Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Jan 28.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2008 Fall;20(4):1023–1027. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000485

Imaging Brain Systems in Normality and Psychopathology

Kathleen M Thomas 1, Dante Cicchetti 1
PMCID: PMC3556908  NIHMSID: NIHMS434781  PMID: 18838029

Neurobiological development is a complex process that originates at conception and extends throughout the lifespan (Casey, Tottenham, Liston, & Durston, 2005; Cicchetti & Cannon, 1999; Nowakowski, 1987; Rakic, 1996; Thompson & Nelson, 2001). The course of brain development can be altered by a host of factors, ranging from genetic liabilities to psychosocial stressors, and mental disorders are thought to eventuate from etiologic factors that modify the normal progression of brain development.

Perturbations that take place in the developing brain can trigger a cascade of growth and function changes that lead the neural system down a pathway that deviates from that taken in normal neurobiological development (Cicchetti & Tucker, 1994). Accordingly, abnormal perturbations at one stage of brain development likely impede the creation of some new structures and functions, distort the form of later-emerging ones, bring about the construction of structures and functions that would never become manifest, and hinder or limit the elaboration and usage of ones that had appeared earlier (Cicchetti, 2002; Courchesne, Chisum, & Townsend, 1994). Subsequently, abnormal neural network configurations and operations likely develop that may cause aberrant connections to be retained or created (Courchesne et al., 1994). Such early developmental abnormalities may eventuate in the development of aberrant neurocircuity and often compound themselves into enduring forms of psychopathology (Cicchetti & Cannon, 1999; Nowakowski & Hayes, 1999).

An outgrowth of systems theorizing in neuroscience has been a growing acceptance that neurobiological development and experience are mutually influencing (Cicchetti & Tucker, 1994; Eisenberg, 1995; Greenough, Black, & Wallace, 1987; Kandel, 1998; Nelson & Bloom, 1997). Pathological experience may become part of a vicious cycle, as the pathology induced in the brain structure may distort the child’s experience, with subsequent alterations in cognition or social interactions causing additional pathological experience and added brain pathology (Black, Jones, Nelson, & Greenough, 1998). Because experience-expectant and experience-dependent processes may continue to operate during psychopathological states, children who incorporate pathological experience during these processes may add neuropathological connections into their developing brains instead of functional neuronal connections (Black et al., 1998; Cicchetti & Tucker, 1994).

Basic research in neuroscience has begun to elucidate the neural events that mediate the relation between experience and behavior. Researchers in the field of developmental psychopathology have begun to use this knowledge base to inform their investigations aimed at uncovering the neural mechanisms that might subserve the dynamic, multiple-level interactions that exist among genes, brain, behavior, and experience (Gottlieb, 2002; Gottlieb & Willoughby, 2006).

Despite the major influence that embryology exerted upon the leading systematizers in the field of developmental psychology (Fishbein, 1976; Sameroff, 1983; Waddington, 1957; Weiss, 1961, 1969), the majority of the classic theories of normal development that were prominent throughout much of the twentieth century accorded little attention to neurobiological processes (Cicchetti, 2002; Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Johnson, 1998; Nelson, Thomas, & de Haan, 2006; Segalowitz, 1994). Undoubtedly, the paucity of information that existed about the structural and functional organization of the brain contributed to the relative neglect of neurobiology in the formulation of developmental theorizing on the ontogenesis and epigenesis of behavior (Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Johnson, 1998; Kandel, 1998, 1999).

Although extant knowledge of the nature of the relations between neurobiology and behavior in both normal and abnormal development across the lifespan is far from complete, in recent decades a number of technological advances have emerged that have greatly enhanced the ability of neuroscientists and psychopathologists to discover normal and abnormal pathological processes in the brain (Amso & Casey, 2006; Casey, Giedd, & Thomas, 2000; Casey, Tottenham, Liston, & Durston, 2005; Durston et al., 2006; Johnson, Halit, Grice, & Karmiloff-Smith, 2002). This rapid growth in the development of sophisticated techniques that permit the anatomical and physiological imaging of the nervous system has enabled researchers to uncover diverse information about the brain, including brain metabolic processes, glucose metabolic rate, the ability to distinguish between cerebrospinal fluid and white and gray matter, the capacity to detect biochemical changes within brain cells, such as changes in neurotransmitter receptors, and the examination of brain connectivity through tracing white matter tracts and detecting brain functional connectivity (Hunt & Thomas, this issue; Thomas, 2003).

Because developmental psychopathology and neuroscience share fundamental principles, the connection between neuroscience and developmental psychopathology can provide a compelling framework to support the study of normal and abnormal neurobiological development (Cicchetti & Posner, 2005). For example, one of the central principles of developmental psychopathology – that the study of normality and pathology are mutually informative – also is embraced by developmental neuroscientists (Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Johnson, 1998). Scientists in each of these disciplines believe that a firm knowledge base of normative developmental processes is essential for understanding both psychopatholgy and resilient functioning (Cicchetti, 1993; Sroufe, 1990). Moreover, scientists in these two fields have long asserted that one can gain valuable information about an organism’s normal functioning by investigating its abnormal condition (Cicchetti & Cannon, 1999; Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Johnson, 1998; Nelson, Thomas, & de Haan, 2006).

The theme of this Special Issue, “Imaging Brain Systems in Normality and Psychopathology,” addresses a timely and important topic that has the potential to augment the understanding of the etiology, developmental course, and pathogenesis of high-risk conditions and mental disorders across the lifespan. Furthermore, in the present era, where interdisciplinary and multiple-levels-of-analysis perspectives are receiving increased attention and emphasis (see, e.g., Cacioppo et al., 2007; Cicchetti & Posner, 2005; Gottlieb, Wahlsten, & Lickliter, 2006; Masten, 2007; Pellmar & Eisenberg, 2000), the incorporation of neuroimaging into the research armamentarium of developmental psychopathologists may contribute to an increased comprehension of the mechanisms underlying maladaptive, psychopathological, and resilient adaptation (Cicchetti & Curtis, 2007). Furthermore, because psychopathology and resilience cannot be understood fully unless all levels of analysis are examined, the integration of neuroimaging into basic multi-level empirical investigations will be critical to suggesting future opportunities for translational research in neuroscience and developmental psychopathology (Cicchetti & Gunnar, 2008; Gunnar & Cicchetti, in press).

As illustrated through the contributions to this Special Issue, neuroimaging methods are being used by neuroscientists and developmental psychopathologists to enhance the understanding of normal and abnormal neurobiological development and to augment knowledge concerning the processes and pathways linking neurodevelopment and outcomes, typical and disordered (Casey et al., 2005; Giedd, Shaw, Wallace, Gogtay, & Lenroot, 2006; Nelson & Bloom, 1997). For example, it is now thought that the increasing cognitive capacities that emerge in childhood may take place concurrently with a gradual loss rather than with the formation of new synapses and a presumed strengthening of the synaptic connections that remain (Casey, Giedd, & Thomas, 2000). Moreover, regions of the brain that are associated with basic functions (e.g., sensory and motor processes) have been shown to mature earliest, followed by maturation of the association regions that are involved in the top-down control of behavior (Casey et al., 2005). In addition, as cortical systems undergo a fine-tuning, it has been shown that there is a corresponding enhancement of connectivity with cortical and subcortical circuitry (Amso & Casey, 2006). Thus, neuroimaging research has helped the field of cognitive development transcend the questions of what develops and when, to how these transformations in cognition across time may take place (Amso & Casey, 2005; Casey et al., 2000). Furthermore, neuroimaging research has aided in the discovery of the underlying neural mechanisms of a number of psychopathological conditions.

These scientific gains may allow us to develop therapeutic strategies that may lead to advances in the treatment, and even in the prevention of, behavioral and emotional symptoms, as well as coping strategies, that may have been adaptive in their initial context, but proved to be ultimately maladaptive. Through investigating brain structure and function developmentally, we may get closer to specifying etiological pathways or a set of necessary precursors for the development of symptoms associated with various mental disorders. Regional differences in structural brain development or patterns of brain activity may serve as an endophenotype (Gottesman & Gould, 2003), providing an alternate means of identifying those individuals who are more likely to respond positively to various treatments, those whose symptoms may be more refractory to intervention, or even those at risk for developing disorder. Moreover, the use of neuroimaging methods may allow for more precise subclassification of behavioral symptoms and syndromes. In addition, examination of the brain systems associated with specific cognitive, emotional, and social behaviors across development may aid in identifying key symptoms that are common among individuals with different behavioral or emotional profiles. Neuroimaging may indicate previously unstudied overlap between seemingly disparate symptoms.

Further investigating the activity of brain systems associated with disrupted behavior or emotion dysregulation can aid in understanding typical individual variability in brain function and organization, and in comprehending processes of risk and resilience. Neuroimaging also provides a unique method for examining the impact of various environmental and experiential factors on brain development and biological instantiations of behavior. In the future, emphasis should be placed on longitudinal data, especially on functional neuroimaging. In order to truly comprehend the emergence of behavioral and affective symptoms, it is critical for the field to conduct prospective longitudinal investigations. The prohibitive financial cost of neuroimaging research and the vast amount of human effort expended on coding and data analysis have thus far rendered the accumulation of prospective data to be a challenging goal.

Clearly, however, brain imaging technology cannot solve the complex issues inherent to the relation between typical and atypical development alone. Sound theory, in conjunction with experimental paradigms that permit the investigation of cognitive, emotional, and social behavior, has enabled research on neuroimaging to enhance the understanding of the relation between typical and atypical development across multiple levels of analysis. Since individual levels of analysis constrain other levels, researchers conducting their work at each level will need to develop theories that are consistent across all levels. Interdisciplinary research will become increasingly prevalent, necessitating that the challenge of establishing communication between scientists from different fields be confronted and solved (Gunnar & Cicchetti, in press; Pellmar & Eisenberg, 2000). This will enable the field of developmental psychopathology to make optimal use of the advances in technology that have occurred. If disciplines function in isolation, then it is likely that the theories they promulgate will ultimately prove to be incorrect because existing vital information from other disciplines will either have been unknown or have been ignored. Just as has been witnessed in the development of the discipline of systems neuroscience (Kandel & Squire, 2000), it is essential that we strive to develop an integrative framework that incorporates all levels of analysis about complex systems in typical development, as well as the development of psychopathology, and resilience. The sophisticated and comprehensive portrayals of adaptation, maladaptation, and resilience that ensue will serve not only to advance scientific understanding, but also to inform efforts to prevent, ameliorate, and, ultimately, develop cures for, mental disorders (Insel & Scolnick, 2006).

Acknowledgments

Our work on this Special Issue was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (NS34458 and MH79513, (Thomas); MH54643 and MH067792-01, (Cicchetti) and the Spunk Fund, Inc. (Cicchetti).

References

  1. Amso D, Casey BJ. Beyond what develops when: Neuroimaging may inform how cognition changes with development. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2006;15:24–29. [Google Scholar]
  2. Black J, Jones TA, Nelson CA, Greenough WT. Neuronal plasticity and the developing brain. In: Alessi NE, Coyle JT, Harrison SI, Eth S, editors. Handbook of child and adolescent psychiatry. New York: Wiley; 1998. pp. 31–53. [Google Scholar]
  3. Cacioppo JT, Amaral DG, Blanchard JJ, Cameron JL, Carter CS, Crews D, Fiske S, Heatherton T, Johnson MK, Kozak MJ, Levenson RW, Lord C, Miller EK, Ochsner K, Raichle ME, Shea MT, Taylor SE, Young LJ, Quinn KJ. Social neuroscience: Progress and implications for mental health. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2007;2:99–123. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00032.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Casey BJ, Giedd JN, Thomas KM. Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development. Biological Psychology. 2000;54:241–257. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(00)00058-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Casey BJ, Tottenham N, Liston C, Durston S. Imaging the developing brain: What have we learned about cognitive development? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9:104–110. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Cicchetti D. Developmental psychopathology: Reactions, reflections, projections. Developmental Review. 1993;13:471–502. [Google Scholar]
  7. Cicchetti D. How a child builds a brain: Insights from normality and psychopathology. In: Hartup W, Weinberg R, editors. Minnesota symposia on child psychology: Child psychology in retrospect and prospect. Vol. 32. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2002. pp. 23–71. [Google Scholar]
  8. Cicchetti D, Cannon TD. Neurodevelopmental processes in the ontogenesis and epigenesis of psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11:375–393. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499002114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cicchetti D, Curtis WJ, editors. A multi-level approach to resilience [Special Issue] Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19(3):627–955. doi: 10.1017/s0954579407000314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cicchetti D, Gunnar MR. Integrating biological processes into the design and evaluation of preventive interventions. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20 doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cicchetti D, Posner MI. Cognitive and affective neuroscience and developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17(3):569–575. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cicchetti D, Tucker D. Development and self-regulatory structures of the mind. Development and Psychopathology. 1994;6:533–549. [Google Scholar]
  13. Courchesne E, Chisum H, Townsend J. Neural activity-dependent brain charges in development: Implications for psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 1994;6:697–422. [Google Scholar]
  14. Durston S, Davidson MC, Tottenham N, Galvan A, Spicer J, Fossella JA, et al. A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development. Developmental Science. 2006;9:1–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00454.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Eisenberg L. The social construction of the human brain. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1995;152:1563–1575. doi: 10.1176/ajp.152.11.1563b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fishbein H. Evolution, development, and children's learning. Pacific Palisades, CA: Goodyear Publishing Company; 1976. [Google Scholar]
  17. Giedd JN, Shaw P, Wallace G, Gogtay N, Lenroot RK. Anatomic brain imaging studies of normal and abnormal brain development in children and adolescents. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen D, editors. Developmental psychopathology (2nd ed.), Vol. 2: Developmental neuroscience. New York: Wiley; 2006. pp. 127–196. [Google Scholar]
  18. Goldman-Rakic PS. Development of cortical circuitry and cognitive function. Child Development. 1987;58:601–622. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:636–645. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gottlieb G. Developmental-behavioral initiation of evolutionary change. Psychological Review. 2002;109:211–218. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.109.2.211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gottlieb G, Wahlsten D, Lickliter R. The significance of biology for human development: A developmental psychobiological systems view. In: Damon W, Lerner R, editors. Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development. New York: Wiley; 1998. pp. 233–273. (Series Ed.) (Vol. Ed.) [Google Scholar]
  22. Gottlieb G, Willoughby MT. Probabilistic epigenesis of psychopathology. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen D, editors. Developmental Psychopathology (2nd ed.) Vol. 1. New York: Wiley; 2006. pp. 673–700. [Google Scholar]
  23. Greenough W, Black J, Wallace C. Experience and brain development. Child Development. 1987;58:539–559. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gunnar MR, Cicchetti D. Meeting the Challenge of Translational Research in Child Psychology: Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology (Vol. 35) New York: Wiley; Meeting the challenge of translational research in child psychology. (in press). [Google Scholar]
  25. Hunt RH, Thomas KM. MRI methods in developmental science : A primer. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20 doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Insel TR, Scolnick EM. Cure therapeutics and strategic prevention: Raising the bar for mental health research. Molecular Psychiatry. 2006;11:11–17. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Johnson MH. The neural basis of cognitive development. In: Kuhn D, Siegler R, editors. Handbook of child psychology: Cognition, perception, and language. Vol. 2. New York: Wiley; 1998. pp. 1–49. [Google Scholar]
  28. Johnson MH, Halit H, Grice SJ, Karmiloff-Smith A. Neuroimaging of typical and atypical development: A perspective from multiple levels of analysis. Development and Psychopathology. 2002;14:521–536. doi: 10.1017/s0954579402003073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kandel ER. Biology and the future of psychoanalysis: A new intellectual framework for psychiatry revisited. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156:505–524. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.4.505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kandel ER, Squire L. Neuroscience: Breaking down scientific barriers to the study of brain and mind. Science. 2000;290:1113–1120. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5494.1113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Masten AS. Competence, resilience and development in adolescence: Clues for prevention science. In: Romer D, Walker EF, editors. Adolescent psychopathology and the developing brain: Integrating brain and prevention science. New York: Oxford University Press; 2007. pp. 31–52. [Google Scholar]
  32. Nelson CA, Bloom FE. Child development and neuroscience. Child Development. 1997;68:970–987. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01974.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Nelson CA, Thomas KM, de Haan M. Neuroscience and cognitive development: Experience and the developing brain. Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  34. Nowakowski RS. Basic concepts of CNS development. Child Development. 1987;58:568–595. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Nowakowski RS, Hayes NL. CNS development: An overview. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11:395–418. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499002126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Pellmar TC, Eisenberg L, editors. Bridging disciplines in the brain, behavioral, and clinical sciences. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2000. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Rakic P. Development of the cerebral cortex in human and non-human primates. In: Lewis M, editor. Child and adolescent Psychiatry: A comprehensive textbook. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins; 1996. pp. 9–30. [Google Scholar]
  38. Sameroff AJ. Developmental systems: Contexts and evolution. In: Mussen P, editor. Handbook of Child Psychology. Vol. 1. New York: Wiley; 1983. pp. 237–294. [Google Scholar]
  39. Segalowitz SJ. Developmental psychology and brain development: A historical perspective. In: Dawson G, Fischer KW, editors. Human behavior and the developing brain. New York: Guilford; 1994. pp. 67–92. [Google Scholar]
  40. Sroufe LA. Considering normal and abnormal together: The essence of developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 1990;2:335–347. [Google Scholar]
  41. Thomas KM. Assessing brain development using neurophysiologic and behavioral measures. Journal of Pediatrics. 2003;143:S46–S53. doi: 10.1067/s0022-3476(03)00401-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Thompson RA, Nelson CA. Developmental science and the media: Early brain development. American Psychologist. 2001;56(1):5–15. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.56.1.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Waddington CH. The strategy of genes. London: Allen & Unwin; 1957. [Google Scholar]
  44. Weiss PA. Deformities as cues to understanding development of form. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine. 1961;4:133–151. doi: 10.1353/pbm.1961.0035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Weiss PA. Principles of development. New York: Hafner; 1969. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES