Abstract
Introduction
Neuroconstructivist theories of development highlight the potential effect one developmental domain may have on constraining or facilitating another. Empirical validation of this theory requires further testing in autism spectrum disorders (ASD), and may illuminate the complex interplay of developmental trajectories, particularly in the relationship between predictor and outcome variables. In ASD, language ability is an early predictor of important functional outcomes such as communication and socialization. We aimed to investigate whether theory of mind (ToM) mediates the relation between language ability and adaptive functioning in more cognitively able children with ASD (IQ > 70).
Methods
Thirty-nine children were followed prospectively every two years from 4–6 years to 12–14 years. Their language and theory of mind abilities and adaptive functioning were tested using the Test of Language Development-2 (the independent variable, at age 6–8 years), the “Eyes Test” (a measure of ToM, the mediator, at age 10–12) and the Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales (the outcome variable, at age 12–14).
Results
ToM mediated an association between language and adaptive functioning in the communication domain, but not in the social domain.
Conclusion
These results challenge the usefulness of ToM as a unifying theory for ASD deficits and highlight the potential usefulness of a neuroconstructivist framework for prospective studies.
Keywords: autism, Asperger Syndrome, language, theory of mind, mediation
Résumé
Introduction
Les théories neuroconstructivistes du développement soulignent l’effet potentiel de restriction ou de facilitation qu’un domaine développemental peut exercer sur un autre domaine. La confirmation empirique de cette théorie exige plus de vérifications dans les troubles du spectre de l’autisme (TSA), et peut faire la lumière sur l’interaction complexe des trajectoires développementales, en particulier sur la relation entre les variables de prédiction et celles des résultats. Dans le TSA, l’aptitude linguistique est un prédicteur précoce d’importants résultats fonctionnels comme la communication et la socialisation. Nous visions à rechercher si la théorie de l’esprit (TdE) aide la relation entre l’aptitude linguistique et le fonctionnement adaptatif chez les enfants cognitivement aptes souffrant d’un TSA (IQ > 70).
Méthodes
Trente-neuf enfants ont été suivis prospectivement tous les deux ans, de 4 à 6 ans jusqu’à 12 à 14 ans. Leurs aptitudes de langage et à la théorie de l’esprit ainsi que leur fonctionnement adaptatif ont été vérifiés à l’aide du test du développement du langage-2 (la variable indépendante, à l’âge de 6 à 8 ans), du « Eyes Test » (une mesure de la TdE, la médiatrice, à l’âge de 10 à 12 ans) et de l’Échelle de comportement adaptatif Vineland (la variable de résultat, à l’âge de 12 à 14 ans).
Résultats
La TdE facilitait une association entre le langage et le fonctionnement adaptatif dans le domaine de la communication, mais pas dans le domaine social.
Conclusion
Ces résultats remettent en question l’utilité de la TdE en tant que théorie unificatrice des déficits du TSA, et font valoir l’utilité potentielle d’un cadre neuroconstructiviste pour les études prospectives.
Keywords: autisme, syndrome d’Asperger, langage, théorie de l’esprit, médiation
Functional outcomes in autism spectrum disorders (ASD) differ significantly across individuals over time; many continue to require significant support in adulthood, whereas others progress toward independent living. Research into early predictors of outcomes has begun to explain this heterogeneity. For example, early language abilities are important indicators of later adaptive functioning - defined as practical abilities in social and communication skills and self-care (Bennett et al., 2008; Rutter, Greenfeld, & Lockyer, 1967; Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984; Szatmari, Bryson, Boyle, Streiner, & Duku, 2003; Szatmari et al. 2000; Venter, Lord, & Schopler, 1992). Identifying early predictors is an important task in longitudinal research, but “how” or “why” predictors such as early language ability are associated with variation in outcomes such as adaptive functioning is an important issue.
From a theoretical perspective, constraints or growth in one developmental domain may “spill over” to influence change across other important domains. For example, the additional burden of language impairment in young children with ASD may lead to further lags in their social understanding - there is evidence that verbal children with ASD rely more upon language to understand social tasks than do typically developing children (Fisher, Happé, & Dunn, 2005). This may, in turn, constrain their social and communication skills later on. Conversely, one domain may facilitate another by opening up new opportunities for growth. For example, better language skills may facilitate more opportunities for social interaction, which may in turn lead to better social skills.
“Neuroconstructivism” (Karmiloff-Smith, 1998; Sirois et al., 2008), a particularly promising theoretical framework that can capture this dynamic interplay between developmental domains, has gained increasing recognition in cognitive and developmental neuroscience research. The neuroconstructivist approach focuses on the developmental process itself as “key to understanding developmental disabilities” (Karmiloff-Smith, 1998). Therefore particular attention is paid to early differences - genetic alterations, environmental exposures, brain trauma - that may constrain multiple interacting developmental trajectories over time (Karmiloff-Smith, 1998; Farran & Karmiloff-Smith, 2012). This approach would therefore enhance longitudinal research in ASD by providing a more complete and dynamic understanding of the developmental processes that occur in the causal chain between predictors and outcomes.
Well-designed prospective cohort studies also allow us to more thoughtfully address “how” predictors lead to particular outcomes. This requires an understanding of mediating factors and their underlying mechanisms. Mediation explains how such associations may “flow through” an intervening process in a presumably causal sequence; statistically, the predictor “X” affects the mediator “M”, which then affects the outcome “Y” (Baron & Kenny, 1986; Kraemer, Stice, Kazdin, Offord, & Kupfer, 2001; MacKinnon, 2008). Understanding mediating pathways is important both to understanding developmental processes and to designing process-oriented clinical treatment; for example, learning how more advanced early language leads to improved adaptive functioning helps to understand brain processes in ASD and to develop more effective interventions targeted at neurocognitive mediating mechanisms rather than more “distant” predictors.
Theory of mind (ToM) - the ability to reason about the mental states of self and others - may be an important mediator in the association between early language abilities and later developmental outcomes in ASD. First, deficits in ToM have consistently distinguished children and adults with ASD from controls (e.g., Baron-Cohen, Leslie, & Frith, 1985; Baron-Cohen, Tager-Flusberg, & Cohen, 2000). Secondly, studies of typically developing children and individuals with ASD have shown ToM to be strongly associated with language abilities across different ages (Astington & Baird, 2005; see review by Milligan, Astington, & Dack, 2007). Some researchers have reported that, in fact, these two developmental abilities (language and ToM) may be more strongly related in people with ASD compared to typically developing children (Fisher et al., 2005; Tager-Flusberg & Joseph, 2005). Language skills may facilitate particularly important compensatory pathways given the social impairment associated with ASD. Children who demonstrate early language advantages will likely be exposed to a wider range of social situations and be better equipped to decode them than those who have poorer verbal abilities. This in turn would confer learning advantages in a range of ToM skills, including reading facial expressions of others, which would enhance the development of adaptive communication and social skills. ToM may therefore act as an important link mediating early language abilities to long-term outcomes in social and communication skills in ASD.
Several questions remain, however. First, we do not know how language and ToM are related over longer periods of time, and more specifically across developmental stages in individuals with ASD, for example from preschool to adolescent years. Furthermore, we do not know how ToM may relate to both early language and later functional outcomes. Understanding the ways in which children with ASD develop these cognitive abilities will help us to understand why individuals with ASD embark on different trajectories and also to design more effective interventions matched to specific trajectories.
Previous published work by our group found significant relationships between early language abilities and adolescent adaptive functioning in more cognitively able children with ASD (Bennett et al., 2008; Szatmari et al. 2003). Therefore, in this study, we aimed to determine whether ToM would mediate the association between language abilities in children assessed at ages 6–8 years old, and communication and social adaptive functioning skills assessed at age 12–14 years old, using data from a longitudinal inception cohort study of 39 children with ASD and nonverbal intelligence quotients (IQ) of greater than 68 standard score points (autistic disorder and Asperger Syndrome). We hypothesized that:
Language abilities, measured at age 6–8 years via the Test of Language Development-2 (TOLD-2), would be significantly associated with both ToM measured at age 10–12 years (using a modified version of the “Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test”) and later communication and social functioning (age 12–14) indexed on the Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales (VABS); and, that
ToM would mediate the relationship between early language and later adaptive functioning in communication and socialization.
Methods
For a detailed description of the participant sample and measures used in this study, as well as a graphic description of the mediation approach outlined in the analyses section, please see the supplementary online materials (SOM).
Analyses
The aim of these analyses was to determine whether ToM, as measured by the “Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test” at T2, mediated, or might account for, an association between the predictor variable - language abilities measured at Time 1 - and the two outcome variables, socialization and communication adaptive behaviours at T3. The effects of language and ToM on each adaptive functioning outcome variable were tested in two separate mediation models for assessing and comparing indirect effects, as outlined in Preacher and Hayes (2008) and depicted in Supplementary Figure 1. The indirect effects approach to testing simple mediation (i.e. one proposed mediating variable) apportions an independent variable X’s hypothesized causal effect into direct effects on the dependent variable Y (labeled path c) and indirect effects of X on Y through the proposed mediator M. Each path is quantified by an unstandardized regression coefficient. The effect of X on M is represented by path a, and path b represents the effect of M on Y while accounting for the effect of X. The indirect effect is then quantified by multiplying a × b. Bootstrapping procedures were used to test indirect effects, standard errors and confidence intervals. The estimate of indirect effect would be considered to be statistically significant if 95% confidence intervals did not include zero, which would support the hypothesis of mediation. Model 1 tested the direct and indirect effects of T1 language on T3 adaptive communication and Model 2 tested the direct and indirect effects of T1 language on T3 adaptive socialization. Nonverbal IQ was included as a covariate of all paths in both models. Calculations were performed using the INDIRECT plug-in by A. F. Hayes in SPSS 20.0 (http://www.afhayes.com/spss-sas-and-mplusmacros-and-code.html).
Results
Within the cohort of 39 children, one individual enrolled but was unavailable for Time 1 data collection (family moved), however returned at T2. Three children were lost to follow-up at T3 (moved or declined follow-up). Boys comprised 90% of the available sample of 35 participants (n=31). Table 1 provides further descriptive statistics. Tests of normality indicate that the variable distributions were within normal limits.
Table 1.
Descriptive statistics of sample (n = 35)
Mean (SD) | Minimum | Maximum | |
---|---|---|---|
Age T1 (mo.) | 89.00 (11.56) | 73.00 | 112.00 |
Age T2 (mo.) | 182.30 (14.27) | 158.00 | 211.00 |
Age T3 (mo.) | 208.54 (17.76) | 175.00 | 254.00 |
TOLD-2 T1 | 5.21 (3.34) | 1.50 | 13.50 |
Leiter NV IQ T1 ** | 86.48 (17.34) | 36.00 | 127.00 |
“Eyes” Test T2 † | 12.03 (3.45) | 4.00 | 19.00 |
VABS COMM T3 †† | 57.06 (21.21) | 19.00 | 89.00 |
VABS SOC T3 ‡ | 49.78 (18.86) | 19.00 | 91.00 |
Test of Language Development-2, standard score points
Leiter nonverbal intelligence quotient score, standard score points
“Seeing the Mind in the Eyes Test”, modified for current study, maximum attainable score = 30.
Vineland Adaptive Communication Score, standard score points
Vineland Adaptive Socialization Score, standard score points
Mediation analyses
Adaptive Communication Outcome
Figure 1 depicts the mediation model, partitioned into its component regression associations. The overall model accounted for 70% of the variance explained in the VABS Communication Subscale at Time 3 (R2=0.70, p<0.01). As illustrated by path a1, 41% of the variance in the mediator variable - T2 ToM as measured by the Eyes Test - was predicted by individual language abilities at T1 (B=0.41(0.13), β = 0.65, p < 0.001). In Path b1, T2 ToM was also significantly associated with T3 VABS Communication, controlling for language at T1 (B=2.09(1.00), p=0.05). There was a significant indirect effect of T1 language on T3 adaptive communication through T2 ToM, as depicted by the product term of the regression coefficients for paths a1 and b1 (a1b1=0.87(0.54), CI=0.01,2.17). Language competency at T1 also demonstrated a significant direct association with T3 Adaptive Communication (Path c1: B=3.21(0.86), p<0.001). The analyses controlled for nonverbal IQ, which demonstrated a non-significant effect on the outcome variable (B=0.11(0.17), p=0.49). In summary, in more cognitively able youth with ASD, structural language as measured by the TOLD-2 at age 6–8 was directly associated with adolescent adaptive communication as measured by the Vineland at age 12–14 years and indirectly through the effects of ToM as measured using the Eyes Test.
Figure 1.
Mediation Model 1
β=regression coefficient (with standard error)
Adaptive socialization outcome
In contrast, language ability was not a statistically significant predictor of variance in the Vineland Socialization domain scores at Time 3, through either a direct path (path c2: B=1.57(1.14), p=0.18) or indirect path through ToM (a2b2=0.043(0.56); CI= −1.18,1.13), when controlling for nonverbal IQ (see Figure 2). Overall the entire model predicted a much smaller amount of variance in adaptive socialization (R2=0.29, p=0.01) compared to the results for adaptive communication. ToM as measured using the Eyes test at T2 was not significantly associated with adaptive social skills as measured by the Vineland.
Figure 2.
Mediation model 2
β=regression coefficient (with standard error)
Discussion
Results of these analyses indicate that ToM mediated an association between early structural language and later adaptive communications skills as measured in early adolescence (12–14 years). Neither language ability nor ToM uniquely predicted a significant amount of variance in later adaptive socialization however. Each of these results from the mediation analysis will now be discussed, in turn.
Language and the development of ToM
Strong associations were found between language ability as measured by the TOLD-2 at age 6–8 and the Eyes Task performed approximately four years later in higher-IQ individuals with ASD. The results of the current study reiterate earlier findings of longitudinal associations between language ability and later ToM skills as indexed by narrative or false-belief tasks (Pellicano, 2010; Tager-Flusberg & Joseph, 2005). However they diverge from results obtained by Kaland et al. (Kaland, Callesen, Moller-Nielsen, Moretensen, & Smith, 2008), who failed to find cross-sectional associations between language ability and the Eyes Test in individuals with Asperger Syndrome.
Our results may demonstrate the developmental nature of associations between language and ToM. For example, the Eyes test has been described as an index of “social-perceptual” ToM skills, which involve more immediate decoding of interpersonal information (e.g. deciphering facial expressions) (Tager-Flusberg & Sullivan, 2000). False-belief and similar tasks, meanwhile, may involve more developmentally advanced “social-cognitive” ToM abilities and sophisticated linguistic skills (Tager-Flusberg & Sullivan, 2000). However, the strength of the associations between different components or “building blocks” of ToM and language may depend on the developmental stage of the individual with ASD. Early language advantages in younger children with ASD may enable them to engage more fully in social situations, leading over time to advantages in socio-perceptual ToM. For older verbal individuals with ASD, superior language ability may instead be a stronger requirement for understanding and reasoning through more complex social-cognitive ToM tasks (Tager-Flusberg, 2005).
Adaptive Communication Outcome
ToM mediated the association between language ability at age 6–8 (T1) and adaptive communication measured six years later. This suggests that structural language (grammar and vocabulary), ToM and later adaptive communication are related over the course of development in children with ASD. These longitudinal mediation mechanisms provide empirical support for developmental processes such as bootstrapping, in which early abilities such as language aptitude scaffold later-emerging skills such as ToM, which may then in turn facilitate growth in other domains such as adaptive functioning. As in typically developing children, linguistic understanding and awareness of others’ emotions and intentions appear to build on each other over time, leading to more successful adaptive communication functioning in more cognitively able children and adolescents with ASD.
Adaptive Socialization Outcome
Together, language, non-verbal IQ and ToM predicted a relatively small but significant amount of variance in adaptive functioning on the Vineland socialization domain six years later. However, neither language at age 6–8 nor ToM as indexed at age 10–12 were uniquely predictive of variance in adaptive socialization in early adolescence, once accounting for IQ. This diverges from the results of previous studies (Hale & Tager-Flusberg, 2005; Tager-Flusberg, 2003). The present results are consistent with those of certain earlier studies that failed to find an association between false-belief ToM tasks and the Vineland Socialization domain (Frith, Happé, & Siddons, 1994; Fombonne, Siddons, Achard, Frith, & Happé, 1994). They diverge, however, from those of Tager-Flusberg (2003), who found that ToM was significantly associated with the VABS Socialization scale (measured in the same year) in 69 verbal autistic children aged 4–14. This may reflect a difference in sample size, follow-up time or measurement issues.
This lack of association between ToM and adaptive social functioning reflects important methodological and conceptual issues with respect to measuring social development in individuals with ASD. First, while the communication domain of the Vineland generally depicts individual competencies (e.g. “Reads books”, Writes advanced letters”), the socialization domain, particularly approaching adolescence, describes functioning in a more ecologically valid way (e.g. “Going on double dates”; “Playing on school teams”). Social adaptive functioning may therefore measure the inclusiveness of the environment of an individual with ASD as much as his or her own individual social competencies. Second, the ToM skills measured by the Eyes test may be necessary but insufficient in navigating the complex social world of adolescents (Tager-Flusberg, 2003; Tager-Flusberg & Sullivan, 2000); higher scores on the Vineland may also reflect the development of a wide range of cognitive and other abilities (Frith et al., 1994; Fombonne, Siddons, Achard, Frith & Happé 1994). For example, the battery of false-belief and narrative-based ToM measures in Tager-Flusberg’s (2005) study may have tapped more advanced ToM abilities as well as competencies such as executive function and language. From a neuroconstructivist perspective, the successful acquisition of much earlier building blocks of ToM – such as social attunement and joint attention in infancy and toddlerhood – or other early-emerging domain-general skills such as executive function may act as more primary constraints on developmental pathways (Dawson, Meltzoff, Osterling, Rinaldi, & Brown, 1998; Dawson et al., 2004; Mundy, Sigman, & Kasari, 1990; Pellicano, 2010) than socioperceptual ToM skills in early adolescence. Longitudinal studies that follow younger children into adolescence and that chart interacting developmental pathways such as language, social cognition (including ToM), IQ and executive functioning will be key to clarifying such developmental questions.
Limitations
The mediation model proposed in this study is preliminary and correlational, with several important limitations. First, power issues due to the relatively small sample size may have affected the results, specifically the lack of significant associations with socialization functioning. A larger sample size would also have enabled the use of more sophisticated mediation analyses using structural equation models. Multiple methods and informants and a range of developmentally sensitive ToM tests (ideally including tests requiring minimal language ability) would further strengthen the findings. Furthermore, because the “Eyes” test was first published after the initiation of the study, it was only available at T2, so we were unable to determine the association between language at age T1 and ToM at T2 controlling for baseline ToM. Ideally, all tests (including outcome measures) should be measured at all time points. Finally, more complex models would enable one to test the growth of multiple interacting competencies, such as social skills, IQ, language, executive function and ToM - as well as contextual factors - in children with ASD.
Controlled intervention trials that include mediating and moderating variables can more definitively clarify how and under what circumstances interventions may shape developmental trajectories in children with ASDs. The present data support the idea that among children with ASD and higher cognitive functioning, interventions aimed at improving ToM in late childhood would have an impact on communication but not necessarily on socialization. However, barriers in translating gains made in ToM skills to real-world outcomes may exist. Multiple intervention studies have shown that learning conceptual ToM skills frequently fails to translate to improvements in daily social interactions among individuals with ASD (Begeer et al., 2011; Ozonoff, & Miller, 1995; Williams, Gray, & Tonge, 2012). Promising, albeit preliminary, studies suggest that social cognition-based interventions may overcome such barriers if they are developmentally sensitive (e.g. assessing a child’s readiness to learn; focusing on emerging social cognition in younger children) and ecologically valid (incorporating naturalistic settings; regular coaching in spontaneous situations; engaging parents, teachers, peers as facilitators or co-participants) (Bauminger, 2007; Kasari, Gulsrud, Wong, Kwon, & Locke, 2010). ToM may be included as one part of a battery of interventions to improve social communication along multiple dimensions among children with ASD. However we need to target multiple skills during developmentally sensitive periods in order to address multiple outcomes.
SUPPLEMENTARY ONLINE MATERIALS
http://www.cacap-acpea.org/uploads/documents//Theory_of_Mind_Bennett_2013_02.pdf
Online materials include: further information on participants; figure 1 and table 1 (Bivariate correrlations of study variables); measures; time measures.
Acknowledgements / Conflicts of Interest
This work was supported by grants from the Vellum Foundation, the Ontario Mental Health Foundation and Health and Welfare Canada. Dr. Szatmari was supported by a personnel award from the Ontario Mental Health Foundation. Dr. Bennett was supported by a training award from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research.
References
- Astington JW, Baird JA. Introduction: Why Language Matters. In: Astington JW, Baird JA, editors. Why Language Matters for Theory of Mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2005. pp. 3–25. [Google Scholar]
- Baron RM, Kenny DA. The Mediator-Moderator Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Research: Conceptual, Strategic, and Statistical Considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51(6):1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition. 1985;21(1):37–46. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen D. Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience. 2nd edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2000. pp. 306–333. [Google Scholar]
- Bauminger N. Brief report: Individual social-multi-model intervention for HFASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37:1593–1604. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0245-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Begeer S, Gevers C, Clifford P, Verhoeve M, Kat K, Hoddenbach E, Boer F. Theory of mind training in children with autism: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2011;41:997–1006. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1121-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett T, Szatmari P, Bryson S, Volden J, Zwaigenbaum L, Vaccarella L, Boyle M. Differentiating autism and Asperger syndrome on the basis of language delay or impairment. Journal of Autism Developmental Disorders. 2008;38(4):616–625. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0428-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dawson G, Meltzoff AN, Osterling J, Rinaldi J, Brown E. Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1998;28(6):479–485. doi: 10.1023/a:1026043926488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dawson G, Toth K, Abbott R, Osterling J, Munson J, Estes A, Liaw J. Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology. 2004;40(2):271–283. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farran EK, Karmiloff-Smith A. Williams syndrome: A model for the neuroconstructivist approach. In: Farran EK, Karmiloff-Smith A, editors. Neurodevelopmental Disorders Across the Lifespan: A Neuroconstructivist Approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2012. pp. 1–13. [Google Scholar]
- Fisher N, Happé F, Dunn J. The relationship between vocabulary, grammar, and false belief task performance in children with autistic spectrum disorders and children with moderate learning difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2005;46(4):409–419. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00371.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fombonne E, Siddons F, Achard S, Frith U, Happé Adaptive behaviour and theory of mind in autism. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1994;3(3):176–186. doi: 10.1007/BF02720324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frith U, Happé F, Siddons F. Autism and theory of mind in everyday life. Social Development. 1994;3(2):108–124. [Google Scholar]
- Hale CM, Tager-Flusberg H. Social communication in children with autism: The relationship between theory of mind and discourse development. Autism. 2005;9(2):157–178. doi: 10.1177/1362361305051395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaland N, Callesen K, Moller-Nielsen A, Moretensen EL, Smith L. Performance of children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism on advanced ToM tasks. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2008;38(6):1112–1123. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0496-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karmiloff-Smith A. Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 1998;2(10):389–398. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(98)01230-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kasari C, Gulsrud AC, Wong C, Kwon S, Locke J. Randomized controlled caregiver mediated joint engagement intervention for toddlers with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2010;40:1045–1056. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0955-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraemer HC, Stice E, Kazdin A, Offord D, Kupfer D. How do risk factors work together? Mediators, moderators, and independent, overlapping, and proxy risk factors. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;158(6):848–856. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.6.848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacKinnon DP. Introduction to Statistical Mediation Analysis. New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2008. pp. 19–20. [Google Scholar]
- Milligan K, Astington JW, Dack LA. Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development. 2007;78(2):622–646. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mundy P, Sigman M, Kasari C. A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1990;20(1):115–128. doi: 10.1007/BF02206861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ozonoff S, Miller J. Teaching theory of mind: A new approach to social skills training for individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1995;25:415–433. doi: 10.1007/BF02179376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellicano E. Individual differences in executive function and central coherence predict developmental changes in theory of mind in autism. Developmental Psychology. 2010;46(2):530–544. doi: 10.1037/a0018287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods. 2008;40(3):879–891. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.3.879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M, Greenfeld D, Lockyer L. A five to fifteen year follow-up study of infantile psychosis. II. Social and behavioural outcome. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1967;113(504):1183–1199. doi: 10.1192/bjp.113.504.1183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sirois S, Spratling M, Thomas MS, Westermann G, Mareschal D, Johnson MH. Precis of neuroconstructivism: How the brain constructs cognition. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2008;31(3):321–331. 331–356. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0800407X. discussion. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sparrow S, Balla D, Cicchetti D. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (Survey Form) Circle Pines, Minn: American Guidance Service; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Szatmari P, Bryson SE, Boyle MH, Streiner DL, Duku E. Predictors of outcome among high functioning children with autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2003;44(4):520–528. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szatmari P, Bryson SE, Streiner DL, Wilson F, Archer L, Ryerse C. Two-year outcome of preschool children with autism or Asperger’s syndrome. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;157(12):1980–1987. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.12.1980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tager-Flusberg H. Exploring the relationship between theory of mind and social-communicative functioning in children with autism. In: Repacholi B, Slaughter V, editors. Individual Differences in Theory of Mind: Implications for Typical and Atypical Development. New York, NY: Psychology Press; 2003. pp. 208–222. [Google Scholar]
- Tager-Flusberg H, Joseph RM. How Language Facilitates the Acquisition of False-Belief Understanding in Children With Autism. In: Astington JW, Baird JA, editors. Why Language Matters for Theory of Mind. New York, NY: Oxford Press; 2005. pp. 298–318. [Google Scholar]
- Tager-Flusberg H, Sullivan K. A componential view of theory of mind: Evidence from Williams Syndrome. Cognition. 2000;76:59–89. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00069-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Venter A, Lord C, Schopler E. A follow-up study of high-functioning autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1992;33(3):489–507. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00887.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams BT, Gray KM, Tonge BJ. Teaching emotion-recognition skills to young children with autism: A randomized controlled trial of an emotion training programme. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2012;53(12):1268–1276. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02593.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
http://www.cacap-acpea.org/uploads/documents//Theory_of_Mind_Bennett_2013_02.pdf
Online materials include: further information on participants; figure 1 and table 1 (Bivariate correrlations of study variables); measures; time measures.