Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Feb 8.
Published in final edited form as: J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2012 Apr 3;55(6):1600–1612. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/10-0153)

Cognitive, Environmental, and Linguistic Predictors of Syntax in Fragile X Syndrome and Down Syndrome

Bruno Estigarribia 1,2,3, Gary E Martin 4,5, Joanne E Roberts 4,5,6,+
PMCID: PMC3567480  NIHMSID: NIHMS385796  PMID: 22473836

Abstract

Purpose

We examined which cognitive, environmental, and speech/language variables predict expressive syntax in boys with fragile X syndrome (FXS), Down syndrome (DS), and typical development (TD), and whether predictive relationships differed by group.

Method

We obtained Index of Productive Syntax scores for 18 boys with FXS only, 20 boys with both FXS and autism spectrum disorder, 27 boys with DS, and 25 younger TD boys of similar nonverbal mental age (MA). Predictors included group (diagnosis), nonverbal cognition, phonological working memory (PWM), maternal education, speech intelligibility, and expressive vocabulary. We addressed the research questions via hierarchical linear regression.

Results

Diagnostic group, nonverbal cognition, and PWM predicted 56% of the variance in syntactic ability, with approximately three-fourths of the predicted variance explained by group membership alone. The other factors did not contribute any additional significant variance in this final model. There was no evidence that predictor effects differed by group.

Conclusions

Nonverbal cognition and PWM have an effect on expressive syntax beyond that of diagnostic group. These effects are estimated to be the same in FXS, DS, and TD. We discuss explanations for residual variance and the relative role of different predictors.

Keywords: Fragile X Syndrome, Down Syndrome, Syntax, Phonological Working Memory, Modularity, Autism Spectrum Disorders

Cognitive, Environmental, and Linguistic Predictors of Syntax in Fragile X Syndrome and Down Syndrome

Syntax is particularly vulnerable in many developmental disorders (Down syndrome (DS): Abbeduto & Chapman, 2005; SLI: Rice et al., 2004; high-functioning autism: Roberts et al., 2004). Of note, syntactic difficulties have an enormous impact on the daily living of individuals with disabilities (Hewitt, Hinkle, & Miccio, 2005). Recent studies have demonstrated weaknesses in expressive syntax in fragile X syndrome (FXS) as well (Estigarribia, Roberts, Price, & Sideris, 2010; Roberts, Hennon, Price, Dear, Anderson, & Vandergrift, 2007; Price, Roberts, Hennon, Berni, Anderson, & Sideris, 2008). Although the average syntactic performance of boys with FXS might be delayed, there is still enormous individual variability in syntactic skills. Understanding which factors underlie this variability is crucial. In fact, one of the main challenges to an etiology-specific approach to intervention is the lack of understanding of individual variation in the development and outcome of language phenotypes both within and across disabilities (Fidler et al., 2007). A predictor model is a first step from descriptive models of impairment to positing explanatory mechanisms of impairment that could potentially be targeted in intervention. The present study has adopted this analytic approach.

Accordingly, we investigate which cognitive, social-environmental, and speech-linguistic factors predict individual variability and syntactic skill in FXS. We consider predictors that have been shown or hypothesized to be related to language level, or to syntactic level in particular: nonverbal cognition, phonological working memory (PWM), maternal education, speech intelligibility, and expressive vocabulary. Additionally, we examine differences in syntax predictors between boys with FXS, DS, and typically developing boys (TD) to clarify whether impaired systems in DS and FXS result from deviant mechanisms underlying syntactic development and whether patterns of association differ according to diagnosis or are common across intellectual disabilities (ID). Differences in predictors would suggest mechanisms of impairment different from typical pathways or even specific to each disorder. Conversely, if the same predictors are significant in all groups, this would support a classic delay scenario, where lower performance on other aspects of cognition/language, or particular combinations of environmental factors, if present in TD boys, would entail similar lower levels of skill.

Syntax in FXS and DS

Young children with FXS produce shorter sentences, fewer grammatical morphemes, and a narrower range of syntactic constructions than mental age or mean length of utterance (MLU) matches (Estigarribia et al., 2010; Levy et al., 2006; Paul et al., 1984; Price et al. 2008; Roberts, Hennon, et al. 2007).

Likewise, considerable evidence points to syntax delays in DS. Both receptive and expressive syntax are at lower levels than expected for nonverbal cognitive ability, vocabulary, or MLU (Abbeduto et al., 2003; Berglund & Eriksson, 2000; Berglund, Eriksson, & Johansson, 2001; Caselli et al., 2008; Chapman et al., 1991, 1998; Eadie et al., 2002; Hesketh & Chapman, 1998; Iverson et al., 2003; Laws & Bishop, 2003; Price et al., 2007, 2008; Rutter & Buckley, 1994). In fact, young individuals with DS demonstrate even more syntactic delay than do those with FXS (Finestack & Abbeduto, 2010; Price et al., 2008).

Cognitive profile of FXS and DS

FXS

Approximately 80% of males with FXS have ID, with most in the moderate to severe range (Dykens, Hodapp, Ort, Finucane, Shapiro, & Leckman, 1989). Of interest, nonverbal mental age has been consistently found to predict expressive and receptive language in FXS (Abbeduto & Chapman, 2005; Roberts, Mirrett, & Burchinal, 2001), and language production in children with DS (e.g., Chapman, Seung, Schwartz, & Kay-Raining Bird, 2000). Other important cognitive deficits in FXS include weaknesses in working memory, and inhibitory control and attention (Baker, Hooper, Skinner, Hatton, Schaaf, Ornstein, et al., 2011; Cornish, Turk, & Hagerman, 2008; Freund & Reiss, 1991; Hodapp, Dykens, Ort, Zelinsky, & Leckman, 1991; Hooper, Hatton, Sideris, Sullivan, Hammer, Schaaf, et al., 2008; Ornstein, Schaaf, Hooper, Hatton, & Mirrett, 2008; Sullivan, Hatton, Hammer, Sideris, Hooper, Ornstein, et al., 2007). All these factors have been suggested to impact language development and processing in FXS (Abbeduto et al., 2005; Cornish et al., 2008; Roberts, Hennon, et al. 2007; Roberts, Price, et al. 2007; Price et al, 2007), but no studies to date have investigated these hypothesized links.

DS

Individuals with DS have ID ranging from mild to severe, with around 80% of individuals in the moderate range, and some with IQ scores in the average range (Pueschel, 1995; Roizen, 2007). Verbal short-term memory impairments are well-documented in DS (Jarrold & Baddeley, 2001; Laws, 2002) and predict concurrent difficulties in syntactic production in individuals with DS ages 5–20 (Chapman et al., 2002). Weaknesses in auditory or phonological working memory have been proposed as an explanation of language deficits in DS (Jarrold, Hewes, & Baddeley, 2000; Laws, 2004; Tager-Flusberg, 2006). Still, further research on the relationship between short-term memory and language in DS and other populations is needed (Chapman et al., 2002; Hick, Botting, & Conti-Ramsden, 2005), especially studies that control for other variables that might have an impact at the syntactic level.

Speech and expressive language in FXS and DS

Speech intelligibility is an area of concern in FXS (Barnes et al., 2009). Expressive vocabulary is also delayed in FXS, but its relationship with concurrent syntactic delays is unclear (Roberts, Hennon, et al., 2007).

Expressive language is an area of particular weakness in DS as well (Martin, Klusek, Estigarribia, & Roberts, 2009), and is more impaired than receptive skills in young individuals (Caselli, Vicari, Longobardi, Lami, Pizzoli, & Stella, 1998; Chapman, Hesketh, & Kistler, 2002; Laws & Bishop, 2003). Young individuals with FXS outperformed those with DS on a global measure of expressive language in one recent study (Finestack & Abbeduto, 2010). Expressive vocabulary is delayed with respect to mental age (Caselli et al., 2008; Chapman et al., 1998; Hick et al., 2005; Roberts, Price et al., 2007). Intelligibility is also lower than for mental age-matched TD children (Barnes et al., 2009; Chapman, 1997; Chapman & Hesketh, 2000; Chapman, Seung, Schwartz, & Kay-Raining Bird, 1998; Roberts et al., 2005) but not different from that seen in FXS according to one study (Barnes et al., 2009). Intelligibility can affect syntax multiply: (a) by hindering children’s monitoring of their own speech, potentially limiting feedback and practice in acquisition; and (b) by “masking” underlying syntactic skills––more complex syntactic structures are more likely to be impacted by unintelligibility. In fact, Bray & Woolnough (1988) found children with DS to be less intelligible in more complex syntactic structures than in simple structures. Syntactic simplification, then, might be an adaptive skill to compensate for unintelligibility (Martin et al., 2009).

In sum, boys with DS or FXS have mild to severe levels of ID, deficits in verbal short-term or phonological working memory, reduced intelligibility, and general expressive language deficits, including deficits in vocabulary and syntax. Whether these similar cognitive backgrounds affect syntactic skill equally in both populations is at present unknown.

Social-environmental factors in FXS and DS

The importance of environmental factors in typical language development is well established (e.g., Dollaghan et al., 1999; Gallaway & Richards, 1994; Hoff-Ginsberg, 1991). In FXS, higher quality home environments are associated with higher IQs and fewer autistic features (Dyer-Friedman et al., 2002; Glaser et al., 2003). High maternal responsivity, in particular, is associated with better expressive language in toddlers with FXS (Warren et al., 2010), and preschoolers with autism (Siller & Sigman, 2002, 2008).

Several studies have highlighted positive relationships between more organized home environments and higher maternal education, and the developmental scores of young children with DS (Piper & Ramsay, 1980; Sharav, Collins, Shlomo, 1985). However, Chapman et al. (2000) found maternal education did not predict expressive syntax in DS beyond cognitive and age variables.

Autism in FXS

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is present in one-half to three quarters of males with FXS, with boys with FXS and ASD showing lower language levels than boys with FXS alone (Bailey et al., 2001; Clifford et al., 2007; Hall et al., 2008; Kaufmann et al., 2004; Philofsky et al., 2004; Roberts, Martin, Moskowitz, Harris, Foreman, & Nelson, 2007; Rogers et al., 2001). Notably, morphosyntactic impairments have been detected in individuals with idiopathic autism (Landa & Goldberg 2005; Roberts et al., 2004). Recent studies show contradictory evidence as to the existence of syntactic differences when boys with both FXS and ASD are examined as a group separate from boys with only FXS (FXS-O) (Estigarribia et al., in press; Price et al., 2008). We will analyze these two groups separately since it is possible that group differences in syntax predictors exist, even though the outcome levels in the groups may not differ.

Study questions

This study aims to answer the following questions:

  1. Which cognitive, environmental, and speech/language factors predict syntactic skill in FXS and DS?

  2. Are there differences between the FXS groups (with and without ASD), the DS group, and the TD group in these predictive relationships?

Method

Ninety-eight boys participated in this study as part of a larger longitudinal investigation of speech and language in school-age boys with fragile X syndrome FXS, DS, and TD (Roberts, Price, et al., 2007). This study examines concurrent predictors of expressive syntax at baseline during the first year (i.e., all measures come from the first data collection session). These baseline findings are the first step in investigating possible changes in syntax predictors over time. There were 33 boys with fragile X syndrome only (FXS-O, mean age 10.56, SD 2.41), 28 boys with fragile X and autism spectrum disorder (FXS-ASD, mean age 9.98, SD 3.04), 31 boys with DS (mean age 10.19, SD 2.80), and 46 TD boys (mean age 5.03, SD 1.18). The TD group was recruited to establish differences between FXS and TD at similar mental age stages, and the DS group was recruited to determine which language characteristics were common in both groups with ID as opposed to specific to each syndrome.

Participants came from English-speaking homes, and were at or beyond Brown’s (1973) Stage III (MLU 2.5–3.0). All boys with FXS had full mutation. ASD was excluded from the DS and TD groups if children scored above ADOS (Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, and Risi 2001) cutoffs for “spectrum.” Boys were excluded if their average hearing threshold was greater than 25 dB HL in the better ear (screened across 500, 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz using a Grason Stadler GSI 16 or 17, or MAICO MA 40 audiometer). Participants’ ethnic distribution was 81% white (non-Hispanic), 14% African-American, 4% Hispanic, 1% Asian-American, and 1% Native American. Ethnicity did not vary significantly with group. The data do not include females because FXS is an X-linked disorder with a considerably higher incidence among males, and the degree of ID and developmental consequences for males are substantially greater. The other groups exclude females to preserve gender-matching. Boys with DS were screened for FXS and vice versa using a project-developed parent questionnaire, as were TD boys for FXS or DS.

Group matching

The FXS, DS, and TD groups were not significantly different on nonverbal mental age determined by Leiter-R Age Equivalents (Roid & Miller, 1997). The mean mental age for the FXS-O group was 62.48 months (SD=7.75); for the FXS-ASD group, 62.45 months (SD = 7.38); for the DS group, 65.27 (SD=11.56); and for the TD group, 59.74 months (SD=9.91). Group differences in mean mental age were not significant, F(3,94) = 1.44, p = .23. Although Mervis and Robinson (2003) recommend that groups be considered well-matched only if the p value is equal to or greater than .50, we do not believe our lower p-value is a problem because: a) mental age is a covariate in the analysis; and b) there is substantial overlap in mental age in all groups, making a regression covarying mental age adequate from the point of view of interpretability.

Autism status

We determined autism status in the boys with FXS with the ADOS, a 45-minute series of structured and semi-structured interactions with an examiner that provide contexts to elicit behaviors characteristic of autism. Trained examiners (clinically-trained professionals holding either a B.S. or a Ph.D.) scored videotapes of the ADOS sessions. We administered three modules of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule based on each child’s developmental and language levels. Each child received a categorical score of ‘no autism,’ ‘spectrum,’ or ‘autism’, with the latter two scores combined to form a single group (FXS-ASD). To be classified in the FXS-ASD group, each child had to meet the autism spectrum cutoff score for each subtotal: Communication and Social, and the autism spectrum cutoff for the Communication + Social total. For Module 1, communication cutoff is two or higher, social interaction four or higher, and total seven or higher. For Module 2, the respective cutoffs are three, four, and eight. For Module 3, they are two, four, and seven. A second examiner scored 16% of the interactions, with reliability equaling .89 for the individual items (range .83 to .96) and .93 for diagnosis (range .81 to 1.00).

Data collection

Depending on family preference, we tested children in their homes, at school, or at the Frank Porter Graham (FPG) Child Development Institute at UNC-Chapel Hill. Sessions were audiotaped with a portable Digital Auditory Tape TASCAM (DA-P1) recorder and a Shure WBH headset microphone system. Videotaping was done using a Sony Digital8 videocamera (DCR-TVR 27).

Conversational sampling procedures

Trained research assistants transcribed 100 usable spontaneous child utterances from videotapes of ADOS sessions using CLAN software and CHAT conventions (MacWhinney, 2000). This is not substantially different from the common practice of eliciting language samples using age-appropriate toys or prompts, except that the semi-structured character of the ADOS interview (a) facilitates language elicitation from children with ID, and (b) adds a measure of consistency in the sampling context for all participants. The ADOS has recently been recommended especially for collecting natural language samples (Tager-Flusberg, Rogers, Cooper, Landa, Lord, Paul, et al., 2009). Partially or fully unintelligible utterances, exact self-repetitions, parts of routines (e.g., reciting the alphabet, songs), exact imitations of examiner utterances, and yes/no responses to questions were excluded from analysis. Utterances with more than two independent clauses conjoined by and were split at the second conjunction, following SALT guidelines (Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts; Miller & Chapman, 1996) to avoid higher MLUs due to clausal chaining. A second researcher with transcription training verified and corrected all original transcripts from audiotapes.

Transcription reliability

A third researcher independently verified via audiotape a randomly selected 19% (i.e., four boys with FXS-O, five boys with FXS-ASD, five boys with DS, and five TD boys) of the original transcripts for reliability. Morpheme-to-morpheme agreement (including agreement on unintelligible segments) between the second and third researchers was 91% overall, 85% for the FXS-O group, 85% the FXS-ASD group, 90% for the DS group, and 95% for the TD group.

Syntax outcome variable

We measured expressive syntax via the Index of Productive Syntax (IPSyn: Scarborough, 1990). The IPSyn measures the range of different syntactic and morphological constructions in language samples from children in the preschool age range. It has proven useful in investigating syntactic skill in typical children, as well as in children with disabilities, including FXS, DS, and autism (Condouris, Meyer, & Tager-Flusberg, 2003; Hewitt, Hammer, Yont, & Tomblin, 2005; Oetting, Cantrell, & Horohov, 1999; Price et al., 2008; Roberts, Hennon et al., 2007; Scarborough, Rescorla, Tager-Flusberg, Fowler, Sudhalter, 1991; Sudhalter, Scarborough, & Cohen, 1991). Two trained research assistants, blind to diagnosis, coded all transcripts with very high reliability (Shrout & Fleiss’ (1979) intraclass correlation coefficient (fixed set) = .99, Cohen’s kappa = .65). The first author subsequently verified all transcripts to resolve discrepancies.

Predictor variables

Nonverbal cognition

We computed age equivalent scores from the Leiter International Performance Scale-Revised (Leiter-R; Roid and Miller 1997) to measure nonverbal cognition. We administered four subtests: Figure Ground (identifying figures or designs within a complex stimulus), Form Completion (recognizing an object from its fragmented parts), Sequential Order (selection of the next item in a logical sequence of items), and Repeated Patterns (supplying the missing portion of a repeated pattern). We used age equivalents, following previous research (Chapman et al., 2002). However, being cognizant of the limitations of age equivalent scores (Maloney and Larrivee 2007), we ran the exact same analyses using raw scores, with results completely unchanged.

Phonological working memory

PWM was measured with the raw scores from the Nonword Repetition Task of the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP: Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999). Raw scores were used because standard scores were at floor for many of the participants with intellectual disability (ID).

Maternal education

Total years of education completed by participants’ primary caregivers (all mothers in our sample) at the time of enrollment were used as a proxy for the effect of environmental and family factors on verbal and nonverbal skills, since studies have shown that higher levels of maternal education correlate with more advanced speech and language development in TD children and children with developmental disabilities (Campbell et al. 2003, Dollaghan et al. 1999).

Speech intelligibility

The proportion of completely intelligible utterances was computed using CLAN by dividing the total number of completely intelligible utterances by the total number of utterances in the transcript (i.e., fully intelligible plus partially or fully unintelligible utterances).

Expressive vocabulary

Expressive vocabulary was measured with age equivalent scores from the Expressive Vocabulary Test (EVT: Williams, 1997). In this test, the individual is asked to label a picture or give a synonym for the word provided by the examiner that also labels the picture.

Hierarchical regression

Our goal was to investigate the successive contributions to explaining variance in expressive syntax of diagnosis, cognitive factors, social-environmental factors, and speech-language factors, in that order. This can be accomplished with sequential regression, where an order of entry of predictor variables is specified a priori, or with statistical regression, which selects the predictors that explain more variance first, and continues doing this for the remaining predictors at each step (Tabachnik & Fidell, 2007). We employed sequential regression to avoid capitalizing on the particular characteristics of our sample (i.e., “overfitting” the data). The order of entry was decided a priori, taking into consideration the relative timing of the effects. Clearly, the “genetic” effect, represented by diagnosis (DS/FXS, or no ID), is temporally prior to all the rest. We assumed, furthermore, that cognitive effects (nonverbal cognition, PWM) are prior to social-environmental and to speech-language effects, and that social-environmental effects are prior to speech-language effects. Of course, this ordering is arguable.

Out of the original pool of 138 subjects, 40 had to be excluded because they had missing nonword repetition (PWM) scores (4 boys with FXS-O, 8 with FXS-ASD, 9 with DS, and 19 TD). An additional 8 (2 from each group) were excluded because their nonword repetition task was scored by a trainee who did not meet reliability criteria. These 48 subjects were included in a post hoc analysis using multiple imputation that we report for completeness at the end of the results section. There remained 18 boys in the FXS-O group, 20 in the FXS-ASD group, 27 in the DS group, and 25 in the TD group, for the regression reported here.

Results

Descriptive statistics for predictor and outcome variables

Figure 1 shows the distribution of scores for the 5 continuous predictors and the outcome variable. There is a slight concern about the lack of overlap in the intelligibility scores, which should be kept in mind when interpreting the model. The groups do not differ significantly in nonverbal mental age, F(3,86) = 1.30, p = .28, or expressive vocabulary, F(3,86) = .46, p = .71 (Both of these are age equivalents.) There are group differences in PWM, F(3,86) = 3.69, p = .01, maternal education, F(3,86) = 7.91, p = .0001, intelligibility, F(3,86) = 17.03, p < .0001, and syntax (unadjusted scores), F(3,86) = 18.26, p < .0001.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Descriptive plots for all variables in the full model (with mean values provided and standard deviations indicated by bars)

Table 1 shows the results of the hierarchical linear regression model. The table uses sequential sums of squares. The first step was a regression of IPSyn scores on diagnostic group. One outlier in the FXS group was identified via examination of residuals and normality plots, and removed. Although removal of this data point did not change the model estimates or significance tests for individual variables appreciably, it had a small influence on the total explained variance. As we expected it, the effect of group was significant, F(3,85) = 20.08, p < .0001. This predictor alone accounted for 41.5% of the variance in IPSyn scores.

Table 1.

Coefficients and significance tests for the hierarchical linear regression (sequential sums of squares).

Model IVs df1 df2 F p R2 R2 change F change Sig F
change
1 Group 3 85 20.08 <.0001 .415 -- -- --

2 Group 3 83 26.32 <.0001 .564 .149 14.21 <.0001
Mental Age 1 83 14.51 .0003
PWM 1 83 13.91 .0004

3 Group 3 82 26.09 <.0001 .565 .001 .26 .61
Mental Age 1 82 14.38 .0003
PWM 1 82 13.78 .0004
Mat Ed 1 82 0.26 .61

4 Group 3 80 27.27 <.0001 .594 .029 2.85 .06
Mental Age 1 80 15.03 .0002
PWM 1 80 14.41 .0003
Mat Ed 1 80 0.28 .60
Intelligibility 1 80 4.11 .046
Exp Vocab 1 80 1.60 .21

We examined interactions of all predictors with group in all steps following the first. They were never significant and were not retained in the models. Step 2 showed significant main effects of nonverbal mental age, F(1,83) = 14.51, p = .0003, and PWM, F(1,83) = 13.91, p = .0004. The contribution to explained IPSyn variance of these two variables combined was 14.9% beyond the variance explained by group. This change in R2 was significant, Fchange(2,83) = 14.21, p < .0001. This model accounted for 56.4% IPSyn variance.

Step 3 found no significant main effect of maternal education, after group, nonverbal mental age, and PWM are taken into account, F(1,82) = .26, p = .61. Step 4, finally, showed a main effect of intelligibility, F(1,80) = 4.11, p = .046, but not of expressive vocabulary, F(1,80) = 1.60, p = .21. These two variables combined explained an additional 2.9% IPSyn variance. This change, however, was just short of significance, Fchange(2,80) = 2.85, p = .06. This last model accounted for a total of 59.4% variance in syntax scores.

Therefore, for prediction, we retained model 2, the last model for which adding variables increased the amount of explained variance significantly. The estimated effects for covariates and model-predicted means (outlier included, full dataset) are shown in table 2. After Tukey’s Honestly Significant Difference adjustment for multiple comparisons, the boys in the TD group scored significantly higher than those in the three groups with ID (diffTD-DS = 15.39, t = 5.89, p < .0001, Cohen’s d = 1.68; diffTD-FXS.O = 17.06, t = 6.04, p < .0001, Cohen’s d = 1.87; diffTD-FXS.ASD = 17.19, t = 5.94, p < .0001, Cohen’s d = 1.88), with very large effect sizes. There were no significant differences between the three groups with ID. This model predicted 56.4% of the variance in IPSyn scores.

Table 2.

Regression coefficients and model-predicted means from retained model (model 2).Means with different superscripts are pairwise significantly different after adjustment for multiple comparisons.

Estimate SE t p
Intercept 88.20 1.89 46.76 < .0001
DS −15.39 2.61 −5.89 < .0001
FXS-O −17.06 2.83 −6.04 < .0001
FXS-ASD −17.19 2.89 −5.94 < .0001
Nonverbal Mental Age 0.25 0.11 2.31 .02
Nonword Repetition 1.49 0.40 3.73 .0003
Model Residual Standard
Error 8.73

Adjusted
Means

TD (n = 25) 88.20a
DS (n = 22) 72.81b
FXS-O (n = 18) 71.14b
FXS-ASD (n = 20) 71.02b

TD: Typically developing

DS: Down syndrome

FXS-O: Fragile X syndrome without autism spectrum disorder

FXS-ASD: Fragile X syndrome with autism spectrum disorder

As explained in the method section, 48 participants had to be excluded due to missing or unreliable nonword repetition scores. Missing data are always a concern, biasing regression coefficients and impacting generalizability. Therefore, we created a dataset with imputed PWM scores (using the scores for the eight excluded children and predictive mean matching for the remaining 40 children). This allowed us to use data from the full sample of 138 subjects. A regression analysis on the imputed dataset showed the same pattern of significance and similar effect sizes for the different factors.

Discussion

This paper provides a model of predictors of expressive syntax in boys with FXS, DS and TD. Although some predictor models have been proposed for DS (Chapman et al., 2002), this is the first such model for FXS. We asked which cognitive, environmental, and speech/language factors predict syntactic skill in FXS. We found that the relative importance of predictors did not differ by group, that is, we found no evidence of deviant pathways of development. Accordingly, in what follows we will discuss a general delay model that applies across all groups. Having FXS or DS affects syntax independently from nonverbal cognition, PWM, and intelligibility level, which are estimated to be the same in TD boys as in the boys with ID. Although we may have been underpowered to detect interactions with group, let us note that we imputed 40 missing PWM scores and the interactions remained non-significant.

We found that diagnostic group, nonverbal cognition, and PWM predicted 56% of the variance in syntactic ability in our sample, with each predictor significant while controlling for all others. Approximately three-fourths of the variance explained was predicted by differences in diagnostic group. This is to be expected, given the documented syntactic impairments in FXS and DS, compared to TD boys of similar nonverbal cognition. However, even after the group effect was accounted for, our cognitive measures explained an additional 15% variance. Nonverbal mental age and PWM had an explanatory effect beyond that of FXS, DS, or typical development. They both contributed unique variance to syntactic scores.

The relationship between working memory and language development is well established in typical populations. Working memory, in particular, has been linked to first and second language syntax learning in typical populations (Ellis & Sinclair, 1996; Williams & Lovatt, 2005), and to the adult processing of long-distance dependencies (e.g., Lewis, Vasishth, & Van Dyke, 2006). Given the fast-paced demands of conversation, higher levels of auditory working memory presumably allow more efficient on-line language processing. In turn, this would facilitate the kind of categorization and co-occurrence analyses needed for syntactic learning. Note that, although the PWM measure we used, nonword repetition, has a phonological/articulatory component and is influenced by lexicon size, an additional post-hoc analysis found 8.4% variance accounted for by PWM when entered last in the model. We are then confident that the effect of PWM is at least partially independent from other speech/language factors. Moreover, the effect of PWM was assessed covarying intelligibility: thus, it was also independent from it.

Beyond diagnostic and cognitive factors, no other factors contributed any significant variance. Although adding intelligibility to our model did not result in a significant increase in explained variance, this factor reaches significance after controlling for all others (Table 1, model 4). This suggests that lower intelligibility may underlie lower syntactic scores due to an artifact of assessment. For example, less complex utterances might be easier to understand, and therefore only simpler structures would be transcribed fully and coded. However, we would like to suggest deeper, causal reasons for this connection. Children with lower intelligibility see their communicative ability compromised (e.g., Chapman et al. 1998), may simplify their syntax to increase their intelligibility (Martin et al., 2009), and may have fewer opportunities for positive and negative feedback from adults, or perhaps adults might use language of lower complexity with them (e.g., Conti-Ramsden, 1990). Either scenario leads to impoverished language input, practice, and possible subsequent language delays. Furthermore, if the ability of unintelligible speakers to monitor their own language productions is reduced, a probable result is an impoverished capacity for learning by matching one’s productions to intended targets (Clark, 1993). These hypothetical connections deserve to be specifically examined in future studies.

We were surprised that our environmental proxy, maternal education, and expressive vocabulary did not have an effect after diagnostic and cognitive variables were included. There is substantial research documenting effects of the environment on children’s language (Dollaghan et al., 1999; Fewell & Deutscher, 2003). Because the total correlation of maternal education with IPSyn scores is not significant in any group (rTD = .04; rDS = .17; rFXS-O = .06; rFXS-ASD = .06), it is unlikely that its effect is merely masked by concurrent predictors. Rather, maternal education just seems a poor proxy for environmental effects, at least in the age ranges studied here. Moreover, SES effects have been demonstrated for younger, typical populations, not older, clinical populations. Finally, maternal education is only one of many factors that define children’s environments (Chapman et al., 2000). Some other dimensions not considered here are family socioeconomic status, amount of language exposure, maternal responsivity.

The lack of significance of expressive vocabulary was also unexpected, since Roberts et al. (2007) had found that between-group differences in syntactic scores disappeared when number of different words in conversation (a type-frequency measure of vocabulary) was controlled for. In this study, the total correlation of EVT with IPSyn was significant in all groups except DS (rTD = .47; rDS = .22; rFXS-O = .50; rFXS-ASD = .53), but so were the correlations of EVT with MA and PWM except for DS (for MA: rTD = .54; rDS = .55; rFXS-O = .50; rFXS-ASD = .76; for PWM: rTD = .56; rDS = .01; rFXS-O = .73; rFXS-ASD = .40). This seems to suggest that the shared variance between EVT and IPSyn is shared also with our cognitive variables. Cognitive level would then (partially) explain both expressive vocabulary and expressive syntax.

We were also interested in assessing between-group differences after adjustment for significant predictors, in an attempt to pin down possible specific syntactic impairments in the populations with ID. Adjusted means showed no difference in IPSyn scores between the three groups with ID, which all scored significantly lower than the TD controls. This is surprising since Price et al. (2008) had found lower IPSyn scores for boys with DS than those for boys with FXS. It is possible, then, that differences in syntax skill are explained away when PWM or intelligibility are covaried (they were not in Price’s study). This would imply that any apparent syndrome-specificity in FXS or DS is not due to syntax but rather to the effect of other cognitive and speech factors which may be differentially affected, supporting again a delay interpretation.

There was a remaining 44% unexplained variance in syntactic skills in the retained model. While we do not intend to claim our final model as theoretically complete, we would still like to interpret this residual variance as indicating possibly syntactic specific differences between children with intellectually disability and typical children, plus some random variation. The issue of whether there is a syntactic impairment independent of cognitive impairments, environmental factors, and interactions with other linguistic variables, is of great theoretical import. It speaks to the issue of modularity, that is, whether syntax (or subdomains thereof) develop and function truly independently from general cognitive and environmental influences. Only by developing more comprehensive and more accurate predictor models will we be able to decide whether the remaining variability is a reflection of the difficulty of measuring syntax in conversation, or related to specific syntactic challenges.

Lastly, the importance of validation of a model with independent samples cannot be overemphasized. We based our hierarchical regression on the following assumptions: that diagnosis could affect all other factors; that cognitive factors could affect social-environmental and speech-language variables; that social-environmental factors could affect speech-language variables; and that these effects were unidirectional. Clearly, the assumption of unidirectional causality is too simplistic, and other scenarios should be considered in future studies. If larger samples were available, researchers could carry out structural equation modeling to test causal hypotheses. However, the gold standard for causation is experimental control of independent variables. One could manipulate phonological working memory availability in an artificial grammar learning study with a TD group and a FXS group to test causation, but it is currently difficult to envisage a methodology that would work in the clinical population.

Strengths and future directions

This study proposes the first predictor model of syntax skills in FXS, and supplements the results of Chapman et al. (2000, 2002) in DS. We collected data from a rather large sample of boys with FXS and DS to examine diagnostic, cognitive, social-environmental, and speech-linguistic variables, all of which were theoretically well founded. This study focuses on males with FXS (as do most) because of the relatively higher severity of impairment, and also because syntactic deficits in FXS have been shown only for boys. We are cognizant of this limitation and are currently collecting language, cognitive, and environmental data from girls affected with FXS for future analyses.

Future work should explore other possible predictors of syntax, for example receptive syntax (or language), since Chapman et al (2000, 2002) demonstrated syntax comprehension was the best (indeed, only) predictor of concurrent syntax production in individuals with DS, aged 5–20. In that study, auditory short-term memory predicted syntax production at study start, as long as comprehension was not in the model. Perhaps comprehension mediates the effect of short-term memory in FXS as well. Other important predictors for FXS are perseveration, hyperarousal, attention, and family SES. Perseveration (excessive repetition of topics, words, or phrases) is common in individuals with FXS (Kau et al., 2002; Levy et al., 2006; Roberts, Martin, et al., 2007, Sudhalter et al., 1990). Hyperarousal is common in FXS and might slow the decay of sentence representations in memory, leading to perseveration (Belser & Sudhalter, 1995). Presumably, an inability to shift from a particular behavior can affect syntactic parsing in conversation and, therefore, syntax learning (Mazuka et al., 2009). Like intelligibility, perseveration can lower test performance by masking skills (Helm-Estabrooks & Albert, 2004). Furthermore, there are known sequential processing, attention, and inhibition deficits in FXS that have potential effects on language development. As for SES, Hoff (2003) showed effects of SES and maternal language on vocabulary development. Lower SES is related to lower syntax complexity for TD children (Huttenlocher et al., 2002). Finally, we included no biomarker or genetic information for FXS (e.g. FMRP/cortisol levels, CGG repeats). Even though excluded variables potentially bias regression estimates, it is impossible to know the impact of these omitted variables (Berry, 1993, Gelman & Hill, 2007).

Finally, to the extent that obtainable sample sizes allow, future models should move towards understanding causation paths in a more complex fashion, perhaps through the examination of possible mediation and moderation scenarios between variables identified in predictor studies.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (1 R01 HD038819, 1 R01 HD044935, 1 R03 HD40640, and 5 T32 HD40127), by an Odum Institute for Research in Social Science Seed Grant to first author, by a UNC University Research Council Grant to first author, and by the Cognitive Science Program at UNC-Chapel Hill. The authors wish to thank Jan Misenheimer for her help with data entry, and Kristin Cooley, Anne Harris, Cheryl Malkin, and Sabrina Smiley for their assistance with data verification and reliability. The authors would also like to remember Dr. Joanne Erwick Roberts, a visionary in the field of language in fragile X syndrome, without whom this research would have never been possible.

References

  1. Abbeduto L, Brady N, Kover ST. Language development and fragile X syndrome: Profiles, syndrome-specificity, and within-syndrome differences. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2007;13:36–46. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Abbeduto L, Chapman RS. Language development in Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome: Current research and implications for theory and practice. In: Fletcher P, Miller JF, editors. Developmental theory and language disorders. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing; 2005. pp. 53–72. [Google Scholar]
  3. Abbeduto L, Murphy MM, Cawthon SW, Richmond E, Weissman MD, Karadottir S, O’Brien A. Receptive language skills of adolescents and young adults with Down or fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2003;108:149–160. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2003)108<0149:RLSOAA>2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Abbeduto L, Pavetto M, Kesin E, Weissman MD, Karadottir S, O’Brien A, et al. The linguistic and cognitive profile of Down syndrome: Evidence from a comparison with fragile X syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice. 2001;7:9–15. doi: 10.3104/reports.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bailey DB, Hatton DD, Skinner M, Mesibov G. Autistic behavior, FMR1 protein, and developmental trajectories in young males with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2001;31:165–174. doi: 10.1023/a:1010747131386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baker S, Hooper S, Skinner M, Hatton D, Schaaf J, Ornstein P, Bailey D. Working memory subsystems and task complexity in young boys with Fragile X syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2011;55:19–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01343.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Barnes E, Roberts J, Long SH, Martin GE, Berni MC, Mandulak KC, et al. Phonological accuracy and intelligibility in connected speech of boys with fragile X syndrome or Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2009;52:1048–1061. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0001). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Belser RC, Sudhalter V. Arousal difficulties in males with fragile X syndrome: A preliminary report. Developmental Brain Dysfunction. 1995;8:270–279. [Google Scholar]
  9. Berglund E, Eriksson M. Communicative development in Swedish children 16–28 months old: The Swedish Early Communicative Inventory–Words and Sentences. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 2000;41:133–144. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.00181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Berglund E, Eriksson M, Johansson I. Parental reports of spoken language skills in children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2001;44:179–191. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Berry WD. Series Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications; 1993. Understanding Regression Assumptions. [Google Scholar]
  12. Bray M, Woolnough L. The language skills of children with Down’s syndrome aged 12 to 16 years. Child Language Teaching and Therapy. 1988;4:311–324. [Google Scholar]
  13. Brown R. A First Language: the Early Stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1973. [Google Scholar]
  14. Calculator SN. Promoting the acquisition and generalization of conversational skills by individuals with severe disabilities. Augmentative and Alternative Communication. 1988;4:94–103. [Google Scholar]
  15. Camarata SM, Nelson KE, Camarata MN. Comparison of conversational-recasting and imitative procedures for training grammatical structures in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1994;37:1414–1423. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3706.1414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Campbell TF, Dollaghan CA, Rockette HE, Paradise JL, Feldman HM, Shriberg LD, Sabo DL, Kurs-Lasky M. Risk factors for speech delay of unknown origin in 3-year-old children. Child Development. 2003;74:346–357. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.7402002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Caselli MC, Monaco L, Trasciani M, Vicari S. language in italian children with down syndrome and with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology. 2008;22:27–35. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.1.27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Caselli MC, Vicari S, Longobardi E, Lami L, Pizzoli C, Stella G. Gestures and words in early development of children with down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1998;41:1125–1135. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4105.1125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Improved national prevalence estimates for 18 selected major birth defects—United States, 1999–2001. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 2006;54:1301–1332. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Chapman RS. Language development in children and adolescents with Down syndrome. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 1997;3:307–312. [Google Scholar]
  21. Chapman RS, Hesketh LJ. Behavioral phenotype of individuals with Down syndrome. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2000;6:84–95. doi: 10.1002/1098-2779(2000)6:2<84::AID-MRDD2>3.0.CO;2-P. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Chapman RS, Hesketh LJ, Kistler DJ. Predicting longitudinal change in language production and comprehension in individuals with Down syndrome: Hierarchical linear modeling. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2002;45:902–915. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/073). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Chapman RS, Schwartz SE, Kay-Raining Bird E. Language skills of children and adolescents with Down syndrome: I. Comprehension. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1991;34:1106–1120. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3405.1106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Chapman RS, Seung H, Schwartz SE, Kay-Raining Bird E. Language skills of children and adolescents with Down syndrome: II Production deficits. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1998;41:861–873. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4104.861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Chapman RS, Seung H, Schwartz SE, Kay-Raining Bird E. Predicting language production in children and adolescents with Down syndrome: The role of comprehension. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2000;43:340–350. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4302.340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Clark EV. The Lexicon in Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  27. Clifford S, Dissanayake C, Bui QM, Huggins R, Taylor AK, Loesch DZ. Autism spectrum phenotype in males and females with fragile X full mutation and premutation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37:738–747. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0205-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Coffee B, Keith K, Albizua I, Malone T, Mowrey J, Sherman SL, Warren ST. Incidence of fragile X syndrome by newborn screening for methylated FMR1 DNA. American Journal of Human Genetics. 2009;85:503–514. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.09.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Condouris K, Meyer E, Tager-Flusberg H. The relationship between standardized measures of language and measures of spontaneous speech in children with autism. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2003;12:349–358. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2003/080). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Conti-Ramsden G. Maternal recasts and other contingent replies to language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1990;55:262–274. doi: 10.1044/jshd.5502.262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Cornish K, Turk J, Hagerman R. The fragile X continuum: new advances and perspectives. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2008;52:469–482. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2008.01056.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Dodd B, Thompson L. Speech disorder in children with Down’s syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disabilities Research. 2001;45:308–316. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2001.00327.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Dollaghan CA, Campbell TF, Paradise JL, Feldman HM, Janosky JE, Pitcairn DN, Kurs-Lasky M. Maternal education and measures of early speech and language. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1999;42:1432–1443. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4206.1432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Dyer-Friedman J, Glaser B, Hessl D, et al. Genetic and environmental influences on the cognitive outcomes of children with fragile X syndrome. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002;41:237–244. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200203000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Dykens EM, Hodapp RM. Research in Mental Retardation: Toward an Etiologic Approach. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry. 2001;42:41–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Dykens EM, Hodapp RM, Finucane B. Genetics and mental retardation syndromes: A new look at behavior and treatments. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  37. Dykens EM, Hodapp RM, Ort S, Finucane B, Shapiro LR, Leckman JF. The trajectory of cognitive development in males with fragile X syndrome. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1989;28:422–426. doi: 10.1097/00004583-198905000-00020. PMID: 2786867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Eadie PA, Fey ME, Douglas JM, Parsons CL. Profiles of grammatical morphology and sentence imitation in children with specific language impairment and Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2002;45:720–732. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/058). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Ellis N, Sinclair S. Working memory in the acquisition of vocabulary and syntax: putting language in good order. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A. 1996;49:234–250. [Google Scholar]
  40. Estigarribia B, Roberts JE, Price JR, Sideris J. Expressive morphosyntax in boys with fragile X syndrome with and without autism spectrum disorder. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 2010 doi: 10.3109/13682822.2010.487885. EPub ahead of print. PMID: 20569173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Faraway JJ. Linear Models in R. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  42. Ferrier LJ, Bashir AS, Meryash DL, et al. Conversational skills of individuals with fragile X syndrome: A comparison with autism and Down syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 1991;33:776–788. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1991.tb14961.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Fewell RR, Deutscher B. Contributions of early language and maternal facilitation variables to later language and reading abilities. Journal of Early Intervention. 2003;26:132–145. [Google Scholar]
  44. Fey MM, Cleave PL, Long SH. Two models of grammar facilitation in children with language impairments: Phase 2. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1997;40:5–19. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4001.05. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Fey MM, Cleave PL, Long SH, Hughes DI. Two approaches to the facilitation of grammar in language-impaired children: An experimental evaluation. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1993;36:141–157. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3601.141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Fidler DJ, Philofsky A, Hepburn SL. Language phenotypes and intervention planning: Bridging research and practice. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2007;13:47–57. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Finestack LH, Abbeduto L. Expressive language profiles of verbally expressive adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome or fragile X syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2010;53:1334–1348. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0125). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Freund L, Reiss A. Cognitive profile associated with the fra(X) syndrome in males and females. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 1991;38:542–547. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1320380409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Gallaway C, Richards BJ, editors. Input and interaction in language acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  50. Gelman A, Hill J. Data Analysis Using Regresssion and Multilevel/Hierarchical Models. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  51. Glaser B, Hessl D, Dyer-Friedman J, et al. Biological and environmental contributions to adaptive behavior in fragile X syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics A. 2003;117:21–29. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.10549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Goldstein H. Communication intervention for children with autism: A review of treatment efficacy. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2002;32:373–396. doi: 10.1023/a:1020589821992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hall SS, Lightbody AA, Reiss AL. Compulsive, self-injurious, and autistic behavior in children and adolescents with fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2008;113:44–53. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2008)113[44:CSAABI]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Hassold T, Sherman S. The origin and etiology of trisomy 21. In: Cohen WI, Nadel L, Madnick ME, editors. Down syndrome: Visions for the 21st century. New York: Wiley-Liss; 2002. pp. 295–301. [Google Scholar]
  55. Helm-Estabrooks N, Albert ML. Manual of Aphasia and Aphasia Therapy. (Second Edition) Austin, Texas: PRO-ED; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  56. Hesketh LJ, Chapman RS. Verb use by individuals with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 1998;103:288–304. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(1998)103<0288:VUBIWD>2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Hewitt LE, Hammer CS, Yont KM, Tomblin JB. Language sampling in kindergarten children with and without SLI: Mean length of utterance, IPSYN, and NDW. Journal of Communication Disorders. 2005;38:197–213. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Hewitt LE, Hinkle AS, Miccio AW. Intervention to improve expressive grammar for adults with Down syndrome. Communication Disorders Quarterly. 2005;26:144–155. [Google Scholar]
  59. Hick RF, Botting N, Conti-Ramsden G. Short term memory and vocabulary development in children with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2005;47:532–538. doi: 10.1017/s0012162205001040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Hodapp RM, Dykens EM, Ort SI, Zelinsky DG, Leckman JF. Changing Patterns of Intellectual Strengths and Weaknesses in Males with Fragile X Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1991;21:503–516. doi: 10.1007/BF02206873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Hoff E. The Specificity of Environmental Influence: Socioeconomic Status Affects Early Vocabulary Development via Maternal Speech. Child Development. 2003;74:1368–1378. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Hoff-Ginsberg E. Mother-child conversation in different social class and communicative settings. Child Development. 1991;62:782–796. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01569.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Hooper SR, Hatton D, Sideris J, Sullivan K, Hammer J, Schaaf J, Mirrett P, Ornstein PA. Executive Functions in Young Males With Fragile X Syndrome in Comparison to Mental Age-Matched Controls: Baseline Findings From a Longitudinal Study. Neuropsychology. 2008;22:36–47. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.1.36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Huttenlocher J, Vasilyeva M, Cymerman E, Levine S. Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology. 2002;45:337–374. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0285(02)00500-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Iverson JM, Longobardi E, Caselli MC. Relationship between gestures and words in children with Down’s syndrome and typically developing children in the early stages of communicative development. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 2003;38:179–197. doi: 10.1080/1368282031000062891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Jarrold C, Baddeley AD. Short-term memory in Down syndrome: Applying the working memory model. Down Syndrome Research and Practice. 2001;7:17–23. doi: 10.3104/reviews.110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Jarrold C, Hewes AK, Baddeley AD. Do two separate speech measures constrain verbal short term memory in children? Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2000;26:1626–1637. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.26.6.1626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Kau AS, Meyer W, Kaufman WE. Early development in males with fragile X syndrome: A review of the literature. Microscopy Research and Technique. 2002;57:174–178. doi: 10.1002/jemt.10069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Kaufmann WE, Cortell R, Kau ASM, Bukelis I, Tierney E, Gray RM, et al. Autism spectrum disorder in fragile X syndrome: Communication, Social Interaction, and Specific Behaviors. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2004;129A:225–234. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.30229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Landa RJ, Goldberg MC. Language, social, and executive functions in high functioning autism: A continuum of performance. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2005;35:557–573. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0001-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Laws G. The use of nonword repetition as a test of phonological memory in children with Down syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 1998;39:1119–1130. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Laws G. Working memory in children and adolescents with Down syndrome: Evidence from a colour memory experiment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2002;43:353–364. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Laws G. Contributions of phonological memory, language comprehension and hearing to the expressive language of adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2004;45:1085–1095. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.t01-1-00301.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Laws G, Bishop DVM. A comparison of language abilities in adolescents with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2003;46:1324–1339. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2003/103). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Levy Y, Gottesman R, Borochowitz Z, Frydman M, Sagi M. Language in boys with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Child Language. 2006;33:125–144. doi: 10.1017/s030500090500718x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Lewis RL, Vasishth S, Van Dyke JA. Computational principles of working memory in sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2006;10:447–454. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore PC, Risi S. Autism diagnostic observation schedule. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  78. Losh M, Piven J. Social-Cognition and the Broad Autism Phenotype: Identifying Genetically Meaningful Phenotypes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48:105–112. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01594.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. MacWhinney B. The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk. 3rd Edition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  80. Madison LS, George C, Moeschler JB. Cognitive functioning in the fragile-X syndrome: a study of intellectual, memory and communication skills. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research. 1986;30:129–148. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1986.tb01306.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Maloney ES, Larrivee LS. Limitations of age-equivalent scores in reporting the results of norm-referenced tests. Contemporary Issues in Communication Science and Disorders. 2007;34:86–93. [Google Scholar]
  82. Martin GE, Klusek J, Estigarribia B, Roberts JE. Language characteristics of individuals with Down syndrome. In: Abbeduto Leonard J., editor. Topics in Language Disorders 29, special issue “Language and Literacy Development in People with Intellectual Disabilities: A Focus on Genetic Syndromes”. 2009. pp. 112–132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Mazuka R, Jincho N, Oishi H. Development of executive control and language processing. Language and Linguistics Compass. 2009;3:59–89. [Google Scholar]
  84. Mercaldo V, Descalzi G, Zhuo M. Fragile X Mental Retardation Protein in Learning-Related Synaptic Plasticity. Molecules and Cells. 2009;28:501–507. doi: 10.1007/s10059-009-0193-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Mervis CB, Robinson BF. Methodological issues in cross-group comparisons of language and cognitive development. In: Levy Y, Schaeffer J, editors. Language competence across populations: Toward a definition of specific language impairment. Mahwah, NJ US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2003. pp. 233–258. [Google Scholar]
  86. Miller JF, Chapman RS. SALT: Computer Program for the Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts. Madison, WI: Language Analysis Lab, Waisman Center; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  87. Oetting JB, Cantrell JP, Horohov JE. A study of specific language impairment (SLI) in the context of non-standard dialect. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics. 1999;13:25–44. [Google Scholar]
  88. Ornstein PA, Schaaf JM, Hooper SR, Hatton DD, Mirrett P. Memory skills of boys with fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2008;113:453–465. doi: 10.1352/2008.113:453-465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Paul R, Cohen DJ, Breg WR, Watson M, Herman S. Fragile X syndrome: Its relation to speech and language disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1984;49:328–332. doi: 10.1044/jshd.4903.328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Perovic A. Syntactic deficit in Down syndrome: More evidence for the modular organisation of language. Lingua. 2006;116:1616–1630. [Google Scholar]
  91. Philofsky A, Hepburn SL, Hayes A, Hagerman R, Rogers S. Linguistic and cognitive functioning and autism symptoms in young children with fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2004;109:208–218. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2004)109<208:LACFAA>2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Piper MC, Ramsay MK. Effects of early home environment on the mental development of Down syndrome infants. American Journal of Mental Deficiencies. 1980;85:39–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Price JR, Roberts JE, Hennon EA, Berni MC, Anderson KL, Sideris J. Syntactic complexity during conversation of boys with fragile X syndrome and Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2008;51:3–15. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Price JR, Roberts JE, Vandergrift N, Martin G. Language comprehension in boys with fragile X syndrome and boys with Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2007;51:318–326. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00881.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Pueschel SM. Down syndrome. In: Parker S, Zuckerman B, editors. Behavioral and developmental pediatrics: A handbook for primary care. New York: Little Brown; 1995. pp. 116–119. [Google Scholar]
  96. Rice ML, Tomblin JB, Hoffman L, Richman WA, Marquis J. Grammatical tense deficits in children with SLI and nonspecific language impairment: Relationships with nonverbal IQ over time. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2004;47:816–834. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/061). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Roberts JA, Rice ML, Tager-Flusberg H. Tense marking in children with autism. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2004;25:429–448. [Google Scholar]
  98. Roberts JE, Hennon EA, Price JR, Dear E, Anderson K, Vandergrift NA. Expressive language during conversational speech in boys with fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2007;112:1–15. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2007)112[1:ELDCSI]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Roberts JE, Long SH, Malkin C, Barnes E, Skinner M, Hennon EA. A comparison of phonological skills of boys with fragile X syndrome and Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2005;48:980–995. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/067). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Roberts JE, Martin GE, Moskowitz L, Harris AA, Foreman J, Nelson L. Discourse skills of boys with fragile X syndrome in comparison to boys with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2007;50:475–492. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/033). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Roberts JE, Mirrett P, Burchinal M. Receptive and expressive communication development of young males with fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2001;106:216–230. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2001)106<0216:RAECDO>2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Roberts JE, Price JR, Barnes EF, Nelson L, Burchinal M, Hennon E, et al. Receptive vocabulary, expressive vocabulary, and speech production of boys with fragile X syndrome and Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2007;112:177–193. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2007)112[177:RVEVAS]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Rogers SJ, Wehner DE, Hagerman R. The behavioral phenotype in fragile X: Symptoms of autism in very young children with fragile X syndrome, idiopathic autism, and other developmental disorders. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2001;22:409–417. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200112000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Roid GH, Miller LJ. Leiter International Performance Scale-Revised. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  105. Roizen NJ. Down syndrome. In: Batshaw ML, Pellegrino L, Roizen NJ, editors. Children with disabilities. 6th ed. Baltimore: Brookes; 2007. pp. 263–273. [Google Scholar]
  106. Rutter T, Buckley S. The acquisition of grammatical morphemes in children with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome: Research and Practice. 1994;2:76–82. [Google Scholar]
  107. Scarborough HS. Index of productive syntax. Applied Psycholinguistics. 1990;11:1–22. [Google Scholar]
  108. Scarborough HS, Rescorla L, Tager-Flusberg H, Fowler AE, Sudhalter V. The relation of utterance length to grammatical complexity in normal and language-disordered groups. Applied Psycholinguistics. 1991;12:23–45. [Google Scholar]
  109. Sharav T, Collins R, Shlomo L. Effect of maternal education on prognosis of development in children with Down syndrome. Pediatrics. 1985;76:387–391. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Sherman S. Epidemiology. In: Hagerman RJ, Hagerman PJ, editors. Fragile X syndrome: Diagnosis, treatment, and research. (3rd ed.) Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press; 2002. pp. 136–168. [Google Scholar]
  111. Shrout PE, Fleiss JL. Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin. 1979;2:420–428. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.86.2.420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Siller M, Sigman M. The behaviors of parents of children with autism predict the subsequent development of their children’s communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2002;32:77–89. doi: 10.1023/a:1014884404276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Siller M, Sigman M. Modeling longitudinal change in the language abilities of children with autism: Parent behaviors and child characteristics as predictors of change. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44:1691–1704. doi: 10.1037/a0013771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Sudhalter V, Cohen I, Silverman W, Wolf-Schein E. Conversational analyses of males with fragile X, Down syndrome, and autism: Comparison of the emergence of deviant language. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 1990;94:431–441. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Sudhalter V, Scarborough HS, Cohen IL. Syntactic delay and pragmatic deviance in the language of fragile X males. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 1991;38:493–497. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1320380270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Sudhalter V, Maranion M, Brooks P. Expressive semantic deficit in the productive language of males with fragile X syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 1992;43:65–71. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1320430110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Sullivan K, Hatton DD, Hammer J, Sideris J, Hooper SR, Ornstein PA, Bailey DB. Sustained attention and response inhibition in boys with fragile X syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 144B. 2007:517–532. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using Multivariate Statistics. 5th Edition. Boston: Allyn and Bacon; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  119. Tager-Flusberg H. Atypical Language Development: Autism and Other Neurodevelopmental Disorders. In: Hoff Erika, Shatz Marilyn., editors. Blackwell Handbook of Language Development. Blackwell Publishing; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  120. Tager-Flusberg H, Rogers S, Cooper J, Landa R, Lord C, Paul R, Rice M, Stoel-Gammon C, Wetherby A, Yoder P. Defining spoken language benchmarks and selecting measures of expressive language development for young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2009;52:643–652. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0136). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Thordardottir ET, Chapman RS, Wagner L. Complex sentence production by adolescents with Down syndrome. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2002;23:163–183. [Google Scholar]
  122. Tyler AA, Lewis KE, Haskill A, Tolbert LC. Efficacy and cross-domain effects of a morphosyntax and a phonology intervention. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 2002;33:52–66. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2002/005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Verkerk AJMH, Pieretti M, Sutcliffe JS, Fu YH, Kuhl DP, Pizzuti A, Reiner O, Richards S, Victoria MF, Zhang FP, et al. Identification of a gene (FMR-1) containing a CGQ repeat coincident with a breakpoint cluster region exhibiting length variation in fragile X syndrome. Cell. 1991;65:905–914. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(91)90397-h. PMID: 1710175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Wagner R, Torgesen J, Rashotte C. Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  125. Warren SF, Brady N, Sterling A, Fleming K, Marquis J. Maternal responsivity predicts language development in young children with fragile x syndrome. American Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. 2010;115:54–75. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-115.1.54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Williams JN, Lovatt P. Phonological memory and rule learning. Language Learning. 2005;55:177–233. [Google Scholar]
  127. Williams KT. Expressive Vocabulary Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  128. Wilkinson L Task Force on Statistical Inference. Statistical methods in psychology journals: Guidelines and explanations. American Psychologist. 1999;54:594–604. [Google Scholar]
  129. Yoder P, Compton D. Identifying predictors of treatment response. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2004;10:162–168. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES