Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Jan 22.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2012 Aug;24(3):705–721. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000326

Supportive parenting mediates widening neighborhood socioeconomic disparities in children’s antisocial behavior from ages 5 to 12

Candice L Odgers 1, Avshalom Caspi 1, Michael A Russell 1, Robert J Sampson 1, Louise Arsenault 1, Terrie E Moffitt 1
PMCID: PMC3551477  NIHMSID: NIHMS428209  PMID: 22781850

Abstract

In this article we report a graded relationship between neighborhood socioeconomic status (SES) and children’s antisocial behavior that (1) can be observed at school entry, (2) widens across childhood, (3) remains after controlling for family-level SES and risk, and (4) is completely mediated by maternal warmth and parental monitoring (defined throughout as supportive parenting). Children were participants in the Environmental Risk (E-Risk) Longitudinal Twin Study (n=2232), which prospectively tracked the development of children and their neighborhoods across childhood. Direct and independent effects of neighborhood-level SES on children’s antisocial behavior were observed as early as age 5 and the gap between children living in deprived versus more affluent neighborhoods widened as children approached adolescence. By age 12, the effect of neighborhood socioeconomic status on children’s antisocial behavior was as large as the effect observed for our most robust predictor of antisocial behavior – sex! (Cohen’s d = .51 when comparing children growing up in deprived versus more affluent neighborhoods in comparison to Cohen’s d = .53 when comparing antisocial behavior among boys versus girls). However, differences in children’s levels and rate of change in antisocial behavior across deprived versus more affluent neighborhoods were completely mediated by supportive parenting practices. Implications of our findings for studying and reducing socioeconomic disparities in antisocial behavior among children are discussed.

Keywords: childhood antisocial behavior, neighborhood poverty, socioeconomic status, social inequalities, maternal warmth, parental monitoring, supportive parenting


Children who grow up in poor neighborhoods are less likely to graduate high school, more likely to spend time in prison, and can expect to suffer from more health-related problems when compared to their peers in more affluent settings. Even within the wealthiest nations, the proportion and absolute numbers of children confronting the conditions associated with poverty are staggering. In the United States, an estimated 14 million children - approximately 20% of all children - are living in families that have incomes below the federal poverty level (Wight, Chau, & Aratani, 2010). In the United Kingdom, national statistics indicate that up to 1 in 3 children (3.8 million) live in relative poverty once housing costs are considered (Joyce, Phillips, & Sibieta, 2010). A commission assembled by the World Health Organization (WHO) recently concluded that these types of inequalities are ‘killing people on a grand scale’ and called for investment during the early years of life to reduce social inequalities within the next generation (Marmot et al., 2008). Throughout their call, the Commission emphasized the need to extend the current focus on social inequalities in child development beyond physical and cognitive aspects to include the crucial domains of social and emotional well being (Commission on Social Determinants of Health, 2008).

Child well being has been conceptualized as encompassing the broad domains of physical, educational, behavioral and emotional status (Bradshaw, 2011). Poverty is considered the single biggest threat to child well being and efforts directed at reducing socioeconomic inequalities that begin in childhood are sorely needed. To date, the majority of information on how poverty affects children has emerged from studies focusing on physical health (e.g., infant mortality, birth weight, asthma) and academic achievement (e.g., test scores, school readiness and academic performance). These streams of research have been invaluable in shaping policy initiatives in public health and education. However, the general consensus is that efforts aimed at reducing social inequalities will require a more complete understanding of how the long reach of childhood poverty influences all aspects of development, including emotional and behavioral well being (Brooks-Gunn, Duncan, & Aber, 1997; Commission on Social Determinants of Health, 2008; Duncan & Magnuson, 2011).

Antisocial behavior is a key indicator of children’s well being that has been linked to socioeconomic disparities at both the neighborhood and family-level (Duncan, Brooks-Gunn, & Klebanov, 1994). Antisocial behavior is a relatively common problem among school-aged children and, if it persists across childhood, signals a host of costly physical health, mental health, social and economic difficulties (Cohen & Piquero, 2009; Moffitt, Caspi, Harrington, & Milne, 2002). In this study we defined antisocial behavior as consisting of aggressive and delinquent acts which result in physical or psychological harm to others or their property (e.g., ‘stealing’, ‘lying’, and ‘getting into fights’). Such behaviors violate the rights of others and, in some cases, violate legal codes. Children’s antisocial behavior was assessed using the Achenbach family of instruments (Achenbach, 1991a, 1991b), which is the most widely used and well validated assessment scheme for assessing antisocial behavior problems among children and adolescents. Operationally, the measure of antisocial behavior referenced throughout this article was derived based on items from the Delinquent Behavior (e.g., ‘lying or cheating’ ‘swearing or bad language’) and Aggressive Behavior (e.g., ‘hot temper’, ‘physically attacks people’) scales of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) and the Teacher Report Form.

Unfortunately, most neighborhood effects research on antisocial behavior has not focused specifically on children due to the belief that contexts outside of the family are more likely to influence the behavior of adolescents and adults (Klebanov, Brooks-Gunn, Chase-Lansdale, & Gordon, 1997). As a result, the effects of neighborhood SES on delinquency and crime has been widely studied among older adolescents (for a review see Sampson, Morenoff, & Gannon-Rowley, 2002; Shaw & McKay, 1942) and there is a long history of linking neighborhood features to crime among adults (Sampson, Raudenbush, & Earls, 1997; Wilson, 1987). However, the question of whether and how SES influences children’s antisocial behavior has received less attention (for notable exceptions see Caspi, Taylor, Moffitt, & Plomin, 2000; Caughy, Nettles, & O’Campo, 2008; Chase-Lansdale & Gordon, 1996; Criss, Shaw, Moilanen, Hitchings, & Ingoldsby, 2009; Duncan et al., 1994; Ingoldsby et al., 2006; Mrug & Windle, 2009; Reijneveld et al., 2010; Schneiders et al., 2003; Vanderbilt-Adriance & Shaw, 2008).

In this paper we ask whether neighborhood-level socioeconomic disparities make an independent contribution to children’s involvement in antisocial behavior across childhood. More specifically, we test whether a graded relationship between neighborhood-level SES and antisocial behavior can be detected at school entry and whether the effects of growing up in a deprived versus more affluent neighborhood on antisocial behavior increase as children approach adolescence and begin to spend more time in their neighborhoods. Many people wonder whether neighborhood SES is merely a proxy for family-level SES and question whether associations between neighborhood poverty and the health of residents is simply due to the type of people who live in a given neighborhood (social selection) versus the effects of neighborhood characteristics per se (social causation). To address this concern, we tested whether differences in children’s antisocial behavior across deprived versus more affluent neighborhoods remain after accounting for more proximal family-level factors, including family-level SES and parent’s own history of antisocial behavior. Finally, most theories assume that the effects of neighborhood-level poverty, especially for young children, are transmitted through parents (Klebanov et al., 1997). In this paper we ask the policy-relevant question of whether supportive parenting practices, including maternal warmth and parental monitoring, mediate the effects of socioeconomic deprivation on children’s developmental course of antisocial behavior. Additional details regarding our three main research questions are outlined below.

Question 1. Does the widely observed SES gradient in health underlie children’s antisocial behavior? The SES gradient in health is pervasive. The seminal Whitehall Studies in the United Kingdom provided evidence of a steep inverse association between social class and mortality for a wide range of diseases (Marmot et al., 1991). Similarly, the last two decades of work by the Macarthur Network on SES and Health have provided countless illustrations of how the gradient in health reproduces itself over time, populations and different types of diseases (Adler & Ostrove, 1999). Over the last decade, the field has moved to a new era of research focused on mechanisms linking SES and health via multiple levels of influence, from characteristics of the individual to features of the larger community (Adler & Stewart, 2010). Within this discussion, neighborhoods have emerged as potentially relevant contexts for understanding the illusive SES gradient as they contain both physical and social attributes known to influence health status (Diez Roux & Mair, 2010). More specifically, neighborhood settings are believed to influence resident’s lives through two broad domains of influence: (1) the physical environment (e.g., physical spaces, air quality, safe places to for children to play) and (2) the social environment (e.g., social norms, safety and violence, the type and nature of connections between residents).

In this study we test whether the SES gradient can be observed across neighborhood settings for children’s antisocial behavior, beginning at school entry (age 5) and persisting through to age 12. Children from low- versus high SES families typically have higher levels of socio-emotional problems, including antisocial behavior (for a review see Bradley & Corwyn, 2002). However, less is known about how neighborhood-level SES shapes children’s developmental trajectories. The neighborhood settings that children are embedded in are believed to shape children’s antisocial behavior in at least three (interrelated) ways.

First, low-versus high- SES neighborhoods are more likely to be characterized by high levels of crime, disorder and exposure to other threatening and uncontrollable events that are hypothesized to have downstream effects on families and children. Chronic stress exposure is believed to account for much of the difference in outcomes between children across these settings (e.g., Adler & Ostrove, 1999; McLoyd, 1998; Shonkoff & Phillips, 2000), with evidence that young people growing up in poverty confront more stressors in their daily lives when compared to their more affluent counterparts (Evans, Vermeylen, Barash, Lefkowitz, & Hutt, 2009). The stressors, uncertainties, and low social standing that accompany living in a low versus high SES neighborhood are also believed to deplete energy, predict negative emotional states, and lead to a pattern of behavior known as ‘reactive responding’. Reactive responding is often characterized by chronic vigilance, emotional reacting and sense of powerlessness when confronted with environmental stressors (Taylor & Seeman, 1999). In line with this theory, parents living within low-SES contexts have been found to overuse negative control strategies, fail to adequately monitor children and exhibit low warmth and responsiveness (McLoyd, 1990). Prior research also suggests that it is the absence of supportive parenting, not just the presence of negative parenting that mediates the relationship between low SES and child well being (for a review see Bradley & Corwyn, 2002).

Second, children growing up in low- versus high-SES neighborhoods are more likely be exposed to delinquent peer groups and spend greater amounts of unsupervised time with antisocial peers (Haynie & Osgood, 2005; Osgood, Wilson, O'Malley, Bachman, & Johnston, 1996). Research has also shown that exposure to delinquent peers within low SES contexts may overwhelm the effects of family-level factors on children’s conduct problems (Schonberg & Shaw, 2007) and that association with delinquent peers may serve as a way in which early starting conduct problems are reinforced and maintained over time (Ingoldsby et al., 2006). In short, peers have been shown to shown to mediate, exacerbate and directly affect antisocial behavior among children and are believed to play an increasing role as children reach and move through adolescence. Most theories and studies of children’s development assume an interactive pattern of influence of neighborhood effects through both family and peer settings (Criss et al., 2009; Eamon, 2002; Loeber & Farrington, 2000) and, for the most part, operate on the assumption that neighborhood effects operate primarily through family contexts in early childhood and that peers begin to play a larger role as children approach adolescence.

Third, low- versus high SES neighborhoods typically have lower levels of community based resources, such as collective efficacy, which are believed to facilitate the successful socialization of children into school and other community contexts, increase the amount of social support available to parents, enhance parents’ feelings of safety and trust in their communities. High levels of cohesion and trust within a community are also believed to play a role in establishing local norms discouraging harsh parenting and domestic violence, while also informally monitoring levels of antisocial behavior among children and adolescents (Browning & Cagney, 2003; Sampson, 2003; Sampson, Morenoff, & Earls, 1999).

As described in more detail below, in the present study we test whether a graded relationship between neighborhood SES and children’s antisocial behavior is present at school entry, increases over time and can be mediated, in least in part, by supportive parenting. While we acknowledge that community-based resources and peers are likely to play a significant role in the transmission of neighborhood effects on children, we focus primarily on the role of supportive parenting practices. Our focus on parenting behaviors is motivated by the fact that most early childhood interventions aim to enhance supportive parenting and the family context is considered to be one of the primarily ways in which neighborhood effects are transmitted to young children (Klebanov et al., 1997). In addition, there is an emerging belief among those in policy and intervention circles that the most effective way to enhance the life prospects of vulnerable children (while one waits for the unlikely scenario where child poverty is eliminated) is to build capacities and strengths within families that are capable of mitigating the effects of toxic stress on the developing child (Shonkoff, 2012).

Our longitudinal study also offers a unique opportunity to test whether the effects of neighborhood socioeconomic status on children’s antisocial behavior increase over time. The effects of neighborhood settings on children’s antisocial behavior are expected to increase as children begin to spend more time in their neighborhoods, making their way to and from school, participating in afterschool activities and acquiring more freedom to spend unsupervised time in their local communities with peers (Brooks-Gunn, Duncan, Klebanov, & Sealand, 1993). Antisocial behavior is relatively common in early childhood and has been shown to decline as children make the transition into primary school and move across middle childhood (Stanger, Achenbach, & Verhulst, 1997; Tremblay & Nagin, 2005). This normative pattern of decline is believed to result from a host of positive socialization experiences that children encounter as they learn to navigate social relationships in their homes, schools and neighborhoods and acquire the skills needed to achieve their aims through pro-social means (Cairns, Cairns, Neckerman, Ferguson, & Gariepy, 1989; Tremblay, 2000). Children who do not follow this normative trajectory of decline in behavioral problems are at risk for a host of poor outcomes in adulthood (Moffitt et al., 2002; Odgers et al., 2008).

To address this question, we mapped the developmental course of antisocial behavior separately for children growing up in relatively deprived, middle and high SES areas and tested whether children in deprived versus more affluent neighborhoods exhibited a slower rate of decline in antisocial behavior between the ages of 5 and 12. The influence of neighborhood SES on the developmental course of antisocial behavior was tested separately among boys versus girls and we were especially interested in testing whether the ‘gap’ in antisocial behavior between children living in more deprived versus affluent settings widened across childhood. We expected to observe a general decrease in antisocial behavior from middle to late childhood, were especially concerned about children who were not exhibiting the expected decline in antisocial behavior, and asked whether neighborhood social inequalities play a role in placing children on an early-onset and persistent pathway of antisocial behavior.

Question 2. Does the level of socioeconomic deprivation in a neighborhood continue to influence children’s antisocial behavior after considering more proximal family-level factors? Prior research has demonstrated small, but robust, effects of neighborhood conditions on children’s educational, emotional and behavioral well being (for a summary see Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2000). However, questions remain as to whether these effects are sensitive to unmeasured variables that may influence the types of families that live in a specific neighborhood. Neighbourhood- and family-level SES are often closely linked and it is possible that neighbourhood SES serves only as a proxy for family-level status. Levels of neighbourhood- and family-level deprivation were moderately, but not completely correlated in the present sample (r=.51), which provided the opportunity to evaluate the relative contribution of each form of SES and test whether neighborhood and family-level SES exerted cumulative effects on children’s antisocial behavior (e.g., see Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2000; Wasserman, Shaw, Selvin, Gould, & Syme, 1998).

Because families are not randomly selected into neighbourhoods we also tested whether the effects of neighbourhood-level deprivation were robust to conditions that are believed to influence both where families are able to live and children’s levels of antisocial behaviour, including the mother’s and father’s history of antisocial behaviour and mental health problems among family members. Controlling for family history information in our analyses helped to account for two important causes of antisocial behavior: familial genetic loading and parents’ environmental influences on their children’s antisocial behavior (Moffitt et al., 2007). Our analyses also controlled for exposure to family violence, which is more heavily concentrated in deprived neighbourhoods and has demonstrated robust associations with children’s antisocial behavior in prior research (Coulton, Crampton, Irwin, Spilsbury, & Korbin, 2007). More specifically, we asked whether neighbourhood-level socioeconomic status independently predicted children’s levels of antisocial behaviour and/or the rate of change in children’s antisocial behaviour between the ages of 5 and 12.

Question 3. Can supportive parenting practices mediate the effects of neighborhood SES on children’s developmental course of antisocial behavior? Poverty and its associated conditions are believed to exact a cumulative toll on children through two broad categories of influence: (1) access to environmental resources/constraints (‘what money can buy’ pathway), and (2) increases in unfavorable environmental exposures associated with declining SES, such as exposure to family violence and harsh parenting (Adler & Ostrove, 1999; Cohen, Janicki-Deverts, Chen, & Matthews, 2010). That is, childhood SES is often viewed as a dual risk factor capable of exerting a direct effect on behavior as well as setting the scene for more proximal factors, such as parenting behaviors, to exert their influence (Poulton & Caspi, 2005). Among adolescents, parenting factors such as harsh discipline, low parental monitoring and weak parent-child attachment have been shown to mediate the effects of poverty and other structural neighborhood characteristics on delinquency and violence (Chung & Steinberg, 2006; Sampson & Laub, 1994; Tolan, Gorman-Smith, & Henry, 2003). Much less attention has been paid to how the effects of neighborhood poverty are transmitted to pre-adolescent children (Mrug & Windle, 2009). However, it is assumed that neighborhood effects at this young age are largely mediated by family-level processes and parenting (Klebanov et al., 1997; Kohen, Leventhal, Dahinten, & McIntosh, 2008).

Parental monitoring has been identified as a key mediator of neighborhood effects on children’s behavior due to the fact that neighborhood conditions are likely to influence the degree to which parents perceive threats in the local community and, in turn, the extent to which they monitor and keep track of their children (Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2000). Although there is evidence of both child elicited and parent driven effects (Laird, Pettit, Bates, & Dodge, 2003; Pettit, Laird, Dodge, Bates, & Criss, 2001; Stattin & Kerr, 2000), effective parental monitoring (1) has consistently been associated with lower levels of antisocial behavior among children and adolescents, (2) has been identified as a mediator of treatment effects in intervention studies, where increases in parental monitoring have been associated with reductions in adolescent substance use, and (3) is considered to be particularly important, but difficult to implement, in high-risk neighborhoods (for a recent review see Racz & McMahon, 2011).

Other types of supportive parenting practices, including parental warmth and responsivity have also emerged as likely candidates in the transmission of neighborhood effects among both adolescents and young children (Brooks-Gunn et al., 1997; Gershoff, Aber, Raver, & Lennon, 2007). For example, one of the reasons that poverty is believed to be so detrimental to children’s lives is that it has been shown to reduce a mother’s ability to respond to her child (Evans, Boxhill, & Pinkaya, 2008; Grant et al., 2003; Magnuson & Duncan, 2002). Mothers who are trying to parent in poverty are often faced with a wide range of physical and psychosocial stressors that may interfere with their ability to be responsive to their children’s needs. Mothers living in low-income neighborhoods also tend to have lower levels of social support, lack positive parenting role models in the community, and are more prone to depression and other mental health problems that are known to compromise supportive parenting practices (Conger et al., 1992; Kiernan & Huerta, 2008; McLoyd, 1998).

There are numerous examples of large-scale interventions that target parents in low income communities and focus on enhancing supportive parenting practices. The Nurse-Family Partnership (NFP) is one of the most widely implemented and evaluated early intervention programs. The NFP targets first time and low income mothers through home visitations prior to and following the birth of their first child and, among other things, aims to help the mother to provide responsible and competent care for her child (Olds, 2006). Similarly, other behavioral family intervention programs, such as Triple P-Positive Parenting Program (Sanders, 1999) and the Oregon Model of Parent Management Training (Patterson, Forgatch, & DeGarmo, 2010) focus on strengthening parenting skills including, parental supervision and monitoring and building warm and supportive parent–child relationships. In this study we test whether two of the main targets of these targeted interventions, parental monitoring and maternal warmth, mediate the effects of neighborhood SES on children’s levels and rate of change in antisocial behavior between the ages of 5 and 12 years. If we find that the supportive parenting practices mediate the effect of neighborhood-SES on children’s antisocial behavior, it would both aid our understanding of how community level resources are translated into child outcomes and point to malleable targets for intervention research.

This study is unique in that it provides the opportunity to: (1) map the developmental course of antisocial behavior in a large prospectively followed cohort of children (n= 2232) followed with high retention across childhood (over 95% at all ages), (2) trace the influence of neighborhood SES on the lives of families representing the full range of socioeconomic conditions in the United Kingdom, (3) test whether differences in antisocial behavior between children living in deprived versus more affluent neighborhoods widen across childhood using a standardized assessment of antisocial behavior at the ages of 5, 7, 10 and 12, and (4) evaluate the influence of neighborhood SES on children’s antisocial behavior using multi-informant and longitudinal assessments of children’s behavior (mother and teacher reported), neighborhood SES (census and resident surveys) and family-level parenting and risk (both rater coded and mother reported).

Our prior work in this sample mapped the developmental course of antisocial behavior between the ages of 5 and 10 years and illustrated how community-level cohesion and informal social control can protect children from the negative effects of neighborhood deprivation at school entry (Odgers et al., 2009). In this study we extend our prior work by: (1) following children to the age of 12 (a time when neighbourhood effects on behaviour are believed to strengthen), (2) asking whether differences in antisocial behaviour between children living in deprived versus more affluent neighbourhoods remain after considering more proximal familylevel risks, (3) testing whether the influence of neighbourhood SES on children’s antisocial behaviour increases over time, and (4) responding to one of our calls for future research by testing how the effects of neighbourhood characteristics are transmitted to children via more proximal family-level processes such as parental monitoring and maternal warmth.

Methods

Participants

Participants were members of the Environmental Risk (E-Risk) Longitudinal Twin Study, which tracks the development of a nationally representative birth cohort of 2,232 British children (see Figure 1a for a description of the geographical distribution of the families in the study). The sample was drawn from a larger birth register of twins born in England and Wales in 1994–1995 (Trouton, Spinath, & Plomin, 2002). Details about the sample have been reported previously (Moffitt, 2002). Briefly, the E-risk sample was constructed in 1999–2000, when 1,116 families with same-sex 5-year old twins (93% of those eligible) participated in home-visit assessments. Families were recruited to represent the UK population of families with newborns in the 1990’s, based on (a) residential location throughout England and Wales and (b) mother’s age (i.e., older mothers having twins via assisted reproduction were under-selected and teenage mothers with twins were over-selected). Follow-up home visits were conducted when the children were aged 7 years (98% participation), 10 years (96% participation), and, most recently, 12 years (96% participation). With parents’ permission, questionnaires were mailed to the children’s teachers, who returned questionnaires for 94% of children at age 5, 91% of the 2,232 E-risk children (93% of those followed up) at age 7, 86.3% of the 2,232 E-risk children (90.1% of those followed up) at age 10, and 80% of the 2,232 E-risk children at age 12 (83% of those followed up). The sample includes 55% monozygotic (MZ) and 45% dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs. Sex is evenly distributed within zygosity (51% female). Parents gave informed consent and children gave assent. The Maudsley Hospital Ethics Committee approved each phase of the study.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Figure 1

a: Location of E-Risk Study families at the Phase 12 home-visit

b: E-Risk families represent the full range of neighborhood SES in Britian

Measures

Table 1 provides the means and standard deviations for all of the measures described below separately for children growing up in deprived, middle and high income neighborhood contexts.

Table 1.

Children’s levels of antisocial behavior, family-level risk, exposure to violence and supportive parenting by neighborhood socioeconomic status

ACORN 3-Category Neighborhood SES
Deprived SES Middle SES High SES


Measure n Mean(SD) Mean(SD) Mean(SD)


Antisocial behavior, age 5 2232 25.98 (18.95) 23.07 (17.07) 19.50 (14.58)
Antisocial behavior, age 7 2178 23.30 (19.83) 20.42 (16.41) 16.40 (13.82)
Antisocial behavior, age 10 2138 23.26 (20.37) 18.49 (16.89) 15.67 (14.08)
Antisocial behavior, age 12 2141 23.48 (20.29) 19.01 (17.47) 14.33 (14.06)
Family SES 2232 2.43 (1.79) 0.94 (1.40) 0.52 (1.08)
Parental antisocial behavior 2222 0.38 (0.48) 0.27 (0.44) 0.17 (0.37)
Family history of disorder 2138 0.39 (0.28) 0.38 (0.27) 0.34 (0.25)
Domestic violenceexposure 2086 2.03 (3.50) 1.21 (2.51) 0.75 (1.75)
Child harm 2232 0.29 (0.58) 0.23 (0.48) 0.20 (0.48)
Parental monitoring 2136 18.40 (2.28) 18.97 (1.64) 19.03 (2.12)
Maternal warmth 2134 3.44 (0.95) 3.73 (0.88) 3.87 (0.82)

Antisocial behavior consists of aggressive and delinquent acts which result in physical or psychological harm to others or their property (e.g., ‘stealing’, ‘lying’, and ‘getting into fights’). Such behaviors violate the rights of others and, in some cases, violate legal codes. Children’s antisocial behavior was assessed using the Achenbach family of instruments (Achenbach, 1991a, 1991b), which is the most widely used and well validated assessment scheme for assessing antisocial behavior problems among children and adolescents. The antisocial behavior construct reported in this article was derived from items from the Delinquent Behavior (e.g., ‘lying or cheating’ ‘swearing or bad language’) and Aggressive Behavior (e.g., ‘hot temper’, ‘physically attacks people’) scales of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) and the Teacher Report Form. We combined mother interviews and teacher reports of children’s behavior on the aggression and delinquency scales by summing the items from each rater (items scored from 0 – 2). The antisocial behavior scale was administered at age 5 (M = 23.0, SD = 17.3, α = .94, range = 0 – 130.4, N = 2232), age 7 (M = 20.3, SD = 17.2, α = .95, range = 0 – 132.0, N = 2178) age 10 (M = 19.5, SD = 17.8, α = .92, range = 0 – 150.0, N = 2138) and age 12 (M = 19.4, SD = 18.03, α = .93, range = 0 – 139.0, N = 2141).

Neighborhood-level socioeconomic status (SES) was assessed using geo-demographic discriminators developed by CACI Limited for commercial use in Great Britain. ACORN (A Classification of Residential Neighborhoods) classifications were built using over 400 variables from the 2001 census and an extensive consumer research database (e.g., age, educational qualifications, unemployment, single-parent status, housing tenure and dwelling type, and car availability) combined to give a comprehensive picture of socioeconomic differences between different areas (see Figure 1b). Classifications are provided at the Enumeration District (ED) level (~ 150 households) and were geo-coded to match the location of each family’s home (Odgers, Caspi, Bates, Sampson, & Moffitt, in press). Hierarchical cluster analysis was used to group EDs into 56 neighborhood types and five distinct and homogeneous ordinal groups ranging from ‘Wealthy Achievers’ (category 1) with high incomes, large single-family houses and access to many amenities (25.6% of E-risk families and 25.3% of the UK population) to ‘Hard Pressed’ neighborhoods (category 5) dominated by government-subsidized housing estates, low incomes, high unemployment and single parents (26.1% of E-risk families and 20.7% of the UK population). ACORN classifications are typically sold to businesses, health and local health authorities for marketing and planning purposes but were shared with our research team by their developers CACI Ltd (http://www.caci.co.uk/) for educational and research purposes. Findings are displayed for children growing up in relatively high SES (ACORN categories 1, 2), middle SES (ACORN category 3) and deprived (low) SES (ACORN categories 5 and 6) neighborhoods (see Figures 24). All regression analyses were performed using the ACORN-defined five category measure of neighborhood SES.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Graded relationship between neighborhood socioeconomic status and children’s antisocial behavior at ages 5, 7, 10 and 12 (n= 2055)

Deprived = ACORN categories 5 (hard pressed) and 4 (moderate means) Middle SES = ACORN category 3 (comfortably off); High SES = ACORN categories 2 (urban prosperity) and 1 (wealthy achievers). Results held at the p<.001 level at all ages for boys [5 (b = 2.19, β = .17), 7 (b = 2.24, β = .17), 10 (b = 2.35, β = .18), and 12 (b = 2.87, β = .21)] and girls [5 (b = 1.21, β = .13), 7 (b = 1.23, β = .14), 10 (b = 1.74, β = .17), and 12 (b = 1.96, β = .20)].

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Neighborhood-level socioeconomic disparities in antisocial behavior increase across childhood for boys and girls

Model fit for 6-group latent basis model: Chi-square (df) =22.19 (18), RMSEA = 0.02 (0.00–0.05), CFI = 1.00, SRMR = 0.03. Cohen’s d: .2 = small, 5. = medium, .8 = large.

Family socioeconomic disadvantage was assessed at age 5 using a count of six socioeconomic disadvantages, defined as: (1) head of household has no educational qualifications; (2) head of household is employed in an unskilled occupation or is not in the labor force; (3) total household gross annual income is less than £10,000; (4) family receives at least one government benefit, excluding disability benefit; (5) family housing is government subsidized; (6) family has no access to a vehicle. The average socioeconomic disadvantage score was 1.38 (SD = 1.70, α = .79, range = 0 – 6, N = 2232). Full details of this measure in this sample are reported elsewhere (Kim-Cohen, Moffitt, Caspi, & Taylor, 2004).

Father’s and mother’s history of antisocial behavior was reported by the mothers when the children were 5 years old. Mothers were interviewed using the Young Adult Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1997) which was modified to obtain lifetime data. Full details of father’s and mother’s history of antisocial behavior within this sample are reported elsewhere (Jaffee, Moffitt, Caspi, & Taylor, 2003). A study of mother-father agreement about men’s antisocial behavior in this sample showed that women provided reliable information about their children’s father’s behavior (Caspi et al., 2001). A symptom of antisocial behavior was considered to be present if the mother endorsed the symptom as being "very true or often true." Antisocial behavior symptoms scores for mothers (M = 0.67, SD = 1.16, range = 0 – 6.0, N = 2226) and fathers (M = 1.42, SD = 2.00, range = 0 – 7.0, N = 2216) were combined into a single score that indexed whether at least one of the parents had 3 or more antisocial behavior symptoms (27.6% of the children had at least one parent who exceeded this threshold). A similar pattern of results emerged when either the continuous antisocial problem scores or a dichotomous measure of parental antisocial behavior were entered into the models. For the ease of presentation, results are presented for the dichotomous measure only.

Family history of mental health problems was assessed at age 12 by asking the mother to report on her own mental health history and the mental health history of her biological mother, biological father, biological sisters, biological brothers, as well as the twins’ biological father. Mother’s were asked to report if anyone on the aforementioned list experienced difficulties with substance-use problems, alcohol problems, depression, psychosis, and suicide attempts (Milne et al., 2008; Weissman et al., 2000). The mental health history items comprised four items of substance use derived from the short Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (MAST; Selzer, Vinokur, & Vanrooijen, 1975) and the Drug Abuse Screening Test DAST (DAST; Skinner, 1982), and one each on problems with drinking and drugs derived from the Family History Screen (FHS; Weissman et al, 2000); five items on depression derived from the FHS; one item on suicide ideation derived from the FHS; and two items asking about hospitalization and treatment for ‘other’ mental health disorders. For each of the four domains (substance, depression, suicide, other), family members were considered to have a positive history if any items within that domain were answered positively, and a negative history otherwise. For the purposes of our analyses, we calculated the proportion of family members with a positive history of any disorder. The average proportion of family members with a history of mental health problems was .37 (SD = 0.27, range = 0 – 1.0, N = 2138).

Physical child maltreatment was assessed separately for each twin at ages 5, 7 and 10 by interviewing mothers with the standardized clinical interview protocol from the Multi-Site Child Development Project (Dodge, Pettit, Bates, & Valente, 1995).1 Full details of the child physical maltreatment measure within this sample are reported elsewhere (Jaffee, Caspi, Moffitt, & Taylor, 2004). Examples of such maltreatment included being victim of adjudicated assault by a teenaged sibling, being burned with matches, injuries (e.g., fractures) from neglectful or abusive parental care, and/or formal registration with a social-services child protection team for physical abuse (scored 0 = no harm, 1 = probable harm, 2 = definite harm). Prior to age 10, there was no evidence of harm for 80.3% of the sample, probable harm for 15.2% of the sample, and definite harm for 4.5% of the sample. Similar results were reported for both the 3 category and dichotomous classification of physical harm (no harm versus probable/definite harm).

Adult domestic violence was assessed by the Conflict Tactics Scale (Straus, 1990), inquiring about 12 acts of physical violence (e.g., kicking a partner, threatening a partner with a knife). Mothers were asked about their own violence toward any partner and about partners' violence toward them. The physical violence acts reported at the ages of 5, 7 and 10 were summed to yield a measure of cumulative exposure to domestic violence. The internal consistency reliability of the physical abuse scale was .89. Full details of the adult domestic violence measure in this sample are reported elsewhere (Koenen, Moffitt, Caspi, Taylor, & Purcell, 2003). As previously shown, inter-partner agreement about abuse perpetration is substantial, with latent correlations between perpetrator and victim reports of perpetration ranging from .71 to .83 (Moffitt et al., 1997).

Maternal warmth was assessed at age 10 using a 5-minute speech sample from the mother to elicit expressed emotion about each child. Trained interviewers asked caregivers to describe each of their children (“For the next 5 minutes, I would like you to describe [child] to me; what is [child] like?”). The mother was encouraged to talk freely with few interruptions. However, if the mother found this difficult, the interviewer could aid the mother with a series of semi-structured probes, such as “In what ways would you like [child] to be different?” Interviews about each twin were separated in time by approximately 90 min. All interviews were audiotaped with the mother’s consent. Two trained raters coded the audiotapes (inter-rater reliability = .90) A full description of the procedure is detailed elsewhere (Caspi et al., 2004). Maternal warmth was a global measure of the entire speech sample that captured the warmth expressed about the child during the speech and was coded 6-point scale, ranging from ‘high warmth’ (5) and moderately high warmth (4) to ‘very little’ (1) or ‘no’ (0) warmth. The average maternal warmth score was 3.67 (SD = 0.91, range = 0 – 5, N = 2100).

Parental monitoring was assessed at age 10 by asking the mother to report on how closely she monitors her child’s behaviour when the child is away from home. Ten items were adapted from Stattin & Kerr’s (2000) monitoring and supervision questionnaire to capture whether the mother knew: friends the child hangs out with, where they go in their spare time, how they spend their money, what type of homework the child has, when the child has tests or projects and how the child performs in different subjects. Mothers also reported on whether the child: needs permission to leave home, needs permission before deciding what to do on the week-end, reports on where and who they are going out with, and reports on what they did when they return home (scored 0 = no, never, 1 = sometimes, 2 = yes, always). The average parental monitoring score was 18.77 (SD = 0.91, range = 0 – 20, α = .75, N = 2136).

Statistical Analyses

Our analyses proceeded in four steps. First, we tested whether there was a graded relationship between neighborhood socioeconomic status and children’s antisocial behavior at each assessment age within a regression framework. Second, children’s developmental course of antisocial behavior between the ages of 5 and 12 was estimated using latent growth curve models (LGCMs). LGCMs map inter-individual differences in intra-individual change over time and provide a means of aggregating repeated measures into relatively few parameters, such as estimates of the average rate of growth and variability in development over time (Bollen & Curran, 2006). LGCMs are now commonly applied in developmental research (Kim & Cicchetti, 2006) and offer the opportunity to test whether the developmental course of antisocial behavior differs across sub-populations. Multiple-group LGCMs were used to test: (1) whether children growing up in deprived versus more affluent neighborhoods exhibited a slower rate of decline in antisocial behavior across childhood, (2) whether socioeconomic related disparities in children’s antisocial behavior increased between the ages of 5 and 12, and (3) whether boys and girls followed a different developmental course of antisocial behavior depending on the type of neighborhood that they were living in.

Third, a series of conditional SEM models were applied to test whether the effect of neighborhood-level SES on children’s antisocial behavior (levels and rate of change) held after controlling for family-level factors, including family socioeconomic status, family history of mental health problems, parental history of antisocial behavior, and child exposure to domestic violence and physical harm.

Fourth, we tested whether the effects of neighborhood SES on children’s antisocial behavior were mediated by parental monitoring and maternal warmth, two indicators of supportive parenting that are believed to play a role in the transmission of the effects of neighborhood poverty on children’s behavior. Using procedures outlined by Mackinnon and colleagues (2007) both the direct and indirect effects of neighborhood deprivation on children’s levels of antisocial behavior were estimated within a SEM framework.

All models were fitted in Mplus Version 6.1 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2008) using maximum likelihood estimation. Missing data were handled through Full Information Maximum Likelihood procedure (Raykov, 2005) and presented a minimal threat to the results due to high retention rates in the E-Risk Study (over 95% response rate across occasions). Statistical analyses were complicated by the fact that the E-Risk Study contained two children from each family. Therefore, the COMPLEX option in Mplus was used to compute adjusted standard error estimates and correct for the non-independence of observations due to the fact that children in our study were nested within families.

Results

Is the neighborhood-SES gradient in children’s antisocial behavior observed by age 5? Does neighborhood-SES influence children’s antisocial more strongly over time?

A graded relationship between neighborhood socioeconomic status and children’s antisocial behavior was observed at age 5 (b = 1.78, β = .15, p<.001), 7 (b = 1.82, β = .16, p<.001), 10 (b = 2.16, β = .18, p<.001), and 12 (b = 2.51, β = .21, p<.001). A three group classification of neighborhood context is presented in Figure 2 and contrasts the levels of antisocial behavior among children growing up in relatively affluent (ACORN categories 1 and 2), middle (ACORN category 3) and deprived (ACORN categories 4 and 5) SES neighborhoods. Differences between children living in each type of neighborhood were significant at the p<.001 level and were observed across each of the neighborhood SES categories at each age.

As shown in Figure 3, the gap between children living in deprived versus more affluent neighborhoods widened across childhood. Between the ages of 5 and 12, the standardized mean difference in children’s levels of antisocial behavior across deprived versus more affluent neighborhoods increased from d= .38 to d = .51. A multiple-group latent growth curve model demonstrated a significant degradation in fit when either the initial levels (Δ χ2 = 31.4 /Δdf= 1, p<.001) or the rate of change over time (Δ χ2 = 5.4/Δdf= 1, p<.05) in antisocial behavior were constrained to be equal across low versus high SES neighborhoods. Children living in high SES neighborhoods experienced a steeper decline in antisocial behavior (standardized slope estimate = −.50, p<.001) when compared with children growing up in deprived neighborhoods (standardized slope estimate = −.15, p<.001).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Neighborhood socioeconomic disparities in children’s antisocial behavior increase across childhood (n=2138)

Deprived = ACORN categories 5 (hard pressed) and 4 (moderate means) Middle SES = ACORN category 3 (comfortably off); High SES = ACORN categories 2 (urban prosperity) and 1 (wealthy achievers). Cohen’s d: .2 = small, 5. = medium, .8 = large. Model fit: χ2 = 6.03, df = 8, CFI =1.00, RMSEA = 0.00 (0.00–0.03), SRMR= .01.

The developmental trajectories of antisocial behavior were estimated separately for males and females living in deprived versus more affluent neighborhoods using a latent basis growth curve model.2 Results from a multiple group model are presented in Figure 4, and illustrate three main findings. First, socioeconomic disparities in children’s antisocial behavior widened for both males and females between the ages of 5 and 12, with an increase in the standardized mean difference in antisocial behavior between children in deprived versus more affluent neighborhoods increasing from .42 to .55 amount among males (43% increase in the mean differences in ASB between neighborhoods over time) and from .30 to .47 among females (57% increase in the mean differences in ASB between neighborhoods over time).

Second, males growing up in deprived neighborhoods were the only subgroup who did not experience a significant decline in antisocial behavior. The estimated amount of change between the ages of 5 and 12 was not significantly different than zero (Estimate = −1.23, p = .28) and constraining the amount of change among boys in deprived neighborhoods to equal zero did not result in a significant change in the overall model fit (Δ χ2 = 1.51 /Δdf= 1). Thus, on average, boys in deprived neighborhoods did not experience the expected decline in antisocial behavior across childhood. All five of the remaining groups demonstrated significant reductions in antisocial behavior across time, with girls in deprived and boys in high SES neighborhoods following an indistinguishable pattern of antisocial behavior between the ages of 5 and 12.

Do the effects of neighborhood SES on children’s antisocial behavior hold after controlling for more proximal family-level factors? Does supportive parenting mediate the influence of neighborhood SES on children’s antisocial behavior?

Tables 2 and 3 report the results from a series of conditional SEMs predicting children’s levels of antisocial behavior (Table 2) and their rate of change in antisocial behavior across the ages of 5 and 12 (Table 3). The results presented in Table 2 were derived from a conditional LGCM with the intercept centered at age 12 to estimate the effect of neighborhood deprivation at the age where the effects of neighborhood deprivation on children appeared to be the strongest. Findings in Table 2 illustrate three main findings.

Table 2.

Estimates from conditional SEMs predicting children’s levels of antisocial behavior (ASB) at age 12

Model 1: neighborhood
SES only
Model 2: controlling for
sex
Model 3: controlling for
family-level risk
Model 4: can supportive
parenting mediate the effects
of neighborhood SES?




Estimate
(SE)
St.
Est
p Estimate
(SE)
St.
Est
p Estimate
(SE)
St.
Est
p Estimate
(SE)
St.
Est
p
Neighborhood 2.48 (.30) .23 <.001 2.36 (.29) .22 <.001 0.96 (0.33) .08 .004 0.38 (0.31) .03 .21
SES
Sex 8.05 (.94) .25 <.001 6.99 (0.90) .22 <.001 6.07 (0.84) .19 <.001

Family SES 1.46 (0.38) . 16 <.001 0.67 (0.35) .07 . 06
Parental ASB 2.81 (1.24) .08 .02 2.74 (1.17) .08 .02
Family history 5.74 (1.92) .10 .003 4.68 (1.80) .08 .01
Dom. violence 0.44 (0.21) .08 .04 0.51 (0.21) .09 .01
Child harm 5.08 (1.20) .17 <.001 4.19 (1.12) .14 <.001

Monitoring −1.84 (0.23) −.2 5 <.001
Warmth −4.38 (0.49) −.25 <.001

Model R2
(intercept)
6% of variance 12% of variance 23% of variance 36% of the variance

Estimate = unstandardized estimate; St. Est = standardized estimate; SE = standard error. Model 3 fit: Chi square = 44.6. df = 19, 0.03 (0.02–0.04), CFI = .99, SRMR = 0.01. Model 4 fit: Chi square = 53.25, df =23, RMSEA = 0.03 (0.02–0.04), CFI = .99, SRMR = 0.02.

Table 3.

Estimates from conditional SEMs predicting rate of change in children’s antisocial behavior between 5 and 12 years

Model 1: neighborhood
SES only
Model 2: controlling for
sex
Model 3: controlling for
family-level risk
Model 4: can supportive
parenting mediate the effects
of neighborhood SES?




Estimate
(SE)
St.
Est
p Estimate
(SE)
St.
Est
p Estimate
(SE)
St.
Est
p Estimate
(SE)
St.
Est
p
Neighborhood 0.11 (.04) .10 .003 0.11 (.04) .10 .004 0.11 (0.04) .10 .02 0.07 (0.04) .06 .13
SES
Sex 0.16 (.12) .05 .18 0.11 (0.12) .04 .35 0.05 (0.12) .02 .66

Family SES 0.04 (0.05) .04 . 47 −0.01 (0.05) −.01 . 88
Parental ASB −0.29 (0.16) −.08 .07 −0.30 (0.16) −.09 .06
Family history −0.19 (0.24) −.05 .41 −0.31 (0.24) −.05 .19
Dom. violence 0.02 (0.03) .03 .54 0.03 (0.03) .04 .39
Child harm −0.15 (0.14) −.05 .27 −0.21 (0.14) −.07 .13

Monitoring −0.13 (0.03) −.17 <.001
Warmth −0.24 (0.07) −.13 .001

Model R2
(slope)
1% of variance 1% of variance 2% of variance 7% of the variance

Estimate = unstandardized estimate; St. Est = standardized estimate; SE = standard error.

First, the unconditional effect of neighborhood deprivation on children’s antisocial behavior at age 12 was significant and substantial (standardized coefficient = .23, p=.001, Model 1). Second, the effects of neighborhood deprivation on children’s levels of antisocial behavior remained after controlling for sex (Model 2) as well as family socioeconomic status, parental antisocial behavior, family history of mental health problems, exposure to domestic violence and child harm (standardized coefficient = .08, p=.01, Model 3). Together these variables accounted for 23% of the variation in children’s behavior at age 12.

Third, our findings provided evidence that supportive parenting mediated the persistent effects of neighborhood and family-level deprivation on children’s levels of antisocial behavior. Once maternal warmth and parental monitoring were entered into the model the effects of both neighborhood and family-level socioeconomic deprivation on children’s antisocial behavior became non-significant (Model 4). Meditational models fit within an SEM framework also revealed significant indirect effects of neighborhood deprivation on antisocial behavior via both parental monitoring and maternal warmth (total effect = 1.43 (standardized = 0.14), p <.001, total indirect effect = 0.99 (standardized = .10), p <.001; specific indirect pathway via parental monitoring = .047, p <.001, specific indirect pathway via maternal warmth = .052, p<.011) alongside a non-significant direct effect of neighborhood SES on child antisocial behavior. In other words, supportive parenting practices completely mediated the effects of neighborhood deprivation on children’s levels of antisocial behavior. Together the neighborhood and family level factors included in Model 4 accounted for 36% of the variation in children’s antisocial behavior at age 12.

Table 3 presents the coefficients from the conditional LGCAs predicting the rate of change in antisocial behavior across the ages of 5 and 12. Findings detailed in table 3 illustrate two main points. First, despite the fact that it is notoriously difficult to predict differences in how children change over time, neighborhood deprivation predicted the rate of change in antisocial behavior across the ages of 5 and 12 (standardized coefficient = .10, p=.003, Model 1). That is, as the level of deprivation in the neighborhood increased the rate of change (improvement) in antisocial behavior slowed. The influence of neighborhood deprivation on children’s rate of change in antisocial behavior also held after controlling for sex and family-level risk factors (Model 3). Second, supportive parenting practices completely mediated the influence of neighborhood deprivation on children’s rate of change over time. Follow up analyses revealed a significant indirect effect of neighborhood deprivation on children’s rate of change in antisocial behavior over time through both parental monitoring and maternal warmth (total effect = 0.14 (standardized = 0.13), p =.002, total indirect effect = 0.06 (standardized = 0.06), p <.001; specific indirect pathway via parental monitoring = .03, p =.001, specific indirect pathway via maternal warmth = .03, p=.003) alongside a non-significant direct effect of neighborhood SES on the rate of change in child antisocial behavior.

Discussion

The WHO Commission on Social Determinants of Health concluded that a child born in a poor neighborhood can expect to die sooner and spend more of his life afflicted with a disability than if he was born into relative affluence. Results of our study augment this narrative by illustrating that by the time child from a poor neighborhood enters school he will be more likely than his same aged peers in more affluent settings to exhibit antisocial behavior and is expected to fall further behind on this marker of well being as he approaches adolescence. In this paper, we add to the growing body of research tracing the effects of poverty on children’s lives in three main ways.

First, we provided evidence that there is a graded relationship between neighborhood socioeconomic status and children’s antisocial behavior that can be observed as early as age 5. The graded association between neighborhood SES and children’s antisocial behavior was observed across each of the assessment ages and increased across childhood for both boys and girls. By age 12, the standardized mean difference in children’s levels of antisocial behavior across deprived versus more affluent neighborhoods approached the size of one of our best and most robust predictors of antisocial behavior – sex! (Cohen’s d = .51 when comparing children growing up in deprived versus more affluent neighborhoods in comparison to Cohen’s d = .53 when comparing antisocial behavior among boys versus girls). The pattern of increasing neighborhood-level socioeconomic disparities in antisocial behavior held for both boys and girls.

Notably, boys in deprived neighborhoods were the only group that was not experiencing the expected decline in antisocial behavior across childhood. In contrast, girls in deprived neighborhoods were following the normative pattern of decline and mirrored the developmental trajectory of boys in high SES neighborhoods with respect to both their initial levels of antisocial behavior and their rate of change (improvement) in antisocial behavior across childhood (see Figure 4). Overall, our examination of the developmental course of antisocial behavior across neighborhood types is consistent with theories suggesting that neighborhood effects on child outcomes are likely to increase with age and provide yet another piece of evidence that boys growing up in deprived neighborhoods may be the most likely to follow an early onset and persistent course of antisocial behavior. The failure of boys growing up in deprived neighborhoods to exhibit signs of improvement in antisocial behavior is especially troubling as a persistent course of early antisocial behavior has been linked to a wide range of later problems (Moffitt et al., 2002; Odgers et al., 2008).

Second, neighborhood deprivation continued to predict differences in children’s developmental course of antisocial behavior across childhood even after controlling for family-level factors. Questions regarding whether social selection (the type of people who live in certain neighborhoods) versus social causation (the effects of neighborhood factors per se) is responsible for observed relationships between environmental risks and behavior are pervasive throughout much of social science (Rutter, 2003). While it is possible that unmeasured influences are confounding our findings, we were able to control for many of the factors that are believed to influence both the types of neighborhoods that families are able to live in and the development of children’s antisocial behavior. In doing so, we provide evidence that the effect of neighborhood SES on children’s antisocial behavior is robust to many of the ‘usual suspects’, including family SES, parental antisocial behavior, family history of mental health problems, exposure to domestic violence and child harm. Even after accounting for these more proximal risk factors, neighborhood-level socioeconomic status continued to predict both children’s levels of antisocial behavior and the rate of change in antisocial behavior across time. More specially, as the amount of deprivation in the local neighborhood increased, children exhibited higher levels of antisocial behavior and a slower decline in antisocial behavior between the ages of 5 and 12 years.

Fourth, supportive parenting practices completely mediated the effects of neighborhood and family-level deprivation on children’s antisocial behavior

In this study we asked whether supportive parenting practices, such as parental monitoring and maternal warmth, could mediate the persisting effects of neighborhood deprivation on children’s antisocial behavior. We focused on supportive parenting practices, defined here as maternal warmth and parental monitoring, as they have been shown to be responsive to neighborhood conditions, consistently emerge as predictors of children’s antisocial behavior, and are often targeted by early childhood intervention programs within low-income communities (e.g., Nurse-Family Partnership and Triple-P Parenting). Our findings demonstrate that maternal warmth and high levels of parental monitoring completely mediated the effects of neighborhood and family-level deprivation on children’s developmental course of antisocial behavior. Moreover, despite the fact that differences in how children change over time are notoriously difficult to predict, maternal warmth and parental monitoring were able to explain 7% of the variation in how quickly children’s antisocial behavior decreased, while other family-level factors demonstrated no measureable effect.

The complete mediation of the effects of neighborhood and family-level SES on children’s levels and rate of change in antisocial behavior was encouraging as (1) prior intervention work has demonstrated that measureable changes in responsive parenting and maternal warmth are possible early in life (Landry, Smith, Swank, & Guttentag, 2008) and, (2) among adolescents, changes in parental monitoring have been shown to mediate the effects of parent-based interventions on antisocial behavior and substance use (for a review see Racz & McMahon, 2011). However, future research is required to understand how child behavior shapes the development of parental monitoring and other supportive parenting practices (Patrick, Snyder, & Schrepferman, 2005). The reciprocal nature of parenting practices and child behavior may be particularly important to consider among children exhibiting an early onset course of antisocial behavior (where child behavior is likely to evoke parental responses at an early age) and/or among children living in high-risk communities (where negative patterns of parent-child interactions may be amplified against a backdrop of a series of environmental stressors and pressures on both parents and children) (Dishion & McMahon, 1998; Laird et al., 2003).

This study also had limitations. First, unmeasured factors may have confounded our estimates of neighborhood effects on children’s antisocial behavior. We selected our control variables to represent family-level factors that are most likely to influence neighborhood selection and children’s antisocial behavior. Nonetheless, our finding that the effects of neighborhood SES on child antisocial behavior are robust to family-level risk factors may not hold with a broader set of controls. Second, children in our sample were twins and families with twins may experience unique pressures as parents struggle to meet the social and economic demands of caring for two children (Spillman, 1984). Although a number of studies have demonstrated that twins and singletons do not differ on their mean levels of behavioral problems (Gjone & Novik, 1995; Kendler, Martin, Heath, & Eaves, 1995; Moilanen et al., 1999; van den Oord, Koot, Boomsma, Verhulst, & Orlebeke, 1995) and that the association between neighborhood factors and children’s mental health outcomes are similar across singleton versus twin samples (Kim-Cohen et al., 2004), replication of our findings in samples of singletons is required. Third, our definition of supportive parenting in this study was restricted to high levels of parental monitoring and maternal warmth. However, it is likely that other dimensions of supportive parenting, including consistent parenting and positive parent-child communication also play a role in mediating the effects of neighborhood and family-level deprivation. Fourth, we reported evidence consistent with a casual influence of parental monitoring on children’s antisocial behavior. However, prior research has raised questions regarding whether high levels of parental monitoring simply reflect the willingness of certain children to disclose information (Kerr, Stattin, & Burk, 2010) and (as noted above) it is likely that the evolution of childhood behavioral problems and parental monitoring strategies are not independent. Future research is required to isolate the key dimensions of supportive parenting and determine whether changes in either neighborhood conditions and/or supportive parenting practices can break the chain of influence from neighborhood socioeconomic status to child outcomes. Fifth, statistical mediation can also be observed if the same reporter serves as the source of information for the predictor, mediator and outcome at the same assessment point. In the current study we went to great lengths to integrate multiple informants (e.g., child and teacher for our outcome of antisocial behavior, census data for our predictor of neighborhood SES, and audiotapes coded via independent raters for maternal warmth). We also relied on assessments that were separated by multiple years. Nonetheless, we still cannot be certain that supportive parenting is the active pathway through which neighborhood effects are transmitted to children. Sixth, our age 12 assessment did not include comprehensive assessments of peer delinquency and, as such, did not allow for a fair test of the neighborhood-to-peer pathway. As we follow the cohort through adolescence we will have the ability to document the interplay between neighborhood, family and peer-level factors. Seventh, given that less than 10% of our sample self-identified as belonging to an ethnic minority group, replication of our findings with ethnically diverse and non-British samples is required

With these limitations in mind the implications of our findings for research and policy can be considered. Public health officials have long accepted the fact that place matters when trying to understand the origins, progression and endpoints for chronic disease. However, social scientists have been slower to completely embrace the existence of place effects as much of the last 50 years of neighborhood effects research has been spent debating the merits of social causation versus social selection. In the end, most estimates of neighborhood effects on behavior and health are likely the product of forces related to both social selection and social causation (Jaffee, Strait, & Odgers, 2011). Importantly, researchers are now beginning to move from the question of whether early insults and the conditions associated with economic deprivation are causal factors in children’s lives to how and when these effects are transmitted to children. For example, researchers are beginning to trace how features of the neighborhood, such as air and noise quality as well as exposure to violence and other stressors can ‘get under the skin’ and influence health (Miller, Chen, & Cole, 2009). For example, Chen and colleagues (2003) have been testing whether children within low SES neighborhoods differ in their biological profiles related to asthma, including immune and cortisol functioning. More recently, neuroscientists have begun to examine the ways in which specific brain structures and responsivity to stress differ between people living in urban versus rural settings (Lederbogen et al., 2011) and creative experimental paradigms are providing evidence that the presence of neighborhood features such as physical disorder may influence behavior (Keizer, Lindenberg, & Steg, 2008). As our interest in understanding how the neighborhoods in which we work, live and play influence well being across the lifespan continue to grow, we will need to continue to explore new and cost effective ways of assessing the features of neighborhood settings that may influence child development. Recently, our team has been working on the development of virtual assessments of neighborhood settings using Google Street View and has found that these types of virtual versus in-person assessments may provide a reliable and means of gathering information about local neighborhoods (Odgers et al., in press). For those in the field of child development, virtual neighborhood assessments may provide a cost effective alternative to census derived data for capturing key features of neighborhood settings.

The fortunes of many children rise and fall with the resources available in their zip code. Economic inequalities between families are widening (Duncan & Murnane, 2011) and exposure to the adverse conditions associated with poverty early in life is believed to lay the foundations and independently contribute to growing inequalities in mental health, disease and social status across the lifespan (for a review see Cohen et al., 2010; Shonkoff, Boyce, & McEwen, 2009). Unfortunately, the harmful effects of poverty are likely to become even more pronounced in the coming years as programs supporting low-income and poor families are eliminated or scaled back in size. For example, the United Kingdom has been waging an aggressive war against child poverty since 1999 and, despite lifting over half a million children out of poverty since that time, is currently facing the challenge of maintaining public commitment to ending child poverty by 2020 in the face of a global recession and shrinking national resources. In the United States, many cash-transfer programs and policies designed to redistribute wealth to children and families in need have been cut or are being met with increasing opposition (Couch, Smeeding, & Waldfogel, 2010).

Research on whether policies and programs designed to change the socioeconomic conditions of families would be effective are mixed. Creative natural experiments have demonstrated marked improvements in children’s conduct problem symptoms when their family was lifted out of poverty (Costello, Compton, Keeler, & Angold, 2003). Yet, randomized housing experiments have generated mixed support regarding whether movement from a deprived neighborhood can reduce children’s levels of behavioral problems (Kling, Ludwig, & Katz, 2005). Thus, even if the government offered everyone a ‘move to opportunity’ it is not clear whether antisocial behavior among children living in deprived neighborhoods would be affected. What is becoming increasingly clear is that poverty is taking a significant toll on the lives of children and there is a need to identify malleable intervention targets to mediate the downstream effects of structural disadvantages. In this paper we identify two potential targets for intervention and prevention efforts – parental monitoring and maternal warmth – but we also encourage the continued search for factors at the neighborhood, school and family level that may play a role in the lives of children and families who confront the conditions associated with poverty in their daily lives.

As macro-level market and global trends continue to determine how many children are living in poverty on any given day, researchers and child advocates are working to identify ways that children can be protected and thrive in these conditions. Rapid advancements in our understanding of how early experiences become embedded and alter the developmental course of health and behavior are offering clues how we can best support families and alter settings to improve children’s lives. Our findings point to the importance of expanding key outcomes in the fight against childhood poverty to include behavioral as well as health and educational outcomes and support continued investment in bolstering positive parenting practices for all children, but especially for those growing up in deprived neighborhood and family contexts.

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgements and Author notes: The E-Risk Study is funded by the Medical Research Council (UKMRC grants G1002190 and G9806489). Additional support was provided by ESRC grant RES-177-25-0013, NICHD grant HD061298, and by funds from the Jacobs Foundation, the British Academy, the Nuffield Foundation, and NIMH grant MH077874. The Community Strengths project is funded in part by Google. Candice L. Odgers is a William T. Grant Scholar. We are grateful to the study mothers and fathers, the twins, and the twins' teachers for their participation. Our thanks to Michael Rutter and Robert Plomin, to Thomas Achenbach for kind permission to adapt the Child Behavior Checklist, and to members of the E-Risk team for their dedication, hard work, and insights. We also Jill Collins and Paul Langston for their geo-coding assistance.

Footnotes

1

The interview protocol was designed to enhance mothers' comfort with reporting valid child maltreatment information, while also meeting researchers' legal and ethical responsibilities for reporting. Under the UK Children’s Act (1989), our responsibility was to secure intervention if maltreatment was current and ongoing. Such intervention on behalf of E-Risk families was carried out with parental cooperation in all but one case.

2

The growth curve was specified by setting the growth factor loadings for the initial assessment (age 5) equal to zero and the final assessment (age 12) equal to one, with the loadings for ages 7 and 10 freely estimated to provide the percentage of growth that occurred between each assessment point. This model is referred to as a ‘latent basis’ model (Ram & Grimm, 2007) and serves to describe both the functional form of development and the amount of change in antisocial behavior between the ages of 5 and 12 years.

References

  1. Achenbach TM. Manual for the child behavior checklist/4–18 and 1991 profile. Burlington: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry; 1991a. [Google Scholar]
  2. Achenbach TM. Manual for the teacher's report form and 1991 profile. Burlington: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry; 1991b. [Google Scholar]
  3. Achenbach TM. Manual for the young adult self report and young adult behavior checklist. Burlington: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  4. Adler NE, Ostrove JM. Socioeconomic status and health: what we know and what we don't. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1999;896(1):3–15. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08101.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Adler NE, Stewart J. Health disparities across the lifespan: meaning, methods, and mechanisms. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010;1186(1):5–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05337.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bollen KA, Curran PJ. Latent curve models: A structural equation approach. New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002;53(1):371–399. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bradshaw J. The well-being of children in the UK. Bristol: Policy Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  9. Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ, Aber JL. Neighborhood poverty: Context and consequences for children (Vol. 1) New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  10. Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ, Klebanov PK, Sealand N. Do neighborhoods influence child and adolescent development? American Journal of Sociology. 1993;99(2):353–395. [Google Scholar]
  11. Browning CR, Cagney KA. Moving beyond poverty: Neighborhood structure, social processes, and health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2003;44(4):552–571. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cairns RB, Cairns BD, Neckerman HJ, Ferguson LL, Gariepy JL. Growth and aggression: Childhood to early adolescence. Developmental Psychology. 1989;25(2):320–330. [Google Scholar]
  13. Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Morgan J, Rutter M, Taylor A, Arseneault L, et al. Maternal expressed emotion predicts children's antisocial behavior problems: Using monozygotic-twin differences to identify environmental effects on behavioral development. Developmental Psychology. 2004;40(2):149–161. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Caspi A, Taylor A, Moffitt TE, Plomin R. Neighborhood deprivation affects children's mental health: Environmental risks identified in a genetic design. Psychological Science. 2000;11(4):338. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Caspi A, Taylor A, Smart M, Jackson J, Tagami S, Moffitt TE. Can women provide reliable information about their children's fathers? Cross-informant agreement about men's lifetime antisocial behaviour. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2001;42(7):915–920. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Caughy MOB, Nettles SM, O’Campo PJ. The effect of residential neighborhood on child behavior problems in first grade. American Journal of Community Psychology. 2008;42(1):39–50. doi: 10.1007/s10464-008-9185-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chase-Lansdale PL, Gordon RA. Economic hardship and the development of five and six-year olds: Neighborhood and regional perspectives. Child Development. 1996;67(6):3338–3367. [Google Scholar]
  18. Chen E, Fisher EB, Bacharier LB, Strunk RC. Socioeconomic status, stress, and immune markers in adolescents with asthma. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2003;65(6):984–992. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000097340.54195.3c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Chung HL, Steinberg L. Relations between neighborhood factors, parenting behaviors, peer deviance, and delinquency among serious juvenile offenders. Developmental Psychology. 2006;42(2):319–331. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cohen MA, Piquero AR. New evidence on the monetary value of saving a high risk youth. Journal of Quantitative Criminology. 2009;25(1):25–49. [Google Scholar]
  21. Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D, Chen E, Matthews KA. Childhood socioeconomic status and adult health. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010;1186(1):37–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05334.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Commission on Social Determinants of Health. Closing the gap in a generation: health equity through action on the social determinants of health. Final Report of the Commission on Social Determinants of Health. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  23. Conger RD, Conger KJ, Elder GH, Jr, Lorenz FO, Simons RL, Whitbeck LB. A family process model of economic hardship and adjustment of early adolescent boys. Child Development. 1992;63(3):526–541. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01644.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Costello EJ, Compton SN, Keeler G, Angold A. Relationships between poverty and psychopathology. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2003;290(15):2023. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.15.2023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Couch K, Smeeding TM, Waldfogel J. Fighting poverty: Attentive policy can make a huge difference. Journal of Policy Analysis and Management. 2010;29(2):401–407. [Google Scholar]
  26. Coulton CJ, Crampton DS, Irwin M, Spilsbury JC, Korbin JE. How neighborhoods influence child maltreatment: A review of the literature and alternative pathways. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2007;31(11–12):1117–1142. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.03.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Criss MM, Shaw DS, Moilanen KL, Hitchings JE, Ingoldsby EM. Family, neighborhood, and peer characteristics as predictors of child adjustment: A longitudinal analysis of additive and mediation models. Social Development. 2009;18(3):511–535. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00520.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Diez Roux AV, Mair C. Neighborhoods and health. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010;1186(1):125–145. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05333.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Dishion TJ, McMahon RJ. Parental monitoring and the prevention of child and adolescent problem behavior: A conceptual and empirical formulation. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 1998;1(1):61–75. doi: 10.1023/a:1021800432380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE, Valente E. Social information-processing patterns partially mediate the effect of early physical abuse on later conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1995;104(4):632–643. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.104.4.632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J, Klebanov PK. Economic deprivation and early-childhood development. Child Development. 1994;65(2):296–318. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Duncan GJ, Magnuson K. The long reach of early childhood poverty. Pathways Magazine. 2011:22–27. [Google Scholar]
  33. Duncan GJ, Murnane R. Whither opportunity? Rising inequality, schools, and children. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  34. Eamon MK. Poverty, parenting, peer, and neighborhood influences on young adolescent antisocial behavior. Journal of Social Service Research. 2002;28(1):1–23. [Google Scholar]
  35. Evans GW, Boxhill L, Pinkaya M. Poverty and maternal responsiveness: The role of maternal stress and social resources. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2008;32(3):232–237. [Google Scholar]
  36. Evans GW, Vermeylen FM, Barash A, Lefkowitz EG, Hutt RL. The experience of stressors and hassles among rural adolescents from low-and middle-income households in the USA. Children, Youth and Environments. 2009;19(2):164–175. [Google Scholar]
  37. Gershoff ET, Aber JL, Raver CC, Lennon MC. Income is not enough: Incorporating material hardship into models of income associations with parenting and child development. Child Development. 2007;78(1):70–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00986.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Gjone H, Novik TS. Parental ratings of behavior problems - A twin and general-population comparison. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 1995;36(7):1213–1224. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb01366.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Grant KE, Compas BE, Stuhlmacher AF, Thurm AE, McMahon SD, Halpert JA. Stressors and child and adolescent psychopathology: Moving from markers to mechanisms of risk. Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129(3):447. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Haynie DL, Osgood DW. Reconsidering peers and delinquency: How do peers matter? Social Forces. 2005;84(2):1109–1130. [Google Scholar]
  41. Ingoldsby EM, Shaw DS, Winslow E, Schonberg M, Gilliom M, Criss MM. Neighborhood disadvantage, parent–child conflict, neighborhood peer relationships, and early antisocial behavior problem trajectories. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2006;34(3):293–309. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9026-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Jaffee SR, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Taylor A. Physical maltreatment victim to antisocial child: Evidence of an environmentally mediated process. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113(1):44–55. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Jaffee SR, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A. Life with (or without) father: The benefits of living with two biological parents depend on the father's antisocial behavior. Child Development. 2003;74(1):109–126. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Jaffee SR, Strait LB, Odgers CL. From correlates to causes: Can quasi-experimental studies and statistical innovations bring us closer to identifying the causes of antisocial behavior? Psychological Bulletin. 2011 doi: 10.1037/a0026020. (online first) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Joyce R, Phillips D, Sibieta L. Poverty and inequality in the UK: 2011. London: The Institute for Fiscal Studies; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  46. Keizer K, Lindenberg S, Steg L. The spreading of disorder. Science. 2008;322(5908):1681. doi: 10.1126/science.1161405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kendler KS, Martin NG, Heath AC, Eaves LJ. Self-report psychiatric symptoms in twins and their nontwin relatives: Are twins different? American Journal of Medical Genetics. 1995;60(6):588–591. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1320600622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kerr M, Stattin H, Burk WJ. A reinterpretation of parental monitoring in longitudinal perspective. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2010;20(1):39–64. [Google Scholar]
  49. Kiernan KE, Huerta MC. Economic deprivation, maternal depression, parenting and children's cognitive and emotional development in early childhood. The British Journal of Sociology. 2008;59(4):783–806. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-4446.2008.00219.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Kim-Cohen J, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A. Genetic and environmental processes in young children's resilience and vulnerability to socioeconomic deprivation. Child Development. 2004;75(3):651–668. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00699.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kim J, Cicchetti D. Longitudinal trajectories of self-system processes and depressive symptoms among maltreated and nonmaltreated children. Child Development. 2006;77(3):624–639. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00894.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Klebanov PK, Brooks-Gunn J, Chase-Lansdale PL, Gordon RA. Are neighborhood effects on young children mediated by features of the home environment. In: Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ, Aber JL, editors. Neighborhood poverty: Context and consequences for children. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 1997. pp. 119–145. [Google Scholar]
  53. Kling JR, Ludwig J, Katz LF. Neighborhood effects on crime for female and male youth: Evidence from a randomized housing voucher experiment. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2005;120(1):87–130. [Google Scholar]
  54. Koenen KC, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A, Purcell S. Domestic violence is associated with environmental suppression of IQ in young children. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15(2):297–311. doi: 10.1017/s0954579403000166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kohen DE, Leventhal T, Dahinten VS, McIntosh CN. Neighborhood disadvantage: Pathways of effects for young children. Child Development. 2008;79(1):156–169. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01117.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Laird RD, Pettit GS, Bates JE, Dodge KA. Parents' monitoring-relevant knowledge and adolescents' delinquent behavior: Evidence of correlated developmental changes and reciprocal influences. Child Development. 2003;74(3):752–768. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Landry SH, Smith KE, Swank PR, Guttentag C. A responsive parenting intervention: The optimal timing across early childhood for impacting maternal behaviors and child outcomes. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44(5):1335. doi: 10.1037/a0013030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Lederbogen F, Kirsch P, Haddad L, Streit F, Tost H, Schuch P, et al. City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans. Nature. 2011;474(7352):498–501. doi: 10.1038/nature10190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Leventhal T, Brooks-Gunn J. The neighborhoods they live in: The effects of neighborhood residence on child and adolescent outcomes. Psychological Bulletin. 2000;126(2):309–337. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Loeber R, Farrington DP. Young children who commit crime: Epidemiology, developmental origins, risk factors, early interventions, and policy implications. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12(4):737–762. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400004107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. MacKinnon DP, Fairchild AJ, Fritz MS. Mediation analysis. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58(58):593–614. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Magnuson KA, Duncan GJ. Parents in poverty. In: Marc H, editor. Handbook of parenting: Vol. 4: Social conditions and applied parenting. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2002. pp. 95–121. [Google Scholar]
  63. Marmot M, Friel S, Bell R, Houweling TAJ, Taylor S Commission on Social Derterminants of Health. Closing the gap in a generation: Health equity through action on the social determinants of health. Lancet. 2008;372(9650):1661–1669. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61690-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Marmot MG, Stansfeld S, Patel C, North F, Head J, White I, et al. Health inequalities among British civil servants: the Whitehall II study. The Lancet. 1991;337(8754):1387–1393. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(91)93068-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. McLoyd VC. The impact of economic hardship on Black families and children: Psychological distress, parenting, and socioemotional development. Child Development. 1990;61(2):311–346. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02781.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. McLoyd VC. Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psychologist. 1998;53(2):185. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.53.2.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Miller G, Chen E, Cole SW. Health psychology: Developing biologically plausible models linking the social world and physical health. Annual Review of Psychology. 2009;60:501–524. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Milne B, Moffitt T, Crump R, Poulton R, Rutter M, Sears M, et al. How should we construct psychiatric family history scores? A comparison of alternative approaches from the Dunedin Family Health History Study. Psychological Medicine. 2008;38(12):1793–1802. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Moffitt TE. Teen-aged mothers in contemporary Britain. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2002;43(6):727–742. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Jaffee SR, Kim-Cohen J, Koenen KC, Odgers CL, et al. DSM-V Conduct Disorder: Research needs for an evidence base. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2007;49:3–33. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01823.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Harrington H, Milne BJ. Males on the life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways: Follow-up at age 26 years. Development and Psychopathology. 2002;14(1):179–207. doi: 10.1017/s0954579402001104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Krueger RF, Magdol L, Margolin G, Silva PA, et al. Do partners agree about abuse in their relationship?: A psychometric evaluation of interpartner agreement. Psychological Assessment. 1997;9(1):47. [Google Scholar]
  73. Moilanen I, Linna SL, Ebeling H, Kumpulainen K, Tamminen T, Piha J, et al. Are twins' behavioural/emotional problems different from singletons'? European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1999;8:62–67. doi: 10.1007/pl00010702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Mrug S, Windle M. Mediators of neighborhood influences on externalizing behavior in preadolescent children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2009;37(2):265–280. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9274-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Muthén B, Muthén LK. Mplus (Version 5.0) Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén; 1998–2008. [Google Scholar]
  76. Odgers CL, Caspi A, Bates C, Sampson RJ, Moffitt TE. Systematic social observation of children’s neighborhoods using Google Street View: A reliable and costeffective method. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02565.x. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Odgers CL, Moffitt TE, Broadbent JM, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H, et al. Female and male antisocial trajectories: From childhood origins to adult outcomes. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20(2):673–716. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Odgers CL, Moffitt TE, Tach LM, Sampson RJ, Taylor A, Matthews CL, et al. The protective effects of neighborhood collective efficacy on British children growing up in deprivation: A developmental analysis. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45(4):942–957. doi: 10.1037/a0016162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Olds DL. The nurse–family partnership: An evidence-based preventive intervention. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2006;27(1):5–25. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Osgood DW, Wilson JK, O'Malley PM, Bachman JG, Johnston LD. Routine activities and individual deviant behavior. American Sociological Review. 1996:635–655. [Google Scholar]
  81. Patrick MR, Snyder J, Schrepferman LM. The joint contribution of early parental warmth, communication and tracking, and early child conduct problems on monitoring in late childhood. Child Development. 2005;76(5):999–1014. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00893.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Patterson GR, Forgatch MS, DeGarmo DS. Cascading effects following intervention. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22(4):949–970. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Pettit GS, Laird RD, Dodge KA, Bates JE, Criss MM. Antecedents and behavior-problem outcomes of parental monitoring and psychological control in early adolescence. Child Development. 2001;72(2):583–598. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Poulton R, Caspi A. Commentary: How does socioeconomic disadvantage during childhood damage health in adulthood? Testing psychosocial pathways. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2005;34(2):344. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyi044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Racz SJ, McMahon RJ. The relationship between parental knowledge and monitoring and child and adolescent conduct problems: A 10-year update. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2011;14(4):377–398. doi: 10.1007/s10567-011-0099-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Ram N, Grimm K. Using simple and complex growth models to articulate developmental change: Matching theory to method. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2007;31(4):303–316. [Google Scholar]
  87. Raykov T. Analysis of longitudinal studies with missing data using covariance structure modeling with full-information maximum likelihood. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2005;12(3):493–505. [Google Scholar]
  88. Reijneveld SA, Veenstra R, de Winter AF, Verhulst FC, Ormel J, de Meer G. Area deprivation affects behavioral problems of young adolescents in mixed urban and rural areas: The TRAILS study. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2010;46(2):189–196. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Rutter M. Poverty and child mental health. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2003;290(15):2063. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.15.2063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Sampson RJ. The neighborhood context of well-being. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine. 2003;46(3):S53–S64. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Sampson RJ, Laub JH. Urban poverty and the family context of delinquency: A new look at structure and process in a classic study. Child Development. 1994;65(2):523–540. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Sampson RJ, Morenoff JD, Earls F. Beyond social capital: Spatial dynamics of collective efficacy for children. American Sociological Review. 1999;64(5):633–660. [Google Scholar]
  93. Sampson RJ, Morenoff JD, Gannon-Rowley T. Assessing "neighborhood effects": Social processes and new directions in research. Annual Review of Sociology. 2002;28:443–478. [Google Scholar]
  94. Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW, Earls F. Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science. 1997;277(5328):918–924. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5328.918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Sanders MR. Triple P-Positive Parenting Program: Towards an empirically validated multilevel parenting and family support strategy for the prevention of behavior and emotional problems in children. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 1999;2(2):71–90. doi: 10.1023/a:1021843613840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Schneiders J, Drukker M, Van der Ende J, Verhulst F, Van Os J, Nicolson N. Neighbourhood socioeconomic disadvantage and behavioural problems from late childhood into early adolescence. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2003;57(9):699–703. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.9.699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Schonberg MA, Shaw DS. Do the predictors of child conduct problems vary by high-and low-levels of socioeconomic and neighborhood risk? Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2007;10(2):101–136. doi: 10.1007/s10567-007-0018-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Selzer ML, Vinokur A, Vanrooijen L. Self-Administered Short Michigan Alcoholism Screening-Test (Smast) Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1975;36(1):117–126. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1975.36.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Shaw CR, McKay HD. Juvenile delinquency and urban areas. Chicago, Ill: 1942. [Google Scholar]
  100. Shonkoff JP. Leveraging science to catalyze innovative early childhood policy and practice; Paper presented at the National Academy of Sciences Sackler Colloquium Biological Embedding of Early Social Adversity: From Fruit Flies to Kindergartners; Irvine, CA. 2012. [Google Scholar]
  101. Shonkoff JP, Boyce WT, McEwen BS. Neuroscience, molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2009;301(21):2252–2259. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Shonkoff JP, Phillips D. From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2000. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Skinner HA. The Drug-Abuse Screening-Test. Addictive Behaviors. 1982;7(4):363–371. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(82)90005-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Spillman JR. Double exposure - Coping with newborn twins at home. Midwife, Health Visitor and Community Nurse. 1984;20:92–94. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Stanger C, Achenbach TM, Verhulst FC. Accelerated longitudinal comparisons of aggressive versus delinquent syndromes. Development and Psychopathology. 1997;9(1):43–58. doi: 10.1017/s0954579497001053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Stattin H, Kerr M. Parental monitoring: A reinterpretation. Child Development. 2000;71(4):1072–1085. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Straus MA. Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The Conflict Tactics (CT) Scales. In: Straus MA, Gelles RJ, editors. Physical violence in American families: Risk factors and adaptations to violence in 8,145 families. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers; 1990. pp. 403–424. [Google Scholar]
  108. Taylor SE, Seeman TE. Psychosocial resources and the SES-health relationship. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1999;896(1):210–225. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08117.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Tolan PH, Gorman-Smith D, Henry DB. The developmental ecology of urban males' youth violence. Developmental Psychology. 2003;39(2):274. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.39.2.274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Tremblay RE. The development of aggressive behaviour during childhood: What have we learned in the past century? International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2000;24(2):129–141. [Google Scholar]
  111. Tremblay RE, Nagin DS. The developmental origins of physical aggression in humans. In: Tremblay RE, Hartup WW, Archer J, editors. Developmental origins of aggression. New York: Guilford Press; 2005. pp. 83–106. [Google Scholar]
  112. Trouton A, Spinath FM, Plomin R. Twins Early Development Study (TEDS): A multivariate, longitudinal genetic investigation of language, cognition and behavior problems in childhood. Twin Research. 2002;5(5):444–448. doi: 10.1375/136905202320906255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. van den Oord EJ, Koot HM, Boomsma DI, Verhulst FC, Orlebeke JF. A twin-singleton comparison of problem behavior in 2 to 3 year-olds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 1995;36(3):449–458. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb01302.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Vanderbilt-Adriance E, Shaw DS. Protective factors and the development of resilience in the context of neighborhood disadvantage. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36(6):887–901. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9220-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Wasserman CR, Shaw GM, Selvin S, Gould JB, Syme SL. Socioeconomic status, neighborhood social conditions, and neural tube defects. American Journal of Public Health. 1998;88(11):1674. doi: 10.2105/ajph.88.11.1674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Weissman MM, Wickramaratne P, Adams P, Wolk S, Verdeli H, Olfson M. Brief screening for family psychiatric history: the family history screen. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2000;57(7):675. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.7.675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Wight VR, Chau M, Aratani Y. Who are America's Poor Children?: The Official Story. New York: National Center for Children in Poverty; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  118. Wilson WJ. The truly disadvantaged: The inner city, the underclass, and public policy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1987. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES