Smith R, Michael J, Sundberg M. Automatic reinforcement and automatic punishment in infant vocal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior. (1996);13:39–48. doi: 10.1007/BF03392905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stock R. A, Schulze K. A, Mirenda P. A comparison of stimulus-stimulus pairing, standard echoic training, and control procedures on the vocal behavior of children with autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior. (2008);24:123–133. doi: 10.1007/BF03393061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sundberg M. L, Michael J, Partington J. W, Sundberg C. A. The role of automatic reinforcement in early language acquisition. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior. (1996);13:21–35. doi: 10.1007/BF03392904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sundberg M. L, Partington J. W. Teaching language to children with autism or other developmental disabilities. Pleasant Hill, CA: Behavior Analysts; (1998). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward S. J, Osnes P. J, Partington J. W. The effects of a delay of noncontingent reinforcement during a pairing procedure in the development of stimulus control of automatically reinforced vocalizations. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior. (2007);23:103–111. doi: 10.1007/BF03393050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoon S, Bennett G. Effects of a stimulus-stimulus pairing procedure on conditioning vocal sounds as reinforcers. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior. (2000);17:75–88. doi: 10.1007/BF03392957. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoon S, Feliciano G. Stimulus-stimulus pairing and subsequent mand acquisition of children with various levels of verbal repertoires. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior. (2007);23:3–16. doi: 10.1007/BF03393042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]