Abstract
Extinction of contextual fear in rats is enhanced by exposure to a novel environment at 1–2 h before or 1 h after extinction training. This effect is antagonized by administration of protein synthesis inhibitors anisomycin and rapamycin into the hippocampus, but not into the amygdala, immediately after either novelty or extinction training, as well as by the gene expression blocker 5,6-dichloro-1-beta-D-ribofuranosylbenzimidazole administered after novelty training, but not after extinction training. Thus, this effect can be attributed to a mechanism similar to synaptic tagging, through which long-term potentiation can be enhanced by other long-term potentiations or by exposure to a novel environment in a protein synthesis-dependent fashion. Extinction learning produces a tag at the appropriate synapses, whereas novelty learning causes the synthesis of plasticity-related proteins that are captured by the tag, strengthening the synapses that generated this tag.
Fear memories are essential for survival; however, their expression out of context or in inadequate instances of daily life can lead to anxiety or phobias (1) or to the serious and incapacitating condition known as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (1–4). Thus, it is desirable to extinguish fear memories when they are uncalled for (2), and to restrict their retrieval to appropriate circumstances (2, 3).
Extinction, the learned inhibition of retrieval (1–6), is widely used in the treatment of PTSD, often under the term “exposure therapy” (2–4). First described for alimentary learning (5), extinction involves not the erasure of original memories (1, 3, 4, 6), but merely the attenuation of responses to these memories (1–9). It relies on the activation of NMDA receptors in the hippocampus, basolateral amygdala (BLA), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (4) and requires protein synthesis in these three structures shortly after its acquisition (7–9).
Given the wide use of extinction in psychotherapy, determining whether it is modulatable, like other forms of learning, is desirable. In recent work, we showed that extinction indeed can be modulated by several neurotransmitter systems acting on the hippocampus, BLA, or prefrontal cortex (4). Here we show that extinction also can be modulated by the hippocampal process known as synaptic tagging (10–19).
Frey and Morris (10, 11) and their collaborators (12–16) (see also ref. 17) have proposed a mechanism in which relatively weak “early” long-term potentiation (LTP) at hippocampal synapses lasting only a few minutes may “tag” these synapses with proteins synthesized ad hoc, allowing other proteins produced at other sets of synapses by other LTPs (10–16) or by behaviors that require the hippocampus (16–19) to be captured by the tagged synapses, thereby strengthening their activity to a “long LTP” lasting hours or days (12–15). These proteins are referred to as plasticity-related proteins (PRPs) (10–17). This tagging mechanism also applies to, and has been reported in, hippocampal long-term depression (LTD), and “cross-tagging” between LTP and LTD, by which an LTP can turn a weak LTD into a strong LTD, has been described (16).
Long-lasting synaptic changes underlying long LTP or long-term memory (LTM) are widely believed to result from synaptic remodeling (20–23). Hippocampal long LTP has been repeatedly proposed (24–26) and actually was recently shown to underlie LTM formation of both aversive tasks (27–29) and object recognition learning (30). Thus, unsurprisingly, hippocampal-dependent behavioral procedures that influence early LTP to become long LTP (10–12, 17) also may influence other hippocampal memories to make them stronger (10–12). This is the case for the effect of exposure to a novel environment on single-trial contextual fear conditioning (CFC) (15), inhibitory avoidance (19), object recognition (15), and spatial learning in a water maze (14). Exposure to a new environment triggers exploration, followed by habituation of the exploration (11–14). The enhancing effect of novelty on task retention has been interpreted as related to behavioral tagging by task retention, an expression used to denote the induction of synaptic tagging (10, 11) by behavioral manipulations (15, 18, 19). Memories based mainly on the BLA or other brain areas are not influenced by tag-and-capture processes (13, 17).
The question of whether learning tasks can influence one another has been sporadically investigated sporadically over the past 3 decades (30–36). Exposure to a novel environment has been reported to cause retrograde amnesia of inhibitory avoidance when presented posttraining (32) and also to enhance retrieval of many memories when presented before retention testing (33–36). These two effects are unrelated to synaptic tagging or to the capture of PRPs (30, 31, 33).
Extinction requires glutamatergic NMDA receptor-dependent mechanisms in the hippocampus and other brain areas at the time of consolidation (4), followed by hippocampal ribosomal (37) and nonribosomal (38, 39) protein synthesis. Although several studies have suggested that memory consolidation of extinction learning relies on hippocampal LTP (1–6), the possibility exists that it may depend on the mechanistically related LTD (1–3). LTD is also initiated by NMDA receptors, is transformed by protein synthesis from an early brief form into a late long-lasting form, and generates and undergoes the effects of tagging (16, 40) (see also ref. 41).
In the present work, we studied whether extinction can produce behavioral tagging and whether it can be enhanced by novelty, presumably through PRPs (10–17), for capture by the tags, as occurs with LTP and with the various types of learning mentioned above. Novelty, as used here and in related experiments on behavioral tagging, refers to exposure to an open field (OF) (15, 18, 19) or a holeboard (12), the exploration of which causes habituation learning and engenders hippocampal biochemical changes (42–44) that usually lead to protein synthesis.
Results
Extinction of Contextual Fear.
Rats were trained in a CFC task (4, 43) and 24 h later were subjected to an extinction training session without reinforcement. At 30 min or 24 h after this training session, the rats were subjected to a test session involving short-term memory (STM) and LTM of extinction (4) (Fig. 1). Those two training test intervals are frequently used to evaluate the STM and LTM of various behaviors and of early LTP and long LTP (13, 44–47). The first retrieval session can be viewed as a training session of extinction learning given that it is carried out without reinforcement (4), and the second session can be viewed as a test session of extinction (4, 44). A CFC procedure involving three instead of two foot shocks and a shorter extinction training session (10 min instead of 20 min) (4) was used to make extinction presumably weaker compared with that seen in other recent studies (4, 44).
Effect of Novelty (Exposure to an OF) on Extinction of Contextual Fear.
The rats were exposed for 5 min to an OF at various intervals either before or after a weak extinction training session, or left unexposed (Fig. 2A). In the OF, the rats were allowed to explore freely, then gently withdrawn and replaced in their home cages (15, 18, 19). Presentation of the OF for 1 h or 2 h, but not for less than 1 h or for 3 h or longer, before the extinction session significantly enhanced extinction (Fig. 2 A and B). Exposure to the OF for 1 h but not for less or more than 1 h after extinction had a similar effect (Fig. 2A). These results are similar to those reported by others (19) on the effect of novelty on consolidation of CFC rather than its extinction.
The enhancing effect of exposure to the OF was not seen in rats that had been exposed before training, because when they were again exposed before extinction, it was familiar rather than novel (Fig. 2B). This is in agreement with the findings of Ballarini et al. (19) on the enhancing effect of a single exposure but not of a second exposure to an OF on acquisition of a spatial object recognition task. Spatial object recognition requires the hippocampus (47), probably because it correlates with CA1 LTP (29), like other tasks that are subject to enhancement by novelty (26–28), as apparently does spatial exploration of an OF as used here (15, 19, 43).
Inhibitor of Gene Transcription and Two Different Inhibitors of Protein Synthesis in Hippocampus Hinder Behavioral Tagging of Extinction.
The enhancing effect on extinction of exposure to a novel OF was fully blocked by the bilateral administration of anisomycin (80 μg per side), rapamycin (5 pg per side), or 5,6-dichloro-1-beta-D-ribofuranosylbenzimidazole (DRB; 8 ng per side) into the dorsal CA1, but not into the BLA (Fig. S1), immediately after exposure to the OF (Fig. 3 A, C, and E) or by anisomycin and rapamycin (Fig. 3 B and F), but not DRB (Fig. 3D), given immediately after the extinction training session. Thus, gene activation appears to be necessary for the production of the PRPs by the novelty, but not of the tag by the extinction training, whereas ribosomal and mammalian target of rapamycin (mTOR)-dependent protein synthesis seems to be important for both. Infusion of any of the three compounds into the BLA immediately after the novelty had no effect on the influence of the novelty on CFC extinction (Fig. S1). As in the preceding experiment, preexposure to the OF at 24 h before CFC training rendered it the exposure familiar rather than novel, and thus cancelled its effect on extinction performance; none of the drugs used had any effect on behavior (Fig. 3 A–C, E, and F).
Thus, the effect of anisomycin on the behavioral tagging of extinction is similar to that reported for the behavioral tagging of aversive, spatial, or recognition learning (14, 15, 18, 19) and certainly like that seen in synaptic tagging (10–19). The effect of rapamycin on synaptic tagging has not yet been studied, and our finding that it blocked the effect of novelty on extinction suggests that it should be (Fig. 3 E and F). The localization of the mTOR system in nerve cells appears to be dendritic (48, 49), which in principle could lead to better synaptic specificity of the synthesized tag proteins and PRPs.
Discussion
In a recent article on the enhancement by novelty of spatial learning weakened by stress, Frey et al. (14) noted that “novelty processing can transform short-term into long-term memory. We propose that this memory-reinforcing effect of novelty could be explained by mechanisms outlined in the ‘synaptic tagging hypothesis.’ Initial STM is sustained by a transient plasticity change at activated synapses and sets synaptic tags. These tags are later able to capture and process the PRPs, which are required to transform a short-term synaptic change into a long-term one.” This explanation may apply ipsis litteris to the interpretation of our present findings on the effect of novelty on extinction learning by tag-and-capture mechanisms, and also of course to the effect of novelty on hippocampal LTP (18) and on aversive, recognition, or spatial learning (14, 15, 18, 19, 50).
Here we add the extinction of CFC to the list of memory types that may tag hippocampal synapses so that they can be enhanced by novelty. The addition of extinction to this list is an important argument in favor of the generality of the tagging process in learning (10, 11, 13, 14). Indeed, even though extinction is now widely regarded as one more form of learning (1–4, 6), it clearly is very different in terms of response requirement and other factors from all of the other learning situations to which the tag-and-capture process has been applied so far (1, 14, 15, 18, 19).
As is the case with the synaptic tagging of LTP (13), the effect of novelty on extinction can be attenuated by treatments administered into the hippocampus, but not by treatments administered into the BLA. In the present study, the enhancement of extinction by novelty was blocked by intrahippocampal DRB, anisomycin, or rapamycin given immediately after exposure to the OF, which would be expected to inhibit synthesis of PRPs, and also by anisomycin or rapamycin given after the extinction training session, which would be expected to block the production of a tag (10–18). This result suggests that both tagging and production of PRPs use regular ribosomal protein synthesis (38) as well as nonribosomal, probably dendritic, mTOR-mediated synthesis processes (38, 39), whereas tagging also requires gene expression.
The present work provides an important addition to the knowledge of modulation of extinction, which is important for the use of extinction in exposure therapy for PTSD (4, 51). So far, extinction has been shown to be modulated both by the interpolation of a retrieval session and the resulting reconsolidation (52, 53), as well as by drugs that modify the action of modulatory neurotransmitters (4), including the serine analog D-cycloserine (54). Both reconsolidation (53) and D-cycloserine (54) have been used successfully in humans to enhance extinction (52). Here we add modulation through the interpolation of novelty, which should be easy to apply in humans as the simplest procedure with the fewest side effects.
Concerning the old question of whether one type of learning can influence other types (30, 31), our present results clearly support an affirmative response. The novelty of the OF, which leads to exploration and subsequent habituation, enhances the extinction of CFC. This effect can be explained by the tagging of hippocampal synapses by extinction and the production of hippocampal PRPs by the former, as has been suggested for LTP (10, 11), and for other types of learning (12, 14, 15). In learning experiments, presenting one task too soon after another may cause mutual inhibition owing to mechanisms unrelated to tagging (32), which may explain why here, as in other behavioral observations, the timing of tagging processes is not continuous (18, 19) as it is in LTP experiments (12–16). This point merits further investigation.
Materials and Methods
Mice.
The animals used in these experiments were male Wistar rats (3 mo old, 300–320 g), purchased from Fundação Estadual de Produção e Pesquisa em Saúde do Rio Grande do Sul, our regular provider. The rats were housed five to a cage with water and food (Purina Laboratory Pellets) ad libitum, under a 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 AM). The temperature of the animal room was maintained at 22–24 °C. All procedures were in accordance with the National Institutes of Health’s Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by Pontifical Catholic University of Rio Grande do Sul's Bioethics Committee.
Surgery.
At least 1 wk after their arrival, some of the animals were implanted under anesthesia [75 mg/kg ketamine (König, Sao Paulo) plus 10 mg/kg/xylazine (Coopers, São Paulo)] with bilateral 27-g guide cannulae inserted 1.0 mm above the pyramidal cell layer of the dorsal CA1 area of the hippocampus (A, −4.2; L, ±3.0; V, −2.0 mm) or 1.0 mm above the BLA (A, −2.8; L, ±4.7; V, −7.5 mm) (coordinates according to refs. 55 and 56). At 10 d after arrival or, in the animals that underwent implantation, 4–7 d after surgery, the rats were subjected to daily handling for 3 consecutive days and then trained in the CFC procedure between 8:00 and 11:00 AM (see below).
CFC Training.
The training apparatus was a 35 × 35 × 35 cm aluminum box with a floor made of parallel caliber bronze bars spaced 0.8 mm between them. This training box was placed within another, larger box with soundproof walls to attenuate external sounds. The percentage of time that the rat spent freezing in the apparatus was measured automatically by a counter connected to photocells (Panlab).
On the day of training, the rat was allowed to explore the apparatus freely for 2 min, after which it was given three 2-s, 0.5-mA scrambled foot shocks separated by 30-s intervals. The rat was left in the conditioning chamber for another 30 s and then placed back into its home cage. Basal freezing behavior was recorded before administration of the shocks. After 24 h, the rat was exposed to the same apparatus without any stimulation for 10 min (extinction training session), then subjected to a retention test after 30 min (for STM) or after 24 h (for LTM). The percentage of time spent freezing (i.e., with no movement) was measured during all sessions.
Exposure to an OF.
The testing apparatus was a 50 × 50 × 40 cm white-painted wooden box with a clear glass front wall. For a novel experience, the rat was exposed to the OF for 5 min at various times before or after the extinction training session. For a familiar experience, the rat was exposed to the OF for 30 min at 24 h before the extinction training session (19).
Drug Treatments.
Less than 1 min after the end of the exposure to the OF or the extinction training session, the 30-gauge infusion cannula was fitted tightly into the guides so that its tip protruded 1.0 mm beyond the guide, first on one side and then on the other (4, 57). Drug or vehicle infusions were performed at a rate of 0.5 μL/30 s. The infusion cannula was left in place for an additional 60 s to minimize backflow, then carefully withdrawn and placed on the other side, after which the foregoing procedure was repeated. The entire bilateral infusion procedure took approximately 90 s. Drugs used included the inhibitor of ribosomal translation anisomycin (80 μg per side) (38), the extraribosomal protein synthesis inhibitor rapamycin (5 pg per side) (39, 40), and the inhibitor of gene expression DRB (8 ng per side) (38). These doses have been reported to be effective (38–40). Rapamycin acts on an extraribosomal, presumably dendritic protein synthesis system that uses preexistent mRNA molecules and includes as a central component the protein kinase mTOR (39). At the doses used, these drugs affect the consolidation of avoidance conditioning and extinction when administered into rat hippocampus or amygdala, but have no effect on locomotion, exploration, or performance in an elevated plus maze (32, 33, 42). The drugs were dissolved in 2% DMSO, and 1.0 µL per side was infused into the dorsal CA1 area of the hippocampus and 0.5 µL per side was infused into the BLA.
Cannula Placement.
Correct cannula placement was verified by infusion of a 4% (wt/vol) methylene blue solution over 30 s into the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus (1 µL per side) or the BLA (0.5 µL per side) at the aforementioned coordinates at 2 d after the last behavioral experiment. The extent of the spread of the dye was considered to represent an estimate of the amount of drug infused. Cannula placement was considered correct when the spread was ≤l mm from the intended infusion site (3); this occurred in 97% of the animals. As explained elsewhere (4), despite the uncertainty inherent in the unknown rate of solubility of the drugs used relative to methylene blue, this approach is an improvement over the mere determination of cannula tip location.
Statistics.
Data were analyzed by one-way ANOVA followed by the Newman–Keuls or Dunnet test using Graphpad Prism software. P < 0.05 was considered to indicate statistical significance.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by grants from the National Research Council of Brazil.
Footnotes
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1220875110/-/DCSupplemental.
References
- 1.Orisini CA, Maren S. Neural and cellular mechanisms of fear and extinction memory formation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36(7):773–802. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Sher L, Vilens A. Neurobiology of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. New York: Nova; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Milad MR, Quirk GJ. Fear extinction as a model for translational neuroscience: Ten years of progress. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:129–151. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Fiorenza NG, Rosa J, Izquierdo I, Myskiw JC. Modulation of the extinction of two different fear-motivated tasks in three distinct brain areas. Behav Brain Res. 2012;232(1):210–216. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Pavlov IP. Conditioned Reflexes. London: Oxford Univ Press; 1927. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Bouton ME. Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learn Mem. 2004;11(5):485–494. doi: 10.1101/lm.78804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Santini E, Ge H, Ren K, Peña de Ortiz S, Quirk GJ. Consolidation of fear extinction requires protein synthesis in the medial prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2004;24(25):5704–5710. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0786-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Vianna MR, Szapiro G, McGaugh JL, Medina JH, Izquierdo I. Retrieval of memory for fear-motivated training initiates extinction requiring protein synthesis in the rat hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98(21):12251–12254. doi: 10.1073/pnas.211433298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Vianna MR, Coitinho AS, Izquierdo I. Role of the hippocampus and amygdala in the extinction of fear-motivated learning. Curr Neurovasc Res. 2004;1(1):55–60. doi: 10.2174/1567202043480170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Frey U, Morris RG. Synaptic tagging and long-term potentiation. Nature. 1997;385(6616):533–536. doi: 10.1038/385533a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Frey S, Frey JU. “Synaptic tagging” and “cross-tagging” and related associative reinforcement processes of functional plasticity as the cellular basis for memory formation. Prog Brain Res. 2008;169:117–143. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)00007-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Uzakov S, Frey JU, Korz V. Reinforcement of rat hippocampal LTP by holeboard training. Learn Mem. 2005;12(2):165–171. doi: 10.1101/lm.89305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Almaguer-Melian W, Bergado JA, Martínez-Martí L, Duany-Machado C, Frey JU. Basolateral amygdala stimulation does not recruit LTP at depotentiated synapses. Physiol Behav. 2010;101(4):549–553. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Almaguer-Melian W, et al. Novelty exposure overcomes foot shock-induced spatial-memory impairment by processes of synaptic-tagging in rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109(3):953–958. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1114198109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Moncada D, Ballarini F, Martinez MC, Frey JU, Viola H. Identification of transmitter systems and learning tag molecules involved in behavioral tagging during memory formation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108(31):12931–12936. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1104495108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Sajikumar S, Frey JU. Late-associativity, synaptic tagging, and the role of dopamine during LTP and LTD. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2004;82(1):12–25. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2004.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Barco A, Lopez de Armentia M, Alarcon JM. Synapse-specific stabilization of plasticity processes: The synaptic tagging and capture hypothesis revisited 10 years later. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32(4):831–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Moncada D, Viola H. Induction of long-term memory by exposure to novelty requires protein synthesis: Evidence for a behavioral tagging. J Neurosci. 2007;27(28):7476–7481. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1083-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Ballarini F, Moncada D, Martinez MC, Alen N, Viola H. Behavioral tagging is a general mechanism of long-term memory formation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106(34):14599–14604. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0907078106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ramón y Cajal S. New representation of the histological structure of the central nervous system. Arch Anat Physiol. 1893;17:319–428. German. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Greenough WT. Morphological and molecular studies of synaptic memory mechanisms: Links to the fragile X mental retardation syndrome. In: Gold PE, Greenough WT, editors. Memory Consolidation: Essays in Honor of James L. McGaugh. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2000. pp. 59–77. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Geinisman Y, Berry RW, Disterhoft JF, Power JM, Van der Zee EA. Associative learning elicits the formation of multiple-synapse boutons. J Neurosci. 2001;21(15):5568–5573. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-15-05568.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Morris RG, Anderson E, Lynch GS, Baudry M. Selective impairment of learning and blockade of long-term potentiation by an N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist, AP5. Nature. 1986;319(6056):774–776. doi: 10.1038/319774a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Izquierdo I, et al. Memory retrieval and its lasting consequences. Neurotox Res. 2002;4(5-6):573–593. doi: 10.1080/10298420290031441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Reymann KG, Frey JU. The late maintenance of hippocampal LTP: requirements, phases, “synaptic tagging,” “late-associativity,” and implications. Neuropharmacol. 2007;52(1):24–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2006.07.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Izquierdo I, et al. Different molecular cascades in different sites of the brain control memory consolidation. Trends Neurosci. 2006;29(9):496–505. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Gruart A, Muñoz MD, Delgado-García JM. Involvement of the CA3-CA1 synapse in the acquisition of associative learning in behaving mice. J Neurosci. 2006;26(4):1077–1087. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2834-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Whitlock JR, Heynen AJ, Shuler MG, Bear MF. Learning induces long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Science. 2006;313(5790):1093–1097. doi: 10.1126/science.1128134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Clarke JR, Cammarota M, Gruart A, Izquierdo I, Delgado-García JM. Plastic modifications induced by object recognition memory processing. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107(6):2652–2657. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0915059107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Netto CA, Dias RD, Izquierdo I. Interaction between consecutive learnings: Inhibitory avoidance and habituation. Behav Neural Biol. 1985;44(3):515–520. doi: 10.1016/s0163-1047(85)91048-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Izquierdo I, Netto CA. Role of beta-endorphin in behavioral regulation. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1985;444:162–177. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1985.tb37587.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Izquierdo I, Schröder N, Netto CA, Medina JH. Novelty causes time-dependent retrograde amnesia for one-trial avoidance in rats through NMDA receptor- and CaMKII-dependent mechanisms in the hippocampus. Eur J Neurosci. 1999;11(9):3323–3328. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00742.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Izquierdo I, McGaugh JL. Effect of a novel experience prior to training or testing on retention of an inhibitory avoidance response in mice: Involvement of an opioid system. Behav Neural Biol. 1985;44(2):228–238. doi: 10.1016/s0163-1047(85)90240-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Izquierdo LA, Barros DM, Medina JH, Izquierdo I. Novelty enhances retrieval of one-trial avoidance learning in rats 1 or 31 days after training unless the hippocampus is inactivated by different receptor antagonists and enzyme inhibitors. Behav Brain Res. 2000;117(1-2):215–220. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(00)00286-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Izquierdo LA, Barros DM, Medina JH, Izquierdo I. Exposure to novelty enhances retrieval of very remote memory in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2003;79(1):51–56. doi: 10.1016/s1074-7427(02)00006-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Chaves ML, Izquierdo I. Previous exposure to a novel experience enhances performance in two simple memory tests in humans. Braz J Med Biol Res. 1986;19(2):211–219. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Zimmerman JM, Maren S. NMDA receptor antagonism in the basolateral but not central amygdala blocks the extinction of Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2010;31(9):1664–1670. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07223.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Igaz LM, Vianna MR, Medina JH, Izquierdo I. Two time periods of hippocampal mRNA synthesis are required for memory consolidation of fear-motivated learning. J Neurosci. 2002;22(15):6781–6789. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-15-06781.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Myskiw JC, et al. On the participation of mTOR in recognition memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2008;89(3):338–351. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Slipczuk L, et al. BDNF activates mTOR to regulate GluR1 expression required for memory formation. PLoS ONE. 2009;4(6):e6007. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Parvez S, Ramachandran B, Frey JU. Functional differences between and across different regions of the apical branch of hippocampal CA1 dendrites with respect to long-term depression induction and synaptic cross-tagging. J Neurosci. 2010;30(14):5118–5123. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5808-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Viola H, et al. Phosphorylated cAMP response element-binding protein as a molecular marker of memory processing in rat hippocampus: Effect of novelty. J Neurosci. 2000;20(23):RC112. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-23-j0002.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Vianna MR, et al. Role of hippocampal signaling pathways in long-term memory formation of a nonassociative learning task in the rat. Learn Mem. 2000;7(5):333–340. doi: 10.1101/lm.34600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Alvares LO, Pasqualini Genro B, Diehl F, Molina VA, Quillfeldt JA. Opposite action of hippocampal CB1 receptors in memory reconsolidation and extinction. Neurosci. 2008;154(4):1648–1655. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Izquierdo I, et al. Mechanisms for memory types differ. Nature. 1998;393(6686):635–636. doi: 10.1038/31371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Izquierdo I, Medina JH, Vianna MR, Izquierdo LA, Barros DM. Separate mechanisms for short- and long-term memory. Behav Brain Res. 1999;103(1):1–11. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(99)00036-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.McGaugh JL. Memory—a century of consolidation. Science. 2000;287(5451):248–251. doi: 10.1126/science.287.5451.248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Kindler S, Kreienkamp HJ. Dendritic mRNA targeting and translation. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2012;970:285–305. doi: 10.1007/978-3-7091-0932-8_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Urbanska M, Gozdz A, Swiech LJ, Jaworski J. Mammalian target of rapamycin complex 1 (mTORC1) and 2 (mTORC2) control the dendritic arbor morphology of hippocampal neurons. J Biol Chem. 2012;287(36):30240–30256. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M112.374405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Broadbent NJ, Gaskin S, Squire LR, Clark RE. Object recognition memory and the rodent hippocampus. Learn Mem. 2010;17(1):5–11. doi: 10.1101/lm.1650110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Izquierdo I, Myskiw JC. Brain interactions between processes triggered by memory retrieval and their use in the treatment of fear memory. Future Neurol. 2011;6(3):307–309. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Monfils MH, Cowansage KK, Klann E, LeDoux JE. Extinction-reconsolidation boundaries: Key to persistent attenuation of fear memories. Science. 2009;324(5929):951–955. doi: 10.1126/science.1167975. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Schiller D, et al. Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms. Nature. 2010;463(7277):49–53. doi: 10.1038/nature08637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Davis M, Ressler K, Rothbaum BO, Richardson R. Effects of D-cycloserine on extinction: Translation from preclinical to clinical work. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(4):369–375. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Paxinos G, Watson C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. San Diego: Academic; 1986. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Izquierdo I, et al. Sequential role of hippocampus and amygdala, entorhinal cortex and parietal cortex in formation and retrieval of memory for inhibitory avoidance in rats. Eur J Neurosci. 1997;9(4):786–793. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1997.tb01427.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Quirk GJ, Mueller D. Neural mechanisms of extinction learning and retrieval. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33(1):56–72. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.