Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2013 Jan 22;8(1):e54197. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054197

Imaging Conditioned Fear Circuitry Using Awake Rodent fMRI

Nichola M Brydges 1, Heather C Whalley 2, Maurits A Jansen 1,3, Gavin D Merrifield 3, Emma R Wood 4, Stephen M Lawrie 2, Sara-Madge Wynne 3, Mark Day 5, Sue Fleetwood-Walker 2, Douglas Steele 6, Ian Marshall 2,3,, Jeremy Hall 1,2,*,, Megan C Holmes 1,2,3,*,
Editor: Yann Herault7
PMCID: PMC3551953  PMID: 23349824

Abstract

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) is a powerful method for exploring emotional and cognitive brain responses in humans. However rodent fMRI has not previously been applied to the analysis of learned behaviour in awake animals, limiting its use as a translational tool. Here we have developed a novel paradigm for studying brain activation in awake rats responding to conditioned stimuli using fMRI. Using this method we show activation of the amygdala and related fear circuitry in response to a fear-conditioned stimulus and demonstrate that the magnitude of fear circuitry activation is increased following early life stress, a rodent model of affective disorders. This technique provides a new translatable method for testing environmental, genetic and pharmacological manipulations on emotional and cognitive processes in awake rodent models.

Introduction

There is a clear requirement for improved translation of preclinical neuroscience research into clinical impact. One particular area of need is the development of measures of neural activation that can be translated directly from animals to humans. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) provides a system-wide measure of neuronal activation which is non-invasive, repeatable and lends itself to paradigms translatable across species. Until now however, rodent fMRI has been largely limited to anaesthetized animals [1], [2] or animals at rest or responding to an innate stimulus [3][6], and has not been applied to the study of learned behaviours in awake animals, limiting its use as a translational tool. Here we have used fMRI to assess emotional function in awake rodents with the aim of generating translatable methods for investigating the aetiology and treatment of neuropsychiatric disorders.

In order to develop awake rodent fMRI of a learned task, we focused on Pavlovian fear conditioning. Pavlovian fear conditioning produces robust behavioural and physiological responses and has been shown to elicit amygdala activation in human imaging studies [7][9]. Furthermore abnormal fear conditioning and altered amygdala activation have been implicated in a number of human neuropsychiatric conditions including affective disorders, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and schizophrenia [7], [10]. The development of rodent imaging models of fear conditioning is therefore of utility for studying translational models of these conditions. Early life stress (ELS), known to increase fear and anxiety responses in adulthood, has been extensively used as an animal model of human affective disorders [11][13]. Furthermore ELS in humans is associated with later affective disorders and increased amygdala activation in adulthood [14][17]. Here we therefore report both the development of awake rodent fMRI imaging and its modulation by ELS.

Materials and Methods

Animals

32 male Lister Hooded rats (Harlan, UK) weighing 260 g ±20 g were housed in small groups (4–5 per cage) with water and chow available ad libitum in a humidity (45–60%), temperature (21°C) and light (on 07.00–19.00 h) controlled environment. Prior to experimentation, all animals were handled daily for 14–21 days. All studies were carried out in strict accordance with local ethics guidelines (approved by The University of Edinburgh Ethical Review Committee), the UK Home Office Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 and under a personal Home Office license.

Acclimitisation of Animals to the MRI Scanning environment

Prior to scanning, animals were acclimatised to the restraint apparatus and scanning environment on days 1 and 3 using a mock MRI scanner [18]. This procedure produced stable physiological responses within the normal range for adult rats (Table 1). Acclimatisation was carried out in a purpose built mock scanner, with recordings of scanner noise played through speakers in the apparatus at a level equivalent to that measured in the scanner itself. All animals were placed in the mock scanner within a rodent restraint apparatus in order to minimize motion artefacts during subsequent MRI scanning (InsightMRI, Worcester, MA). The restraint procedure is described in King et al. [18]. Briefly, a plastic semicircular headpiece with blunted ear supports that fit into the ear canals was positioned over the ears. The head was placed into a cylindrical head holder with the animal's canines secured over a bite bar and ears positioned inside the head holder with adjustable screws fitted into lateral sleeves. An adjustable surface coil built into the head holder was pressed firmly on the head and locked into place. The body of the animal was placed into a body restrainer. The head-piece locks into a mounting post on the front of the chassis. This design isolates all of the body movements from the head restrainer and minimizes motion artifact. Rats were lightly anaesthetized using 1–2% isoflurane in air while being placed into and removed from the restraint apparatus and then allowed to recover consciousness thereafter.

Table 1. Heart rate levels obtained during preliminary testing using a mock scanner.

Day Heart rate (beats per min)
1 300+/−27 S.E.
3 284+/−8.7 S.E.
5 311+/−9.8 S.E.

Data Mean ±SEM, n = 6 rats. Previous telemetry evidence suggests a resting heart rate of around 300–380 bpm in adult rats.

Animal preparation and conditioning protocol

Animals were subsequently conditioned to a fear-associated cue on day 4 in a separate and distinct context, and were scanned in the real MRI scanner on day 5 (Figure 1). The conditioning apparatus comprised a rat shock chamber (30 cm ×25 cm ×32 cm), and the conditioned stimulus (CS) was a 10 s flashing light (10s of 5 Hz maximum intensity flashes, 50/50 duty cycle). The unconditioned stimulus (US) was a 0.5 s, 0.5 mA foot shock delivered through the bars of the floor. Fear conditioning was conducted on experimental day 4, allowing retrieval of the conditioned response in the scanner 24 h later. Animals were trained in paired and unpaired groups (PG (n = 14) and UG (n = 10) respectively). The PG received 5 presentations of the CS, each co-terminating with the US. These pairs were presented at intervals of 5±1min for a total of 25 min. The UG received 5 presentations of the CS (intervals 5±1 min) with 5 presentations of the US in a pseudorandom order (explicitly avoiding any pairing) over 25 min.

Figure 1. Training and scanning procedure.

Figure 1

Animals were acclimatised to the scanning procedure using a mock scanner, conditioned and then tested during retrieval in the scanner 24 h later.

We confirmed that conditioned fear responses are produced to subsequent presentations of the CS in PG animals but not in UG animals in a separate behavioural experiment using conditioned freezing as a dependent variable. 12 rats were conditioned using the above fear conditioning protocol. 6 experienced the PG procedure, 6 the UG procedure. Rats were conditioned on day 1, and placed into a novel context (to reduce contextual freezing) on the following day for retrieval. The novel context was a chamber (30 cm ×25 cm ×32 cm) with the walls and floor decorated with coloured plastic shapes, to distinguish it contextually from the training chamber. During retrieval, 12 blue LED lights, mounted in the ceiling, were flashed (5 Hz maximum intensity flashes, 50/50 duty cycle – identical to the training lights) for a duration of 2 min, followed by a rest of 2 min, and this was repeated twice more. The percentage of time the animals spent freezing during all periods was calculated. Only rats that had been conditioned to associate the conditioned stimulus (CS) with the aversive shock (US) exhibited freezing behaviour upon each presentation of the CS 24 h later (p<0.01). Animals from the unpaired group did not show a significant freezing response to the presentation of CS (Figure 2).

Figure 2. Behavioural response to the fear conditioning paradigm.

Figure 2

Animals were trained with paired CS-US presentations (paired group (PG), open bars; n = 6) and unpaired CS and US presentations (unpaired group (UG), black bars; n = 6) and freezing behaviour was assessed 24 h later in a separate context. A two-way ANOVA revealed that PG spent a greater proportion of time freezing than UG animals during all three stimulus presentations (P<0.01). Data is represented as mean ± SEM.

Image acquisition and analysis

The day after fear conditioning (day 5), rats were lightly anaesthetized, put into the restraint apparatus and placed into a 7T MRI scanner (Agilent Technologies; formerly Varian, Oxford, UK) equipped with a 400 mT/m gradient set, a 72 mm volume RF coil for transmission and a surface coil for signal reception (InsightMRI, Worcester, MA). Anaesthesia was maintained until the animal was positioned in the coil assembly in the scanner and whole-brain structural images were acquired using a Fast Spin Echo (FSE) sequence with a matrix of 256×256, FOV 30 mm (in-plane resolution 117 μm), TR = 3000 ms, Echo Train Length (ETL) 8, effective TE = 48 ms, 4 signal averages, and 22×1 mm coronal slices. Animals were then allowed to regain consciousness (respiration level >85 breaths/min) before the test paradigm and functional scanning commenced.

After a 5 min acclimatisation period, bilateral visual CS stimuli were presented in the scanner using a custom-built array of high-intensity blue LEDs fitted with a diffuser. The activation protocol consisted of 2 min periods with the LEDs flashing at 5 Hz with a 50/50 duty cycle (“on”) alternating with 2 min periods during which the LEDs were not illuminated (“off”) with a randomized starting order. A total of three CS presentations were delivered in each scanning session. Functional image acquisition used a FSE sequence with a matrix of 64×64 (in-plane resolution 469 μm), TR = 2500ms, ETL 16, and effective TE = 36ms. Each functional volume consisted of 16×1 mm slices covering the brain excluding cerebellum and took 10s to acquire. Functional scanning was run for a total of 103 volumes (17 min 10 s).

Data analysis

SPM5 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, UK) was used for image analysis. Structural and functional data for each individual animal was manually checked for artifacts. To facilitate SPM processing, images were scaled by a factor of 10 in the x, y and z dimensions to allow for differences between rat and human brain sizes. A structural study-specific rat brain template was created using 18 Lister Hooded rats based on the Karolinska template (consistent with the Paxinos Atlas space [19]). A priori tissue segments were generated for SPM5 preprocessing procedures. Functional data for each animal was realigned to the mean volume in the series, and then spatially normalized according to SPM5 co-registration procedures using each individual animal's structural scan. Finally, all realigned and normalized images were smoothed with an 8×8×8 mm full width half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian filter. Global effects were removed using a voxel level linear model of the global signal [20].

First level analysis was performed using a general linear model implemented in SPM5. At an individual subject level, the 3 on/off periods were modelled separately along with the initial acclimatisation condition. This permitted analysis of individual blocks and therefore habituation effects. Each condition was modelled by a boxcar convolved with the SPM5 canonical hemodynamic response function. Contrasts were constructed to determine (i) the first CS block responses > all ‘off’ blocks (baseline), (ii) the first CS block responses > the last CS block, and (iii) the last CS block responses > the first CS block. For second level analysis one contrast image per subject for each contrast of interest (i–iii) was entered into a second level random effects model. Within group effects were modelled using a one sample t-test. Between-group differences were investigated using a two-sample t-test. Second level images were thresholded at a whole brain cluster level of Pcorrected<0.05. Monte Carlo simulations [21] showed that this was achieved by a simultaneous requirement for a voxel level significance of P<0.05 and activation clusters exceeding 387 contiguous voxels. A region of interest (ROI) analysis was applied for the lateral amygdalae using an anatomical rat bilateral lateral amygdala mask generated using MRIcro (http://www.cabiatl.com/mricro/mricro). Further Monte Carlo simulations indicated that when only the amygdala mask was of a priori interest, cluster level Pcorrected<0.05 was satisfied by a simultaneous requirement for a voxel level significance of p<0.05 and activation clusters exceeding 56 contiguous voxels.

Early Life Stress protocol

In order to examine the effects of early life stress on fear circuitry activation a further group of animals was subjected to a repeated variable stress paradigm between postnatal day (PND) 25–27 using previously established protocols [11][13]. This consisted of a 10min swim in a bucket of water on PND 25 (25 cm height, 34 cm diameter, temperature 25+/−1°C), restraint in a Perspex rat restraint tube on PND 26 (15 cm length, 5 cm diameter) for 3×30 mins with a 30 min break between restraint sessions, and footshock on PND 27 (6×0.5 mA, 0.5 s footshocks over 3 min in a 30 cm ×25 cm ×32 cm shock chamber (Coulbourn Instruments, PA)). This protocol has previously been shown to produce enhanced anxiety responses and depressive-like behaviours in rodent models [12], [13]. The rats were then left undisturbed in their home cages in groups of 4–5 until testing. As adults, the ELS rats (n = 8) were exposed to fear conditioning (as PG) and fMRI as outlined above and their activation patterns were compared to control (CON) rats that had not received ELS.

Results

fMRI activation maps in response to Conditioned Stimulus

Analysis of neural responses (BOLD activation) in animals from the PG during the first CS presentation in the scanner compared to the rest periods revealed activation of a neural system implicated in fear processing, comprising the right lateral amygdala (LA), hypothalamus (Hyp), somatosensory cortex (SSC) and granular insular cortex (GI) (Figure 3a; Table 2). In contrast the UG did not show activation of any of these fear-associated brain regions during the CS presentation (Table 2). Direct comparison of activation to the first CS in the PG compared to the UG confirmed greater activation in the PG of the right LA, Hyp, GI, left SSC and ventral pallidum (Figure 3b, Table 2). Analysis of brain responses across the three CS presentations revealed evidence of amygdala habituation over time, consistent with previous human MRI studies [7], [8] (Figure 3c–d; Table 3).

Figure 3. Brain activation maps in response to a fear conditioned stimulus.

Figure 3

(a) Activation in PG in response to CS presentation is observed in right lateral amygdala (LA) and hypothalamus (Hyp). (b) Comparison of response between groups (PG:UG) confirms LA, Hyp and granular insular cortex (GI) activation in PG in response to the CS presentation. Extinction modeling of (c) the response across CS presentations within the PG or (d) between PG and UG groups reveals right LA, Hyp, GI and somatosensory cortex activation (SSC). PG, n = 14; UG, n = 10.

Table 2. Brain activation during first CS presentation versus baseline.

KE T Region
PG within group (First CS> Baseline)
Whole brain analysis 495 3.18 R lateral hypothalamus R lateral amygdala L somatosensory cortex R + L granular insular cortex
Lateral amygdala mask 117 4.17 R lateral amygdala*
UG within group (First CS> Baseline)
Whole brain analysis 12020 15.43 Peak R subiculum extends to adjacent cortex

Significant regional brain activation (BOLD responses) to the first CS presentation compared to baseline. PG  =  paired group, UG  =  unpaired group. KE  =  cluster extent in voxels. T  =  peak voxel t-statistic. Clusters reaching P corrected <0.05 are shown.*  =  significant (P corrected <0.05) within lateral amygdala ROI. R =  right, L =  left, PVN =  paraventricular nucleus. PG, n =  14; UG, n = 10.

Table 3. Brain activation across extinction.

KE T Region
PG within group analysis (First CS >Last CS)
Whole brain analysis 826 5.61 R lateral amygdala
582 3.48 Hypothalamus
Lateral amygdala mask 147 3.35 R lateral amygdala*
PG within group analysis (Last CS> First CS)
Whole brain analysis 661 5 Thalamus (extending to central amygdala)
1589 4.19 Sensory cortex
1242 3.7 Ventral subiculum, midbrain
444 3.09 Ventral pallidum
Between Group Analysis
PG>UG (First CS>Last CS)
Whole brain analysis 1211 4.21 Sensory cortex
485 4.11 R lateral amygdala
1295 3.99 Granular insular cortex
825 3.26 Hypothalamus
Lateral amygdala mask 91 3.35 R lateral amygdala*
PG>UG (Last CS> First CS)
1542 3.97 RSG
1221 3.9 Ventral subiculum includes medial geniculate nucleus
457 3.89 Thalamus

Significant regional brain activation (BOLD responses) to the first CS presentation compared to last CS presentation and the reverse. PG  =  paired group, UG  =  unpaired group. KE  =  cluster extent in voxels. T  =  peak voxel t-statistic. Clusters reaching P corrected <0.05 are shown.*  =  significant (P corrected <0.05) within lateral amygdala ROI. R =  right, L =  left, RSG =  retrospenial granular cortex. PG, n =  14; UG, n = 10.

Early Life Stress causes increased activation of fear networks

We next investigated fear circuitry activation following early life stress (ELS). Analysis of neural responses during the presentation of the fear-associated CS in the ELS group revealed activation of a neural system comprising the left LA, SSC, retrosplenial granular cortex, ventral pallidum and caudate putamen during the first CS presentation compared to the rest periods (Table 4). Comparison of the ELS group to control animals showed greater activation of fear circuitry including both left and right amygdalae and hypothalamus in the ELS group compared to the CON group during the first CS presentation (Figure 4 and Table 4). These results demonstrate that ELS increases amygdala activation during adulthood, consistent with human studies [14][17].

Table 4. Effects of early life stress on brain activation.

KE T Region
ELS Within Group Analysis (First CS>Baseline)
Whole brain analysis 684 8.62 L ventral pallidum/cortex
1097 6.36 RSG/dorsal subiculum
725 5.66 R somatosensory cortex
793 5.42 Ventral pallidum extending to L lateral amygdala
662 4.98 R caudate putamen
Between Group Analysis
ELS>CON (First CS>Baseline)
Whole brain analysis 3808 4.23 Somatosensory cortex extending to thalamus and R lateral amygdale later latere lateral amygdala
1107 4.04 RSG/Dorsal subiculum
919 3.63 L ventral pallidum
2447 3.08 L central and lateral amygdala, lateral hypothalamus
CON>ELS (First CS>Baseline)
Whole brain analysis 541 3.15 R subiculum extending to brainstem

Within and between group analysis comparing BOLD response to first CS presentation over baseline in rats previously exposed to early life stress (ELS) compared to unstressed controls (CON). KE  =  cluster extent in voxels. T  =  peak voxel t-statistic. Clusters reaching P corrected <0.05 are shown. R =  right, L =  left, RSG =  retrospenial granular cortex. CON, n =  14; ELS, n = 8.

Figure 4. Consequence of early life stress on fear associated brain activation.

Figure 4

Greater activation in response to CS presentation is observed in right and left lateral amygdalae (LA), hypothalamus (Hyp) and optic grey (optic) when ELS is compared to CON. (a) Coronal, (b) axial and (c) sagittal sections of rat brain. Control, n = 14; ELS, n = 8.

Discussion

Here we have developed a novel fMRI paradigm for examining neural activation in awake rodents processing a learned emotional stimulus. Using this method we demonstrate activation of fear-related circuitry, including the amygdala and hypothalamus, to an aversively conditioned CS. We also show that this fear response is exacerbated in an animal model of human affective disorders based on prior exposure to ELS. This represents the first demonstration of the delineation of regional brain function in response to a learnt association in awake rodents using fMRI, opening the door to the use of fMRI for translational research into human affective and neuropsychiatric disorders.

In order to demonstrate specific changes in neural activation related to fear learning, we used a tightly controlled fear conditioning paradigm. We were able to show that only rats in which the CS was specifically paired with the US, leading to a conditioned association, demonstrated increased activation of the right LA, Hyp, SSC and GI in response to re-exposure to the CS. In contrast no such responses were seen in animals exposed to the same stimuli in an unpaired (non-contingent) manner. The LA is known to be a key brain region involved in the formation of fear associations and emotional learning, whilst the Hyp is central to the mediation of behavioural responses to fear-associated stimuli [22], [23]. The GI and SSC are constituents of pain pathways, which are recruited into the conditioned network in response to fear-associated CS [24], [25]. These results closely parallel previous human fear conditioning fMRI studies [8] and also indicate an unexpected, but not unrecognised, lateralization of amygdala activation during the expression of conditioned fear [14], [26].

We additionally investigated whether our fMRI paradigm was sensitive to the effects of early life stress. ELS has previously been shown to result in increased anxiety and depression-like behaviour in rodent models [11][13]. In our current study we show that pre-pubertal ELS increases bilateral LA and Hyp activation to fear-associated stimuli in adulthood. This demonstrates the utility of rodent fMRI for investigating the aetiology and treatment of affective disorders using methods that can be directly translated into human imaging studies.

Although previous studies have used fMRI in animals, and the behavioural and neural circuitry of fear conditioning are well validated, the novel combination of these techniques in the present study allows a system wide analysis of brain activation during such cognitive and emotional responses. These findings demonstrate that it is possible to apply fMRI to the analysis of regional brain activation during learnt tasks in awake rodents, paving the way to translational investigations of the effects of experimental manipulations and of the neural basis of fMRI signal changes that would be difficult, if not impossible, to achieve using human studies alone.

Acknowledgments

We express thanks to C. Ferris, T Stolberg and P Kulkarni, Northeastern University, Boston for help and advice for imaging awake rats.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by a Wyeth-TMRC grant NS-EU-136 (IM, SF-W, MCH, JH, ERW, SML, MAJ). JH is supported by a Scottish Senior Clinical Fellowship. Further support was provided by the Dr Mortimer and Theresa Sackler Foundation JH) and the University of Edinburgh Centre for Cognitive Aging and Cognitive Epidemiology, part of the cross council Lifelong Health and Wellbeing Initiative (G0700704/84698)(MCG, IM and ERW). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1. Kim T, Masamoto K, Fukuda M, Vazquez A, Kim SG (2010) Frequency-dependent neural activity, CBF, and BOLD fMRI to somatosensory stimuli in isoflurane-anesthetized rats. Neuroimage 52: 224–233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Governo RJ, Morris PG, Marsden CA, Chapman V (2008) Gabapentin evoked changes in functional activity in nociceptive regions in the brain of the anaesthetized rat: an fMRI study. Br J Pharmacol 153: 1558–1567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Ferris CF, Kulkarni P, Sullivan JM Jr, Harder JA, Messenger TL, et al. (2005) Pup suckling is more rewarding than cocaine: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and three-dimensional computational analysis. J Neurosci 25: 149–156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Chin C, Upadhyay J, Marek G, Baker S, Zhang M, et al. (2011) Awake rat pharmacological magnetic resonance imaging as a translational pharmacodynamic biomarker: metabotropic glutamate 2/3 agonist modulation of ketamine-induced blood oxygenation level dependence signals. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 336: 709–715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Becerra L, Chang PC, Bishop J, Borsook D (2011) CNS activation maps in awake rats exposed to thermal stimuli to the dorsum of the hindpaw. Neuroimage 54: 1355–1366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Zhang N, Rane P, Huang W, Liang Z, Kennedy D, et al. (2010) Mapping resting-state brain networks in conscious animals. J Neurosci Methods 189: 186–196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Romaniuk L, Honey GD, King JR, Whalley HC, McIntosh AM, et al. (2010) Midbrain activation during Pavlovian conditioning and delusional symptoms in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 67: 1246–1254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Buchel C, Morris J, Dolan RJ, Friston KJ (1998) Brain systems mediating aversive conditioning: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron 20: 947–957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Yaniv D, Desmedt A, Jaffard R, Richter-Levin G (2004) The amygdala and appraisal processes: stimulus and response complexity as an organizing factor. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 44: 179–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Shin LM, Liberzon I (2010) The neurocircuitry of fear, stress, and anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 35: 169–191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Toledo-Rodriguez M, Sandi C (2007) Stress before puberty exerts a sex- and age-related impact on auditory and contextual fear conditioning in the rat. Neural plasticity 2007: 71203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Jacobson-Pick S, Richter-Levin G (2010) Differential impact of juvenile stress and corticosterone in juvenility and in adulthood, in male and female rats. Behav Brain Res 214: 268–276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Avital A, Richter-Levin G (2005) Exposure to juvenile stress exacerbates the behavioural consequences of exposure to stress in the adult rat. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 8: 163–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Dannlowski U, Stuhrmann A, Beutelmann V, Zwanzger P, Lenzen T, et al. (2012) Limbic scars: long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment revealed by functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging. Biol Psychiatry 71: 286–293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Meewisse ML, Olff M, Kleber R, Kitchiner NJ, Gersons BP (2011) The course of mental health disorders after a disaster: Predictors and comorbidity. J Trauma Stress 24: 405–413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Weich S, Patterson J, Shaw R, Stewart-Brown S (2009) Family relationships in childhood and common psychiatric disorders in later life: systematic review of prospective studies. Br J Psychiatry 194: 392–398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Bremne JD, Vermetten E (2001) Stress and development: behavioral and biological consequences. Dev Psychopathol 13: 473–489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.King JA, Garelick TS, Brevard ME, Chen W, Messenger TL, et al.. (2005) Procedure for minimizing stress for fMRI studies in conscious rats. J Neurosci Methods 148; 154–160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Schweinhardt P, Fransson P, Olson L, Spenger C, Andersson JLR (2003) A template for spatial normalisation of MR images of the rat brain. J Neurosci Methods 129: 105–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Macey PM, Macey KE, Kumar R, Harper RM (2004) A method for removal of global effects from fMRI time series. Neuroimage 22: 360–366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Slotnick SD, Moo LR, Segal JB, Hart J (2003) Distinct prefrontal cortex activity associated with item memory and source memory for visual shapes. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 17: 75–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Johansen JP, Wolff SB, Luthi A, LeDoux JE (2012) Controlling the elements: an optogenetic approach to understanding the neural circuits of fear. Biol Psychiatry 71: 1053–1060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. LeDoux JE (2000) Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 23: 155–184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Sacco T, Sacchetti B (2010) Role of secondary sensory cortices in emotional memory storage and retrieval in rats. Science 329: 649–656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Shi C, Davis M (1999) Pain pathways involved in fear conditioning measured with fear-potentiated startle: lesion studies. J Neurosci 19: 420–430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Phelps EA, LeDoux JE (2005) Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models to human behavior. Neuron 48: 175–187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES