Abstract
Objective
To examine the quality of communication likely to be experienced by parents when being first informed about how newborn screening identified heterozygous “carrier” status for cystic fibrosis or sickle cell disease.
Methods
Primary care providers (PCPs) of infants found to have carrier status were telephoned over a 48-month period, and asked to rehearse with a standardized patient how they would inform the infants’ parent(s). 214 rehearsal transcripts were abstracted using explicit criteria methods to measure the frequency of five categories of high-quality communication behaviors.
Results
Overall, PCPs used large amounts of jargon and failed to use high quality communication behaviors. On average, PCPs used 18.6 total jargon words (8.7 unique words), but explained 2.4 jargon words. The most frequent assessment of understanding was the close-ended version, although it was only seen in 129 of 214 transcripts. The most common organizing behavior was importance emphasis (121/214). Precautionary empathy was rare; the most frequent behavior was “instruction about emotion” (33/214).
Conclusions
The limited use of high-quality communication behaviors in rehearsals raises concern about parental understanding, decision-making, and psychosocial outcomes after newborn screening.
Practice Implications
Measurement of specific behaviors may help PCPs to improve communication, and thereby improve the patient experience.
1. Introduction
Newborn screening (NBS) is a population-scale public health program which includes testing of infants’ blood specimens that are applied to a special filter paper, dried, and tested at a centralized laboratory for a panel of genetic and metabolic diseases [1]. Cystic fibrosis (CF) and sickle cell disease (SCD) are included on NBS panels because the diseases’ risk of death and disability can be reduced if identified before becoming symptomatic [2-5]. Both CF and SCD are autosomal recessive conditions, and heterozygous “carrier” infants are identified in far greater numbers than infants with the actual diseases [6]. Unfortunately, many families of carrier infants develop psychosocial complications after NBS, ranging from clinical levels of parental anxiety or depression to impaired parent-child bonding and the vulnerable child syndrome [1-3, 7, 8]. Some authors have referred to these carrier conditions as “Nondiseases,” [9] although there is increasing interest in people being aware about some carrier states [10-12]. In the United States, NBS programs typically provide carrier results to the child’s primary care provider (PCP) for disclosure to the parent. NBS programs have developed educational and support materials for families, but it is also important to work on PCPs communication because first conversations can be critical for understanding [13], and because the quality of PCP’s communication has been criticized by parents and NBS officials [8, 14-16].
Psychosocial problems after carrier identification have been cited by bioethicists and others as grounds for delaying or discontinuing some NBS activities [17-20]. In contrast, we see psychosocial risks as a matter of NBS safety. To manage safety and allow NBS to expand, we have been developing techniques for assessing and improving PCPs’ communication. We adapt methods from traditional Quality Improvement, so that the methods will be affordable and feasible for use on the same population scale that is covered by NBS [7, 21-31]. A key part of this effort has been to develop “communication quality indicators” that operationalize important communication behaviors for quantitatively reliable, objective measurement [21-28].
In this paper, we describe communication quality indicator data from a 48-month statewide sample of PCPs’ “rehearsals” with a standardized patient prior to informing an actual parent about a NBS result showing carrier status for CF or SCD. We have previously used patient simulations in other studies of communication quality [22-27] and patient simulations are known to be an effective tool for physician education and assessment [32-36].
2. Methods
2.1 Context
This analysis was done as part of the “Wisconsin Project on Improvement of Communication Process and Outcomes after Newborn Screening” (hereafter called the “Project”) [7, 29, 30]. The Project is a statewide research study and quality improvement effort by the NBS program of the Wisconsin State Laboratory of Hygiene and the Department of Health Services, with the Medical College of Wisconsin as a contracted project agent. Project methods are approved by Institutional Review Boards at the Medical College of Wisconsin and University of Wisconsin, Madison. The phase of the Project that this analysis covers ran over the 48 months from December 1, 2007 through November 30, 2011. A previous manuscript goes into more detail about the complex methodology for the Project’s recruiting methods, as well as the acceptability of the methods to the PCPs and parents [30].
As one part of the Project, we telephoned PCPs of infants found by NBS to be carriers for SCD or likely carriers for CF, and invited the PCPs to rehearse over the telephone how they would inform parents about the results[30]. Parents were called during another part of the Project; these results will be reported elsewhere.
2.2 Participants
All participants for this analysis were PCPs, recruited by a multi-step process that was designed to function within the usual practice of the NBS laboratory [30]. The Project focused on two NBS results: the presence of fetal, adult, and sickle hemoglobin (the F-A-S result), or an elevated immunoreactive trypsinogen (IRT) followed by a single CF-associated mutation. The hemoglobin F-A-S result is 100% specific for the most common type of SCD carrier status. Infants with an elevated IRT and a single mutation are said by the Project to be “likely CF carriers” because they have a 2-5% chance of having CF due to a mutation that was not included on the NBS panel [3]. Thus, infants with the likely CF carrier result require sweat testing to confirm that they are actually carriers [37, 38].
During the 48 months of the Project reported in this paper, when an infant was found to have either of these results the NBS laboratory faxed the result to the Project team at the same time it contacted the clinician listed on the NBS card. When the listed clinician was not the correct PCP [39], the Project team telephoned the birthing hospital and used other search techniques to identify the responsible PCP. At any point during this process, exclusion criteria could be applied: (a) NBS lists more than one abnormality, (b) gestational age < 35 weeks, (c) >5 days in neonatal intensive care, (d) hospitalization after nursery discharge, or (e) evaluation for some other medical abnormality. We also excluded infants by asking the PCP to identify any other contraindications to contact by asking, “Can you think of any reason why it would not be appropriate to contact this family later this year?”
Finally, we excluded PCPs and infants from the entire Project when the PCP informed us that the parents require a translator. We were concerned that we would not have the sample size to analyze the effect of a language barrier on communication outcomes. We also did not have the resources to conduct parent interviews in other languages.
2.3 Data collection
After the infant’s actual PCP was located and the lack of contraindications was verified, PCPs were asked if they have any questions about the NBS result, and when they planned to inform the parent. PCPs were then invited to rehearse over the telephone how they would inform the infant’s parent(s) about the result. For this rehearsal the interviewer pretended to be the infant’s parent, following the protocol for our Brief Standardized Communication Assessment (BSCA) method [40]. Interviewers were instructed to maintain neutral vocal tone and avoid leading questions. Some artificiality is inevitable with the BSCA, but analysis is standardized and reduces confounding effects of patient variation.
PCPs were not asked to rehearse if they had previously rehearsed for another infant in the Project, or if they had declined to rehearse in the past. PCPs recorded only one rehearsal for analysis even if they had more than one patient in the project, both to save resources and also to avoid double-counting those PCPs who have a greater volume of patients. If the PCP had previously expressed interest but was unable to participate (e.g. because of time limits) then the PCP was invited to rehearse again.
Rehearsals were digitally audio-recorded, transcribed, and de-identified. To facilitate abstraction, transcripts are subjected to a parsing procedure to separate them into individual “strings” of text, each of which has a single subject and predicate that give the string a distinct meaning.
2.4 Measures (Communication quality indicators)
Transcripts of the PCP rehearsals were abstracted for communication quality indicator data using techniques that we previously demonstrated with smaller samples [21-28, 31]. As outlined elsewhere [21-28, 31], communication quality indicators operationalize important communication behaviors into specific, measurable targets for clinicians to improve. Indicators are independent of each other, so that clinicians may perform well on one indicator, but poorly on another [41].
2.4.1 Abstraction procedures
Our abstraction procedures are adapted from techniques used in traditional Quality Improvement, with abstractors reviewing transcripts in much the same way that hospital records are abstracted [41]. An explicit-criteria data dictionary is derived from published evidence and guidelines [13, 42-46], and contains detailed explanations and examples to reduce need for subjective judgment. Abstractors read though transcripts one string at a time, searching for strings that meet criteria outlined in the explicit-criteria data dictionary. To focus abstractors’ attention, abstraction is done for one group of communication quality indicators at a time. Abstraction is facilitated by our self-developed software application, Transcript Abstraction System (TAS), and is done twice for one third of the transcripts for quality control and reliability (following the suggestion by Feinstein [47]).
In this manuscript we focus on four out of our five previously-described groups of communication quality indicators: jargon [23, 24], assessment of understanding [21, 22], precautionary empathy [25], and organizing behaviors [28]. The fifth group of indicators (which focuses on content messages) [26, 27] is complex enough that data from that portion of the Project will be presented in another manuscript [48].
2.4.2 Communication behavior group #1: Jargon and explanations
“Jargon” refers to medical, scientific, or other words that may be unknown or misunderstood by the patient [23, 24]. Patients are known to complain about the amount of jargon used by health care providers (in NBS and elsewhere) [42-46]. Since jargon may sometimes be necessary for a full explanation, this group also includes criteria for jargon word explanations.
The number of jargon words was counted via a previously described, automated search procedure that we adapted from corpus linguistics [23, 24]. Abstractors then verified the automated results, with an amendment process allowing abstractors to propose or ratify additions to the jargon list. When such a word was identified, an electronic search of previously abstracted transcripts was used to verify that the newly designated jargon word was not missed in previous abstractions [23, 24].
During abstraction, jargon explanations were also identified by following the abstraction procedure described above. This group of communication quality indicators also includes the ratio of explained to unexplained jargon words and the explanation lag, defined as the number of concepts (measured in strings) between the first use of the word and its explanation [23, 24].
2.4.3 Communication behavior group #2: Assessment of understanding
Assessment of understanding (AU) refers to behaviors where the PCP seeks to gauge the effectiveness of his or her counseling about the NBS result. AUs are especially important for communication about NBS because, as with other complex topics, shock or intimidation may inhibit parents’ questions or reduce the chance of a confused facial expression [49]. As described elsewhere [21, 22], our data dictionary includes definitions and examples for four types of AUs: close-ended, open-ended, request for a teach-back, and the relatively ambiguous “OK?” Ideally, the PCP will include a new AU periodically throughout longer conversations [22]; if a PCP moves on to new information before verifying understanding, evidence suggests the patient may puzzle over statements and miss the new information as it is presented [21, 22, 50].
2.4.4 Communication behavior group #3: “Precautionary empathy”
We coined this term to refer to behaviors in which the physician addresses emotion that may be present, but which is not apparent in the parent’s speech, facial expressions, or body language [25]. Communication about “hidden” emotions is important because patients’ facial expressions do not always correlate with their emotional states for cultural reasons or because of a sense of shock [51, 52].
Abstraction for the precautionary empathy group was done following the procedure described above. As described elsewhere [25], our data dictionary for precautionary empathy includes eight behaviors, one of which (anticipate/validate) is a subgroup of two behaviors merged to increase reliability. Illustrative examples will be provided in the Results section’s Table 4.
Table 4.
Transcripts | |||
---|---|---|---|
Indicator | Example | n | (%) |
Assess for prior emotion |
How have previous
experiences with screening tests made you feel? |
0 | (0.0) |
Anticipate/ Validate emotion |
Many people get sad when they hear this kind of news. |
14 | (6.5) |
Assess for emotion (closed) |
Are you feeling sad about this
news? |
2 | (1.0) |
Assess for emotion (open) |
What feelings are you
experiencing right now? |
0 | (0.0) |
Instruction about emotion |
Don’t worry about this. Don’t cry. |
33 | (15.4) |
Assess for possible future emotion (closed) |
Are you going to worry about
this after you get home? |
0 | (0.0) |
Assess for possible future emotion (open) |
Based on your past
experience, what emotions might you experience when you think about this later? |
0 | (0.0) |
Caution about future emotion |
When you think some more about the implication of this, you might get more worried. |
10 | (4.7) |
2.4.5 Communication behavior group #4: Organizing behaviors
Organizing behaviors refer to verbal cues in a physician’s speech that help the patient keep up with the conversation, which influences the degree to which patients comprehend and retain verbal information [28, 53], or use the information in subsequent medical decision-making [54-56]. Organizing behaviors may be especially important for NBS because organization may help patients with the mental demands of simultaneously processing new information, experiencing emotions, and holding several unfamiliar concepts in mind [50, 57]. In contrast, disorganized speech may lead to confusion, recall problems, annoyance, or problems with the patient-provider relationship [54].
As described elsewhere [28], our data dictionary for organizing behaviors includes three subgroups of behaviors that have been grouped together to increase reliability: opening behaviors, structuring behaviors, and importance emphasis.
Abstraction for the organizing behaviors group was also done following the procedure described above. In contrast to the other four groups in this analysis, the data dictionary for organizing behaviors was derived exclusively from general communication and public speaking guidelines and training materials [50, 53-56] because they have not previously been studied in a health care setting.
2.5 Analysis
Data were archived using Access and SQLExpress software (Microsoft, Redmond WA). JMP software (SAS Institute, Cary, North Carolina) was used for statistical comparison with t-tests, Wilcoxon rank-sum, Cohen kappa comparisons, and the Chi-squared test, depending on the characteristics of the data being compared.
3. Results
Over the 48 months described in this paper, we logged 1642 infants with the hemoglobin F-A-S result, and 686 infants with the likely CF carrier result. After exclusion criteria and other sources of lossage were applied, we were able to reach 537 PCPs in time to invite a rehearsal, who together were responsible for 2075 of the infants we logged (89%).
Of the 537 PCPs invited to rehearse at least one time, 217 agreed (40.4%) and another 162 PCPs (30.2%) deferred participation to an interview about a future patient, but never had the opportunity to record a rehearsal. The remaining physicians declined to rehearse, but agreed for us to contact their patient’s parent at 3 months of age (29.4%). Since many PCPs are responsible for more than one infant, the PCPs who recorded a rehearsal corresponded with 442 of the logged infants (19%).
Three recorded rehearsals were excluded from analysis because of technical issues with the audio recording. Further details about rehearsal enrollment and associated factors are reported elsewhere [30]. Descriptive characteristics of the enrolled PCPs are in Table 1.
Table 1.
No. responding | ||
---|---|---|
Characteristics | n | (%) |
Gender | ||
Male | 88 | 41.1 |
Female | 126 | 58.9 |
Ethnicity (self-reported) | ||
White | 179 | 83.6 |
Black | 13 | 6.1 |
Asian | 11 | 5.1 |
Latino | 2 | 0.9 |
South Asian or Pacific Islander | 6 | 2.8 |
Middle Eastern | 1 | 0.5 |
Other | 1 | 0.5 |
Unknown or not reported | 1 | 0.5 |
Years since graduation | ||
10 years or less | 80 | 37.4 |
11-30 | 118 | 55.1 |
Greater than 30 | 16 | 7.5 |
Provider type | ||
Physician | 203 | 94.8 |
Nurse practitioner | 9 | 4.2 |
Physician Assistant | 1 | 0.5 |
Lay midwife | 1 | 0.5 |
Last time discussed a NBS result | ||
One year or less | 116 | 54.2 |
More than a year | 63 | 29.4 |
Unknown or not reported | 35 | 16.4 |
3.1 Communication behavior group #1: Jargon and explanations
In this sample, PCPs used a large amount of jargon and failed to explain much of the jargon they used (Table 2).
Table 2.
Indicator | Average | (SD) |
---|---|---|
Total jargon words | 18.5 | (9.8) |
Unique jargon words | 8.7 | (3.4) |
No Jargon words explained | 2.4 | (3.2) |
Jargon explanation ratio | 0.33 | (0.44) |
Jargon explanation time lag | 2.4 | (3.8) |
There was no difference in jargon use based on PCP gender, provider race, years since graduation, months since last discussing an NBS result, or type of PCP (physician, physician assistant or nurse practitioner). PCPs discussing SCD trait results included significantly more unique jargon words (t-test, p<0.05) and significantly fewer jargon explanations (Wilcoxon, p<0.05).
3.2 Communication behavior group #2: Assessment of understanding
While many PCPs attempted assessments of understanding, they tended to use close-ended questions rather than the more effective open-ended assessment (Table 3). Only 2 PCPs (1.0%) used the most effective assessment, the request for a teach-back. Some additional comments about these two PCPs’ transcripts are included in section 3.5.
Table 3.
Transcripts | |||
---|---|---|---|
Indicator | Example | n | (%) |
Closed-ended assessment of understanding |
Do you have any questions? | 129 | 60.3 |
Open-ended assessment of understanding |
What questions for you have
for me? |
21 | 9.8 |
Request for teach-back |
It would be helpful to me if you could repeat back that last point in your own words. |
2 | 1.0 |
OK? | OK? | 43 | 20.1 |
There were no significant differences in use of this behavior based on NBS result, PCPs’ gender, race, years since graduation, months since last discussing an NBS result, and whether the PCP was a physician, nurse practitioner, or physician assistant.
3.3 Communication behavior group #3: Precautionary empathy
As shown in Table 4, very few PCPs in this sample communicated about the potential for emotion. The most common type of precautionary empathy (15.4%) was the diverse behavior we group under the term “instruction about emotion.” It is unclear, however, whether statements like “don’t cry” or “I wouldn’t worry about this” are helpful for parents.
There were no significant differences in use of precautionary empathy behaviors based on NBS condition, PCPs’ gender, race, years since graduation, months since last discussing an NBS result, and whether the PCP was a physician, nurse practitioner, or physician assistant.
3.4 Communication behavior group #4: Organizing behaviors
Use of organizing behaviors was variable in this sample (Table 5). Slightly less than half of the transcripts included an opening behavior, and slightly more than half included at least one importance emphasis. Structuring behaviors (e.g. outlining, and summarizing) were rare.
Table 5.
Transcripts | |||
---|---|---|---|
Indicator | Example | n | (%) |
Opening behaviors |
I called you today to talk to you about your baby’s test results. |
104 | (48.6) |
Structuring behaviors |
First we’ll go over what newborn screening is, then we’ll talk about your baby’s results. |
17 | (7.9) |
Importance emphasis |
The most important thing to remember is that your baby is healthy. |
121 | (56.5) |
There were no significant differences in use of this behavior based on NBS result, PCPs’ gender, race, years since graduation, months since last discussing an NBS result, and whether the PCP was a physician, nurse practitioner, or physician assistant.
3.5 Examination of PCPs’ overall performance
The variability of the PCPs’ behavior on the rehearsals led us to do some further analysis, looking for patterns. The two transcripts that included a request for teach-back were studied closely, since this type of assessment of understanding may require unusual insight and skill. We hypothesized that the presence of a request for teach-back might prove to be a signal for a greater degree of skill. Although there was insufficient power for such a hypothesis to be tested, we did note qualitatively that one transcript included an importance emphasis and a close-ended assessment of understanding, and the other included an opening behavior, 3 importance emphases, and 3 close-ended assessments of understanding. Neither transcript included a precautionary empathy behavior.
We then used nonparametric correlation and Cohen kappa comparisons to determine if the absence or inclusion of any given behavior predicted the probability of absence or inclusion of another behavior. There was a weak but significant correlation between the absence of organizing behavior and absence of assessment of understanding (r=0.286, p<0.001). Even so, detailed inter-behavior comparisons using Cohen kappa failed to find any other associations. Inter-observation comparisons are limited when the instances are rare, but even without correction for random chance it appeared equally likely that a given behavior would appear when another was present. A reasonable statistical interpretation of this finding is that communication behaviors were, quantitatively speaking, randomly-occuring phenomena in this sample.
4. Discussion and Conclusions
4.1 Discussion
The purpose of this paper was to comment on two important issues. First, we further demonstrated the feasibility of our explicit-criteria approach to communication for a population scale [30]. Second, and most importantly for NBS and patients, our findings point to likely widespread problems with the quality of communication likely to be experienced by parents after NBS identifies carrier status for CF or SCD.
Many of our findings were consistent with the results of our previous, smaller studies which found that PCPs tend to use too much jargon [23, 24] and may fail to assess understanding [21, 22], organize their communication [58], and discuss emotion [25]. PCPs’ attempts to explain jargon suggest that some PCPs are aware that words can be confusing and they knew they should attempt to explain those words [23, 24] . Likewise, the inclusion of some assessments of understanding, organizing behaviors, and precautionary empathy behaviors suggests that PCPs may be aware of the need for these behaviors, but they may not be aware of the most effective techniques for including them. We are therefore optimistic about the potential for behavior-specific interventions with PCPs. That there were no differences in communication quality based on physician gender, ethnicity or years since graduation demonstrates that it is appropriate to target communication interventions to physicians at all stages of training and practice.
The current analysis does not provide data about the PCPs’ actual communication or parents’ psychosocial outcomes after the PCPs’ rehearsals. Future analyses from this data set will examine physician communication quality and subsequent parental anxiety. Even so, there have been many studies of associations between outcomes and various aspects of communication [8, 59-64]. Since these more subjective measures are associated with benefits for patients, we anticipate that communication quality indicators will also benefit patients.
We acknowledge several limitations for the current analysis and for our methods in general. A significant limitation is that our data come from rehearsals with standardized patients, instead of recordings of actual PCP-parent encounters. Simulated patient encounters may reduce generalizability because of artificial circumstances or because a sense of observation may prompt research participants to greater efforts [65, 66]. On the other hand, the Hawthorne effect helps our Quality Improvement goals because the resulting ceiling on performance helps us to identify targets most in need of improvement. We included patient simulation in our data collection methods because of our aim to develop techniques that can be immediately, affordably disseminated to settings where communication improvement has not previously been feasible. Patient simulation is also an effective mechanism for assessing physician communication skills because many physicians cannot tell the difference between real and simulated patients in practice [67]. Finally, simulation avoids practical barriers (e.g. logistics, privacy, and consent), which otherwise would pose tremendous challenges for routine Quality Improvement professionals [68]. With Quality Improvement methods also come a track record for improving other clinician behaviors on population scales, as well as concrete methods that are transparent to clinicians and easy to implement by Quality Improvement professionals with typical training [69]. Finally, the standardization that is possible with simulation allows for comparisons across clinicians to be made fairly and equally without confounding effects of variation in patients’ medical risks and communication challenges [32].
It is also worth pointing out that the current manuscript only reports data from four groups of communication quality indicators, and that here are many other facets to communication “quality” [26, 27, 41]. Indeed, content messages are some are of most important quality indicators [26, 27], but had to be removed from this manuscript because the combined analysis was too long for a single journal article. We want to emphasize for providers, that even if they explain jargon, assess understanding, talk about the potential for emotion, and begin with an outline and end with a summary – even then, there still may be a long way to go to achieve high-quality communication. As with more traditional quality measures, the inclusion of these behaviors can be taken to “indicate” high-quality communication.
4.2 Conclusions
The implications of these communication problems are troubling. Since many psychosocial complications after NBS are a result of misconceptions or emotional distress [7, 70-73], then NBS may be less safe than it needs to be. Where some bioethicists might interpret our findings as an argument against the viability of genetic NBS, however, we see an opportunity for risk management. As we wrote in 2003, straightforward techniques like those in this study may be used to manage psychosocial risk, so that NBS can result in “more good than harm” over entire populations [74]. High quality physician communication may be an effective technique to manage the risk of psychosocial complication after newborn screening. With an effective way to manage the psychosocial risk, by measuring and improving the quality of physician communication, researchers can continue to pursue future innovations in newborn screening. Further research is testing how targeted feedback tools adapted from Quality Improvement can be used at a lower cost than would be needed for traditional training programs [29, 75]. If psychological problems and misconceptions can be addressed after NBS identifies carrier status for SCD and CF, then we have hope for addressing many of the ethical, legal, and social implications of forthcoming genetic tests [76].
4.3 Practice Implications
Helping physicians to improve the quality of their communication may help to improve patient understanding of test results and treatment options, and allow patients to actively participate in their care.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
This project was funded by grants from the National Health Lung and Blood Institute of the National Institutes of Health (R01-HL086691 and K01-HL072530). We acknowledge the contribution of abstractors who worked on the project: Nadia Ahmad, Lisa Bradford, Jenelle Collins, Nadine Desmarais, Kerry Eskra, Alison La Pean, Faith O’Tool, Justin Riederer and Sara Roedl. We also gratefully acknowledge the collaboration of the Wisconsin State Laboratory of Hygiene and the Wisconsin Newborn Screening Laboratory.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Allen DB, Farrell PM. Newborn screening: principles and practice. Adv Pediatr. 1996;43:231–70. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Pass K, et al. Update: newborn screening for sickle cell disease--California, Illinois, and New York, 1998. MMWR. 2000;49(32):729–31. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bobadilla JL, Farrell MH, Farrell PM. Applying CFTR molecular genetics to facilitate the diagnosis of cystic fibrosis through screening. Adv Pediatr. 2002;49:131–90. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Farrell PM, et al. Acquisition of Pseudomonas aeruginosa in children with cystic fibrosis. Pediatrics. 1997;100(5):E2. doi: 10.1542/peds.100.5.e2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Farrell PM, et al. Early diagnosis of cystic fibrosis through neonatal screening prevents severe malnutrition and improves long-term growth. Wisconsin Cystic Fibrosis Neonatal Screening Study Group. Pediatrics. 2001;107(1):1–13. doi: 10.1542/peds.107.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.National Newborn Screening and Genetics Resource Center [cited 2011 October 26];National Newborn Screening Information System (NNSIS) 2008 report and ongoing reports from 2010. 2010 Available from: http://nnsis.uthscsa.edu/xreports.aspx?XREPORTID=10&FORMID=13&FCLR=1.
- 7.Collins JL, et al. Factors that Influence Parents’ Experiences with Results Disclosure after Newborn Screening Identifies Genetic Carrier Status for Cystic Fibrosis or Sickle Cell Hemoglobinopathy. Patient Education and Counseling. 2012 Jan 11; doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2011.12.007. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kai J, et al. Communication of carrier status information following universal newborn screening for sickle cell disorders and cystic fibrosis: qualitative study of experience and practice. Health Technol Assess. 2009;13(57):1–82. iii. doi: 10.3310/hta13570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hampton ML, et al. Sickle cell “nondisease”. A potentially serious public health problem. Am J Dis Child. 1974;128(1):58–61. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Hosick MB. Protocol decided for sickle cell testing. The NCAA News. 2010 [Google Scholar]
- 11.Stein R. Washington Post. Washington: 2010. Colleges mandate sickle cell testing. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Thomas K, Zarda B. The New York Times. New York: 2010. N.C.A.A., question of bias over a test for a genetic trait. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Roter DL, et al. Improving physicians’ interviewing skills and reducing patients’ emotional distress. A randomized clinical trial. Arch Intern Med. 1995;155(17):1877–84. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Tluczek A, Murphy-Orland K, Cavangh L. Psychosocial Consequences of False-Positive Newborn Screens for Cystic Fibrosis: A Relational Family Systems Perspective. Qualitative Health Research. 2010;21(2):174–186. doi: 10.1177/1049732310382919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Farrell MH, Certain L, Farrell PM. Genetic counseling and risk communication services of newborn screening programs. Arch Ped Adol Med. 2001;155(2):120–6. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.155.2.120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Ciske D, et al. Genetic counseling and neonatal screening for cystic fibrosis: an assessment of the communication process. Pediatrics. 2001;107(4):699–705. doi: 10.1542/peds.107.4.699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Ross LF, Moon MR. Ethical issues in genetic testing of children. Arch Ped Adol Med. 2000;154(9):873–9. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.154.9.873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Fost N, Kaback NM. Why do sickle screening in children? Pediatrics. 1973;51(4):742–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.McNeil TF, et al. Identifying children at high somatic risk: long-term effects on mother-child interaction. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1986;74(6):555–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1986.tb06284.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Swerts A. Impact of genetic counseling and prenatal diagnosis for Down syndrome and neural tube defects. Birth Defects: Original Article Series. 1987;23(2):61–83. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Farrell MH, Kuruvilla P. Assessment of parental understanding by pediatric residents during counseling after newborn genetic screening. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2008;162(3):199–204. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2007.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Farrell MH, et al. A method to quantify and compare clinicians’ assessments of patient understanding during counseling of standardized patients. Patient Educ Couns. 2009;77(1):128–35. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2009.03.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Deuster L, et al. A Method to Quantify Residents’ Jargon Use During Counseling of Standardized Patients About Cancer Screening. J Gen Intern Med. 2008;23(12):1947–52. doi: 10.1007/s11606-008-0729-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Farrell MH, et al. Pediatric residents’ use of jargon during counseling about newborn genetic screening results. Pediatrics. 2008;122(2):243–9. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-2160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Farrell MH, et al. Child Health Providers’ Precautionary Discussion of Emotions during Communication about Results of Newborn Genetic Screening. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166(1):62–67. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Farrell MH, La Pean A, Ladouceur L. Content of communication by pediatric residents after newborn genetic screening. Pediatrics. 2005;116(6):1492–8. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-2611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.La Pean A, Farrell MH. Initially misleading communication of carrier results after newborn genetic screening. Pediatrics. 2005;116(6):1499–505. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-0449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Christopher SA, et al. A method to assess the organizing behaviors used in physicians’ counseling of parents. 2011. Submitted manuscript. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 29.La Pean A, et al. A Qualitative Secondary Evaluation of Statewide Follow-Up Interviews for Abnormal Newborn Screening Results for Cystic Fibrosis and Sickle Cell Hemoglobinopathy. Genet Med. 2011 Sep 13; doi: 10.1038/gim.0b013e31822dd7b8. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Farrell MH, et al. Improving communication between doctors and parents after newborn screening. Wisconsin Medical Journal. 2011;110(5):221–227. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Farrell MH, et al. A structured implicit abstraction method to evaluate whether content of counseling before prostate cancer screening is consistent with recommendations by experts. Patient Educ Couns. 2009;77(3):322–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2009.09.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Norman GR, Tugwell P, Feightner JW. A comparison of resident performance on real and simulated patients. Journal of Medical Education. 1982;57:708–715. doi: 10.1097/00001888-198209000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rethans J-J, et al. Six formats in simulated and standardized patients use, based on experiences of 13 undergraduate medical curricula in Belgium and the Netherlands. Medical Teacher. 2012;34(9):710–6. doi: 10.3109/0142159X.2012.708466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lane C, Rollnick S. The use of simulated patients and role-play in communication skills training: a review of the literature to August 2005. Patient Education & Counseling. 2007;67(1-2):13–20. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2007.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Whelan GP, et al. Scoring standardized patient examinations: lessons learned from the development and administration of the ECFMG Clinical Skills Assessment (CSA) Med Teach. 2005;27(3):200–6. doi: 10.1080/01421590500126296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Dudley F. The Simulated Patient Handbook: A Comprehensive Guide for Facilitators and Simulated Patients. Radcliffe Medical Press; London: 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Comeau AM, et al. Guidelines for implementation of cystic fibrosis newborn screening programs: Cystic Fibrosis Foundation workshop report. Pediatrics. 2007;119(2):e495–518. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wisconsin State Laboratory of Hygiene Health Professionals Guide to Newborn Screening, Cystic Fibrosis. 2011 Mar 14; Available from: http://www.slh.wisc.edu/newborn/guide/cystic_fibrosis.dot.
- 39.Christopher S, Collins J, Farrell M. Effort required to contact primary care providers after newborn screening identifies sickle trait. Journal of the National Medical Association. 2011 doi: 10.1016/s0027-9684(15)30219-4. [accepted manuscript] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Farrell MH, et al. The Brief Standardized Communication Assessment: A Patient Simulation Method Feasible for Population-Scale Use in Communication Quality Assurance. Medical Encounter. 2009;23(1):64. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Mainz J. Defining and Classifying Clinical Indicators for Quality Improvement. Internat J Qual Health Care. 2003;15(6):523–30. doi: 10.1093/intqhc/mzg081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Coulehan JL, Block MR. The medical interview : mastering skills for clinical practice. 5th ed xix. F.A. Davis Co.; Philadelphia: 2006. p. 409. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Roter DL, Hall JA, editors. Doctors Talking with Patients, Patients Talking with Doctors: Improving Communication in Medical Visits. Auburn House; Westport, Connecticut: 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Makoul G. Essential elements of communication in medical encounters: the Kalamazoo consensus statement. Acad Med. 2001;76(4):390–3. doi: 10.1097/00001888-200104000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Smith RC, et al. Evidence-based guidelines for teaching patient-centered interviewing. Patient Educ Couns. 2000;39(1):27–36. doi: 10.1016/s0738-3991(99)00088-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Braddock CH, 3rd, et al. Informed decision making in outpatient practice: time to get back to basics. JAMA. 1999;282(24):2313–20. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.24.2313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Feinstein A. Clinical Epidemiology: The Architecture of Clinical Research. WB Saunders; Philadelphia: 1985. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Farrell MH, La Pean A, Christopher SA. Content messages included by primary care providers in counseling about newborn screening results. 2012. Manuscript in preparation.
- 49.Bensing JM, Kerssens JJ, van der Pasch M. Patient-directed gaze as a tool for discovering and handling psychosocial problems in general practice. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 1995;19(4):223–242. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Seel N. Mental Models in Learning Situations. Advances in Psychology. 2006;138:85–110. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Quill TE. Barriers to effective communication. In: Lipkin M, Putnam SM, Lazare A, editors. The Medical Interview: Clinical care, education and research. Springer; New York: 1995. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Johnson TM, Hardt EJ, Kleinman A. Cultural factors in the medical interview. In: Lipkin M, Putnam SM, Lazare A, editors. The Medical Interview: Critical care, education and research. Springer; New York: 1995. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Lucas S. The Art of Public Speaking. 8th ed McGraw Hill; Boston: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Thompson EC. An Experimental Investigation of the Relative Effectiveness of Organizational Structure in Oral Communication. Southern Speech Journal. 1960;26:59–69. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Titsworth BS. The Effects Teacher Immediacy, Use of Organizational Lecture Cues, and Students’ Notetaking on Cognitive Learning. Communication Education. 2001;50(4):283–297. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Chesebro JJ, McCroskey James C. The Relationship of Teacher Clarity and Immediacy with Student State Receiver Apprehension, Affect, and Cognitive Learning. Communication Education. 2001;50(1):59–68. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Morgan M, et al., editors. Risk Communication: A Mental Models Approach. Cambridge Univ Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Christopher S, et al. A method to assess the use of organizing behaviors in physicians’ conversation. Medical Encounter. 2010;24(3):224. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Clark NM, et al. The clinician-patient partnership paradigm: outcomes associated with physician communication behavior. Clinical Pediatrics. 2008;47(1):49–57. doi: 10.1177/0009922807305650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Clever SL, et al. Does doctor-patient communication affect patient satisfaction with hospital care? Results of an analysis with a novel instrument variable. HSR: Health Services Research. 2008;43(5):1505–1519. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6773.2008.00849.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Ong LM, et al. Doctor-Patient Communication: a Review of the Literature. Soc Sci Med. 1995;40(7):903–18. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)00155-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Stewart M, et al. The Impact of Patient-Centered Care on Outcomes. Journal of Family Practice. 2000;49(9):796–804. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Cegala DJ, Marinelli T, Post D. The effects of patient communication skills training on compliance. Arch Fam Med. 2000;9(1):57–64. doi: 10.1001/archfami.9.1.57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Kaplan SH, Greenfield S, Ware JE., Jr. Assessing the effects of physician-patient interactions on the outcomes of chronic disease. Med Care. 1989;27(3 Suppl):S110–27. doi: 10.1097/00005650-198903001-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Miller GE. The assessment of clinical skills/competence/performance. Acad Med. 1990;65(9 Suppl):S63–7. doi: 10.1097/00001888-199009000-00045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Roethlisberger FJ, Dickson WK. Management and the worker. Harvard University Press; Cambridge, MA: 1939. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Sanson-Fisher RW, Poole AD. Simulated patient and the assessment of medical students’ interpersonal skills. Medical Education. 1980;14:249–253. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2923.1980.tb02269.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Donabedian A. An introduction to quality assurance in health care. Oxford University Press; New York: 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 69.Jencks SF, Huff ED, Cuerdon T. Change in the Quality of Care Delivered to Medicare Beneficiaries, 1998-1999 to 2000-2001. JAMA. 2003;289(3):305–12. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.3.305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Tluczek A, et al. Psychosocial risk associated with newborn screening for cystic fibrosis: parents’ experience while awaiting the sweat-test appointment. Pediatrics. 2005;115(6):1692–703. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Tluczek A, et al. Parents’ knowledge of neonatal screening and response to false-positive cystic fibrosis testing. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1992;13(3):181–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Tluczek A, et al. Psychological impact of false-positive results when screening for cystic fibrosis. Pediatr Pulmonol Suppl. 1991;7:29–37. doi: 10.1002/ppul.1950110707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Mischler EH, et al. Cystic fibrosis newborn screening: impact on reproductive behavior and implications for genetic counseling. Pediatrics. 1998;102(1 Pt 1):44–52. doi: 10.1542/peds.102.1.44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Farrell MH, Farrell PM. Newborn screening for cystic fibrosis: ensuring more good than harm. J Pediatr. 2003;143(6):707–12. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2003.09.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Farrell M, et al. A rapid-throughput feedback intervention using a record card for population-scale communication quality assurance. 2012. manuscript in preparation.
- 76.Burke W, et al. Genetic screening. Epidemiol Rev. 2011;33(1):148–64. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxr008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]