Abstract
This paper addresses recent calls to narrow the micro–macro gap in management research (Bamberger, 2008), by incorporating a macro-level context variable (country) in exploring micro-level determinants of board effectiveness. Following the integrated model proposed by Forbes and Milliken (1999), we identify three board processes as micro-level determinants of board effectiveness. Specifically, we focus on effort norms, cognitive conflicts and the use of knowledge and skills as determinants of board control and advisory task performance. Further, we consider how two different institutional settings influence board tasks, and how the context moderates the relationship between processes and tasks. Our hypotheses are tested on a survey-based dataset of 535 medium-sized and large industrial firms in Italy and Norway, which are considered to substantially differ along legal and cultural dimensions. The findings show that: (i) Board processes have a larger potential than demographic variables to explain board task performance; (ii) board task performance differs significantly between boards operating in different contexts; and (iii) national context moderates the relationships between board processes and board task performance. Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Introduction
Most previous governance studies have been criticized for either adopting under- or over-contextualized views. On the one hand, literature on corporate governance and boards of directors has primarily applied agency theory to explore the impact of board demographics on firm financial performance (Aguilera, Filatotchev, & Jackson, 2008; Daily, Dalton, & Cannella, 2003). The reliance on the agency paradigm, characterized by an extensive focus on board demographics, diverted past research from considering the influence of board processes on board effectiveness (Forbes & Milliken, 1999; Hambrick, v. Werder, & Zajac, 2008; Huse, 2007), and hardly allowed scholars to consider the impact of macro-social contexts on governance practices and behaviors (Aguilera et al., 2008; Aguilera & Jackson, 2003; Lubatkin, 2007). On the other hand, comparative studies in the financial economics and law research traditions relied on arguments based on institutional theory to explore governance mechanisms in different settings; thereby underestimating the importance of organizational contexts in determining the effectiveness of central governance mechanisms such as corporate boards (Lubatkin, 2007).
Following recent calls to narrow the micro–macro gap in management research (e.g., Bamberger, 2008), this paper argues for the co-existence of micro- and macro-level determinants in understanding board effectiveness within corporate governance structures. As such, the study follows the call to go beyond the simple acknowledgment of surrounding phenomena as “error variance” (Bamberger, 2008: 840) and incorporates context- or macro-level determinants in exploring board effectiveness.
Specifically, our purpose is to investigate board processes as micro-level determinants of board effectiveness (Gomez-Mejia & Wiseman, 2007; Zahra, 2007). We measure board effectiveness as board task performance in control and advice, and relate these to three antecedents of board task performance; effort norms, cognitive conflicts and the use of knowledge and skills (Forbes & Milliken, 1999). These constructs are solidly grounded in traditional organizational research (Hackman & Morris, 1975), but find renewed vigor in explaining the task performance of boards of directors, which differ in their characteristics from other decision-making workgroups.
Additionally, we adopt a cross-national or cross-cultural lens (Gomez-Mejia & Wiseman, 2007) to complement existing board research by introducing macro-level determinants of board effectiveness. According to the institutional perspective, human and social behaviors are driven by the influence of country-level institutions, such as norms, routines and historical patterns, which determine isomorphism among individuals and organizations (DiMaggio & Powell, 1983; Scott, 2001). Building on this logic, we investigate the impact of national contexts on board tasks, as well as how context moderates the relationship between processes and tasks.We test our hypotheses on a dataset from a cross-country survey of medium and large Norwegian and Italian firms of comparable sizes across countries. We selected countries that reflect the Scandinavian and French civil law traditions, respectively, because these two contexts differ from each other along several important dimensions, such as shareholders' and creditors' protection (e.g., La Porta, Lopez-de-Silanes, Shleifer, & Vishny, 1998; Lubatkin, Lane, Collin, & Very, 2005), as well as cross-cultural aspects (e.g., Hofstede, Van Deusen, Mueller, & Charles, 2002; Polley, 1988; Waldman et al., 2006). Hence, we follow a long tradition of the cross-national, cross-cultural organizational behavior research, which has considered national contexts as proxies for cultures and institutions (Tsui, Nifadkar, & Ou, 2007).
This paper contributes to existing literature in three related ways. First, we investigate board processes as determinants of board effectiveness in conducting its tasks (Forbes & Milliken, 1999; Huse, 2007; Hambrick et al., 2008) through a large-scale cross-national survey. This approach helps opening the ‘black box’ of corporate boards (Zona & Zattoni, 2007) by avoiding exclusive reliance on secondary data in board research (Daily et al., 2003; Hambrick et al., 2008). Second, following calls to determine “how institutions shape corporate governance” (Aguilera & Jackson, 2003: 449), we offer insights to comparative institutional analysis. Focusing on board processes allows us to go beyond the recognition of cross-national differences among board structures (Aguilera & Cuervo-Cazurra, 2004). Specifically, we investigate how boards engage in their tasks in different contexts, providing evidence for the actual influence of dominant institutional norms and values on board internal mechanisms (Hambrick et al., 2008). Finally, we contribute to the transitioning from a-contextual research (Bamberger, 2008) to the explicit consideration of contexts within the domain of boards and governance research. As such, the study offers novel inputs to the ongoing debate on the ‘embedded model of governance’ (Lubatkin, Lane, Collin, & Very, 2007), which argues for the existence of a close interplay of firm- and institutional- levels in defining governance mechanisms (Aguilera & Jackson, 2003; Lubatkin, 2007; Lubatkin et al., 2007; Scharpf, 1999; Williamson, 2000).
The paper proceeds as follows. We first introduce the organizational-level relationship between board processes and board task performance. Next, we discuss the macro-level institutional settings governing board behavior, leading to our hypotheses on the moderating effects of country contexts on the above relationship. After presenting our methods and results, the paper concludes with a discussion of its contribution to the extant literature and suggestions for future research.
Theoretical Background
Board task performance
We follow the tradition of boards and governance research by considering board effectiveness in relation to board task performance (Forbes & Milliken, 1999; Johnson, Daily, & Ellstrand, 1996; Stiles & Taylor, 2001; Zahra & Pearce, 1989). According to this tradition, boards are expected to perform control and advisory tasks. The control tasks follow predictions of agency theory (Fama, 1980; Jensen & Meckling, 1976), suggesting that the primary responsibility of corporate boards is to safeguard shareholders from management misappropriation (Shleifer & Vishny, 1997). From this perspective, boards are groups of independent people with the duty to actively control top executives and the incentives to operate in the interests of firm's shareholders (Fama & Jensen, 1983). To accomplish control tasks, board members should scrutinize top executives' behaviors and actively monitor firm performance to satisfy both shareholders' and stakeholders' expectations (Hillman & Dalziel, 2003). The attention to board control tasks is regaining interest in light of the current global financial crisis. As such, board control is increasingly considered a primary measure of boards' effectiveness, and is thus subject to severe public scrutiny.
At the same time, from a resource dependence perspective (Pfeffer & Salancik, 1978), board advisory tasks relate to the ability of board members to bring additional resources to the firm (Hillman & Dalziel, 2003). According to this view, corporate boards are groups of competent people contributing to boardroom debate by bringing in their experiences, competences and pluralistic perspectives (Forbes & Milliken, 1999). In this respect, board members contribute to strategic decision-making by providing valuable advice and counsel to firm top executives (Daily & Dalton, 1994; Hillman, Cannella, & Paetzold, 2000; Judge & Zeithaml, 1992). The advisory tasks are conceptually rooted in a collaborative model of board of directors, complementing the independent model according to which boards have the primary responsibility to control managers on behalf of shareholders (Westphal, 1999).
Board processes and board task performance
The understanding of board control and advisory task performance requires identifying predictors of board effectiveness. To this end, we follow calls to explore board behaviors beyond the reliance of demographics as proxies of actual processes (Daily et al., 2003; Forbes & Milliken, 1999; Pettigrew, 1992). The need to explore processes in boards and governance research has been recently advocated by Hambrick et al. (2008), who argue that “behavioral processes” represent the main determinants of governance at the micro-level of analysis. Accordingly, we build on the integrated model of board effectiveness proposed by Forbes and Milliken (1999), which identifies three main board processes as predictors of board task performance: Effort norms, cognitive conflicts and use of knowledge and skills. Grounded in organizational theory (Hackman & Morris, 1975), these processes have mainly been tested in different decision-making workgroups (see Bettenhausen, 1991; Cohen & Bailey, 1997 for a review). Notwithstanding, they are thought to have a peculiar meaning within the context of corporate boards, characterized as “large, elite, episodic decision making groups that face complex tasks pertaining to strategic issue processing” (Forbes & Milliken, 1999: 492). Several aspects characterize boards as peculiar decision-making groups. Specifically, boards are: (a) Larger than other workgroups; (b) made up of a majority of “outsiders”, with primary affiliation in other companies (and thus with a more limited knowledge of company issues); (c) episodic, since they meet from 6 to a maximum of 12 times per year and (d) without a concrete and “tangible” outcome, i.e., boards are not responsible for implementing decisions, and their outcome is entirely “cognitive” in nature. These characteristics make boards particularly vulnerable to “process losses” that prevent them from achieving their full potential (Forbes & Milliken, 1999; Steiner, 1972).
Effort norms is a “group-level construct that refers to the group's shared beliefs regarding the level of effort each individual is expected to put toward a task” (Forbes & Milliken, 1999: 493). Recent reviews emphasize the importance of the effort board members devote to preparation, analysis and participation in boardroom debates (e.g., Hambrick et al., 2008). Most board members are busy professionals who face competing demands for their time (Lorsch & MacIver, 1989). As such, the effort devoted to different tasks may vary considerably across boards, with the potential result that board members often act as a “rubber stamp” for managerial proposals (Lorsch & MacIver, 1989; Mace, 1971; Stiles & Taylor, 2001).
As board members often fail to do their “homework”, i.e., do not analyze documents and information provided before meetings (Forbes & Milliken, 1999), the probability of them merely acting as a passive audience in corporate boardrooms increase. The lack of effort is especially relevant in the board context, since boards are decision-making groups composed mostly of outsiders who bring substantial independence at the price of lower inside knowledge of the firm and its strategies (Mallette & Fowler, 1992). Hence, preparation for and participation in board meetings—in terms of carefully scrutinizing information provided by management before meetings, finding autonomously own information regarding issues relevant to the company and actively partaking during meetings with questions—can influence the board's ability to effectively perform its tasks (Forbes & Milliken, 1999; Wageman, 1995). Such efforts ensure constructive and fruitful discussions, thereby improving the quality of decision-making and contributing to the performance of cognitive and intellective tasks (Watson & Michaelsen, 1988). Based on arguments above, we contend that effort norms facilitate both control and advisory tasks. Finding their own information together with careful scrutiny of management reports favor board oversights, whilst in-depth preparation together with active participation during meetings facilitate board advice to management. Thus, we hypothesize the following:
Hypothesis 1: Effort norms will have a positive impact on board control and advisory task performance.
Cognitive conflicts refer to task-oriented differences in judgment among group members, often manifested in “disagreements about the content of the tasks being performed, including differences in viewpoints, ideas and opinions” (Jehn, 1995: 258). As a result of the effects of conflicts on group effectiveness still being equivocal (De Dreu & Weingart, 2003), Jehn (1995, 1997) proposed an alternative perspective by differentiating between task and relationship conflicts. She noticed that, although relationship conflicts generally decrease satisfaction among group members and negatively interfere with task performance, task conflicts can be beneficial to task performance when the group is working on non-routine tasks (Jehn, 1995). The focus on boards of directors as decision-making groups emphasizes the difference between task and relationship conflicts. As groups of highly qualified individuals without hierarchical relationships and meeting only episodically, corporate boards represent a context in which relationship conflicts and personal antagonisms are less likely to take place than in other organizational teams. Rather, the non-routiness of tasks performed by corporate boards, along with the complexity of their decision-making process as well as the interdependence among board members, emphasizes the positive effects of task-related cognitive conflicts (Forbes & Milliken, 1999).
Cognitive conflicts can improve decision-making because they facilitate the exchange of information among board members (Amason & Sapienza, 1997). The presence of cognitive conflicts can increase the quality of debate, forcing board members to consider a broader range of alternatives (Forbes & Milliken, 1999). Moreover, cognitive conflicts increase the group members' tendency to scrutinize task issues and to engage in deliberate processing of task-relevant information (De Dreu & Weingart, 2003). This, in turn, may lead to the consideration of additional alternatives, as well as more careful evaluation of these (Eisenhardt et al., 1997). As such, the presence of disagreements and critical investigation among board members can force top managers to justify their strategic proposals and to consider alternative perspectives.
In other terms, while we recognize that conflicts can be either beneficial or detrimental to group performance depending on group characteristics (De Dreu & Weingart, 2003), we also argue that non-routiness of tasks performed in boardrooms emphasizes the positive sides of conflicts among directors. Based on arguments suggesting that cognitive conflicts may lead to better decision-making processes, we hypothesize the following:
Hypothesis 2: Cognitive conflicts will have a positive impact on board control and advisory task performance.
Use of knowledge and skills refers to “the board's ability to tap the knowledge and skills available to it and then apply them to its tasks” (Forbes & Milliken, 1999: 495). The use of knowledge and skills is associated with the process by which board members' contributions are coordinated, and specifically refers to the flows of information among board members, the clear division of tasks and responsibilities, and the awareness board members should have of each others' competences and areas of expertise (Forbes & Milliken, 1999). This construct differs from the presence of knowledge and skills, which conversely refers to the presence of professional (e.g., finance, marketing, accounting, law) and/or firm specific knowledge and expertise. Boards are usually populated by highly competent and reputed individuals; however, the mere presence of knowledge, we argue, does not per se mean that board members will use their knowledge (Forbes & Milliken, 1999; Zona & Zattoni, 2007). Rather, effective boards require active use and integration of board members' expertise and skills to enhance group decisions. The collective use of knowledge and skills is particularly relevant when groups are highly interdependent, and when the group shares a sense of collective responsibility for performance outcomes (Wageman, 1995). It gains additional relevance when interdependent groups are also episodic, since the use of knowledge may prevent “process losses” and help board members build on each others' professionalism. Corporate boards represent such groups as board members “must elicit and respect each others” expertise, build upon each others' contributions, and seek to combine their insights in creative, synergistic ways' (Forbes & Milliken, 1999: 496). Hence, the proper use of knowledge and skills is believed to enhance task performance in both control and advice:
Hypothesis 3: The use of knowledge and skills will have a positive impact on board control and advisory task performance.
Board effectiveness in control and advisory task performance is also relevant in relation to firm-level outcomes. As discussed above, both control and advice tasks of boards are believed to influence performance by preventing management misappropriation (Shleifer & Vishny, 1997) and bringing qualified advice and counsel to firm top executives (Daily & Dalton, 1994; Judge & Zeithaml, 1992). In doing so, effective boards of directors avoid “distraction” of value by managerial expropriation (through effective control) and, at the same time, allow for maximization of value creation (through effective advice). Hence, we contend that board tasks, as the sole board-level outcome to directly influence corporate financial results (Forbes & Milliken, 1999), are important predictors of firm financial outcomes (Zahra and Pearce, 1989):
Hypothesis 4: Board control and advisory task performance will have a positive impact on firm financial performance.
Governance systems and institutional settings
Governance systems are embedded in national institutional environments (Aguilera et al., 2008; Aguilera & Jackson, 2003; Buck & Shahrim, 2005). Despite law harmonization, financial market integration, and diffusion of codes of best practice, are pushing corporate governance practices to become increasingly similar around the world, they continue to formally and substantially differ across contexts (e.g., Aguilera & Cuervo-Cazurra, 2004; Aguilera & Jackson, 2003; Roe, 2003). Hence, at the macro-level, national contexts potentially exert a significant influence on board task performance and internal processes.
Following institutional arguments, sources of contextual differences are formal institutions and constraints (North, 1990; Whitley, 1992) as well as informal or background institutions (DiMaggio & Powell, 1983). Among the formal institutions, legal regimes are rooted in the legal traditions of nations (La Porta et al., 1998). From a legalistic perspective, Scandinavian (Norway, Sweden, Denmark, and Finland) and French (e.g., France, Spain, Italy, etc.) legal traditions have been shown to differ along several dimensions (La Porta et al., 1998). For instance, national context has been shown to significantly influence the financial systems and consequently the local patterns of corporate ownership (Pedersen & Thomsen, 1997; Thomsen & Pedersen, 1996, 2000; Whitley, 1992).
As for background institutions, different contexts are characterized by specific national cultures. Culture has been defined as ‘a shared meaning system […], during a specific historic period, and in a definable geographic region’ (Triandis, 2000: 146). Culture refers to the “complex of meanings, symbols, and assumptions about what is good or bad, legitimate or illegitimate that underlie the prevailing practices and norms in a society” (Licht et al., 2005: 233). As such, national culture has the potential to influence and shape business practices and styles, and work-related values and behaviors. Based on their national origin, for instance, managers “not only contribute to the collective formulation of cultural norms and views, they experience social reinforcement pressures which bring their individual-level assumptions and preferences into close alignment with those of their native culture” (Geletkanycz, 1997: 617).
Board task performance and processes in different institutional contexts
From a legal perspective, board task performance is dependent on requirements by corporate laws and voluntary codes of good governance based on a “comply or explain” principle. Thus, board task performance may vary in different legal regimes. In this regard, the Scandinavian system is characterized by higher legal protection and efficiency of the judiciary system compared to the Latin system (La Porta et al., 1998). Hence, in Scandinavian countries corporate boards are likely to feel greater pressures on task performance, especially in control; such pressures stem from both disclosure requirements and legal responsibilities that board members must adhere to.
Besides legal constraints, board task performance may be influenced by cultural traits characterizing different national contexts. Comparative studies on cross national differences in cultures show strong dissimilarities among work-related individual values and behaviors as a consequence of the broader political, sociological and psychological influence of nationality (e.g., Hofstede, 1983, 1984, 2001; Hofstede et al., 2002; see also Egri and Ralston, 2004; Kirkman, Chen, Farh, Chen, & Lowe, 2009; Man and Lam, 2003). For instance, in a study of the perceived goals of business leaders within organizations, Hofstede et al. (2002) show how Latin leaders emphasize family interests, personal wealth and power, while the northern European model of business is driven by responsibility toward employees and society at large. Consistent, we argue that the consideration of stakeholders' interests in Latin countries is relatively lower compared to Scandinavia.
Thus, Scandinavian boards are more involved in both control and advisory tasks. Higher performance in control tasks is grounded in the higher institutional pressures Scandinavian board members experience in terms of disclosure and enforcement of their legal responsibilities. Higher performance in advisory tasks is predicated by a business culture in Scandinavia which traditionally emphasizes an active role of boards within firms (Huse, 1990). Hence, we hypothesize the following:
Hypothesis 5: Board performance in both control and advisory tasks will be higher in Scandinavian countries compared to Latin countries.
The impact of context is not limited to board task performance. Isomorphic pressures related to inertial rules and routines embodied in institutional environments (McNulty & Pettigrew, 1999; Ocasio, 1999) and in different national cultures (Hofstede, 2001) also have the potential to significantly influence board processes (Hambrick et al., 2008). In this respect, intriguing insights emerge from the comparison of cultural traits among Scandinavian and Latin countries. In his studies of cultural relativity and its impact on managerial practices and behaviors, Hofstede (1983, 1984, 2001) shows how Latin countries are more individualistic societies, dominated by larger “power distance” and stronger hierarchies in workplaces than Scandinavian countries (Hofstede, 1984). Power distance is related to the “degree of centralization of authority and the degree of autocratic leadership” (Hofstede, 1984: 81), and together with individualism defines the leadership style which is idiosyncratic to a specific national context. In countries characterized by high levels of individualism and power distance (i.e., Latin countries), individual subordinates usually refrain from participating in decision-making. As such, it is expected that leaders will lead “autocratically” (Hofstede, 1984).
In this study, we show how culture may moderate the relationships between board processes and board task performance. In particular, we argue that board processes will be stronger predictors of board effectiveness in individualistic rather than in collectivistic societies. Individualism, for instance, may induce overconfidence in executives' ability to lead the firm, and, as a result, reinforces the importance of individual decision-making over group consensus (Geletkanycz, 1997). Individualism may also parallel opportunism in that it allows room for opportunistic behaviours, since individualistic societies are likely to be characterized by lesser social control than collectivistic ones. In individualistic contexts, the presence of competent and skilful professionals in corporate boardrooms does not ensure the effective use of knowledge. In such contexts, the board leader (CEO and/or Chair) may prefer to assign individual responsibility to him- or herself, rather than eliciting collective use of knowledge and skills. Provided the importance of collective use of such knowledge in highly interdependent groups (Wageman, 1995), the actual active use of knowledge and skills will have a stronger impact on board task performance in individualistic rather than in collectivistic societies.
The opportunistic risks associated with individualism provide further arguments for the higher relevance of effort norms in leading board members' behaviours and force them to fulfil their responsibilities as board members. Hence, boardroom cultures characterized by emphasis on board members' preparation and actual contribution to board discussions (Forbes & Milliken, 1999; Wageman, 1995) will be stronger predictors of board effectiveness in individualistic than in collectivistic societies, since effort norms enforce the commitment that otherwise would be difficult to reach.
Consistent with previous research, hierarchy prevails over active participation in decision-making in high power distant cultures (Geletkanycz, 1997); cognitive conflicts are more likely to be suppressed than encouraged in such cultures. Hence, when conflicts arise, they lower hierarchical and cognitive barriers among board members (e.g., among outsiders and insiders) and create conditions for boards being participative in firm's critical decisions. Under such circumstances, conflicts have a stronger potential to engage board members in valuable discussions, which otherwise might be missing. This is perhaps one of the reasons why boards in Latin countries have a tradition for being passive; especially concerning control tasks (e.g., Brunello, Graziano, & Parigi, 2003).
Furthermore, different leadership styles in dissimilar national contexts will shape group dynamics in that they may stimulate or inhibit effective board processes. Hence, in high collectivist-low power distance cultures (like the ones characterizing Scandinavia), leadership style will encourage board participation, and processes will tend to be naturally more effective. As a consequence, however, it may lead to a “ceiling effect” in Scandinavian boards, since relatively less variance is achievable in a good process context. In sum, previous arguments point at the importance of developing effective board processes especially in those settings where autocratic leadership and low participative business styles prevail. In such settings, the greater variance in processes across boards will determine a higher predictive potential of the board processes themselves on board task performance. Thus, we hypothesize the following:
Hypothesis 6: Effort norms, cognitive conflicts and the use of knowledge and skills will have a stronger positive impact on both control and advisory task performance in Latin countries vis-à-vis Scandinavian countries.
Methods
Sample and collection of data
The hypotheses were tested on data collected in Norway (representing Scandinavian countries) and Italy (representing Latin countries). The selection of two countries as proxies of cultures and institutions follows the tradition in cross-cultural organizational behaviour research. In this vein, issues related to cross-national data collection procedures, matching samples and construct equivalence often lead scholars to a choice of comparing two radically different countries (Tsui et al., 2007). Empirical literature on cross-country investigation has solid roots, and provided several examples of comparative research between two countries (e.g., Egri & Ralston, 2004; Lee, Bobko, Ashford, Zhen Xiong, & Xiaopeng, 2008; Lin, Peng, Yang, & Sun, 2009; Kirkman et al., 2009; Man & Lam, 2003). Cross-national studies typically consider countries featuring polarized characteristics in terms of the phenomenon under investigation, and consistently focus on culture and institutions as sources of variation. Based on previous examples, the selection of Norway and Italy has been inspired by their appropriateness in exploring the effects of national contexts on organizational processes within corporate boards. As argued theoretically, these two countries are markedly different along both the legal, the institutional, and the cultural dimensions. To provide further evidence supporting our arguments, we collected archival data from the World Competitiveness Report along several dimensions related to those traits. Following Wan & Hoskisson (2003), we computed two composite measures of both legal, and institutional and cultural dimensions to provide evidence of the differences between the two countries. Specifically, the legal variable includes: (i) the quality of the legal and regulatory framework and the judiciary system efficiency; (ii) the intellectual property protection and (iii) the competitive legislation (Wan & Hoskisson, 2003). The institutional and cultural variable includes: (i) political transparency; (ii) consumer price inflation and (iii) bribing and corruption (Wan & Hoskisson, 2003). While we could not use these variables in our regression analyses due to the presence of two countries in our dataset, the country-level scores support our theoretical assumptions about the significant differences between the two countries.
As Table 1 shows, the legal variable average score is 4.04 for Italy and 6.97 for Norway (out of a maximum of 10), and the gap is similar for the institutional and cultural variable, with a score of 2.70 for Italy and 5.58 for Norway (out of a maximum of 10). Table 1 also presents the differences on single items in order to show detailed variations on each dimension. This evidence provides additional support for the choice of the two countries, selected for this study.
Table 1.
Legal and regulatory framework | Intellectual property protection | Competitive legislation | Legal variable (1–10) | Transparency | Consumer price inflation | Bribing and corruption | Institutional/cultural variable (1–10) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Italy | 3.09 | 4.85 | 4.19 | 4.04 | 3.05 | 2.21 | 2.85 | 2.70 |
Norway | 6.14 | 8 | 6.76 | 6.97 | 6.57 | 2.48 | 7.68 | 5.58 |
δ | −3.05 | −3.15 | −2.58 | −2.92 | −3.52 | −0.27 | −4.83 | −2.87 |
Source: World Competitiveness Report; scores have been selected at the year the survey was issued.
Data from Norway and Italy was collected within a short time-span (autumn 2003 in Norway, spring 2004 in Italy). The Norwegian sample consists of the publicly listed firms and the one thousand largest industrial firms based on total turnover in 2002, totalling 1140 firms. The Italian sample consists of the 2000 largest Italian industrial firms ranked by turnover in the same year, including all the 240 industrial firms listed in that year at the Italian Stock Exchange. We obtained 379 answers from Norway, with a response rate of 33 per cent, and 301 answers from Italy, with a response rate of 15 per cent, which is in line with previous research on board of directors (Cycyota & Harrison, 2006). Additionally, we collected archival data on non responding firms in order to check for non-respondent bias. Data on firm characteristics were gathered from public sources and company annual reports. Specifically, we performed the non-parametric two independent samples test using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov procedure on firm size. We compared size in terms of annual turnover and number of employees of both respondent and non-respondent firms (Siegel & Castellan, 1988). The results of the tests provide evidence that respondents and non-respondents come from the same population in both sub-samples.
Following recommendations on matching samples in cross-national research (Tsui et al., 2007), we built a unique dataset considering firms of comparable size in both countries. We investigated only firms that in both samples had more than 50 employees, which represents a threshold for identifying medium and large firms (OECD Definition, 2005). The final dataset includes a total of 535 firms: 256 Norwegian firms (out of 379) and 279 Italian firms (out of 301). The two samples are comparable both in terms of number of firms included and in terms of average firm size. The average turnover for Norwegian firms is 1516 million euro, while it is 1977 million euro for Italian firms.
The survey data was based on responses from CEOs on behalf of the entire board. Since it is traditionally difficult to gain access to process data on boards of directors (e.g., Daily et al., 2003; Pettigrew, 1992), governance studies incorporating primary data are often based on a single respondent, typically the CEO (e.g., Pearce & Zahra, 1991; Zahra, 1996; Zahra, Neubaum, & Huse, 2000). In line with previous studies, we consider the CEO as the best possible key informant because he/she is knowledgeable about the issues investigated in our study, while at the same time he/she is also in a better position than other board members to report on them. Board members are part of a group which meets episodically, which suggests potential downsides of having multiple respondents. According to some scholars, having multiple reports in some specific circumstances can enhance the risk of constructing averaged measures which reflect divergence across reports, rather than representing the constructs being investigated (Kumar, Stern, & Anderson, 1993).
The dataset has been built through a survey instrument designed around established scales from the group effectiveness literature. In order to increase reliability of the data, we applied a number of procedural steps in the instrument development and data collection phase. First, we protected the respondents' anonymity by assuring confidentiality of their responses in the cover letter that accompanied the survey. Second, we invested considerable time and effort in improving the scale items and reducing item ambiguity. All survey questions are short, specific and use simple words to avoid ambiguous and vague formulations (Dillman, 2000). In some cases, following suggestions by Forbes and Milliken (1999), we adapted our items to the specific context of boards as decision-making groups. Related to this, in order to enhance the construct validity of the survey measures, we conducted a pre-test (Fowler, 1993: 102) and interviewed board members participating in the pilot study in Norway to assist us in the fine-tuning of the questionnaire, and particularly in identifying potentially misleading items (Carpenter & Westphal, 2001). Each individual was then asked to identify questions that were unclear, ambiguous or difficult to answer. Moreover, using inputs from the pilot interviews, we carefully worded questions to minimize the likelihood of a social desirability bias. Based on the survey instrument, developed in mid-2003, we sent out a traditional mailing to all the firms in our initial populations. In order to increase response rates, we had two rounds of reminders and we re-sent questionnaires to non-responding CEOs after two months. The collection procedure was identical in the two countries.
Measures
Dependent variables
Most of the dependent and independent variables are based on multiple-item constructs measured through a five point Likert-type scale. Control task performance was measured through five items. CEOs were asked to assess the extent to which the board: (i) controls that activities are well organized; (ii) establishes plans and budgets for the firms' operations; (iii) establishes guidelines for the operations of the firms; (iv) keeps itself informed about the financial position of the firm and (v) oversees that the operations are properly controlled (Huse 1993; Zona & Zattoni 2007). The variable control task performance was computed as a mean of these items, and the Cronbach α for this variable is 0.83.
Advisory task performance was also measured using five items which represent the different aspects boards of directors are supposed to contribute to. Accordingly, we used five statements about the degree to which the board provides advice on: (i) Management issues (e.g., organizational structure or company strategy); (ii) financial issues (e.g., leverage or relationships with banks and other financial institutions); (iii) technical issues (e.g., new technologies or products); (iv) market issues (e.g., entry in new industries or consumer behavior) and (v) legal issues and taxation (Minichilli & Hansen, 2007; van Ees, van der Laan, & Postma, 2008). The variable advisory task performance was computed as a mean of these items, and the Cronbach α for this variable is 0.78.
To assess firm financial performance we used Return on Assets (ROA), defined as the net operating income before extraordinary items divided by total assets. ROA is a well understood and common measure used in several studies on the impact of boards and top management teams on firm performance, and is particularly appropriate for manufacturing firms (e.g., Cannella & Shen, 2001; Carpenter, 2002; Finkelstein & D'Aveni, 1994; Geletkanycz & Hambrick, 1997; Henderson, Miller, & Hambrick, 2006). However, since the impact of executives on corporate level outcomes can be detected only within an appropriate performance window, we relied on a lagged value of ROA(t + 1), considering the year following the one in which the survey was administered (Shen, 2003; Westphal, 1999).
Independent variables
The independent variables included in the study are effort norms, cognitive conflicts and use of knowledge and skills. For effort norms we used three-items construct based on Forbes and Milliken (1999: 494). Effort norms was measured by asking the CEO the extent to which board members: (i) Carefully scrutinize information provided by management before the meetings; (ii) find their own information in relation to firm-specific issues and (iii) actively participate with critical questions during meetings. The above items are related to Wageman (1995); however, while Wageman's (1995) operationalization of effort norms seems to capture the level of individual effort expected from each of the members of the group, Forbes and Milliken's (1999) seems to capture more the actual behaviors of board members in terms of the effort they place on doing their tasks. Hence, our choice to rely on the latter items is in line with our purpose to focus on actual board behavior. The variable was computed as a mean of the three items, and the Cronbach α is 0.70. The cognitive conflicts construct has been developed for the purposes of this study based on Jehn's (1995) operationalization of conflicts, and relying on Forbes and Milliken's (1999) arguments to adapt such measure to the reality of corporate boards. Hence, cognitive conflicts was measured by asking the CEOs to evaluate the extent to which conflicts and disagreements emerged in the boardroom on: (i) Decisions to be taken during the board meetings; (ii) how to define what is the best for the firm; (iii) decision processes and (iv) firm's owners and stakeholders' interests. The Cronbach α for this variable is 0.86. Measurement for the use of knowledge and skills relies on what was first elucidated by Hackman & Morris (1975), and has been adapted to the board context following Forbes and Milliken (1999: 496). As such, it was measured by asking the CEO about the extent to which (i) board members know each others competences well; (ii) the division of work in this board is a good match between board members' knowledge/competencies and the character of the work and (iii) when an issue is discussed, the most knowledgeable board members use their knowledge. The Cronbach α for this variable is 0.76.
We used a dummy variable to capture country effects (Italy = 1, Norway = 0) and to assess the influence of the national context on board task performance, as well as its moderating effect on the board processes and task performance relationship. Similar to what the majority of cross-cultural studies do (Tsui et al., 2007), we consider country as a proxy for culture and institutions. To this end, all the interaction variables (country*effort norms; country*cognitive conflicts; country*use of knowledge and skills) were computed as a product of the originating process variables with the country dummy. To avoid collinearity, interaction variables have been built as the product of the country dummy by the mean-centered originating variable.
Control variables
We adopted both firm- and board-related control variables in our analyses. At the firm-level, we controlled for firm size, listing and age. Firm size was measured as firm turnover, and a logarithmic transformation allowed adjustment for skewness. We introduced a dummy variable to control for public listing (1 = listed company). Our firm age variable was a logarithmic transformation of the age the firm.
At the board-level, we controlled for CEO tenure and the traditional board demographic variables. CEO tenure may have an influence on board task performance across life cycle evolution (Shen, 2003). The CEO tenure was computed as the number of years in office the CEO served in the firm. The traditional board demographic variables included are the four “usual suspects”, i.e., board size, non-executive ratio, CEO duality and director shareholding (Finkelstein & Mooney, 2003). Board size was measured as the total number of directors (Zahra, Neubaum, & Huse, 2000). The non-executive ratio was measured as the percentage of non-executive directors over the total number of directors (Mallette & Fowler, 1992). The variable CEO duality was coded 1 if the CEO was also the chair of the board, and 0 otherwise (Finkelstein & D'Aveni, 1994). Director shareholding was measured as the ratio of director shareholding to total shareholding, and it included shareholding by inside directors (Kosnik, 1987; Zahra et al., 2000).
Validity of measures
In order to deal with potential common method bias (Doty & Glick, 1998), we performed some of the statistical remedies suggested by Podsakoff et al. (2003). First, we used the Harman's one factor test. The exploratory factor analysis of the items measuring board control and advisory task performance, as well as items underlying the process constructs (effort norms, cognitive conflicts, use of knowledge and skills), exhibited more than one factor with eigenvalues higher than 1.0, suggesting that the majority of the variance between the variables cannot be accounted for by one general factor (common method variance). Second, we used the partial correlation procedure to control for the effects of method variance (Lindell & Whitney, 2001). The results suggest that common method bias does not appear to be a problem in our data.
Additionally, all perceptual measures were evaluated in terms of reliability and validity. The psychometric properties of the multi-items constructs (effort norms, cognitive conflicts, use of skills and knowledge, control tasks and advisory tasks) were assessed simultaneously in one confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) using MPLUS Version 5.21. The CFA results showed a good model fit (CFI = 0.921; RMSEA = 0.062 with a 90 per cent confidence interval ranging between 0.056 and 0.069). We assessed reliability by calculating a composite reliability for each construct (Fornell & Larcker, 1981). Along with the reliability calculations, we also examined the parameter estimates and their associated t-values as well as the average variances extracted (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988). The reliabilities of the scales range from 0.91 to 0.97, the factor loadings range from 0.47 to 0.91 (p < 0.05), and the average variance extracted range from 76 to 90 per cent (see Table 2). The items were also found to be reliable and valid when evaluated based on each item's error variance, modification index and residual co-variation. Table 2 presents the results of the measurement assessment and reports the average variances extracted, construct reliabilities and factor loadings for all independent and dependent constructs used in our analyses.
Table 2.
Average variance extracted (%) | Composite reliability | Range of loadings | |
---|---|---|---|
1. Effort norms | 78 | 0.91 | 0.47 to 0.85 |
2. Cognitive conflicts | 90 | 0.97 | 0.65 to 0.91 |
3. Use of knowledge and skills | 87 | 0.95 | 0.67 to 0.78 |
4. Control task performance | 86 | 0.97 | 0.67 to 0.73 |
5. Advisory task performance | 76 | 0.94 | 0.49 to 0.76 |
We established discriminant validity by two independent methods. First, we calculated the shared variance between each pair of constructs and verified that it was lower than the variances extracted for the involved constructs (Fornell & Larcker, 1981). The shared variances between pairs of all possible scale combinations indicated that the average variances extracted were higher than the associated shared variances in all cases. Second, we examined all possible pairs of constructs, as suggested by Bagozzi and Philips (1982), in a series of two-factor CFA models. The pairwise analysis tests showed that CFA models representing two factors fitted the data significantly better than one factor models (i.e., Δ χ2 (1) > 3.84 was exceeded in all cases). In addition, we tested for the invariability of the constructs and their measurement across the two national contexts. A multi-group analysis was conducted to validate the factorial structure of the proposed constructs and test whether the regression coefficients were invariant across the two populations (Williams, Edwards, & Vandenberg, 2003). We tested for equality with respect to the measurement model (invariance of factor loadings, factor variance and error terms) including all multi-items constructs across the two populations. The imposition of equality constraints on all freely estimated parameters led to multi-group comparison models with a good fit to the data. Furthermore, the results of the Lagrange-Multiplier test for incremental increase in χ2 when releasing equality constraints were all greater than 0.05, thereby indicating that the hypothesized equality of the specified factor loadings and factor variances held across the two samples. Hence, we found support for the validity of our constructs across the two subsamples.
Results
Table 3 shows means, standard deviations and bivariate correlation coefficients for the variables used in the regression analyses.
Table 3.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | 14 | 15 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Firm size | 1 | ||||||||||||||
2. Listing | 0.14** | 1 | |||||||||||||
3. Firm age | −0.16** | 0.06 | 1 | ||||||||||||
4. CEO tenure | 0.08 | 0.02 | 0.17** | 1 | |||||||||||
5. Country (ITA = 1, NOR = 0) | 0.22** | 0.07 | −0.08 | 0.44** | 1 | ||||||||||
6. CEO duality | 0.03 | 0.05 | 0.02 | 0.42** | 0.34** | 1 | |||||||||
7. Board size | 0.12* | 0.25** | 0.11* | 0.08 | 0.16** | 0.13** | 1 | ||||||||
8. Non-executives ratio | −0.06 | 0.16** | 0.10* | −0.32** | −0.60** | −0.27** | 0.24** | 1 | |||||||
9. Director ownership | −0.06 | −0.10 | 0.05 | 0.29** | 0.27** | 0.14** | −0.21** | −0.37** | 1 | ||||||
10. Effort norms | −0.02 | 0.13** | 0.05 | 0.00 | −0.09 | −0.05 | −0.13** | 0.07 | 0.03 | 1 | |||||
11. Cognitive conflicts | −0.03 | −0.06 | −0.04 | −0.15** | −0.21** | −0.15* | 0.00 | 0.12** | 0.06 | 0.20** | 1 | ||||
12. Use of knowledge and skills | −0.02 | 0.07 | 0.05 | 0.08 | 0.16** | 0.13** | 0.01 | −0.17** | 0.15** | 0.19** | −0.23** | 1 | |||
13. Advisory tasks | 0.01 | 0.11* | 0.01 | 0.03 | −0.04 | 0.05 | −0.13** | −0.06 | 0.12* | 0.40** | 0.01 | 0.34** | |||
14. Control tasks | −0.04 | 0.13** | 0.16** | −0.12* | −0.42** | −0.15** | −0.03 | 0.23** | −0.12* | 0.36** | 0.01 | 0.26** | 0.52** | 1 | |
15. Roa (t + 1) | 0.01 | 0.03 | 0.02 | 0.08 | 0.06 | 0.05 | −0.01 | −0.06 | 0.06 | 0.07 | −0.09 | 0.19** | 0.22** | 0.18** | 1 |
Mean | 11.80 | 0.20 | 3.59 | 11.76 | 0.49 | 0.19 | 6.62 | 0.67 | 27.26 | 3.07 | 1.90 | 3.88 | 3.15 | 3.75 | 6.06 |
SD | 2.20 | 0.40 | 0.92 | 11.36 | 0.50 | 0.36 | 3.00 | 0.34 | 37.18 | 0.81 | 0.77 | 0.68 | 0.84 | 0.85 | 8.81 |
Pearson's product-moment correlation coefficients. 2-tailed: *< 0.05; **< 0.01, N = 414.
Table 3 shows acceptable levels of correlations among the predictors and the dependent variables (board performance in control and advisory tasks). Based on this preliminary analysis, we conducted VIF analyses after each regression to test for multicollinearity. VIF values range from 1 to 3, indicating that multicollinearity is not a problem in our study (Neter, Kutner, Nachtsheim, & Wasserman, 1996).
The hypotheses were tested through hierarchical multiple regression analyses. Before running the analyses we examined potential problems in the distribution of variables with respect to assumptions of hierarchical regression analysis. We performed hierarchical regressions for the board tasks presented in the theory section. Each set of regressions was entered in different steps. The sets of regressions presented in Tables 4 and 5 have been performed in five different models: Model I includes control variables only; model II includes board demographics as board-related controls; model III considers the main effects of our predictors on board task performance; model IV includes the country effect and model V accounts for the three interactions among board processes and the country variable.
Table 4.
Standardized β coefficients N = 415 | Model I | Model II | Model III | Model IV | Model V |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Controls | |||||
Firm size | −0.07† | −0.06 | −0.05 | 0.02 | 0.04 |
Listing | 0.14** | 0.14** | 0.07 | 0.10* | 0.10* |
Firm age | 0.20*** | 0.18*** | 0.15*** | 0.07† | 0.07† |
CEO tenure | −0.15*** | −0.04 | −0.05 | 0.06 | 0.04 |
Board demography | |||||
CEO duality | −0.08 | −0.08† | −0.05 | −0.04 | |
Board size | −0.12* | −0.06 | 0.08† | 0.10* | |
Non-executives ratio | 0.16** | 0.17*** | −0.11† | −0.13* | |
Director ownership | −0.07 | −0.10* | −0.06 | −0.06 | |
Board processes | |||||
Effort norms | 0.29*** | 0.28*** | 0.14* | ||
Cognitive conflicts | −0.02 | −0.07† | −0.13* | ||
Use of knowledge and skills | 0.23*** | 0.24*** | 0.32*** | ||
Country effect | |||||
Country (ITA = 1, Nor = 0) | −0.53*** | −0.55*** | |||
Interactions | |||||
Country (ITA) * effort norms | 0.18** | ||||
Country (ITA) * conflicts | 0.07 | ||||
Country (ITA) * use of knowledge | −0.09 | ||||
R2 | 0.08 | 0.12 | 0.26 | 0.38 | 0.41 |
Adj R2 | 0.07 | 0.10 | 0.24 | 0.37 | 0.39 |
F change | 8.6*** | 4.91*** | 27.2*** | 81.3*** | 5.2** |
F (sign) full model | 7.8*** | 6.9*** | 13.4*** | 21.5*** | 18.7*** |
† = 0.10-level, * = 0.05-level, ** = 0.01-level, ***= 0.001-level.
The listwise deletion procedure determined a number of complete observations of N = 415.
Note: Some changes have been made to this table to correct layout and reporting of decimal places on 7 February 2011 after first publication online on 5 January 2011.
Table 5.
Standardized β coefficients N = 419 | Model I | Model II | Model III | Model IV | Model V |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Controls | |||||
Firm size | −0.01 | 0.01 | 0.02 | 0.04 | 0.06 |
Listing | 0.11* | 0.15** | 0.07 | 0.08† | 0.08† |
Firm age | 0.01 | 0.01 | −0.03 | −0.05 | −0.05 |
CEO tenure | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.03 | 0.01 |
Board demography | |||||
CEO duality | −0.01 | −0.01 | −0.00 | 0.02 | |
Board size | −0.15** | −0.08† | −0.04 | −0.01 | |
Non-executives ratio | −0.00 | 0.01 | −0.08 | −0.12† | |
Director ownership | 0.10† | 0.06 | 0.07 | 0.07 | |
Board processes | |||||
Effort norms | 0.33*** | 0.33*** | 0.13* | ||
Cognitive conflicts | 0.01 | −0.01 | −0.12* | ||
Use of knowledge & skills | 0.28*** | 0.28*** | 0.34*** | ||
Country effect | |||||
Country (ITA = 1, Nor = 0) | −0.17** | −0.20*** | |||
Interactions | |||||
Country (ITA) * effort norms | 0.27*** | ||||
Country (ITA) * conflicts | 0.16** | ||||
Country (ITA) * use of knowledge | −0.05 | ||||
R2 | 0.01 | 0.05 | 0.25 | 0.26 | 0.32 |
Adj. R2 | 0.01 | 0.03 | 0.23 | 0.24 | 0.29 |
F change | 1.2 | 3.9** | 37.5*** | 6.8** | 10.4*** |
F (sign) full model | 1.2 | 2.6** | 12.7*** | 12.3*** | 12.6*** |
† = 0.10-level, * = 0.05-level, ** = 0.01-level, *** = 0.001-level.
The listwise deletion procedure determined a number of complete observations of N = 419.
Note: Some changes have been made to this table to correct layout and reporting of decimal places on 7 February 2011 after first publication online on 5 January 2011.
The first set of regressions was performed considering the control task performance as dependent variable. As evident from Table 4, all the models are significant and adjusted R2 range from 0.08 (model I) to 0.41 (model V). Further, the most significant F-changes are between model II and model III (27.2***) and model III and model IV (81.3***), indicating the relevance of board processes and the country variable to predict control task performance. Table 4 shows results in details.
The second set of regressions refers to the board advisory task performance. As Table 5 shows, all models are significant with the exception of model I including only controls. Adjusted R2 range from 0.01 (model I) to 0.32 (model V), and the most significant F-change is between model II and model III (37.5***), indicating the predictive potential of board processes on board task performance in advice. Further, the R2 change and the F-change statistics between model IV and model V are stronger than the previous set of regressions, indicating that interactions are more relevant for advisory tasks than for control. Detailed results are presented in Table 5.
The third set of regressions tests for the hypothesized positive effects of board effectiveness in performing its tasks on firm's financial outcomes. Table 6 reports results on the sets of tests we performed in two different steps 1. The first model includes control variables, and both control and advisory task performance measures. The second model includes board processes in addition to control variables, and board task performance in control and advice to assess whether results from the first step remain robust after including our main predictors.
Table 6.
Standardized β coefficients | Dependent variable = ROA t + 1 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model I | Model II | ||||||
Control variables | |||||||
Firm size | 0.07 | 0.08 | 0.10† | 0.11† | |||
Listing | 0.04 | 0.01 | 0.03 | 0.01 | |||
Firm age | −0.02 | −0.02 | −0.03 | −0.03 | |||
CEO tenure | 0.05 | 0.09 | 0.05 | 0.09 | |||
CEO duality | 0.04 | 0.06 | 0.02 | 0.04 | |||
Board size | −0.06 | −0.02 | −0.07 | −0.04 | |||
Non-executives ratio | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.02 | 0.01 | |||
Director ownership | 0.03 | 0.06 | −0.01 | 0.02 | |||
Processes | |||||||
Effort norms | −0.02 | −0.04 | |||||
Cognitive conflicts | −0.03 | −0.06 | |||||
Use of know and skills | 0.14* | 0.10 | |||||
Board tasks | |||||||
Control tasks | 0.15* | 0.13† | |||||
Advisory tasks | 0.13† | 0.15† | |||||
R2 | 0.02 | 0.09 | 0.04 | 0.11 | |||
Adj. R2 | 0.00 | 0.06 | 0.00 | 0.07 | |||
F/F change | 0.44 | 10.24*** | 1.89 | 6.02** | |||
N= | 320 | 320 | 304 | 304 |
† = 0.10-level,* = 0.05-level, ** = 0.01-level, *** = 0.001-level.
Note: Some changes have been made to this table to correct layout and reporting of decimal places on 7 February 2011 after first publication online on 5 January 2011.
Overall, our results show that: (i) Board performance in both control and advisory tasks predict firm financial performance measured as Return On Assets in year t + 1; and (ii) the positive effects of board task performance (both control and advice) remain significant even after introducing board processes in the regression equations 2. Overall, these results provide evidence that there is a relationship between self-reported performance measures of board tasks and firm performance, and that this relationship is robust (Simons, Pelled, & Smith, 1999).
The different sets of regressions above provide support for hypotheses 1 and 3, respectively, on effort norms and the use of knowledge and skills, while they do not provide supporting evidence for Hypothesis 2. The results also provide support for Hypothesis 4 on the relationship between task performance and firm financial results. Furthermore, our results provide support for Hypothesis 5 on the higher board performance in both control and advisory tasks in Norway. With respect to Hypothesis 6, results show support for effort norms related to both board performance in control and advisory tasks, while cognitive conflicts show positive interaction with control task performance only. The use of knowledge and skills, however, shows no interactions at the country level with either control or advisory task performance. All the significant relationships are graphically presented in Figures 1–3 illustrating the trends of our process predictors with respect to board control (Figure 1) and advisory task (Figures 2 and 3) performance. Slopes in the figures have been plotted following guidance from Aguinis and Gottfredson (2010), and tests for slopes' significance have been realized using the Johnson–Neyman technique for probing interactions in linear models (Hayes and Matthes, 2009).
We performed an additional set of analyses in order to further validate our main results. Specifically, in order to account for potential perceptual biases of CEO respondents being also Chairs of their boards, we performed the same full set of regression analyses without considering responses of “dual” CEOs (i.e., CEOs being also board Chairs). While separate CEO and Chair positions may indicate CEOs as both “clients” of board advice and “targets” of board control, dual CEOs may be evaluating at least to some extent the quality of his or her own leadership of the board. Empirical analyses showed identical results for analyses excluding dual CEOs (the full analyses are not reported here). Results also show that dual CEOs are not positively biased in their ratings.
Discussion
The main purpose of this study is to introduce a macro-level variable (country) to the boards and governance research on micro-level behavioral processes (Bamberger, 2008; Hambrick et al., 2008). We aim at understanding how board effectiveness is influenced both by micro-level board processes and macro-level institutional contexts simultaneously. We collected board data from two countries which substantially differ along both legal, and institutional and cultural dimensions in order to investigate how contexts moderate the relationship between board processes and task performance (La Porta et al., 1998). In this way, we offer empirical evidence on the co-existence of micro- and macro-level analyses in understanding board effectiveness within corporate governance structures (Lubatkin et al., 2005), and go beyond the mere acknowledgment of context as a potentially relevant variable (Bamberger, 2008).
Micro-level determinants of board task performance
A first set of results relates to the micro-level determinants of board task performance. Following recent calls to go beyond the formal structure of corporate boards (Daily et al., 2003; Hambrick et al., 2008), this study considers board effectiveness as the actual performance of board control and advisory tasks. We focus on micro-level board processes rather than demographics (Finkelstein & Mooney, 2003), and apply an integrated model of board effectiveness which considers three main board processes (Forbes & Milliken, 1999) as determinants of board task performance.
The results of our analyses support the validity of the applied model. First, consistent with our theoretical predictions, board demographics show limited impact on board task performance, indicating that they hardly predict board task performance or even firm performance (Daily et al., 2003; Hambrick et al., 2008). Some evidence emerges only with respect to non-executive ratio, illustrating how the presence of non-executives on boards is beneficial for control (Mallette & Fowler, 1992). However, this evidence applies only to basic models and changes direction of causality when we introduce interactions. Hence, the presence of non-executives seems to be only marginally relevant and even negative for advice, indicating how an unbalanced mix of directors toward outsiders favor board control yet at the price of lower inside knowledge of the firm (Mallette & Fowler, 1992).
Second, and most importantly, our findings provide support for board processes being much stronger predictors of board tasks than board's characteristics in terms of CEO duality, board size, board members' active shareholding or non-executive ratio (Finkelstein and Mooney, 2003). Specifically, effort norms show a consistent positive effect on both control and advisory task performance. This finding supports arguments pertaining to the benefits of board members doing their “homework” to understand firm's specificities and key strategic problems (Forbes & Milliken, 1999). Moreover, these findings are consistent with reviews of best practices to empower corporate boards (Lorsch & MacIver, 1989) and encourage open debate and enactment (Hambrick et al., 2008) in counteracting groupthink, thereby enhancing board effectiveness (Sonnenfeld, 2002). It also supports the importance of engaging board members in discussion, thereby avoiding “rubber stamping” attitudes toward managerial proposals (Lorsch & MacIver, 1989; Mace, 1971; Stiles & Taylor, 2001). Effort behaviors furthermore counteract habits of “pluralistic ignorance” in workgroups that potentially affects outside members in boards, who are often hesitant to express and share their concerns to others (Westphal & Bednar, 2005). Similar considerations apply to the use of knowledge and skills, as our results support the benefits of an active use and integration of board members' expertise and skills for group decisions. The use of knowledge and skills may prevent “process losses” and help interdependent decision-making groups, such as corporate boards, to build on each other's contributions (Forbes & Milliken, 1999).
The findings about cognitive conflicts indicate more complex patterns. While cognitive conflicts are not significant as a standalone effect, models including interactions show a negative association between conflicts and both control and advisory task performance. Although this evidence must be interpreted cautiously given potential perceptual biases related to measurement issues (Staw, 1975), it opens up for debate at both the micro- and the macro-level of analysis. At the micro-level, De Dreu and Weingart (2003) notice that, despite mainstream theory predictions support task-related conflicts as being positive for task effectiveness, a meta-analysis on the most recent empirical research questions this prediction. In terms of corporate boards, the customary reluctance toward open and candid discussion (Hambrick et al., 2008) can make conflicts an anguished experience for board members. The most important theoretical implications result from considering the macro-level of analysis, which suggests cognitive conflicts as being context-specifically relevant. This evidence reinforces our contribution to bring context into organizational research, and will be discussed in the following sections.
Macro-level (context) determinants of board task performance
A second set of results shows the impact of macro-level institutional context on board task performance. According to institutional theory, human and social behaviors are driven by the pervasive influence of institutions such as norms, rules, routines and historical patterns (DiMaggio & Powell, 1983; Scott, 2001). To test the hypothesized relationships we adopted a cross-national or cross-cultural lens (Gomez-Mejia & Wiseman, 2007) to examine how institutional settings and related institutional constraints shape the way boards perform their tasks. Our results provide evidence of the positive effect of the Scandinavian institutional context on board control and advisory task performance. This finding supports arguments of the relevance of legal (La Porta et al., 1998) and cultural (e.g., Hofstede, 1983) dissimilarities to board effectiveness at the micro-level. It further suggests that corporations are influenced not only by legal compliance with governance codes and statutes (Aguilera & Cuervo-Cazurra, 2004), but also by normative conformance with cultural and institutional norms and values on what represents effective governance (Hambrick et al., 2008). As such, this study provides a preliminary answer to theoretical calls for going beyond “context-free” or universalistic approaches to governance research (Aguilera et al., 2008).
The moderating effect of (macro-level) context on the relationship between board processes and board task performance
A third set of evidences reveals how macro- and micro-level determinants of board effectiveness interact to shape board task performance. As such, we address requests to explore how different institutional environments moderate the hypothesized relationships between sets of governance practices and organizational outcomes, such as effectiveness, efficiency or performance (Aguilera et al., 2008).
The strongest finding relates to effort norms, suggesting a stronger relevance of effort norms in contexts characterized by high power distance (Hofstede, 1984). The positive moderating effect on both control and advisory tasks (see Figures 1 and 2) further indicates the importance of a culture of commitment and preparation (Forbes & Milliken, 1999; Hambrick et al., 2008) in contexts where low participative decision-making culture prevails (Hofstede, 1984). Effort norms behaviors will encourage board member discussions rather than free-riding, and will make it more difficult for board members to hide behind “pluralistic ignorance” (Westphal & Bednar, 2005). Additional insights emerge when we consider the interaction effects of context on cognitive conflicts. While conflicts are found to negatively predict control and advisory tasks in the overall cross-country sample, they become positive in predicting advisory task performance of Italian corporate boards (see Figure 3). This evidence supports the assumed ambiguity of conflicts on group performance (De Dreu & Weingart, 2003), thus emphasizing the importance of context variables on organizational processes (Hambrick et al., 2008). We argue that in high power distance cultures, cognitive conflicts might help prevent “social distancing” among board members, and particularly between inside and outside directors (Westphal & Bednar, 2005). In this respect, the lack of evidence in relation to control may be explained by advisory tasks being more related to problem-solving, and hence requiring different perspectives and evaluation of different alternatives than cognitive conflicts may offer (Eisenhardt, 1989).
Finally, the use of knowledge and skills shows similar patterns in both contexts, with moderating effect being non-significant. Contrary to previous findings, our results show that using knowledge and skills in highly interdependent groups is predictive of group task performance regardless of the country context. As such, our findings point to the use of knowledge and skill as a more universalistic practice, which corporate boards should carefully follow. Overall, our results support theoretical predictions that effective board processes are likely to be stronger predictors of tasks in highly individualistic power distance cultures. Our findings may also relate to the greater variance in processes that individualistic cultures experience vis-à-vis collectivistic contexts. In other words, there might be a “ceiling effect” in high collectivist-low power distance cultures (like Scandinavia), since processes tend to be naturally higher in such cultures. Consequently, the relatively lesser variance in the good process context of Scandinavian boards will lower the predictive power of board processes on task performance.
Limitations and future directions
The paper is not without limitations, which may provide opportunities for future research. First, in line with the majority of previous cross-national studies (see Tsui et al., 2007), we investigated only two country contexts. Specifically, we considered Norway and Italy as examples of Scandinavian and Latin cultural and institutional environments which represent different cases in terms of corporate governance standards (La Porta et al., 1998). Including two legally and culturally distinct countries in our analyses assured adequate between-country variation on board effectiveness and internal processes. However, as Kirkman et al. (2009) and Tsui et al. (2007) noticed, including more countries may be appropriate to ascertain the generalizability of results, even in case of largely distinct countries, cultures and phenomena. Such an approach would allow researchers to examine within-country as well as between-country variance in board processes and task performance in a multilevel research design. It might also open up opportunities to investigate which specific factors at the country level serve as moderators of the board process-task performance relationships. Second, studies combining micro-level board process data with the macro-level determinants are rare, and more studies should be encouraged. As such, future studies should attempt to combine access to data with the need to go beyond the formal structure of boards in boards and governance research (Forbes & Milliken, 1999). Third, our study is cross-sectional due to difficulties in collecting cross-country data in relation to matching sample issues and construct equivalence (Tsui et al., 2007). Longitudinal studies may provide complementary and additional insights on how the evolution of the firm- and context- level variables influence the characteristics of governance practices. Future challenges include how processes can be measured across countries and time periods. Finally, this study is based on the perceptions of CEOs, and any generalization must take potential biases into account. Hence, it remains a challenge for future studies to explore the effects of perceptions by other board members and respondents (Hillman, Nicholson, & Shropshire, 2008). While these considerations impose some limitations on the interpretation of our results, they also offer valuable insights for future research in the area of international comparisons of board behaviors and practices, which is still considerably under-investigated.
Conclusion
The primary objective of this study was to incorporate macro-level (country) explanations into the boards and governance research. To this end, we focused on board effectiveness in its tasks to investigate how macro-level determinants (context) influence micro-level relationships. Our analyses offer evidence of how both micro- and macro-level variables influence board task performance, and show how the macro-level country variable moderates relationships between board processes and task performance. As a result, the paper provides a preliminary answer to recent calls to bridge under-contextualized agency theory approaches and over-contextualized views of institutional theory (Aguilera et al., 2008; Aguilera & Jackson 2003; Hambrick et al., 2008), and more generally to incorporate macro-level variables in management research (Bamberger, 2008).
Footnotes
This is to note that the number of observations significantly dropped in this further analysis, due to a number of missing values related to the financial performance measure (ROAt+1).
The same sets of regressions as presented in Table 6 have been performed with past year's ROA as a control variable, with results holding at similar levels of significance. The only exception was for control tasks, the effect of which went from 5 to 10 per cent significance level in model I presented in this table. It enhances robustness of our results indicating board tasks as predicting firm financial performance.
Biographical Information
Alessandro Minichilli is Assistant Professor at Bocconi University in Milan, where he received his Ph.D. Alessandro has a background in corporate governance, with a focus on board characteristics and actual board behaviours. He is also interested in top management teams and executives' leadership characteristics. Based on this background, his recent work directs attention to family controlled firms, and lies in the intersection between corporate governance, TMTs and family business research. He teaches corporate governance and strategy in graduate courses.
Biographical Information
Alessandro Zattoni is Professor of Management at Parthenope University in Naples and Professor of Strategic Management at the SDA Bocconi School of Management in Milan. His research interests focus on boards of directors, codes of good governance, stock incentive plans and ownership structures.
Biographical Information
Sabina Tacheva Nielsen is a post-doctoral research fellow at the Department of International Economics and Management, Copenhagen Business School. She received her Ph.D. from University of St. Gallen, Switzerland. Her research interests include the composition, dynamics and effects of top management teams and boards, as well as diversity in multinational corporations.
Biographical Information
Morten Huse, Professor dr. oecon, is affiliated with the Norwegian School of Management BI and Tor Vergata University in Rome. He was formerly the chairperson of the National Association of Corporate Directors (StyreAkademiet) in Norway, and he has been Director of International Themes at the Academy of Management in the USA. Morten Huse has published a large number of books and articles about behavioral perspectives on boards of directors.
References
- Aguilera RV, Cuervo-Cazurra A. Codes of good governance worldwide: What is the trigger? Organization Studies. 2004;25:415–443. [Google Scholar]
- Aguilera RV, Jackson G. The cross national-diversity of corporate governance. Dimensions and determinants. Academy of Management Review. 2003;28:447–465. [Google Scholar]
- Aguilera RV, Filatotchev I, Gospel H, Jackson G. An organizational approach to comparative corporate governance: Costs, contingencies, and complementarities. Organization Science. 2008;19:475–493. [Google Scholar]
- Aguinis H, Gottfredson RK. Best practice recommendations for estimating interaction effects using moderated multiple regressions. Journal of Organizational Behavior. 2010;31:776–786. [Google Scholar]
- Amason AC, Sapienza HJ. The effects of top management team size and interactions norms on cognitive and affective conflict. Journal of Management. 1997;23:495–516. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson JC, Gerbing DW. Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and recommended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin. 1988;10:411–423. [Google Scholar]
- Bagozzi RP, Phillips LW. Representing and testing organizational theories: A holistic construal. Administrative Science Quarterly. 1982;27:459–489. [Google Scholar]
- Bamberger P. Beyond contextualization: Using context theories to narrow the micro-macro gap in management research. Academy of Management Journal. 2008;51:839–846. [Google Scholar]
- Bettenhausen K. Five years of group research: What we have learned, and what needs to be addressed. Journal of Management. 1991;17:345–381. [Google Scholar]
- Brunello G, Graziano C, Parigi BM. CEO turnover in insider-dominated boards: The Italian case. Journal of Banking and Finance. 2003;27:1027–1051. [Google Scholar]
- Buck T, Shahrim A. The translation of corporate governance changes across national cultures: The case of Germany. Journal of International Business Studies. 2005;36:42–61. [Google Scholar]
- Cannella AA, Shen W. So close and yet so far: Promotion versus exit for CEO heirs apparent. Academy of Management Journal. 2001;44:252–270. [Google Scholar]
- Carpenter M. The implications of strategy and social context for the relationship between Top Management Team heterogeneity and firm performance. Strategic Management Journal. 2002;23:275–284. [Google Scholar]
- Carpenter MA, Westphal JD. The strategic context of external network ties: Examining the impact of director appointments on board involvement in strategic decision making. Academy of Management Journal. 2001;4:639–660. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen S, Bailey D. What makes teams work: Group effectiveness research from the shop floor to the executive suite. Journal of Management. 1997;23:239–290. [Google Scholar]
- Cycyota CS, Harrison DA. What not to expect when surveying executives: A meta-analysis of top manager response rates and techniques over time. Organizational Research Methods. 2006;92:133–160. [Google Scholar]
- Daily CM, Dalton DR. Corporate governance and the bankrupt firm: An empirical assessment. Strategic Management Journal. 1994;15:643–654. [Google Scholar]
- Daily CM, Dalton DR, Cannella AA. Corporate governance: Decades of dialogue and data. Academy of Management Review. 2003;28:371–382. [Google Scholar]
- De Dreu CKW, Weingart LR. Task versus relationship conflict and team effectiveness: A meta analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology. 2003;88:741–749. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.4.741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dillman DA. Mail and internet surveys: The tailored design method. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- DiMaggio P, Powell W. The iron cage revisited: Institutional isomorphism and collective rationality in organizational fields. American Sociological Review. 1983;48:147–160. [Google Scholar]
- Doty DH, Glick WH. Common methods bias: Does common methods variance really bias results? Organizational Research Methods. 1998;14:374–406. [Google Scholar]
- Egri CP, Ralston DA. Generation cohorts and personal values: A comparison of China and the United States. Organization Science. 2004;15:210–220. [Google Scholar]
- Eisenhardt KM. Agency theory: An assessment and review. Academy of Management Review. 1989;14:57–74. [Google Scholar]
- Eisenhardt KM, Kahwajy JL, Bourgeois LJ. Conflict and strategic choice: How top management teams disagree. California Management Review. 1997;39:42–62. [Google Scholar]
- Fama EF. Agency problems and the theory of the firm. Journal of Political Economy. 1980;88:288–307. [Google Scholar]
- Fama EF, Jensen MC. Separation of ownership and control. Journal of Law and Economics. 1983;26:301–325. [Google Scholar]
- Finkelstein S, D'Aveni RA. CEO duality as a double-edged sword: How boards of directors balance entrenchment avoidance and unity of command. Academy of Management Journal. 1994;37:1079–1108. [Google Scholar]
- Finkelstein S, Mooney AC. Not the usual suspects: How to use the board process to make boards better. Academy of Management Executive. 2003;17:101–113. [Google Scholar]
- Forbes DP, Milliken FJ. Cognition and corporate governance: Understanding boards of directors as strategic decision-making groups. Academy of Management Review. 1999;24:489–505. [Google Scholar]
- Fornell C, Larcker D. Structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research. 1981;18:39–50. [Google Scholar]
- Fowler FJ. Survey research methods. Newbury Park: Sage; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Geletkanycz MA. The salience of culture's consequences: The effects of cultural values on top executive commitment to the status quo. Strategic Management Journal. 1997;18:615–634. [Google Scholar]
- Geletkanycz MA, Hambrick DC. The external ties of top executives: Implications for strategic choice and performance. Administrative Science Quarterly. 1997;42:654–681. [Google Scholar]
- Gomez-Mejia LR, Wiseman R. Does agency theory have universal relevance? A reply to Lubatkin, Lane, Collin, and Very. Journal of Organizational Behavior. 2007;28:81–88. [Google Scholar]
- Hackman JR, Morris CG. Group tasks, group interaction process, and group performance effectiveness: A review and proposed integration. In: Berkowitz L, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. Vol. 8. New York: Academic Press; 1975. pp. 45–99. [Google Scholar]
- Hambrick DC, v. Werder A, Zajac EJ. New Directions in corporate governance research. Organization Science. 2008;19:381–385. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes HF, Matthes J. Computational procedures for probing interactions in OLS and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS implementations. Behavior Research Methods. 2009;41(3):924–936. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.3.924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henderson AD, Miller D, Hambrick DC. How quickly do CEOs become obsolete? Industry dynamism, CEO tenure, and company performance. Strategic Management Journal. 2006;27:447–460. [Google Scholar]
- Hillman AJ, Dalziel T. Boards of directors and firm performance: Integrating agency and resource dependence perspectives. Academy of Management Review. 2003;28:383–396. [Google Scholar]
- Hillman AJ, Cannella A, Paetzold R. The resource dependence role of corporate directors: Strategic adaptation of board composition in response to environmental change. Journal of Management Studies. 2000;37:235–256. [Google Scholar]
- Hillman AJ, Nicholson G, Shropshire C. Directors' multiple identities, identification, and board monitoring and resource provision. Organization Science. 2008;19:441–456. [Google Scholar]
- Hofstede G. The cultural relativity of organizational practices and theories. Journal of International Business Studies. 1983:75–89. Fall. [Google Scholar]
- Hofstede G. The cultural relativity of the quality of life concept. Academy of Management Review. 1984;93:389–398. [Google Scholar]
- Hofstede G. Culture's consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations. 2nd. Thousand Oaks: Sage; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Hofstede G, Van Deusen CA, Mueller CB, Charles TA. What goals do business leaders pursue? A study in fifteen countries. Journal of International Business Studies. 2002;33:785–803. [Google Scholar]
- Huse M. Board composition in small firms. Entrepreneurship and Regional Development. 1990;2:363–373. [Google Scholar]
- Huse M. Relational norms as a supplement of neo-classical understanding of directorates. Journal of Socio-Economics. 1993;22:219–240. [Google Scholar]
- Huse M. Boards, governance and value creation: The human side of corporate governance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Jehn K. A multimethod examination of the benefits and detriments of intragroup conflicts. Administrative Science Quarterly. 1995;40:256–282. [Google Scholar]
- Jehn KA. A quantitative analysis of conflict types and dimensions in organizational groups. Administrative Science Quarterly. 1997;42:530–557. [Google Scholar]
- Jensen MC, Meckling WH. Theory of the firm: Managerial behaviour, agency costs and ownership structure. Journal of Financial Economics. 1976;34:305–360. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson JL, Daily CM, Ellstrand AE. Boards of directors: A review and research agenda. Journal of Management. 1996;22:409–438. [Google Scholar]
- Judge WQ, Zeithaml CP. Institutional and strategic choice perspectives on board involvement in the strategic decision process. Academy of Management Journal. 1992;35:766–794. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirkman BL, Chen G, Farh JL, Chen ZX, Lowe KB. Individual power distance orientation and follower reactions to transformational leaders: A cross-level, cross-cultural examination. Academy of Management Journal. 2009;52:744–764. [Google Scholar]
- Kosnik RD. Greenmail: A study of board performance in corporate governance. Administrative Science Quarterly. 1987;32:163–185. [Google Scholar]
- Kumar N, Stern LW, Anderson JC. Conducting interorganizational research using key informants. Academy of Management Journal. 1993;36:1633–1651. [Google Scholar]
- La Porta R, Lopez-de-Silanes F, Shleifer A, Vishny R. Law and finance. Journal of Political Economy. 1998;106:1113–1155. [Google Scholar]
- Lee C, Bobko P, Ashford S, Zhen Xiong C, Xiaopeng R. Cross-cultural development of an abridged job insecurity measure. Journal of Organizational Behavior. 2008;3:373–390. [Google Scholar]
- Licht AN, Goldschmidt C, Schwartz SH. Culture, law, and corporate governance. International Review of Law and Economics. 2005;25:229–255. [Google Scholar]
- Lin Z, Peng MW, Yang H, Sun SL. How do networks and learning drive M&As? An institutional comparison between China and the United States. Strategic Management Journal. 2009;30:1113–1132. [Google Scholar]
- Lindell MK, Whitney DJ. Accounting for common method variance in cross-sectional research designs. Journal of Applied Psychology. 2001;86:114–121. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.86.1.114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorsch JW, MacIver E. Pawns or potentates. The reality of America's corporate boards. Boston: HBS Press; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Lubatkin M. One more time: What is a realistic theory of corporate governance? Journal of Organizational Behavior. 2007;28:59–67. [Google Scholar]
- Lubatkin MH, Lane PJ, Collin S, Very P. Origins of corporate governance in the USA, Sweden and France. Organization Studies. 2005;26:867–888. [Google Scholar]
- Lubatkin M, Lane PJ, Collin S, Very P. An embeddedness framing of governance and opportunism: Towards a cross-nationally accommodating theory of agency. Journal of Organizational Behavior. 2007;28:43–58. [Google Scholar]
- Mace M. Directors: Myth and reality. Boston: Harvard Business School Press; 1971. [Google Scholar]
- Mallette P, Fowler KL. Effects of board composition and stock ownership on the adoption of poison pills. Academy of Management Journal. 1992;35:1010–1035. [Google Scholar]
- Man DC, Lam SSK. The effects of job complexity and autonomy on cohesiveness in collectivistic and individualistic work groups: A cross-cultural analysis. Journal of Organizational Behavior. 2003;24:979–1001. [Google Scholar]
- McNulty T, Pettigrew A. Strategists on the board. Organization Studies. 1999;20:47–74. [Google Scholar]
- Minichilli A, Hansen C. The board advisory tasks in small firms and the event of crisis. Journal of Management and Governance. 2007;11:5–22. [Google Scholar]
- Neter J, Kutner MH, Nachtsheim CJ, Wasserman W. Applied linear statistical models. Boston: McGraw Hill; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- North DC. Institutions, institutional change, and economic performance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Ocasio W. Institutionalized action and corporate governance: The reliance on rules of CEO succession. Administrative Science Quarterly. 1999;44:384–416. [Google Scholar]
- OECD Definition 2005. http://stats.oecd.org/glossary/detail.asp?ID=3123.
- Pearce JA, Zahra SA. The relative power of CEOs and boards of directors: Associations with corporate performance. Strategic Management Journal. 1991;2:135–153. [Google Scholar]
- Pedersen T, Thomsen S. European patterns of corporate ownership: A twelve-country study. Journal of International Business Studies. 1997;28:759–778. [Google Scholar]
- Pettigrew A. On studying managerial elites. Strategic Management Journal. 1992;13:163–182. [Google Scholar]
- Pfeffer J, Salancik GR. The external control of organizations: A resource dependency perspective. New York: Harper and Row; 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Podsakoff PM, Scott B, MacKenzie JL, Podsakoff M. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology. 2003;88:879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polley RB. Intervention and cultural context: Mediation in the US and Norway. Journal of Management. 1988;14:617–629. [Google Scholar]
- Roe M. Political determinants of corporate governance: Political context, corporate impact. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Scharpf FW. Governing in Europe: Effective or democratic? Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Scott RW. Institutions and organizations. 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Shen W. The dynamics of the CEO-Board relationship: An evolutionary perspective. Academy of Management Review. 2003;28:466–476. [Google Scholar]
- Shleifer A, Vishny RW. A survey of corporate governance. Journal of Finance. 1997;52:737–783. [Google Scholar]
- Siegel S, Castellan NJ. Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Simons T, Pelled HL, Smith KA. Making use of difference: Diversity, debate, and decision comprehensiveness in Top Management Teams. Academy of Management Journal. 1999;42:662–673. [Google Scholar]
- Sonnenfeld JA. What makes great boards great. Harvard Business Review. 2002:2–8. September. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steiner I. Group process and productivity. New York: Academic Press; 1972. [Google Scholar]
- Stiles P, Taylor B. Boards at work. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Thomsen S, Pedersen T. Nationality and ownership structures: The one hundred largest companies in six European nations. Management International Review. 1996;36:149–166. [Google Scholar]
- Triandis HC. Culture and conflict. International Journal of Psychology. 2000;35:145–152. [Google Scholar]
- Tsui AS, Nifadkar SS, Ou AY. Cross-national, cross-cultural organizational behavior research: Advances, gaps and recommendations. Journal of Management. 2007;33:426–478. [Google Scholar]
- van Ees H, van der Laan G, Postma T. Effective board behavior in the Netherlands. European Management Review. 2008;26:84–93. [Google Scholar]
- Wageman R. Interdependence and group effectiveness. Administrative Science Quarterly. 1995;40:145–180. [Google Scholar]
- Waldman DA, de Luque MS, Washburn N, House RJ. Cultural and leadership predictors of corporate social responsibility values in top management: A GLOBE study of 15 Countries. Journal of International Business Studies. 2006;37:823–837. [Google Scholar]
- Wan WP, Hoskisson RE. Home country environments, corporate diversification strategies and firm performance. Academy of Management Journal. 2003;46:27–45. [Google Scholar]
- Watson W, Michaelsen L. Group interaction behaviors that affect group performance on an intellective tasks. Group & Organization Studies. 1988;13:495–516. [Google Scholar]
- Westphal JD. Collaboration in the boardroom: Behavioural and performance consequences of CEO-Board social ties. Academy of Management Journal. 1999;42:7–24. [Google Scholar]
- Westphal JD, Bednar MK. Pluralistic ignorance in corporate boards and firm's strategic persistence in response to low firm performance. Administrative Science Quarterly. 2005;50:262–298. [Google Scholar]
- Whitley R. European business systems: Firms and markets in their national contexts. London: Sage; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Williams LJ, Edwards JR, Vandenberg RJ. Recent advances in causal modeling methods for organizational and management research. Journal of Management. 2003;29:903–936. [Google Scholar]
- Williamson OE. The new institutional economics: Taking stock, looking ahead. Journal of Economic Literature. 2000;38:595–613. [Google Scholar]
- Zahra SA. Governance, ownership, and corporate entrepreneurship: The moderating impact of industry technological opportunities. Academy of Management Journal. 1996;39:1713–1735. [Google Scholar]
- Zahra SA. An embeddedness framing of governance and opportunism: Towards a cross-nationally accommodating theory of agency—critique and extension. Journal of Organizational Behavior. 2007;28:69–73. [Google Scholar]
- Zahra SA, Pearce JA. Boards of directors and corporate financial performance: A review and integrative model. Journal of Management. 1989;15:291–334. [Google Scholar]
- Zahra SA, Neubaum DO, Huse M. Entrepreneurship in medium size companies: Exploring the effects of ownership and governance systems. Journal of Management. 2000;26:947–976. [Google Scholar]
- Zona F, Zattoni A. Beyond the black box of demography: Board processes and task effectiveness within Italian firms. Corporate Governance: An International Review. 2007;15:852–864. [Google Scholar]