Abstract
Religiosity is associated with behaviors that reduce the risk of HIV/STI infection among general-population and heterosexual-specific samples. Whether this association is similar for homosexual persons is unknown. Measures of religiosity have not been evaluated psychometrically among men who have sex with men (MSM), a population who, because of stigma, experience religiosity differently than heterosexual persons. We assessed the DUREL and the SWB (short form) in two samples of MSM. Neither instrument produced adequate model fit. To study the association between religiosity and HIV/STI risk behaviors among MSM, scales are needed that measure the religious and spiritual experiences of MSM.
Keywords: religion, spirituality, HIV prevention, gay men
Introduction
Religiosity is associated with sexual health. In general-population or heterosexual-specific samples, persons who are more religious have fewer sexual partners (Billy, Tanfer, Grady, & Klepinger, 1993; Davidson, Moore, & Ullstrup, 2004; Lefkowitz, Gillen, Shearer, & Boone, 2004; Seidman, Mosher, & Aral, 1992), which reduces risk for HIV and other sexually-transmissible infections. Among adolescents, there is evidence of an increase in condom use and some evidence of later sexual debut associated with greater religiosity; this association appears strongest for females (Dodge, Sandfort, Yarber, & de Wit, 2005; Fierros-Gonzalez & Brown, 2002; Galvan, Collins, Kanouse, Pantoja, & Golinelli, 2007; Kramer, Hogue, & Gaydos, 2007; Manlove, Terry-Humen, Ikramullah, & Moore, 2006; Nonnemaker, McNeely, & Blum, 2003; Studer & Arland, 1987; Zaleski & Schiaffino, 2000). Whether religiosity performs similarly among homosexual persons remains largely unknown.
Organized religions differ in their acceptance of homosexuality (Machacek & Wilcox, 2003). Men who have sex with men (MSM) affiliated with a religion not accepting of homosexuality have a different religious experience than their heterosexual peers as they attempt to reconcile their religion's teachings about homosexuality with their same-sex attractions (Garcia, Gray-Stanley, & Ramirez-Valles, 2008; Roseborough, 2006; Shallenberger, 1996; Smith & Horne, 2007). Homosexual persons who reject the tenants of the religious organizations in which they were raised because of a condemnation of homosexuality appear likely to also reject the health-promoting norms of the organization, increasing risk behavior (Rosario, Yali, Joyce, & Gwadz, 2006).
We cannot assume scales developed to measure religious constructs function similarly in heterosexual and homosexual samples because of the difference in how homosexuals experience religion. Most of the commonly used scales of religiosity and spirituality have not been validated in homosexual samples (Hill & Hood Jr., 1999). Valid measures of religiosity and spirituality are needed to determine if the associations between religiosity and HIV/STI risk behaviors are similar between heterosexual and homosexual persons.
In 2010, we collected data from two online samples of MSM. In one survey, we included the Duke Religion Index (DUREL; Koenig, Parkerson, & Meador, 1997), and in another survey we included a recently developed short version of the spiritual well-being scale (SWB; Bufford, 2010) to assess their potential usefulness in samples of MSM. The DUREL has been validated in two samples of college students (Storch et al., 2004; Storch, Strawser, & Storch, 2004) and used to assess religiosity in samples of healthcare providers (Cheever et al., 2005; Kim, Seidlitz, Ro, Evinger, & Duberstein, 2004; McCauley et al., 2005), patients living with HIV (Cotton et al., 2006; Szaflarski et al., 2006), heart disease (Krucoff et al., 2001) or cancer (Sherman et al., 2001), women (Dedert et al., 2004), adolescents (Lee, Miller, & Chang, 2006), collegiate athletes (Storch, Storch, Kovacs, Okun, & Welsh, 2003), and older adults (Klemmack et al., 2007). The full SWB (Ellison, 1983) has been validated in samples of college students (Bassett et al., 2005; Genia, 2001) and has been used to assess spiritual well-being in samples of healthcare providers (Saguil, Fitzpatrick, & Clark, 2011), patients living with HIV (Szaflarski, et al., 2006) or cardiovascular disease (Lawler & Younger, 2002), persons accessing outpatient medical services (Daaleman, 1999), substance users (Borman & Dixon, 1998), adolescents (Davis, Kerr, & Kurpius, 2003; Tshabalala & Patel, 2010), war veterans (Mihaljević et al., 2011), older adults (Eggers, 2003; Gow, Watson, Whiteman, & Deary, 2011), Jews (Musgrave & McFarlane, 2004), Christians (Fee & Ingram, 2004), and clergy (Francis & Stacks, 2003).
One study of 93 gay and lesbian individuals living in the Midwestern US used the SWB to examine associations between the scales' constructs and measures of being well-adjusted (Tan, 2005). The study found increased existential well-being was associated with increased self-esteem and acceptance of one's homosexuality, and associated with decreased feelings of alienation. The same study found religious well-being to be a non-significant variable in regression models. Tan concluded that increased existential well-being was associated with being well-adjusted and that for gay and lesbian persons in his sample, the reconciliation of sexual attraction with religious beliefs was not necessary for being well-adjusted. Tan did not attempt to validate the scale in his sample before using it for his analysis. We found no studies validating the DUREL or the SWB in samples of MSM.
In this study, we used confirmatory factor analyses to examine the internal validity of each measure. Additionally, we estimated the association between the scale measures and hypothesized external correlates, including internalized homonegativity, depressive symptoms, affect, and substance use. Given that internalized homonegativity measures the extent to which a person internalizes negative social attitudes toward homosexuality (Meyer & Dean, 1998) and the extant negative views against homosexuality espoused by several religions, we hypothesized that religiosity would be positively associated with internalized homonegativity. Since religiosity should increase internal conflict in homosexually-oriented persons, we also hypothesized a positive association between religiosity and measures of depressive symptoms and negative affect. Presumably, men who remain affiliated with a faith tradition in spite of the potential negative consequences must have a motivation for doing so. Thus, we also expected religious MSM to experience benefits. Assuming findings from the general population and heterosexual literature are transferable to MSM, we expected more religious MSM to have more positive affect (Koenig, 1998) and less substance use (Chitwood, Weiss, & Leukefeld, 2008; Van der Meer Sanchez, Garcia De Oliveira, & Aparecida Nappo, 2008).
Study One: Validation of the Duke Religion Index
Methods
Study design
In 2008, we began a four-wave prospective, matched sample study of 16 U.S. metropolitan statistical areas (MSAs) with different legislation on homosexuality. The purpose of the larger project was to assess the extent to which legislation about homosexuality influenced alcohol use and risky sexual behavior among MSM. Data for this analysis came from the third wave of data collection. Participants were recruited between April 22, 2010 and July 31, 2010 with geo-targeted banner advertisements and broadcast emails to active members from one of the nation's largest websites for gay men. Banner advertisements and emails directed interested persons to a webpage hosted on a dedicated university server with appropriate encryption to ensure data security. Eligibility criteria included being a man having prior sexual experience with a man, being 18 years or older, and reporting a residential zip code in a MSA under study. For this analysis, we were not interested in differences between MSAs with pro- or anti-gay legislation. We restricted the sample to the 2,060 men from across the 16 MSAs who reported identifying as Christian, atheist/agnostic, or spiritual. We excluded 378 participants affiliated with a variety of other faiths. Within each of the excluded faith traditions, we lacked enough persons to include them in the analysis. The mean survey completion time was 41 minutes. Participants were compensated $30 for their time. A Certificate of Confidentiality was obtained from the National Institutes of Health. The study was conducted under the oversight of the Institutional Review Board of the researchers' home institution. A refuse to answer response option allowed participants to opt out of answering any item.
Measures
The measures relevant to this paper are described below:
Religious or spiritual affiliation
Participants were asked to indicate the primary religious or spiritual tradition with which they currently identify. The list of religious and spiritual traditions was a modification of a list developed by Roof and McKinney (1987).
Duke Religion Index (DUREL)
Developed by Koenig, Parkerson, and Meador (1997), this five-item Likert-type scale measures three dimensions of religiosity. The organizational item asked about frequency of attendance at religious services (six response options: 1=never, 6=more than one time per week). The non-organizational item asked about frequency of private religious activities such as prayer or meditation (six response options: 1=rarely or never, 6=more than one time per day). The three subjective, or intrinsic, religiosity items asked about ever experiencing the presence of the divine, allowing religious beliefs to guide an approach to life, and transporting religion into other areas of life (five response options; 1=definitely not true, 5=definitely true). In our sample of MSM, the Cronbach alpha for the intrinsic religiosity dimension was 0.89.
Revised Reactions to Homosexuality Scale
Internalized homonegativity was assessed using Smolenski, Diamond, Ross, and Rosser's (2010) Revised Reactions to Homosexuality Scale. Responses to the seven 7-point Likert-type questions ranged from strongly disagree to strongly agree and included three constructs: personal comfort with being gay (two items), public identification as gay (three items), and social comfort with gay men (two items). In our sample, the Cronbach alpha for the entire scale was 0.84.
>CES-D short form
The CES-D short form (Cole, Rabin, Smith, & Kaufman, 2004) is a self-report assessment of depressive symptoms comprising four constructs: negative affect (two-items), positive affect (two items), somatic symptoms (three items), and interpersonal symptoms (three items). Participants were asked to respond to ten 4-point Likert-type items ranging from rarely/none to most of the time. In our sample, the overall Cronbach alpha was 0.85.
CAGE
We screened participants for problematic drinking using the four item CAGE questionnaire (Ewing, 1984). Participants who responded “yes” to at least two of four questions were classified as problematic drinkers. In our sample, the Kuder-Richardson 20 for the screener was 0.72, indicating adequate internal consistency.
Demographics
Participants were asked to enter their current age, education, race and ethnicity. To measure sexual orientation, participants could choose from gay/homosexual, bisexual, heterosexual, or could select “other” and type in their preferred identity. We assessed HIV status by asking participants if they had ever received an HIV diagnosis (yes/no).
Analysis
The purpose of this analysis was to validate the DUREL in a sample of MSM with diverse religious and spiritual beliefs. We used Mplus, version 5.2 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2009) to test the three items representing intrinsic religiosity for configural measurement invariance (equivalence of factor model specifications) and metric measurement invariance (equivalence of factor loadings; Meredith, 1993; Meredith & Teresi, 2006; Wu, Li, & Zumbo, 2007) between participants identifying as Christians, atheists/agnostics, and spiritual. The three-item model could not be assessed for absolute fit to the data since the model had an equal number of parameters and observations (i.e., just identified). We used STATA-IC version 11.1 (StataCorp LP, 2009) to calculate correlations between the DUREL constructs, internalized homonegativity, and depression. We used t-tests to compare the means of the DUREL constructs between individuals with problematic and non-problematic drinking as assessed by the CAGE questionnaire.
Results
Our sample was diverse. While 83.6% of the participants were raised Christian, only 43.2% currently identify with this faith tradition; 28.9% identify as atheist or agnostic and 28.0% identify as spiritual (but not Christian). When grouped by religious affiliation, participants differed by age, race, sexual orientation, and HIV-status (Table 1). Participants who identified as atheist or agnostic were younger than participants who identified as Christian or spiritual. The Christian group had a greater proportion of African Americans and fewer persons identifying as gay as compared to the other two groups. Finally, the prevalence of HIV was lowest among the atheist/agnostic group.
Table 1.
Christian n=889 | Atheist/Agnostic n=595 | Spiritual n=576 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
||||||
n (%) | n (%) | n (%) | F | df | P | |
|
||||||
Age [mean (sd)] | 38.4 (12.1) | 32.6 (10.4) | 37.6 (11.3) | 49.4 | 2.0 | <0.001 |
Education | n.a. | n.a. | 0.411 | |||
Less than high school | 6 (0.7) | 2 (0.3) | 3 (0.5) | |||
High school or GED | 82 (9.2) | 41 (6.9) | 49 (8.5) | |||
Technical school | 34 (3.8) | 15 (2.5) | 20 (3.5) | |||
Some college | 270 (30.4) | 181 (30.4) | 200 (34.7) | |||
College degree | 313 (35.3) | 231 (38.8) | 195 (33.9) | |||
Graduate degree | 183 (20.6) | 125 (21.0) | 109 (18.9) | |||
Race | n.a. | n.a. | <0.001 | |||
American Indian/Alaskan Native | 23 (2.6) | 14 (2.4) | 30 (5.2) | |||
Asian | 18 (2.0) | 29 (4.9) | 13 (2.3) | |||
African American | 72 (8.1) | 13 (2.2) | 24 (4.2) | |||
White | 720 (81.0) | 504 (84.7) | 471 (81.8) | |||
Other | 56 (6.3) | 35 (5.9) | 38 (6.6) | |||
Hispanic | ||||||
Yes | 91 (10.2) | 42 (7.1) | 49 (8.5) | n.a. | n.a. | 0.102 |
No | 798 (89.8) | 553 (92.9) | 527 (91.5) | |||
Sexual Orientation | n.a. | n.a. | <0.001 | |||
Gay/Homosexual | 733 (82.5) | 528 (88.7) | 533 (92.5) | |||
Other | 156 (17.6) | 67 (11.3) | 43 (7.47) | |||
Living with HIV | n.a. | n.a. | <0.001 | |||
Yes | 158 (17.8) | 66 (11.1) | 129 (22.4) | |||
No | 731 (82.2) | 529 (88.9) | 447 (77.6) |
Note: α=0.05. For age, statistical significance determined using ANOVA. Scheffe multiple comparison test indicates atheists/agnostic participants differ in age from Christian and spiritual participants (p<0.001). For education, race, Hispanic ethnicity, sexual orientation, and HIV-status, statistical significance determined using Pearson's chi-square.
We were unable to validate the three-item measure of intrinsic religiosity across religious affiliation groups. Constraining the three-item model to have the same factor loadings across Christian, spiritual, and atheist/agnostic participants produced a statistically significant worsening of model fit (ΔX2=17.04, df=4, p=0.002). While the significant ΔX2 could have been an artifact of the large sample size, the higher AIC, SABIC, and RMSEA values suggested that the DUREL performed differently between groups.
After failing to validate the three-item measure of intrinsic religiosity across religious affiliation groups, we attempted to validate the DUREL among a sub-sample of only Christian MSM. This seemed plausible since the DUREL was developed using heterosexual or non-sexual orientation specific samples, which in the U.S. tend to be mostly Christian (The Pew Forum on Religion and Public Life, 2008). As evidenced in Table 2, in a subsample of 889 Christian participants the associations between the constructs and the external validation measures were modest and largely non-significant, with the exception of a small, positive correlation between religious service attendance and internalized homonegativity (r=0.08). In addition, the means of the constructs did not differ between problematic and non-problematic drinkers (data not shown). Overall, these data suggest very limited construct validity of the DUREL among Christian-identified MSM.
Table 2.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | median (IQR) | Mean (sd) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. | Attendance | 1.00 | 3 (2, 4) | --- | ||||
2. | Activity | 0.42 | 1.00 | 1 (1, 4) | --- | |||
3. | Intrinsic | 0.34 | 0.50 | 1.00 | --- | 3.5 (1.1) | ||
4. | Internalized homonegativity | 0.08 | 0.04 | 0.03 | 1.00 | --- | 2.5 (1.3) | |
5. | Depression | −0.02 | 0.06 | 0.04 | 0.07 | 1.00 | --- | 4.1 (2.8) |
Note: Attendance and activity correlations calculated using Spearman. The correlations between intrinsic and internalized homonegativity and depression are calculated using Pearson pairwise. Internalized homonegativity assessed using the Revised Reactions to Homosexuality Scale. Depression assessed using the CES-D short form.
Study Two: Validation of the Spiritual Well-Being Scale
Methods
Study Design
Data for Study Two analyses were obtained from participants who completed the first of two surveys in a reliability study of questions for the Sexually Explicit Media Study. The study examines how watching sexually explicit media might influence HIV risk behavior among MSM. Participants were recruited online between January 4, 2011 and February 10, 2011 using banner advertisements on 148 gay-oriented websites affiliated with an advertising agency specializing in gay consumers. Banner advertisements directed interested persons to a webpage hosted on a dedicated university server with appropriate encryption to ensure data security. A total of 326 MSM met the eligibility criteria, which included having prior sexual experience with a man, being 18 years of age or older, and reporting a residential zip code within the United States. We excluded eight participants affiliated with other faiths because we lacked enough persons to include them in the analysis. The mean completion time for the survey was 53 minutes. Participants were compensated $60 for completing all tasks related to the study. As before, a Certificate of Confidentiality was obtained from the National Institutes of Health, and the study was conducted under the oversight of the Institutional Review Board of the researchers' home institution. A refuse to answer response option allowed participants to opt out of answering any item.
Measures
The measures relevant to this paper are described below:
Religious or spiritual affiliation
Participants indicated their primary religious or spiritual tradition using the same modified list we used in Study One (Roof & McKinney, 1987).
Short version of the Spiritual Well-Being Scale (SWB)
The original SWB is a 20-item scale developed to be a subjective assessment of quality of life. It includes two constructs: existential well-being (EWB), which is a measure of a person's life satisfaction, and religious well-being (RWB), which is a measure of a person's relationship with God (Ellison, 1983; Ellison & Smith, 1991; Smith & Ellison, 1991). Recently, Bufford (2010) proposed a six-item version of the SWB consisting of three existential well-being items (feeling settled about the future, feeling satisfied with life, and enjoying much about life) and three religious well-being items (believing God is concerned about my problems, having a personally satisfying relationship with, God and feeling fulfilled when in close communion with God). Parallel to the 20-item version, participants respond to each item using a six-point Likert-type format (1=strongly agree, 6= strongly disagree).
Revised Reactions to Homosexuality Scale
This is the same internalized homonegativity scale used in Study One (α= 0.83).
International-English Positive and Negative Affect Schedule Short Form (I-PANAS-SF)
The I-PANAS-SF (Thompson, 2007) is a self-report of personality traits. Five items assessed positive affect and five items assessed negative affect. Participants responded to five-point Likert-type items with anchors of “very little or not at all” to “extremely.” The Cronbach alphas were αpositive affect=0.82 and αnegative affect=0.86.
Binge drinking
Participants were asked how many times in the past 30-days they had consumed five or more drinks in one sitting. Persons who reported one or more episodes were classified as having engaged in recent binge drinking.
Analysis
Similar to the purpose of Study One, in this study we attempted to validate the short version of the SWB in a sample of MSM with diverse religious and spiritual beliefs. However, we lacked variation in the responses of the atheist/agnostic participants' and had too few spiritual participants to test for equivalency between groups. Thus, except for a between-group comparison of demographic characteristics, we restricted this analysis to 106 Christian participants. Using Mplus version 5.2, we conducted a confirmatory factor analysis of the SWB structures recommended by Bufford (2010). We used STATA-IC version 11.1 (StataCorp LP, 2009) to calculate correlations between the SWB constructs, internalized homonegativity, positive affect, and negative affect. We used t-tests to compare the means of the SWB constructs between participants who did or did not report recent binge drinking.
Results
Similar to Study One, the majority of participants in the Study Two sample were raised to identify as Christian (89.8%). However, only 38% of the sample currently identifies as Christian; 50.5% identify as atheist or agnostic and 11.5% identify as spiritual. Religious affiliation groups were similar with the exception of spiritual participants being older than participants in the other groups (Table 3).
Table 3.
Christian n=106 | Atheist-Agnostic n=141 | Spiritual n=32 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
||||||
n (%) | n (%) | n (%) | F | df | P | |
|
||||||
Age [mean (sd)] | 32.7 (11.2) | 31.1 (11.5) | 38.2 (11.7) | 5.0 | 2.0 | 0.007 |
Education [mean (sd)] | 15.5 (3.0) | 15.8 (2.9) | 16.2 (2.4) | 0.7 | 2.0 | 0.502 |
Race | n.a. | n.a. | 0.279 | |||
American Indian/Alaskan Native | 4 (3.8) | 1 (0.7) | 1 (3.1) | |||
Asian | 2 (1.9) | 9 (6.4) | 2 (6.3) | |||
African American | 6 (5.78) | 6 (4.3) | --- | |||
White | 87 (82.1) | 120 (85.1) | 28 (87.5) | |||
Other | 7 (6.6) | 5 (3.6) | 1 (3.1) | |||
Hispanic | n.a. | n.a. | 0.295 | |||
Yes | 12 (11.3) | 8 (5.7) | 2 (6.3) | |||
No | 94 (88.7) | 133 (94.3) | 30 (93.8) | |||
Sexual Orientation | n.a. | n.a. | 0.820 | |||
Gay/Homosexual | 84 (79.3) | 126 (89.4) | 27 (84.4) | |||
Other | 22 (2–.8) | 15 (10.6) | 5 (15.6) | |||
Living with HIV | n.a. | n.a. | 1.000 | |||
Yes | 7 (6.6) | 9 (6.4) | 2 (6.3) | |||
No | 99 (93.4) | 132 (93.6) | 30 (93.8) |
Note: α=0.05. For age and education, statistical significance determined using ANOVA. Scheffe multiple comparison test indicates difference in age is between atheist/agnostic and spiritual participants (p=0.007). For race, Hispanic ethnicity, sexual orientation, and HIV-status, statistical significance determined using Fisher's exact.
Since we were interested in a brief assessment tool, we chose to assess the short version in a sample of MSM. We could not confirm the structure of the six-item scale due to a redundancy between the communion and concern religious items. Thus, we removed the communion item, resulting in a five-item scale. We calculated a Cronbach alpha for each sub-scale (αEWB=0.75 and αRWB=0.57). While the reliability statistic was lower than preferred for the religious well-being items, we deemed it acceptable considering only two items were loading on the factor.
Overall, the short version of the SWB did not perform well. Correlation coefficients between the 5-item SWB and validation constructs among Christian participants are reported in Table 4. Increased existential well-being was positively correlated with increased positive affect (r=0.35) and negatively correlated with increased internalized homonegativity (r=−0.36) and negative affect (r=−0.30). An increase in the religious well-being was correlated with an increase in positive affect (r=0.26). The means of the existential and religious subscales and the total SWB did not differ between participants who did or did not report recent binge drinking (data not shown). While the existential well-being items correlated well with the validation constructs, the expected correlations with the religious well-being items did not materialize.
Table 4.
|
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | Mean (sd) | ||
|
|||||||
1. | Existential well-being | 1.00 | 4.1 (1.1) | ||||
2. | Religious well-being | 0.32 | 1.00 | 4.1 (1.3) | |||
3. | Internalized homonegativity | −0.36 | −0.07 | 1.00 | 2.6 (1.4) | ||
4. | Positive affect | 0.35 | 0.26 | −0.17 | 1.00 | 3.6 (0.8) | |
5. | Negative affect | −0.30 | 0.02 | 0.27 | −0.04 | 1.00 | 2.3 (0.8) |
Note: Correlations calculated using Pearson pairwise. Existential and religious well-being are constructs of the Spiritual Well-Being Scale. Internalized homonegativity assessed using the Revised Reactions to Homosexuality Scale. Affect assessed using the International-English Positive and Negative Affect Schedule Short Form.
Discussion
We could not fully validate either scale in samples that included MSM with diverse religious and spiritual beliefs. Among a sub-sample of Christians, the religious service attendance measure of the DUREL had a minimal correlation with internalized homonegativity. The SWB performed better than the DUREL in a sub-sample of Christians. Similar to Tan (2005), the existential well-being construct correlated well with most of the validation constructs whereas the religious well-being construct only correlated with positive affect. There are at least three possible explanations for the lack of correlation between the religious well-being construct and the validation constructs. First, as suggested by Tan, the existential well-being construct might be more relevant than the religious well-being construct when participants are MSM. Second, it is possible that participants in our sample did not experience a dissonance between their religious beliefs and same-gender attraction (we did not specifically ask this question). Third, it is possible participants experienced dissonance in their past but resolved it prior to participating in our study.
A plausible explanation for the inability to validate the scales among our diverse MSM samples is that homosexual persons experience religiosity and spirituality differently than heterosexual persons. While 83.6% of the Study One sample was raised Christian, only 43.2% still identified with the faith tradition. Similarly, in Study Two, 89.8% were raised Christian, but only 38.0% currently identified with the religion. To escape the stigma experienced by being affiliated with a religious organization not accepting of same-gender attraction, many homosexual persons abandon the faith communities in which they were raised (Rosario, et al., 2006). Those that stay affiliated with their faith community often form an understanding of their faith that blends church teachings with a belief in a god that approves of homosexuality, e.g., concluding that the church's teachings on homosexuality are wrong but still finding value in remaining affiliated with the organization (Rosser, 1992; Yip, 1997, 1999, 2002).
Both studies had similar limitations. They used cross-sectional samples of mostly white, young, and gay-identified Internet-using men. In Study One, demographic differences between Christian, atheist/agnostic, and spiritual participants could partially explain the DUREL's lack of measurement invariance between groups. In addition, dichotomous outcomes resulting from the structure of the CAGE in Study One and our binge-drinking question in Study Two were not ideal for correlation studies. Despite these limitations, these analyses bring to the forefront the need for validated scales that assess the religious and spiritual experiences of MSM.
Because homosexual persons appear to experience religion and spirituality differently than their heterosexual peers, new and adapted measures of religiosity and spirituality are necessary to conduct research on religiosity and spirituality among MSM. Formative research is needed to identify those religious and spiritual factors relevant to MSM. With better religiosity and spirituality scales, sexual health programs that include a religious or spiritual component will be better equipped to assess the effectiveness of this component and to meet the needs of religious MSM participants.
Acknowledgments
The National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism funded the Structural Factors to Lower Alcohol-Related HIV Risk Project, grant number R01AA016270-01A1. The National Institute of Mental Health Center for Mental Health Research on AIDS funded the Sexually Explicit Media (SEM) Study, grant number R01MH087231. All research was carried out with the approval of the University of Minnesota Institutional Review Board.
References
- Bassett RL, Smith A, Thrower J, Tindall M, Barclay J, Tiuch K, et al. One effort to measure implicit attitudes toward spirituality and religion. Journal of Psychology & Christianity. 2005;24(3):210–218. [Google Scholar]
- Billy JOG, Tanfer K, Grady WR, Klepinger DH. The Sexual Behavior of Men In the United States. Family Planning Perspectives. 1993;25(2):52–60. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borman PD, Dixon DN. Spirituality and the 12 steps of substance abuse recovery. Journal of Psychology and Theology. 1998;26(3):287–291. [Google Scholar]
- Bufford RK. Short version of the spiritual well-being scale for institutional research. George Fox University; Newberg, OR: 2010. personal communication. [Google Scholar]
- Cheever KH, Jubilan B, Dailey T, Ehehardt K, Blumenstein R, Morin CJ, et al. Surgeons and the spirit: a study on the relationship of religiosity to clinical practice. Journal of Religion and Health. 2005;44(1):67–80. doi: 10.1007/s10943-004-1146-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chitwood DD, Weiss ML, Leukefeld CG. A systemic review of recent literature on religiosity and substance use. Journal of Drug Issues. 2008;38(3):653–688. [Google Scholar]
- Cole JC, Rabin AS, Smith TL, Kaufman AS. Development and Validation of a Rasch-Derived CES-D Short Form. Psychological Assessment. 2004;16(4):360–372. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.16.4.360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cotton S, Puchalski C, Sherman S, Mrus J, Peterman A, Feinberg J, et al. Spirituality and religion in patients with HIV/AIDS. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2006;21(0):S5–S13. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2006.00642.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daaleman TP. Belief and subjective well-being in outpatients. Journal of Religion & Health. 1999;38(3):219. [Google Scholar]
- Davidson JK, Moore NB, Ullstrup KM. Religiosity and sexual responsibility: relationships of choice. American Journal of Health Behavior. 2004;28(4):335–346. doi: 10.5993/ajhb.28.4.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis TL, Kerr BA, Kurpius SER. Meaning, purpose, and religiosity in at-risk youth: the relationship between anxiety and spirituality. Journal of Psychology & Theology. 2003;31(4):356–365. [Google Scholar]
- Dedert EA, Studts JL, Weissbecker I, Salmon PG, Banis PL, Sephton SE. Religiosity may help preserve the cortisol rhythm in women with stress-related illness. International journal of psychiatry in medicine. 2004;34(1):61–77. doi: 10.2190/2Y72-6H80-BW93-U0T6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge B, Sandfort TG, Yarber WL, de Wit J. Sexual health among male college students in the United States and the Netherlands. American Journal of Health Behavior. 2005;29(2):172–182. doi: 10.5993/ajhb.29.2.8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eggers SJ. Older adult spirituality: what is it? A factor analysis of three related instruments. Journal of Religious Gerontology. 2003;14(4):3–33. [Google Scholar]
- Ellison CW. Spiritual well-being: conceptualization and measurement. Journal of Psychology and Theology. 1983;11(4):330–340. [Google Scholar]
- Ellison CW, Smith J. Toward an integrative measure of health and well-being. Journal of Psychology and Theology. 1991;19(1):35–48. [Google Scholar]
- Ewing JA. Detecting alcoholism. The CAGE questionnaire. JAMA : the journal of the American Medical Association. 1984;252(14):1905–1907. doi: 10.1001/jama.252.14.1905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fee JL, Ingram JA. Correlation of the holy spirit questionnare with the spiritual well-being scale and the spiritual assessment inventory. Journal of Psychology & Theology. 2004;32(2):104–114. [Google Scholar]
- Fierros-Gonzalez R, Brown JM. High risk behaviors in a sample of Mexican-American college students. Psychological Reports. 2002;90(1):117–130. doi: 10.2466/pr0.2002.90.1.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francis PC, Stacks J. The association between spiritual well-being and clergy sexual misconduct. Journal of Religion & Abuse. 2003;5(1):79–100. [Google Scholar]
- Galvan FH, Collins RL, Kanouse DE, Pantoja P, Golinelli D. Religiosity, denominational affiliation and sexual behaviors among people with HIV in the United States. Journal of Sex Research. 2007;44(1):49–58. doi: 10.1080/00224490709336792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcia DI, Gray-Stanley J, Ramirez-Valles J. “The priest obviously doesn't know that I'm gay”: the religious and spiritual journeys of Latino gay men. Journal of Homosexuality. 2008;55(3):411–436. doi: 10.1080/00918360802345149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Genia V. Evaluation of the spiritual well-being scale in a sample of college students. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion. 2001;11(1):25–33. [Google Scholar]
- Gow A, Watson R, Whiteman M, Deary I. A stairway to heaven? Structure of the religious involvement inventory and spiritual well-being scale. Journal of Religion & Health. 2011;50(1):5–19. doi: 10.1007/s10943-010-9375-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill PC, Hood RW Jr., editors. Measures of religiosity. Religious Education Press; Birmingham, AL: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Kim Y, Seidlitz L, Ro Y, Evinger JS, Duberstein PR. Spirituality and affect: a function of changes in religious affiliation. Personality & Individual Differences. 2004;37(4):861–870. [Google Scholar]
- Klemmack DL, Roff LL, Parker MW, Koenig HG, Sawyer P, Allman RM. A cluster analysis typology of religiousness/spirituality among older adults. Research on Aging. 2007;29(2):21. [Google Scholar]
- Koenig HG, editor. Handbook of religion and mental health. Academic Press; San Diego, CA: 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Koenig HG, Parkerson GR, Meador KG. Religion Index for Psychiatric Research. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 1997;154(6):885–886. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.6.885b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kramer MR, Hogue CJ, Gaydos LM. Noncontracepting behavior in women at risk for unintended pregnancy: what's religion got to do with it? Annals of Epidemiology. 2007;17(5):327–334. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2006.10.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krucoff MW, Crater SW, Green CL, Maas AC, Seskevich JE, Lane JD, et al. Integrative noetic therapies as adjuncts to percutaneous intervention during unstable coronary syndromes: Monitoring and Actualization of Noetic Training (MANTRA) feasibility pilot. American heart journal. 2001;142(5):760–769. doi: 10.1067/mhj.2001.119138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawler KA, Younger JW. Theobiology: an analysis of spirituality, cardiovascular responses, stress, mood, and physical health. Journal of Religion & Health. 2002;41(4):347. [Google Scholar]
- Lee J, Miller L, Chang ES. Religious identity among Christian Korean-American adolescents. Psychological Reports. 2006;98(1):14. doi: 10.2466/pr0.98.1.43-56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lefkowitz ES, Gillen MM, Shearer CL, Boone TL. Religiosity, sexual behaviors, and sexual attitudes during emerging adulthood. Journal of Sex Research. 2004;41(2):150–159. doi: 10.1080/00224490409552223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Machacek DW, Wilcox MM, editors. Sexuality and the world's religions. ABC-CLIO, Inc.; Santa Barbara, CA: 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Manlove JS, Terry-Humen E, Ikramullah EN, Moore KA. The role of parent religiosity in teens' transitions to sex and contraception. The Journal of Adolescent Health. 2006;39(4):578–587. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCauley J, Jenckes MW, Tarpley MJ, Koenig HG, Yanek LR, Becker DM. Spiritual beliefs and barriers among managed care practitioners. Journal of Religion & Health. 2005;44(2):10. doi: 10.1007/s10943-005-2772-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meredith W. Measurement invariance, factor analysis and factorial invariance. Psychometrika. 1993;58(4):525–543. [Google Scholar]
- Meredith W, Teresi JA. An essay on measurement and factorial invariance. Medical Care. 2006;44(11 (Suppl. 3)):S69–S77. doi: 10.1097/01.mlr.0000245438.73837.89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mihaljević S, Vuksan-Ćusa B, Marčinko D, Koić E, Kušević Z, Jakovljević M. Spiritual well-being, cortisol, and suicidality in Croatian war veterans suffering from PTSD. Journal of Religion & Health. 2011;50(2):464–473. doi: 10.1007/s10943-010-9383-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Musgrave CF, McFarlane EA. Intrinsic and extrinsic religiosity, spiritual well-being, and attitudes toward spiritual care: a comparison of Israeli Jewish oncology nurses' scores. Oncology Nursing Forum. 2004;31(6):1179–1183. doi: 10.1188/04.ONF.1179-1183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén B, Muthén LK. Mplus. Version 5.21 Muthén & Muthén; Las Angeles: 1998–2009. [Google Scholar]
- Nonnemaker JM, McNeely CA, Blum RW. Public and private domains of religiosity and adolescent health risk behaviors: evidence from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. Social Science and Medicine. 2003;57(11):2049–2054. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(03)00096-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roof WC, McKinney WM. American mainline religion. Rutgers University Press; New Brunswick, NJ: 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Rosario M, Yali AM, Joyce H, Gwadz MV. Religion and health among lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths: an emperical investigation and theoretical explanation. In: Omoto AM, Kurtzman HS, editors. Sexual Orientation and Mental Health. American Psychological Association; Washington, DC: 2006. pp. 117–140. [Google Scholar]
- Roseborough D. Coming out stories framed as faith narratives, or stories of spiritual growth. Pastoral Psychology. 2006;55:47–59. [Google Scholar]
- Rosser BRS. Gay Catholics down under: The journeys in sexuality and spirituality of gay men in Australia and New Zealand. Praeger; Westport, Conn.: 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Saguil A, Fitzpatrick AL, Clark G. Is Eevidence able to persuade physicians to discuss spirituality with patients? Journal of Religion & Health. 2011;50(2):289–299. doi: 10.1007/s10943-010-9452-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seidman SN, Mosher WD, Aral SO. Women with Multiple Sexual Partners: United States, 1988. American Journal of Public Health. 1992;82(10):1388–1394. doi: 10.2105/ajph.82.10.1388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shallenberger D. Reclaiming the spirit: The journeys of gay men and lesbian women toward integration. Qualitative Psychology. 1996;19(2):195–215. [Google Scholar]
- Sherman AC, Simonton S, Adams DC, Latif U, Plante TG, Burns SK, et al. Measuring religious faith in cancer patients: reliability and construct validity of the Santa Clara Strength of Religious Faith questionnaire. Psycho-oncology. 2001;10(5):436–443. doi: 10.1002/pon.523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith B, Horne S. Gay, lesbian, bisexual and transgendered (GLBT) experiences with earth-spirited faith. Journal of Homosexuality. 2007;52(3–4):235–248. doi: 10.1300/J082v52n03_11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith J, Ellison CW. Toward an integrative measure of health and well-being. Journal of Psychology and Theology. 1991;19(1):35–48. [Google Scholar]
- Smolenski DJ, Diamond PM, Ross MW, Rosser BRS. Revision, criterion calidity, and multigroup assessment of the reactions to homosexuality scale. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2010;92(6):568–576. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2010.513300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- StataCorp LP. STATA-IC. Version 11 StataCorp LP; College Station, TX: 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Storch EA, Roberti JW, Heidgerken AD, Storch JB, Lewin AB, Killiany EM, et al. The Duke religion index: a psychometric investigation. Pastoral Psychology. 2004;53(2):7. [Google Scholar]
- Storch EA, Storch JB, Kovacs AH, Okun A, Welsh E. Intrinsic religiosity and substance use in intercollegiate athletes. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology. 2003;25(2):5. [Google Scholar]
- Storch EA, Strawser MS, Storch JB. Two-week test-retest reliability of the Duke religion index. Psychological Reports. 2004;94(3):2. doi: 10.2466/pr0.94.3.993-994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Studer M, Arland T. Adolescent Religiosity and Contraceptive Usage. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1987;49(1):117–128. [Google Scholar]
- Szaflarski M, Ritchey PN, Leonard AC, Mrus JM, Peterman AH, Ellison CG, et al. Modeling the effects of spirituality/religion on patients' perceptions of living with HIV/AIDS. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2006;21:11. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2006.00646.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan PP. The importance of spirituality among gay and lesbian individuals. Journal of Homosexuality. 2005;49(2):135–144. doi: 10.1300/J082v49n02_08. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- The Pew Forum on Religion and Public Life U.S. religious landscape survey. 2008 [Google Scholar]
- Thompson ER. Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2007;38(2):227–242. [Google Scholar]
- Tshabalala BG, Patel CJ. The role of praise and worship activities in spiritual well-being: perceptions of a Pentecostal youth ministry group. International Journal of Children's Spirituality. 2010;15(1):73–82. [Google Scholar]
- Van der Meer Sanchez Z, Garcia De Oliveira L. c., Aparecida Nappo S. Religiosity as a Protective Factor Against the Use of Drugs. Substance Use & Misuse. 2008;43(10):1476–1486. doi: 10.1080/10826080802183288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu AD, Li Z, Zumbo BD. Decoding the meaning of factorial invariance and updating the practice of multi-group confirmatory factor analysis: A demonstration with TIMSS data. Practical Assessment, Research and Evaluation. 2007;12(3):1–26. [Google Scholar]
- Yip AKT. Dare to differ: Gay and lesbian Catholics' assessment of official Catholic positions on sexuality. Sociology of Religion. 1997;58(2):165–180. [Google Scholar]
- Yip AKT. The politics of counter-rejection: gay christians and the church. J Homosex. 1999;37(2):47–63. doi: 10.1300/J082v37n02_03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yip AKT. The persistence of faith among nonheterosexual Christians: Evidence for the neosecularization thesis of religous transformation. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion. 2002;41(2):199–212. [Google Scholar]
- Zaleski EH, Schiaffino KM. Religiosity and sexual risk-taking behavior during the transition to college. Journal of Adolescence. 2000;23(2):223–227. doi: 10.1006/jado.2000.0309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]