Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012 Sep 24;37(6):1150–1156. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.09.002

Update: Studies of prepulse inhibition of startle, with particular relevance to the pathophysiology or treatment of Tourette Syndrome

Neal R Swerdlow 1,*
PMCID: PMC3566359  NIHMSID: NIHMS414741  PMID: 23017868

Abstract

Prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex (PPI) is an operational measure of sensorimotor gating, in which the motor response to an abrupt, intense stimulus is inhibited by a weak lead stimulus. PPI is reduced in several brain disorders, including Tourette Syndrome (TS); it is regulated by forebrain circuitry, including portions of the basal ganglia implicated in the pathophysiology of TS, and is also heritable and under strong genetic control. PPI has been the focus of numerous translational models, because it is expressed by most mammalian species, with remarkable conservation of response characteristics and underlying neural circuitry between rodents and primates. Several of these models have recently explored causative factors in TS – from genes to specific basal ganglia perturbations – as well as potential TS therapeutics, including novel pharmacological and neurosurgical interventions. With the focus on Comprehensive Behavioral Interventions for Tics (CBIT) in the evolving treatment model for TS, future studies might apply PPI as a predictive measure for CBIT response, or for identifying medications that might augment CBIT efficacy. In the end, a measure based on a simple pontine-based reflex will have limitations in its ability to explicate any complex behavioral phenotype.

Keywords: Basal ganglia, dopamine, prepulse inhibition, sensorimotor gating, startle reflex, tourette Syndrome

1. Introduction

The startle reflex is a constellation of responses to sudden, relatively intense stimuli. In humans, the blink reflex component of startle is measured using electromyography of orbicularis oculi; in laboratory animals, whole-body startle is quantified by assessing the downward force resulting from the contraction of the skeletal muscles. Prepulse inhibition (PPI) occurs when a weak prestimulus 30–500 msec prior to the startling stimulus inhibits the startle response; this inhibition is an operational measure of sensorimotor gating (Graham, 1975). While the inhibitory effect of the prepulse on the startle reflex is exerted in the pons, studies have described the limbic forebrain circuitry and descending pontine projections that regulate the inhibitory “tone” within the pons and determine the degree to which the prepulse inhibits the subsequent motor response (cf. Swerdlow et al., 2001a, 2008). PPI thus appears to reflect the activation of “hardwired”, centrally mediated behavioral inhibitory processes that are regulated by forebrain neural circuitry.

PPI is a useful experimental measure for understanding brain mechanisms for a number of reasons. It is tested in an automated apparatus, under tight stimulus control, and stimulus parameters can be easily modified by the experimenter to elicit optimal response characteristics for studying a number of different aspects of this measure. Because PPI is a form of startle plasticity, it is measured using a “fight-or-flight” behavior that is simple, robust, and exhibited across all mammalian species tested to date. Of relevance to the present discussion, PPI is easily studied in animal models, including mice (Carter et al., 1999; Francis et al., 2003; Frankland et al. 2004), rats (Swerdlow et al., 2001a), guinea pigs (Vaillancourt & Boksa, 2000), pigs (Lind et al., 2004), and infrahuman primates (Linn et al., 2003), using stimulus parameters and equipment for stimulus delivery and response acquisition that are similar or identical to what are used in humans. While there appear to be differences in the neurochemical regulation of PPI across species (cf. Swerdlow et al. 2008), the basic parametric properties of PPI exhibit striking similarities from rodents to humans (e.g. Swerdlow et al. 1994), and PPI is under significant genetic control in both rodents (Francis et al. 2003) and humans (Greenwood et al. 2007).

This review focuses specifically on the results of studies of PPI in Tourette Syndrome, related clinical conditions, and relevant animal models. Broader reviews of PPI have appeared in this journal (e.g. Li et al. 2009), and more comprehensive analyses of our current understanding of TS pathophysiology can be found in accompanying articles in this special issue.

2. PPI and Tourette Syndrome

Despite its advantages as a laboratory measure of simple brain processes, PPI would likely be a scientific footnote were it not for the fact that it is reduced in a number of different brain disorders. Compared with matched controls, PPI is deficient in patients with schizophrenia (e.g., Braff et al., 1978; Swerdlow et al. 2006b), Huntington’s Disease (Swerdlow et al., 1995; Valls-Sole et al., 2004), Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD) (Swerdlow et al., 1993; Hoenig et al. 2005; Ahmari et al. 2012), nocturnal enuresis (Ornitz et al., 1992), Asperger’s Syndrome (McAlonan et al. 2002), 22q11 Syndrome (Sobin et al. 2005), Kleinfelter Syndrome (Van Rijn et al. 2011), Fragile-X Syndrome (Frankland et al. 2004) and blepharospasm (Gomez-Wong et al., 1998), as well as in patients with TS (Castellanos et al., 1996; Swerdlow et al. 2001b). PPI deficits in TS are one of several forms of reduced “paired pulse inhibition” exhibited in this disorder: TS individuals also exhibit deficits in measures of “blink recovery cycle” (also called “blink excitability”; Smith & Lees, 1989) and “intracortical inhibition” (Ziemann et al., 1997). In each of these measures, the response to a target stimulus (“pulse” or “S2”) is reduced by the presentation of a lead stimulus (“prepulse” or “S1”), and this reduction is blunted in TS patients.

Overlapping neural substrates?

PPI deficits in TS are of particular interest, because the forebrain substrates regulating PPI appear to overlap somewhat with those implicated in the pathophysiology of TS. Thus, two of the neural mechanisms that figure most prominently in current models of TS neuropathology are: 1) disturbances in basal forebrain dopamine (DA) function, and 2) abnormalities within intrinsic striatal circuitry (cf. McNaught & Mink 2011; Kalanithi et al. 2005; Kataoka et al. 2010; Steeves et al. 2010); similarly, basal forebrain DA potently regulates PPI in laboratory animals, and PPI is potently reduced by experimentally-induced damage to intrinsic striatal circuitry (cf. Swerdlow et al. 2008; Kodsi & Swerdlow 1994; Baldan Ramsey et al. 2011a; see below). It is important to acknowledge, however, that there is no clearly elucidated mechanistic link between the various different forms of basal ganglia disturbances noted in different samples of TS patients. In fact, it is conceivable that different forms of TS might reflect distinct, causally and mechanistically unrelated forms of basal ganglia pathology. It is well beyond the scope of this review, or of any report based on our current state of knowledge, to attempt to link at a causal or mechanistic level the myriad reported disturbances of basal ganglia function in TS – e.g. hyperdopaminergic innervation of the ventral striatum in TS (Albin et al. 2003), striatal volumetric reductions in this disorder (Peterson et al. 2003), anti-putamen antibodies (Singer et al. 1998), aberrant distribution of intrinsic basal ganglia cells (Kalanithi et al. 2005; Kataoka et al. 2010) and abnormal neural activation through basal ganglia structures (Wang et al. 2011). Of most relevance to the present review, however, is the loss of PPI detected in a range of models that reproduce aspects of many different forms of reported TS basal ganglia pathology.

Some evidence suggests that the ventral striatum may be one site of convergence between the neural disturbances in TS and the neural substrates of PPI: 1) neurochemical imaging studies in TS patients have suggested increased dopaminergic innervation of this region (Albin et al. 2003), and DA levels in this region are strong determinants of PPI levels (Swerdlow et al. 1992; Zhang et al. 2000); 2) preliminary volumetric and morphometric measures in TS sib-pairs detected significantly reduced ventral striatal volume and density with robust familial patterns (Frey et al. 2006), and experimentally reduced ventral striatal volume and density is associated with PPI deficits in rodents (Kodsi & Swerdlow 1994) as are “familial” strain-based differences in ventral striatal gene expression (Swerdlow et al. 2012); 3) recent reports (discussed below) suggest TS symptom response to deep brain stimulation (DBS) within either the ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens; e.g. Neuner et al. 2009) or within one ventral striatal output field in the internal globus pallidus (Welter et al. 2008), while DBS within an analogous region in rats prevents the PPI-disruptive effects of DA agonists (Posch et al. 2012; see below). Thus, evidence from pharmacologic, neurochemical and volumetric imaging studies, as well as emerging therapeutic reports, suggest an overlap between the many reported (but potentially mechanistically unrelated) brain disturbances in TS and the neurobiology of PPI. Of course, it is highly likely that – as the potentially long list of genetic and non-genetic etiologies of TS is identified – different forms of this syndrome will be differentially associated with disturbances in neuronal function and DA neurotransmission within striatal subregions.

Overlapping psychophysiological substrates?

An intriguing conceptual connection between TS and PPI relates to their psychophysiological underpinnings. PPI is an operational measure of sensorimotor gating, a process of central inhibition that has both automatic (preconscious) and volitional (attentionally sensitive) components. Over the past 20 years, it has become clear that the visible or audible tics in TS, in many instances, may be integrally connected to a form of failed automatic “gating” of sensory information, that is experienced as bothersome, unwanted internal sensory or psychic experiences (Bliss, 1980; Cohen & Leckman, 1992; Leckman et al., 1993; Leckman et al., 1994; Miguel et al., 1997; Miguel et al., 2000; Hollenbeck, 2003). Thus, it is conceivable that the pathological processes responsible for the loss of PPI in TS patients may also be related to, and even contribute to, the processes responsible for intrusive sensory phenomena in this disorder.

In truth, the association of a physiological abnormality such as reduced PPI, with a disorder likely to have multiple different genetic and non-genetic causes, might be based on a number of structurally different biological relationships e.g. mediating, moderating, independent or interactive. The simplest case, which is almost certainly not relevant to most (or even any) brain disorders, is that pathological genes code for neural circuitry that generates deficient PPI, and this PPI deficit causes the symptoms of the disorder. Patients simply do not present to the clinic complaining of “too little PPI”, or that they “startle too much, even when there are prepulses.” A more likely association between PPI and a brain disorder would reflect the fact that pathological genes code for neural circuitry that regulates PPI, and which also regulates other fundamental behavioral or neurocognitive processes. A “simple” example of such an association might underlie PPI deficits in disorders related to pontine dysfunction or developmental delay, such as enuresis, based on the apparent role of neighboring pontine structures in both the control of PPI and bladder function (cf. Swerdlow et al. 2001a); conceivably, such a relationship might also underlie PPI deficits in TS, based on a contribution of striatal dysfunction to both of these “phenotypes.” Certainly, there are more complex and interactive models for an association between PPI and a disorder like TS; for example, TS might be manifested most intensely (or positively moderated) in individuals who also carry genes coding for reduced neural control of a psychophysiological process – sensorimotor gating – that results in a reduced level of motor inhibition in response to sensory input; studies by Wang et al. (2011) suggest such a loss of activity in inter-connected cortico-striatal circuitry that exerts a top-down control of motor activity in TS. The key point is that the experimental association of reduced PPI and TS tells us very little about the biological relationship between these two phenomena; this does not, however, mean that such a relationship will not be a useful one, for testing hypotheses and potential etiologies, and even for developing novel therapeutics.

3. Update of recent PPI findings of relevance to TS

A number of past reviews have described PPI and its applications towards understanding TS (e.g. Swerdlow & Sutherland 2005, 2006). More recent developments in this field have come from several novel uses of PPI in TS-related models; these studies share a common measure (PPI) and target disorder (TS), but otherwise cover a range of different topics:

A. Striatal regulation of PPI in mice

Based on the ability to easily study molecular manipulations in mice, they are the species of favor for modelling genetic contributions to human disorders such as TS. In truth, however, most of the neurobiology of PPI was initially identified in rats, and not in mice (cf. Swerdlow et al. 2001a). While the default assumption had been that the neural regulation of PPI in mice should be quite similar to that found in rats, there is already evidence for species differences in this circuitry, particularly among systems of direct relevance to TS (Ralph & Caine 2005). A number of mutant mice are being used in gene-based models for TS, and it will thus be increasingly important to understand the neural circuit regulation of PPI in mice. In one important recent finding, Baldan Ramsey et al. (2011a) reported that excitotoxic lesions of the dorsomedial striatum in C57Bl/6J mice profoundly disrupted PPI, suggesting that basal ganglia circuitry regulate PPI in mice, in a manner consistent with similar findings in rats (Kodsi & Swerdlow 1995) and with findings in humans with known striatal pathology (Swerdlow et al. 1995). While there is no guarantee that these findings will generalize across all mouse strains (Ralph et al. 2001), or relevant brain regions, they establish at least one background strain with striatal-regulated PPI on which the effects of genetic manipulations could then be interpretted.

B. PPI deficits in histidine carboxylase-deficient TS patients and mice

In a recent use of mutant mice to model TS-related PPI deficits, Baldan Ramsey et al. (2011b) studied PPI in a two-generation TS human pedigree characterized by a nonsense mutation in the histidine decarboxylase (HDC) gene, HDC W317X, and in HDC knockout (KO) mice. PPI was significantly reduced among the nine TS patients carrying the HDC nonsense mutation, compared to control subjects, and in the HDC KO mice, compared to wild type mice; mice heterozygous for the HDC mutant allele exhibited intermediate PPI levels. Neuroimaging and biochemical analyses confirmed midbrain and forebrain D2/3 receptor abnormalities in these TS patients and in HDC KO mice. While these findings do not suggest that HDC is a common causative gene in either TS or its associated PPI deficits, they do provide convergent evidence that across species, HDC deficiencies are associated with reduced PPI. More generally, the use of PPI as a cross-species model to study the neurobiology of HDC mutations exemplifies the potential utility of this measure in investigating the numerous other genes that will ultimately be implicated in the complex genetics of TS.

C. Noradrenergic regulation of PPI in forebrain regions of relevance to TS

In addition to the proposed role of forebrain DAergic systems in tic genesis and therpeutics, there is also both clinical and preclinical evidence for a role of frontal noradrenergic transmission in these processes (cf. Leckman et al. 2010). Consistent with such a role, our group and others have reported effects of noradrenergic drugs in both reducing and restoring PPI in rats. For example, PPI in rats is disrupted by cirazoline, an agonist at the α1 NE receptor (Carasso et al., 1998). We reported that these effects of cirazoline are opposed by acute administration of the alpha-2 agonist, clonidine (Swerdlow et al. 2006a), which after chronic administration, is an effective antitic medication (Leckman et al., 1991). Thus, it is conceivable that antitic properties of noradrenergic agents might be predicted by their ability to reverse the gating-disruptive effects of cirazoline. More recently, Alsene et al. (2011) “mapped” the forebrain noradrenergic regulation of PPI in Sprague-Dawley rats, via intracerebral infusions of a mixture of the α1 NE agonist, phenylephrine plus the β-receptor agonist isoproterenol. PPI was significantly reduced after infusion of the NE agonist “cocktail” into the posterior medial prefrontal cortex, nucleus accumbens shell, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, basolateral amygdala, and the mediodorsal thalamus (MD) disrupted PPI, with particularly strong effects in MD. Several other sites did not support PPI disruptions after NE agonist infusion, but did so after infusion of the dopamine D2 receptor agonist, quinpirole. These findings begin to define a network of forebrain NE terminal fields in which either basal- or stress-induced increases in NE transmission might disrupt PPI in disorders such as TS; conceivably, this circuitry may interface with PPI-regulatory mechanisms in many brain regions, including the nucleus accumbens, where α1 NE receptors are known to regulate DAergic activity (Mitrano et al. 2012).

D. PPI models applied towards TS therapeutics

Several recent studies have utilized PPI in efforts to understand and advance novel TS therapeutics. In one such study, Devoto et al. (2011) utilized PPI to explicate the neural mechanisms that might account for an observed reduction in tic severity in a series of 10 adult male TS patients treated with the 5α-reductase inhibitor, finasteride (FIN) (Muroni et al. 2011). In rats, FIN was shown to prevent the PPI-disruptive effects of DA agonists after systemic, intraventricular and intracerebral administration, the latter being into the nucleus accumbens (NAC) core or shell subregions. FIN infusion into a number of other brain regions failed to prevent DA agonist effects on PPI. The authors interpreted their findings to suggest that FIN effects on PPI in rats – and by extrapolation, potentially its effects on TS symptoms – reflect its actions on cells within the NAC. Here, DA-disrupted PPI was used as a sort of surrogate model for TS symptoms, to understand the clinical observation of positive FIN effects on TS patients.

A similar use of PPI was reported in studies designed to understand and potentially map circuitry for deep brain stimulation (DBS) sites of value to severe, treatment-refractory TS and other disorders. Posch et al. (2012) demonstrated that high frequency DBS of the rat entopeduncular nucleus (EPN) – a rodent analog of the human globus pallidus interna (GPi), an effective site for DBS in TS patients (Welter et al. 2008) – prevented the PPI-disruptive effects of the DA agonist, apomorphine (APO). Taken together with the above findings of Devoto et al. (2011), these effects of DBS underscore that DA-induced PPI deficits can be opposed via interventions at multiple levels of interconnected forebrain circuits, conceivably recapitulating the therapeutic impact of DBS administered to multiple sites within these same circuits.

E. PPI in disorders relevant to TS

Because PPI abnormalities are observed in numerous brain disorders, they are clearly not diagnostically specific. Because of the common comorbidity of TS, it is important that previous findings have determined that – in small numbers of patients –PPI deficits in TS are not dependent on the common comorbid conditions of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) or Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD) (Castellanos et al. 1996; Swerdlow et al. 2001b). However, to the degree that TS is a disorder that shares clinical, neuroanatomical and potentially genetic elements with these and other neurodevelopmental disorders, the findings of PPI deficits within the “TS spectrum” can be informative regarding their relevance to TS. For example, Ahmari et al. (2012) recently confirmed that PPI is deficient in unmedicated OCD patients – as it had been previously reported in smaller groups of medicated OCD patients (Swerdlow et al. 1993; Hoenig et al. 2005). Ahmari et al. (2012) detected significantly reduced PPI in OCD patients vs. comparison subjects, and also identified 3 individuals with a history of tics, whose PPI levels were reduced, on average, well below the levels of the other 19 OCD patients. While a comprehensive review of recent reports of PPI deficits among neurodevelopmental disorders is not within the scope of this paper, it is worth noting that among these disorders, PPI is being applied towards the development of novel therapeutics (e.g. Levenga et al. 2011; Olmos-Serrano et al. 2011), and towards the diagnostic and pathophysiological dissociation of clinical subtypes (e.g. Eggert et al. 2012).

F. Sensory phenomena and TS psychotherapeutics

Some intriguing recent developments in our understanding of TS relate to the sensory phenomenology associated with this disorder. Among the most important breakthroughs in all of TS therapeutics to date has been the recent multi-site finding of the therapeutic benefits of a modified Habit Reversal Therapy protocol –termed “Comprehensive Behavioral Interventions for Tics (CBIT)” – in children with TS (Piacentini et al. 2010), and the upcoming report of comparable findings in TS adults. The efficacy of CBIT in reducing tics is comparable to, or exceeds, that of known antitic medications. Importantly, while this effective “new” therapy does not rely on any models for the molecular or genetic basis of TS, it is critically dependent on our understanding of the TS clinical phenotype, and particular the sensory and premonitory symptoms described by Leckman et al. (1993) and by many subsequent groups. In CBIT, patients identify these sensory events as the antecedent of the motor or phonic event, and initiate a competing opposing response. Thus, the intrusive sensory information plays an important role in the therapeutic impact of CBIT, and indeed, the inability to recognize such sensory events can hinder CBIT’s efficacy.

Based in part on their new, central role in TS therapeutics, TS-related sensory phenomena have become a focus of several recent studies. Sutherland et al. (2011) characterized features of sensory phenomena in TS patients. In addition to the discrete sensory tics and premonitory urges, they reported that TS patients endorse difficulties in specific sensory gating processes, including: 1) the ability to modulate stimulus intensity and prevent perceptual inundation, 2) the ability to focus attention or prevent distractibility, 3) a low threshold of perception (over-inclusion and hyperawareness), and 4) a vulnerability to perceptual and attentional anomalies during periods of fatigue and stress (Sutherland et al. 2011). Belluscio et al. (2011) reported complementary findings, with 80% of their adult TS sample reporting heightened sensitivity to sensory stimulation. Importantly, however, direct assessment of tactile and olfactory thresholds revealed no differences between TS and control subjects, suggesting that subjective sensitivity differences in TS patients reflected altered central information processing rather than enhanced peripheral sensory detection.

The sensitivity of TS symptoms to CBIT depends heavily on the ability of an individual to detect a premonitory event, and to initiate an appropriate preventative response. Structurally, this process is analogous to that used in traditional cognitive and behavioral therapy (CBT), where individuals identify an event (a negative thought, obsession, delusion, etc.) and interrupt the typical consequence (rumination, avoidance, etc.) by initiating a preventative cognitive or behavioral process. Over time, with consistent, effortful application, the volitional components of this inhibitory process become more automatic, integrated into the “default” behavioral profile; conceptually, the regulation of this inhibition shifts from cortical to subcortical circuitry. This structural similarity between CBIT and CBT is of interest because PPI has been reported to be a potent predictor of the therapeutic impact of CBT in schizophrenia patients (Kumari et al. 2012). While PPI has not yet been assessed as a predictor of CBIT response, previous studies of response predictors for Habit Reversal Therapy in TS demonstrated that improvement from HRT correlated significantly with levels of inhibition in a conceptually-related visuospatial priming (VSP) paradigm (Deckersbach et al 2006); an animal model of this VSP paradigm, and its pharmacologic sensitivity, is reported herein by Amitai et al. (2012).

Because of the link between CBIT, sensory phenomena, and motor inhibition, as well as the new evidence that PPI predicts the therapeutic response to CBT (Kumari et al. 2012), it is worth considering how PPI or related measures might be of utility in the application of CBIT or other behavioral interventions for TS. One emerging concept involves the identification of “pro-cognitive therapy” medications that can specifically enhance the therapeutic impact of a cognitive or behavioral intervention, by enabling patients to better meet the specific cognitive demands of that therapy (cf. Swerdlow 2011). A “proof of concept” for such a paradigm comes from the use of the pro-extinction drug, D-cycloserine (DCS), to enhance the therapeutic benefits of CBT in several different brain disorders, including OCD; this ability of DCS was first predicted based on its ability to facilitate extinction of the fear-potentiated acoustic startle reflex (cf. Davis 2011). In the case of TS, such “pro-CBIT” interventions might be specific to both the patient’s symptoms and their cognitive capabilities. In a simple example, one might hypothesize that TS patients with comorbid ADHD are particularly challenged by the attentional demands of CBIT (Woods et al. 2008); in these individuals, stimulants might augment the therapeutic impact of CBIT, even above their independent antitic properties (The Tourette’s Syndrome Study Group 2002). While such synergy has not yet been tested, one small study failed to detect a synergistic effect of acute methylphenidate administration with tic suppression (not CBIT per se); one likely explanation for this outcome appeared to be a floor effect – i.e. near-maximal tic reduction with either tic suppression or methylphenidate (Lyon et al. 2010). Clearly, tic activity after CBIT does not always reach “floor” levels (Piacentini et al. 2010), and there is ample range to assess the potential synergistic impact of medications with CBIT.

One might envision medication-enhanced behavioral interventions for TS via drug effects on other neurocognitive processes. For example, it is indeed conceivable that extinction of the premonitory sensation – which often serves as a “trigger” for tics – might be accelerated with DCS, similar to its ability to enhance extinction of the psychic discomfort associated with obsessions in OCD. Should such a process “disconnect” the urge from the subsequent motor tic, it might obviate a need to initiate a competing event. Alternatively, drugs that enhance sensorimotor gating – assessed by increases in PPI – might potentiate an individual’s ability to suppress the motor (tic) response to the sensory (premonitory) event. Individuals with low levels of PPI are particularly sensitive to PPI-enhancing effects of a number of drugs from different chemical classes – including atypical antipsychotics (Swerdlow et al. 2006b; Vollenweider et al. 2006), direct (Bitsios et al. 2005) or indirect DA agonists (Talledo et al. 2009), the catechol-O-methyl-transferase inhibitor, tolcapone (Giakoumaki et al. 2008) and the low-potency NMDA antagonists, memantine (Swerdlow et al. 2009) and amantadine (Swerdlow et al. 2002; Bitsios et al. 2005). Conceivably, by identifying an individual’s sensitivity to drug-enhanced PPI in the laboratory, it might be possible to predict pharmacological strategies for augmenting sensorimotor gating processes that would potentiate their ability to assert volitional control over semi-automatic motor responses. Whether such a strategy can be applied towards identifying “pro-CBIT” medications, and whether PPI, VSP, “Go - No Go” (e.g. Deckersbach et al. 2006) or related measures of behavioral inhibition can play a predictive role in the development or application of this strategy, are worthwhile topics for future investigation.

4. Understanding the limits of PPI

It is important to have realistic expectations for what can, and cannot be learned, from the use of any laboratory measure, and PPI is no exception to this rule (Swerdlow et al. 2008). As we have noted previously, PPI and its relative deficiencies are not diagnostic of TS or any other condition; levels of PPI do not predict clinical course, specific symptoms, or individual treatment responses (Swerdlow et al. 2008), with the possible exception of one recent report with CBT (Kumari et al. 2012). It appears that PPI is reduced in TS, in a manner that is: 1) independent of stimulus modality and co-morbid conditions; 2) evident in the “general” TS population and in at least one group of individuals whose TS is associated with a specific genetic mutation, as well as in a proposed isomorphic animal model; 3) reproducible in other animal models that appear to be informative about TS neurobiology, and potentially, its treatments. Some applications of this measure are relatively “low-cost/high-yield”, such as its use in a cross-species platform to assess models of genetics and neurobiology. Other uses - particularly those with direct clinical applications – remain under investigation, and in the foreseeable future may remain “a bridge too far.” To some degree, it is astonishing that a laboratory measure of plasticity in a pontine-based reflex has had any utility in the study of the pharmacology, neurobiology and genetics of complex and uniquely human brain disorders like TS; PPI remains an experimental tool of value that should continue to be informative if applied in a rational manner, within its limits of resolution.

Highlights.

  • We review the cross-species utility of prepulse inhibtion (PPI) as an operational measure of sensorimotor gating.

  • PPI deficits in Tourette Syndrome (TS) and in other neurodevelopmental brain disorders are discussed.

  • The neurobiological regulation of PPI by forebrain circuitry implicated in the pathophysiology of TS is described.

  • A conceptual linkage is noted between deficient sensorimotor gating and the intrusive sensory phenomena now recognized to be a core feature of TS.

  • An update of recent findings relevant to the relationship of PPI to TS is provided.

Acknowledgments

NRS is supported by R01-MH059803, R34-MH093453, R01-MH065571, R01-MH042228 and the VA San Diego Healthcare System, VISN22 Mental Illness Research, Education & Clinical Center (MIRECC).

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Ahmari SE, Risbrough VB, Geyer MA, Simpson HB. Impaired sensorimotor gating in unmedicated adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:1216–1223. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Albin RL, Koeppe RA, Bohnen NI, Nichols TE, Meyer P, Wernette K, Minoshima S, Kilbourn MR, Frey KA. Increased ventral striatal monoaminergic innervation in Tourette syndrome. Neurology. 2003;61:310–315. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000076181.39162.fc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Alsene KM, Rajbhandari AK, Ramaker MJ, Bakshi VP. Discrete forebrain neuronal networks supporting noradrenergic regulation of sensorimotor gating. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:1003–1014. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Amitai N, Weber M, Swerdlow NR, Sharp RF, Breier MR, Halberstadt AL, Young JW. A Novel Visuospatial Priming Task for Rats with Relevance to Tourette Syndrome: Evidence for Rate-Dependent Effects of Amphetamine. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.09.007. (in review, this issue) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baldan Ramsey LC, Williams K, Gallezot JD, Crowley M, Anderson G, Leventhal BI, Ohtsu H, Krystal JH, Mayes L, De Araujo I, Ding YS, State MW, Pittenger C. Histidine decarboxylase deficiency produces Tourette Syndrome phenomenology and dopamine dysregulation in humans and mice. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011a;34:S198–199. [Google Scholar]
  6. Baldan Ramsey LC, Xu M, Wood N, Pittenger C. Lesions of the dorsomedial striatum disrupt prepulse inhibition. Neuroscience. 2011b;180:222–228. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.01.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Belluscio BA, Jin L, Watters V, Lee TH, Hallett M. Sensory sensitivity to external stimuli in Tourette syndrome patients. Mov Disord. 2011;26:2538–2543. doi: 10.1002/mds.23977. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bitsios P, Giakoumaki SG, Frangou S. The effects of dopamine agonists on prepulse inhibition in healthy men depend on baseline PPI values. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;182:144–152. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0056-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bliss J. Sensory experiences of Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1980;37:1343–1347. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1980.01780250029002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Braff D, Stone C, Callaway E, Geyer M, Glick I, Bali L. Prestimulus effects on human startle reflex in normals and schizophrenics. Psychophysiology. 1978;15:339–343. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1978.tb01390.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carasso BS, Bakshi VP, Geyer MA. Disruption in prepulse inhibition after alpha-1 adrenoceptor stimulation in rats. Neuropharmacology. 1998;37:401–404. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(98)00051-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carter RJ, Lione LA, Humby T, Mangiarini L, Mahal A, Bates GP, Dunnett SB, Morton AJ. Characterization of progressive motor deficits in mice transgenic for the human Huntington’s disease mutation. J Neurosci. 1999;19:3248–3257. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-08-03248.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Castellanos FX, Fine EJ, Kaysen DL, Marsh WL, Rapoport JL, Hallett M. Sensorimotor gating in boys with Tourette’s Syndrome and ADHD: preliminary results. Biol Psychiatry. 1996;39:33–41. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00101-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cohen DJ, Leckman JF. Sensory phenomena associated with Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome. J Clin Psychiatry. 1992;5:319–323. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Davis M. NMDA receptors and fear extinction: implications for cognitive behavioral therapy. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2011;13:463–474. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2011.13.4/mdavis. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Deckersbach T, Rauch S, Buhlmann U, Wilhelm S. Habit reversal versus supportive psychotherapy in Tourette’s disorder: a randomized controlled trial and predictors of treatment response. Behav Res Ther. 2006;44:1079–1090. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Devoto P, Frau R, Bini V, Pillolla G, Saba P, Flore G, Corona M, Marrosu F, Bortolato M. Inhibition of 5α-reductase in the nucleus accumbens counters sensorimotor gating deficits induced by dopaminergic activation. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011 Oct 24; doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.09.018. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Eggert P, Freischmidt S, Bismarck PV, Schulz-Jürgensen S. Differentiation of subgroups of monosymptomatic enuresis according to prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex. Acta Paediatr. 2012 doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2012.02624.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Francis DD, Szegda K, Campbell G, Martin WD, Insel TR. Epigenetic sources of behavioral differences in mice. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:445–446. doi: 10.1038/nn1038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Frankland PW, Wang Y, Rosner B, Shimizu T, Balleine BW, Dykens EM, Ornitz EM, Silva AJ. Sensorimotor gating abnormalities in young males with fragile X syndrome and Fmr1-knockout mice. Mol Psychiatry. 2004;9:417–425. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Frey KA the TSA Neuroimaging Consortium. Neuroimaging in the pursuit of Tourette Syndrome neurobiology (2006) Proceedings of the Autism Neuroimaging Summit. University of California; Los Angeles: 2006. [Google Scholar]
  22. Giakoumaki SG, Roussos P, Bitsios P. Improvement of prepulse inhibition and executive function by the COMT inhibitor tolcapone depends on COMT Val158Met polymorphism. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:3058–3068. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gomez-Wong E, Marti MJ, Tolosa E, Valls-Sole J. Sensory modulation of the blink reflex in patients with blepharospasm. Arch Neurol. 1998;55:1233–1237. doi: 10.1001/archneur.55.9.1233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Graham F. The more or less startling effects of weak prestimuli. Psychophysiology. 1975;12:238–248. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1975.tb01284.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Greenwood TA, Braff DL, Light GA, Cadenhead KS, Calkins ME, Dobie DJ, Freedman R, Green MF, Gur RE, Gur RC, Mintz J, Nuechterlein KH, Olincy A, Radant AD, Seidman LJ, Siever LJ, Silverman JM, Stone WS, Swerdlow NR, Tsuang DW, Tsuang MT, Turetsky BI, Schork NJ. The Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS): Initial Heritability Analyses of Endophenotypic Measures for Schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:1242–1250. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.11.1242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hoenig K, Hochrein A, Quednow BB, Maier W, Wagner M. Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:1153–1158. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hollenbeck PJ. A Jangling Journey: Life with Tourette’s Syndrome. Cerebrum. 2003;5:47–60. [Google Scholar]
  28. Kalanithi PS, Zheng W, Kataoka Y, DiFiglia M, Grantz H, Saper CB, Schwartz ML, Leckman JF, Vaccarino FM. Altered parvalbumin-positive neuron distribution in basal ganglia of individuals with Tourette syndrome. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:13307–13312. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0502624102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kataoka Y, Kalanithi PS, Grantz H, Schwartz ML, Saper C, Leckman JF, Vaccarino FM. Decreased number of parvalbumin and cholinergic interneurons in the striatum of individuals with Tourette syndrome. J Comp Neurol. 2010;518:277–291. doi: 10.1002/cne.22206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kodsi MH, Swerdlow NR. Prepulse inhibition in the rat is regulated by ventral and caudodorsal striato-pallidal circuitry. Behav Neurosci. 1995;109:912–928. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.109.5.912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kodsi MH, Swerdlow NR. Quinolinic acid lesions of the ventral striatum reduce sensorimotor gating of acoustic startle in rats. Brain Res. 1994;643:59–65. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90008-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kumari V, Premkumar P, Fannon D, Aasen I, Raghuvanshi S, Anilkumar AP, Antonova E, Peters ER, Kuipers E. Sensorimotor gating and clinical outcome following cognitive behaviour therapy for psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2012;134:232–238. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.11.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Leckman JF, Bloch MH, Smith ME, Larabi D, Hampson M. Neurobiological substrates of Tourette’s disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2010;20:237–247. doi: 10.1089/cap.2009.0118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Leckman JF, Hardin MT, Riddle MA, Stevenson J, Ort SI, Cohen DJ. Clonidine treatment of Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48:324–328. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810280040006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Leckman JF, Walker DE, Cohen DJ. Premonitory urges in Tourette’s syndrome. Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150:98–102. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.1.98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Leckman JF, Walker DE, Goodman WK, Pauls DL, Cohen DJ. “Just right” perceptions associated with compulsive behavior in Tourette’s syndrome. Am J Psychiatry. 1994;151:675–680. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.5.675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Levenga J, Hayashi S, de Vrij FM, Koekkoek SK, van der Linde HC, Nieuwenhuizen I, Song C, Buijsen RA, Pop AS, Gomezmancilla B, Nelson DL, Willemsen R, Gasparini F, Oostra BA. AFQ056, a new mGluR5 antagonist for treatment of fragile X syndrome. Neurobiol Dis. 2011;42:311–317. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2011.01.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Li L, Du Y, Li N, Wu X, Wu Y. Top-down modulation of prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex in humans and rats. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;33:1157–67. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lind NM, Arnfred SM, Hemmingsen RP, Hansen AK. Prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex in pigs and its disruption by d-amphetamine. Behav Brain Res. 2004;155:217–222. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Linn GS, Negi SS, Gerum SV, Javitt DC. Reversal of phencyclidine-induced prepulse inhibition deficits by clozapine in monkeys. Psychopharmacology. 2003;169:234–239. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1533-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lyon GJ, Samar SM, Conelea C, Trujillo MR, Lipinski CM, Bauer CC, Brandt BC, Kemp JJ, Lawrence ZE, Howard J, Castellanos FX, Woods D, Coffey BJ. Testing tic suppression: comparing the effects of dexmethylphenidate to no medication in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and Tourette’s disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2010;20:283–289. doi: 10.1089/cap.2010.0032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. McAlonan GM, Daly E, Kumari V, Critchley HD, van Amelsvoort T, Suckling J, Simmons A, Sigmundsson T, Greenwood K, Russell A, Schmitz N, Happe F, Howlin P, Murphy DG. Brain anatomy and sensorimotor gating in Asperger’s syndrome. Brain. 2002;125(Pt 7):1594–1606. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. McNaught KS, Mink JW. Advances in understanding and treatment of Tourette syndrome. Nat Rev Neurol. 2011;7:667–676. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2011.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Miguel EC, Baer L, Coffey BJ, Rauch SL, Savage CR, O’Sullivan RL, Phillips K, Moretti C, Leckman JF, Jenike MA. Phenomenological differences appearing with repetitive behaviours in obsessive-compulsive disorder and Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome. Br J Psychiatry. 1997;170:140–145. doi: 10.1192/bjp.170.2.140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Miguel EC, de Rosario-Campos MC, Prado HS, do Valle R, Rauch SL, Coffey BJ, Baer L, Savage CR, O’Sullivan RL, Jenike MA, Leckman JF. Sensory phenomena in obsessive-compulsive disorder and Tourette’s disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2000;61:150–156. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v61n0213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Mitrano DA, Schroeder JP, Smith Y, Cortright JJ, Bubula N, Paul Vezina P, Weinshenker D. Alpha-1 adrenergic receptors are localized on presynaptic elements in the nucleus accumbens and regulate mesolimbic dopamine transmission. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:2161–2172. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Muroni A, Paba S, Puligheddu M, Marrosu F, Bortolato M. A preliminary study of finasteride in Tourette syndrome. Mov Disord. 2011;26:2146–2147. doi: 10.1002/mds.23810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Neuner I, Podoll K, Lenartz D, Sturm V, Schneider F. Deep brain stimulation in the nucleus accumbens for intractable Tourette’s syndrome: follow-up report of 36 months. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65:e5–6. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Olmos-Serrano JL, Corbin JG, Burns MP. The GABA(A) receptor agonist THIP ameliorates specific behavioral deficits in the mouse model of fragile X syndrome. Dev Neurosci. 2011;33:395–403. doi: 10.1159/000332884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Ornitz EM, Hanna GL, de Traversay J. Prestimulation-induced startle modulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and nocturnal enuresis. Psychophysiology. 1992;29:437–451. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1992.tb01717.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Peterson BS, Thomas P, Kane MJ, Scahill L, Zhang H, Bronen R, King RA, Leckman JF, Staib L. Basal Ganglia volumes in patients with Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60:415–424. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.4.415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Piacentini J, Woods DW, Scahill L, Wilhelm S, Peterson AL, Chang S, Ginsburg GS, Deckersbach T, Dziura J, Levi-Pearl S, Walkup JT. Behavior therapy for children with Tourette disorder: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2010;303:1929–1937. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Posch DK, Schwabe K, Krauss JK, Lütjens G. Deep brain stimulation of the entopeduncular nucleus in rats prevents apomorphine-induced deficient sensorimotor gating. Behav Brain Res. 2012;232:130–136. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.02.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Ralph RJ, Caine SB. Dopamine D1 and D2 agonist effects on prepulse inhibition and locomotion: comparison of Sprague-Dawley rats to Swiss-Webster, 129X1/SvJ, C57BL/6J, and DBA/2J mice. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2005;312:733–741. doi: 10.1124/jpet.104.074468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Ralph RJ, Paulus MP, Geyer MA. Strain-specific effects of amphetamine on prepulse inhibition and patterns of locomotor behavior in mice. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2001;298:148–155. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Singer HS, Giuliano JD, Hansen BH, Hallett JJ, Laurino JP, Benson M, Kiessling LS. Antibodies against human putamen in children with Tourette syndrome. Neurology. 1998;50:1618–1624. doi: 10.1212/wnl.50.6.1618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Smith SJ, Lees AJ. Abnormalities of the blink reflex in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1989;52:895–898. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.52.7.895. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Sobin C, Kiley-Brabeck K, Karayiorgou M. Lower prepulse inhibition in children with the 22q11 deletion syndrome. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:1090–1099. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.6.1090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Steeves TD, Ko JH, Kideckel DM, Rusjan P, Houle S, Sandor P, Lang AE, Strafella AP. Extrastriatal dopaminergic dysfunction in tourette syndrome. Ann Neurol. 2010;67:170–181. doi: 10.1002/ana.21809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sutherland Owens AN, Miguel EC, Swerdlow NR. Sensory gating scales and premonitory urges in Tourette syndrome. ScientificWorldJournal. 2011;11:736–41. doi: 10.1100/tsw.2011.57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Swerdlow NR. Are we studying and treating schizophrenia correctly? Schizophr Res. 2011;130:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Swerdlow NR, Benbow CH, Zisook S, Geyer MA, Braff DL. A preliminary assessment of sensori-motor gating in patients with Obsessive Compulsive Disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1993;33:298–301. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(93)90300-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Swerdlow NR, Bongiovanni MJ, Tochen L, Shoemaker JM. Separable noradrenergic and dopaminergic regulation of prepulse inhibition in rats: Implications for predictive validity and Tourette Syndrome. Psychopharmacology. 2006a;186:246–254. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0374-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Swerdlow NR, Braff DL, Taaid N, Geyer MA. Assessing the validity of an animal model of deficient sensorimotor gating in schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51:139–154. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950020063007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Swerdlow NR, Caine SB, Geyer MA. Regionally selective effects of intracerebral dopamine infusion on sensorimotor gating of the startle reflex in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1992;108:189–195. doi: 10.1007/BF02245306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA, Braff DL. Neural circuit regulation of prepulse inhibition of startle in the rat: Current knowledge and future challenges. Psychopharmacology. 2001a;156:194–215. doi: 10.1007/s002130100799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Swerdlow NR, Karban B, Ploum Y, Sharp R, Geyer MA, Eastvold A. Tactile prepuff inhibition of startle in children with Tourette’s Syndrome: In search of an “fMRI-friendly” startle paradigm. Biol Psychiatry. 2001b;50:578–585. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01164-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Swerdlow NR, Light GA, Cadenhead KS, Sprock J, Hsieh MH, Braff DL. Startle gating deficits in a large cohort of patients with schizophrenia: relationship to medications, symptoms, neurocognition, and level of function. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006b;63:1325–1335. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.12.1325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Swerdlow NR, Paulsen J, Braff DL, Butters N, Geyer MA, Swenson MR. Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic and tactile startle response in patients with Huntington’s Disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1995;58:192–200. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.58.2.192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Swerdlow NR, Shilling PD, Breier M, Trim RS, Light GA, Saint Marie RL. Fronto-temporal-mesolimbic gene expression and heritable differences in amphetamine-disrupted sensorimotor gating in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2012 Jun 15; doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2758-1. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Swerdlow NR, Stephany N, Shoemaker JM, Ross L, Wasserman LC, Talledo J, Auerbach PP. Effects of amantadine and bromocriptine on startle and sensorimotor gating: Parametric studies and cross-species comparisons. Psychopharmacology. 2002;164:82–92. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1172-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Swerdlow NR, Sutherland AN. Preclinical models relevant to Tourette Syndrome. Adv Neurol. 2006;99:69–88. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Swerdlow NR, Sutherland AN. Using animal models to develop therapeutics for Tourette Syndrome. Pharmacol Ther. 2005;108:281–293. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2005.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Swerdlow NR, Talledo J, Sutherland AN, Nagy D, Shoemaker JM. Antipsychotic effects on prepulse inhibition in normal ‘low gating’ humans and rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006c;31:2011–2021. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Swerdlow NR, van Bergeijk DP, Bergsma F, Weber E, Talledo J. The effects of memantine on prepulse inhibition. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:1854–1864. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Swerdlow NR, Weber M, Qu Y, Light GA, Braff DL. Realistic expectations of prepulse inhibition in translational models for schizophrenia research. Psychopharmacology. 2008;199:331–388. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1072-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Talledo JA, Sutherland Owens AN, Schortinghuis T, Swerdlow NR. Amphetamine effects on startle gating in normal women and female rats. Psychopharmacology. 2009;204:165–175. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1446-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Tourette’s Syndrome Study Group. Treatment of ADHD in children with tics: a randomized controlled trial. Neurology. 2002;58:527–536. doi: 10.1212/wnl.58.4.527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Vaillancourt C, Boksa P. Birth insult alters dopamine-mediated behavior in a precocial species, the guinea pig. Implications for schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;23:654–666. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00164-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Valls-Sole J, Munoz JE, Valldeoriola F. Abnormalities of prepulse inhibition do not depend on blink reflex excitability: a study in Parkinson’s disease and Huntington’s disease. Clin Neurophysiol. 2004;115:1527–1536. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. van Rijn S, Swaab H, Magnée M, van Engeland H, Kemner C. Psychophysiological markers of vulnerability to psychopathology in men with an extra X chromosome (XXY) PLoS One. 2011;6:e20292. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Vollenweider FX, Barro M, Csomor PA, Feldon J. Clozapine enhances prepulse inhibition in healthy humans with low but not with high prepulse inhibition levels. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:597–603. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Wang Z, Maia TV, Marsh R, Colibazzi T, Gerber A, Peterson BS. The neural circuits that generate tics in Tourette’s syndrome. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168:1326–1337. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.09111692. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Welter ML, Mallet L, Houeto JL, Karachi C, Czernecki V, Cornu P, Navarro S, Pidoux B, Dormont D, Bardinet E, Yelnik J, Damier P, Agid Y. Internal pallidal and thalamic stimulation in patients with Tourette syndrome. Arch Neurol. 2008;65:952–957. doi: 10.1001/archneur.65.7.952. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Woods DW, Himle MB, Miltenberger RG, Carr JE, Osmon DC, Karsten AM, Jostad C, Bosch A. Durability, negative impact, and neuropsychological predictors of tic suppression in children with chronic tic disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2008;36:237–245. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9173-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Zhang J, Forkstam C, Engel JA, Svensson L. Role of dopamine in prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle. Psychopharmacology. 2000;149:181–188. doi: 10.1007/s002130000369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Ziemann U, Paulus W, Rothenberger A. Decreased motor inhibition in Tourette’s disorder: evidence from transcranial magnetic stimulation. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154:1277–1284. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.9.1277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES