Abstract
Although intracerebral field potential oscillations are commonly used to study information processing during cognition and behavior, the cellular and network processes underlying such events remain unclear. The limited spatial resolution of standard single-point recordings does not clarify whether field oscillations reflect the activity of one or many afferent presynaptic populations. However, multi-site recording devices now provide high-resolution spatial profiles of local field potentials (LFPs) and when coupled to modern mathematical analyses that discriminate signals with distinct but overlapping spatial distributions, they open the door to better understand these potentials. Here we review recent insights that help disentangle certain pathway-specific activities. Accordingly, some oscillatory patterns can now be viewed as a periodic succession of synchronous synaptic currents that reflect the time envelope of spiking activity in given presynaptic populations. These analyses modify our concept of brain rhythms as abstract entities, molding them into mechanistic representations of network activity and allowing us to work in the time domain, reducing the loss of information inherent to data-chopping frequency treatment.
Keywords: local field potentials, gamma oscillations, spatial discrimination, independent component analysis, spontaneous activity
Field potential oscillations: how much do we know?
The incessant processing of information by neural circuits has been investigated using a variety of techniques, of which only a few have sufficient spatial and temporal resolution to grasp the rapid changes in electrical activity associated with brain function. Some record global activity in a non-invasive manner, such as EEG or magnetoencephalograms (MEG), while intracerebral approaches capture more local activity in particular regions, like local field potentials (LFPs). In all cases, one can appreciate the conspicuous presence of repetitive wave-like patterns or oscillations. Despite constituting a small fraction of the total brain activity, oscillations have received much attention as they can be readily associated to cognitive and behavioral tasks. The question is: how much do we know about the identity of the neuron populations involved in their generation?
Brain oscillations appeared well-suited for application of spectral analysis in order to quantify temporal patterns (Fourier analysis and its derivatives; Jenkins and Watts, 1968; Oppenheim and Schafer, 1989). Bioelectrical signals are usually filtered in predefined frequency bands of interest, a process that, it must be remembered, leads to the loss of a considerable amount of information. Such a jump from the time to the frequency domain summarizes temporal information and makes it handy to associate and compare with other measurements of a brain at work. Some paradigmatic cases are the identification of phases in the sleep cycle by their spectral content (Dement and Kleitman, 1957), the predictive character of hippocampal theta in terms of behavioral performance (Buzsáki et al., 1983), or the processing of visual stimuli contingent with cortical gamma activity (Gray et al., 1989).
Importantly, whether oscillatory or irregular, LFPs are complex signals that vary not only in time but also in space, as they are raised by uneven summation of currents originated in different sites, possibly even different oscillatory generators. Despite the practical advantages, frequency bands are not true physiological signals. All too frequently they are thought of and handled as if they were independent from each other, under a reductionist assumption that each constitutes a separate physiological entity. An increasing number of reports are now appearing in the literature challenging, questioning or simply describing inconsistent frequency bands in different or even the same brain areas (Florian et al., 1998; Csicsvari et al., 2003; Schmidt et al., 2009; Ray and Maunsell, 2011). One notable example is the open dispute regarding the physiological role of gamma activity (e.g., Nunez and Srinivasan, 2010) as a temporal reference frame bringing together different features of a visual stimulus (Singer and Gray, 1995; Fries et al., 2007). Recent results challenge this view, arguing that visual stimuli generate gamma activity at different frequencies in sub-regions of the visual cortex (Ray and Maunsell, 2010). Others even questioned their very existence, alleging that the spectral properties of the activity recorded are indistinguishable from filtered noise (Burns and Xing, 2011).
We are moving away from the old view of frequency bands as behavioral or cognitive flags. We now admit that LFP oscillations are highly variable over time and they have a flexible spectrum (Rivas et al., 1996; Bullock et al., 2003; Ray and Maunsell, 2010). However, it is uncertain what this means in terms of the afferent and target populations. For instance, oscillatory 40–50 Hz patterns in the visual cortex (Gray et al., 1989) or the hippocampus (Fernández-Ruiz et al., 2012a) are probably unrelated phenomena with different cellular mechanisms and having distinct computational meaning within their respective networks. On the other hand, the activity of a population of neurons undergoes variable frequency modulation, even during the same behavioral state (Reich et al., 1997; Czurkó et al., 1999; Chang et al., 2012) and hence, the temporal structure of the synaptic currents they originate in target neurons would be expected to change accordingly.
Indeed, when changes of spectral power of LFPs are interpreted, it becomes evident that there is insufficient knowledge on the scaling of unitary to macroscopic activities. The problem is clear for recordings obtained with non-invasive techniques due to the inherent difficulties in identifying deep generators (Gloor, 1985; Baillet et al., 2001; Srinivasan et al., 2006). Even when recording at the physical location of the generating sources, there is significant uncertainty. We can emphasize the dimension of the problem by considering a non-exhaustive list of possible causes that could lead to increased gamma power: (1) increased gamma-modulated excitation, (2) inhibition (3), or both; (4) the enhanced driving force of an unchanged rhythmic input by sustained changes in another input to the same neurons; (5) variations in phase-locking of presynaptic neurons or (6) in the number of units recruited to firing; (7) reduction of a concomitant antiphase rhythm near the recording electrode; (8) the powering of a different in-phase generator or (9) the addition of new ones; (10) variation in resonant intrinsic currents, and so on. The possibilities are many, some of a unitary origin and others network based.
Since the signals from multiple generators overlap spatially, customary measurements of frequency, phase and amplitude may be due to changes in a local or a distant source. These issues stress the importance of identifying the population/s contributing to a given field potential oscillation as a necessary step to infer on its physiological and computational meaning.
Finding the current sources for LFPs: insights from spatially discriminating techniques
As indicated above, the problem of identifying the cellular origin of field oscillations, or the inverse problem as it has become known, is a fundamental issue in Systems Neuroscience. In simple terms, given an experimental macroscopic signal the amplitude of which varies at different sites (e.g., LFP, EEG, MEG), how can the location and extension of the generating source be determined? To put this in more applicable terms, can we reconstruct the characteristics of the electric currents generated by myriads of scattered microscopic sources co-activated as one, if their number (hence their position) change constantly? We know from theory that multiple combinations of independent sources (groups of active neurons) may give rise to a recorded signal with the same spatial pattern. There is no unique solution and in most cases, it is difficult or impossible to confirm the potential solutions experimentally. One would think the way out might be easier for LFPs that can be recorded at the site. However, whereas remote recordings lack necessary detail of the location and extension of current sources, the local recordings can't discriminate between local and remote (volume propagated) contributions.
A common feature of electric fields in the brain is that they vary spatially in a complex manner, on account of the shifting activation of neuron generators with irregular morphology and distribution. It is beyond the scope of this minireview to consider all the factors that are relevant to LFPs (see Elul, 1972 for a starting reading) and thus, we will focus on a few that are important for the discrimination of mixed LFP generators. Neurons with dominant axial geometry act as strong current dipoles (Lorente de No, 1947) and as such are the main contributors to field potentials. LFPs are contributed mostly by synaptic currents that, contrary to propagating spike currents, remain at the site of the synapse for their largest part (Varona et al., 2000; López-Aguado et al., 2002). It is intuitive that a common afferent input to one or another subcellular domain of the neuron population will give rise to different field potential profiles (Makarova et al., 2011). This fact underlies the laminar distribution of pathway-specific LFPs in regular structures (Korovaichuk et al., 2010). The problem arises when several pathways are co-activated, as is usually the case. In such circumstances the electric currents mix unevenly at different sites, and field potential gradients become complex and variable. That is why stable oscillations with seemingly identical waves, one after another, appeared advantageous as they are more accessible to repeated experimental screening. However, they may still be contributed by several inputs with varying magnitude (Makarov et al., 2010; Fernández-Ruiz et al., 2012a). Thus, only high-density recordings simultaneously performed at several depths can correctly map for spatial variations in successive waves originated by modulations in one or more of the contributing sources.
Multisite linear recordings are well-suited to a method that has been employed to find the current generators underlying field potentials, known as current source density (CSD) analysis (Freeman and Nicholson, 1975). This approach has been very useful to determine the contributing cells and the location of synaptic membranes activated by afferent stimuli in laminar structures, such as the hippocampus or neocortex (Leung, 1979; Herreras, 1990; Schroeder et al., 1998). However, while interpreting CSD maps is simple for voltage profiles elicited by stimulating only one afferent pathway (Figures 1A,B, right panels), their application to ongoing LFPs renders complex spatial maps of intermingled inward and outward currents (left panels), and in general it is not feasible to identify the multiple synaptic generators. Partial success has been obtained in a few stereotypic LFP patterns, such as sharp-waves (SPWs: Ylinen et al., 1995), or the theta (Brankačk et al., 1993) and gamma rhythms (Csicsvari et al., 2003) in the hippocampus. But not even in these cases has it been possible to unequivocally determine whether one or several inputs contribute to the field oscillation due to unavoidable technical artifacts.
Amongst the approaches used to address the mixed contribution of inputs to macroscopic patterns, some sought the selective manipulation of parts of a network, such as the activation or the silencing of specific pathways or neuron types through electrical, optogenetic, or pharmacological intervention (Wu et al., 1998; Happel et al., 2010; Kuki et al., 2012). Other approaches pursued the disentanglement of LFPs into their original generators by applying statistical tools and algorithms (Di et al., 1990; Kocsis et al., 1999; Montgomery et al., 2009; Makarov et al., 2010). Blind source separation techniques, like the independent component analysis (ICA: Comon, 1994; Choi et al., 2005), appear to be the best suited by their capacity to find stable groups of sensors picking up a signal whose origin is stationary in the space, a feature that can be assumed for electrical fields generated by synaptic currents.
Independent component analysis as a tool to reveal the cellular generators of brain oscillations
ICA is employed routinely in scalp EEG, MEG, and fMRI to find spatially stable patterns of coherent activity (Makeig et al., 1997; Tang et al., 2002; Kalcher et al., 2012). Recordings distant from the sources are highly sensitive to a variety of distortions in the path of the electrical currents (López-Aguado et al., 2001). Thus, they are limited to a coarse localization of large electrical sources of unknown cellular origin. The disturbing factors are less relevant in intracerebral recordings where electrodes can be placed directly on the generating sources, thereby minimizing the path of the currents to the recording electrode. The ICA operates on multiple simultaneously recorded signals and discriminates the contributing sources on the basis of their distinct spatial distribution. It is important to note that these components may be temporally correlated (Bell and Sejnowski, 1995; Makarova et al., 2011).
We have developed an implementation based on the ICA to separate the different synaptic pathways converging on hippocampal neurons on the evidence that each produces field potentials of stable and distinct spatial distribution (Makarov et al., 2010). The procedure is illustrated in Figure 1, in which each LFP component or generator is defined by two elements: a curve of spatial weights and the time course of the activity over the period analyzed (Figure 1C). We found that only a few macroscopic pathway-specific generators account for most of the LFP variance. Some are easy to identify by the characteristic profile of spatial weights, such as the excitatory Schaffer input from CA3 to CA1 that match the customary profiles of Schaffer-evoked fEPSPs (blue trace). This LFP generator exclusively captures all the activity elicited by this pathway, whether irregular or oscillatory, ongoing (SPWs) or stimulus evoked (Korovaichuk et al., 2010). Once disentangled from native LFPs, a given generator can be reconstructed in isolation and the virtual pathway-specific LFPs subjected to CSD analysis (Figures 1D,E).
A mechanistic use for neural oscillations
Spike activity of a given presynaptic population elicits postsynaptic currents in target regions that if spatially appropriate, may set rhythmic, irregular, and more commonly, behaviorally modulated periods of different LFP patterns. As a case in point, the CA3 input to CA1 pyramidal cells is responsible for (1) low amplitude steady or (2) theta modulated gamma activity, and (3) isolated hyper-synchronous SPWs (Ylinen et al., 1995; Penttonen et al., 1998; Fernández-Ruiz et al., 2012a). The separation of the Schaffer activity from concomitant inputs allowed a detailed study of its time course. A small LFP epoch is presented in Figure 2 to show the independent activity of three different LFP generators. One is very irregular, while another presents high amplitude slow-waves and the third corresponds to the ongoing Schaffer input. In anesthetized animals, whereas epochs of gamma activity can be found in all LFP generators (Makarov et al., 2010) it only has a steady presence in the Schaffer input. Traditionally, gamma activity in the hippocampus was assumed to be mainly inhibitory (Mann and Paulsen, 2007; Buzsáki and Wang, 2012), in compliance with the observations of interneurons firing at that rate. But in phase firing of units to LFPs does not establish cause or effect, as the LFPs may reflect compound synaptic currents generated by spiking in unrecorded distant neurons. In fact, our findings refuted that view, as the gamma activity in the CA1 Schaffer generator is made up of small wave-like LFP events (~120 μV, ~16 ms long) that were specifically time locked with monosynaptic latency to the spikes of CA3 pyramidal cells but not interneurons. Indeed, the CSD analysis of the disentangled Schaffer-specific LFPs returns gamma sequence of current sinks in the CA1 stratum radiatum with a spatial distribution that tightly matched that of SPW events and CA3-evoked fEPSPs (Figure 2B2). These wavelets were blocked by pharmacologically silencing the CA3 or blockade of glutamate receptors in the CA1. Thus, the gamma rhythm recorded in this stratum of the CA1 region is a periodic succession of micro (μ)-fEPSPs triggered in CA1 pyramidal cells by CA3 assemblies firing in gamma sequence. This is probably the clearest demonstration of the cellular nature of a field oscillation in the intact brain presented to date.
The demonstration that Schaffer-LFPs are succession of quantal excitatory packages elicited by synchronous firing of functional assemblies of presynaptic units opens several possibilities. For instance, one may identify pairs of monosynaptically connected CA3 and CA1 neurons, enabling the ongoing spike transfer between pairs of cells of different populations and their plastic modulations to be studied in vivo (Fernández-Ruiz et al., 2012a,b). This was done by correlating pre- (CA3) and postsynaptic (CA1) spikes with the time series of Schaffer μ-fEPSPs. Also, several CA3 units fire synchronously and time-locked to Schaffer μ-fEPSPs in CA1, thus forming a functional assembly. We found that CA3 assemblies are not rigid constructs, as the neurons that comprise them may belong to different clusters and they do not always fire together, rather just a proportion of them (ca. 20%). In the CA1, we found that all pyramidal cells fired a fraction of their spikes time locked to Schaffer μ-fEPSPs. We term these as Schaffer-spikes, emphasizing the origin of the synaptic input that triggers them. The sorting of CA1 spikes by their synaptic drive opens the possibility to investigate the influence of different inputs to the elaboration of the output code of individual neurons. We also found triple coincidences, i.e., a presynaptic CA3 spike elicits a μ-fEPSP that in turn fires a postsynaptic spike. We call this as an effective spike transfer between two nodes in a network. Thus, pairs of pre- and postsynaptic spikes can be selected by their time-locking to a common μ-fEPSP, revealing temporal correlations that would normally remain buried in standard dual correlations of spike trains.
Another practical example of the use of pathway-specific LFPs is to study ongoing correlates of plastic phenomena. Thus, following induction of LTP by CA3 stimulation we found a moderate increase in the gamma power of raw LFPs in the CA1, but a large specific increase in the power of the ICA-separated Schaffer generator (while other components of the LFP remained unchanged: Fernández-Ruiz et al., 2012b). Hence, the ongoing modulations of a given synaptic pathway may not be discernible in raw mixed LFPs, whereas they can be observed, quantified and compared with other activities after their separation into pathway-specific LFP generators.
Concluding remarks
By studying the wide band time course of ICA-isolated LFP generators we gain much more information on network dynamics than by chopping LFPs into narrow frequency bands and analyzing them separately. Hence, we consider that the entity with true physiological meaning coming out of LFPs is the LFP generator, rather than a particular temporal pattern in native LFPs that may still be a composite signal.
The example used here of the CA3 gamma input to CA1 may be generalized to other oscillatory and irregular patterns. The use of pathway-specific LFPs eliminates important concern as to the possibility of an LFP oscillation being a mixture of two or more inputs, and allows a direct interpretation of their fluctuations as time envelopes of synaptic currents with a precise identification of the populations of origin and destination. Thus, we may be close to determining which oscillations are periodic successions of excitatory or inhibitory postsynaptic potentials. The reconstruction of pre- and postsynaptic time sequences represents a step toward a mechanistic understanding of information transfer in identified segments of a network. For these reasons, it may be the moment to recover a feeling for the temporal and spatial domains, so as to complement and extend the spectral characterization of oscillatory patterns.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank A. Oliva, G. Martín-Vazquez, and J. Makarova for useful discussion Mark Sefton at BiomedRed for editorial support. This work was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (BFU2010-19192/BFI).
References
- Baillet S., Mosher J. C., Leahy R. M. (2001). Electromagnetic brain mapping. IEEE Signal. Process. Mag. 18, 14–30 [Google Scholar]
- Bell A., Sejnowski T. (1995). An information-maximization approach to blind separation and blind deconvolution. Neural Comput. 7, 1129–1159 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brankačk J., Stewart M., Fox S. E. (1993). Current source density analysis of the hippocampal theta rhythm: associated sustained potentials and candidate synaptic generators. Brain Res. 615, 310–327 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bullock T. H., McClune M. C., Enright J. T. (2003). Are the electroencephalograms mainly rhythmic? Assessment of periodicity in wide-band time series. Neuroscience 121, 233–252 10.1016/S0306-4522(03)00208-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burns S. P., Xing D. (2011). Is gamma-band activity in the local field potential of V1 cortex a “clock” or filtered noise? J. Neurosci. 31, 9658–9664 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0660-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G., Leung L. W., Vanderwolf C. H. (1983). Cellular bases of hippocampal EEG in the behaving rat. Brain Res. 287, 139–171 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G., Wang X. J. (2012). Mechanisms of gamma oscillations. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 203–225 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang M. H., Armstrong K. M., Moore T. (2012). Dissociation of response variability from firing rate effects in frontal eye field neurons during visual stimulation, working memory, and attention. J. Neurosci. 32, 2204–2216 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2967-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi S., Cichocki A., Park H. M., Lee S. Y. (2005). Blind source separation and independent component analysis: a review. Neural Inf. Process. Lett. Rev. 6, 1–57 [Google Scholar]
- Comon P. (1994). Independent component analysis. A new concept? Signal Process. 138 287–314 [Google Scholar]
- Csicsvari J., Jamieson B., Wise K. D., Buzsáki G. (2003). Mechanisms of gamma oscillations in the hippocampus of the behaving rat. Neuron 37, 311–322 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Czurkó A., Hirase H., Csicsvari J., Buzsáki G. (1999). Sustained activation of hippocampal pyramidal cells by ‘space clamping’ in a running wheel. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 344–352 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00446.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dement W., Kleitman N. (1957). Cyclic variations in EEG during sleep and their relation to eye movements, body motility and dreaming. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 9, 673–690 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di S., Baumgartner C., Barth D. S. (1990). Laminar analysis of extracellular field potentials in rat vibrissa/barrel cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 63, 832–840 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elul R. (1972). The genesis of the EEG. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 15, 228–272 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernández-Ruiz A., Makarov V. A., Benito N., Herreras O. (2012a). Schaffer-specific local field potentials reflect discrete excitatory events at gamma-frequency that may fire postsynaptic hippocampal CA1 units. J. Neurosci. 32, 5165–5176 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4499-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernández-Ruiz A., Makarov V. A., Herreras O. (2012b). Sustained increase of spontaneous input and spike transfer in the CA3-CA1 pathway following long term potentiation in vivo. Front. Neural Circuits 6:71 10.3389/fncir.2012.00071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Florian G., Andrew C., Pfurtscheller G. (1998). Do changes in coherence always reflect changes in functional coupling? Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 106, 87–91 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman J. A., Nicholson C. (1975). Experimental optimization of current source-density technique for anuran cerebellum. J. Neurophysiol. 38, 369–382 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fries P., Nikolic D., Singer W. (2007). The gamma cycle. Trends Neurosci. 30, 309–316 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gloor P. (1985). Neuronal generators and the problem of localization in electroencephalography: application of volume conductor theory to electroencephalography. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2, 327–354 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray C. M., König P., Engel A. K., Singer W. (1989). Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which reflects global stimulus properties. Nature 338, 334–337 10.1038/338334a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Happel M. F., Jeschke M., Ohl F. W. (2010). Spectral integration in primary auditory cortex attributable to temporally precise convergence of thalamocortical and intracortical input. J. Neurosci. 30, 11114–11127 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0689-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herreras O. (1990). Propagating dendritic action potential mediates synaptic transmission in CA1 pyramidal cells in situ. J. Neurophysiol. 64, 1429–1441 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins G. M., Watts D. G. (1968). Spectral Analysis and its Applications. San Francisco, CA: Holden-Day [Google Scholar]
- Kalcher K., Huf W., Boubela R. N., Filzmoser P., Pezawas L., Biswal B., et al. (2012). Fully exploratory network independent component analysis of the 1000 functional connectomes database. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:301 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00301 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kocsis B., Bragin A., Buzsáki G. (1999). Interdependence of multiple theta generators in the hippocampus: a partial coherence analysis. J. Neurosci. 19, 6200–6212 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korovaichuk A., Makarova J., Makarov V. A., Benito N., Herreras O. (2010). Minor contribution of principal excitatory pathways to hippocampal LFPs in the anesthetized rat: a combined independent component and current source density study. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 484–497 10.1152/jn.00297.2010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuki T., Ohshiro T., Ito S., Ji Z. G., Fukazawa Y., Matsuzaka Y., et al. (2012). Frequency-dependent entrainment of neocortical slow oscillation to repeated optogenetic stimulation in the anesthetized rat. Neurosci. Res. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1016/j.neures.2012.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leung L. S. (1979). Potentials evoked by alvear tract in hippocampal CA 1 region in rats. II. Spatial field analysis. J. Neurophysiol. 42, 1571–1589 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- López-Aguado L., Ibarz J. M., Herreras O. (2001). Activity-dependent changes of tissue resistivity in the CA1 region in vivo are layer-specific: modulation of evoked potentials. Neuroscience 108, 249–262 10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00417-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- López-Aguado L., Ibarz J. M., Varona P., Herreras O. (2002). Structural inhomogeneities differentially modulate action currents and population spikes initiated in the axon or dendrites. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 2809–2820 10.1152/jn.00183.2002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorente de No R. (1947). Analysis of the distribution of the action currents of nerve in volume conductors. Stud. Rockefeller Inst. Med. Res. Repr. 132, 384–477 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makarov V. A., Makarova J., Herreras O. (2010). Disentanglement of local field potential sources by independent component analysis. J. Comp. Neurosci. 29, 445–457 10.1007/s10827-009-0206-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makarova J., Ibarz J. M., Makarov V. A., Benito N., Herreras O. (2011). Parallel readout of pathway-specific inputs to laminated brain structures. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 5:77 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00077 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makeig S., Jung T. P., Bell A. J., Ghahremani D., Sejnowski T. J. (1997). Blind separation of auditory event-related brain responses into independent components. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 94, 10979–10984 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mann E. O., Paulsen O. (2007). Role of GABAergic inhibition in hippocampal network oscillations. Trends Neurosci. 30, 343–349 10.1016/j.tins.2007.05.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montgomery S. M., Betancur M. I., Buzsáki G. (2009). Behavior-dependent coordination of multiple theta dipoles in the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 29, 1381–1394 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4339-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nunez P. L., Srinivasan R. (2010). Scale and frequency chauvinism in brain dynamics: too much emphasis on γ band oscillations. Brain Struct. Funct. 215, 67–71 10.1007/s00429-010-0277-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oppenheim A. V., Schafer R. W. (1989). Discrete-Time Signal Processing. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall [Google Scholar]
- Penttonen M., Kamondi A., Acsády L., Buzsáki G. (1998). Gamma frequency oscillation in the hippocampus of the rat: intracellular analysis in vivo. Eur. J. Neurosci. 10, 718–728 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00096.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ray S., Maunsell J. H. R. (2010). Differences in gamma frequencies across visual cortex restrict their possible use in computation. Neuron 67, 885–896 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ray S., Maunsell J. H. R. (2011). Network rhythms influence the relationship between spike-triggered local field potential and functional connectivity. J. Neurosci. 31, 12674–12682 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1856-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reich D. S., Victor J. D., Knight B. W., Ozaki T., Kaplan E. (1997). Response variability and timing precision of neuronal spike trains in vivo. J. Neurophysiol. 77, 2836–2841 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rivas J., Gaztelu J. M., García-Austt E. (1996). Changes in hippocampal cell discharge patterns and theta rhythm spectral properties as a function of walking velocity in the guinea pig. Exp. Brain Res. 108, 113–118 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt R., Diba K., Leibold C., Schmitz D., Buzsáki G., Kempter R. (2009). Single-trial phase precession in the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 29, 13232–13241 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2270-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schroeder C. E., Mehta A. D., Givre S. J. (1998). A spatiotemporal profile of visual system activation revealed by current source density analysis in the awake macaque. Cereb. Cortex 8, 575–592 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer W., Gray C. M. (1995). Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 555–586 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.003011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Srinivasan R., Willian R. W., Nunez P. L. (2006). Source analysis of EEG oscillations using high-resolution EEG and MEG. Prog. Brain Res. 159, 29–42 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)59003-X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang A. C., Pearlmutter B. A., Malaszenko N. A., Phung D. B. (2002). Independent components of magnetoencephalography: single-trial response onset time. Neuroimage 17, 1773–1789 10.1006/nimg.2002.1320 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varona P., Ibarz J. M., López-Aguado L., Herreras O. (2000). Macroscopic and subcellular factors shaping CA1 population spikes. J. Neurophysiol. 83, 2192–2208 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu K., Canning K. J., Leung L. S. (1998). Functional interconnections between CA3 and the dentate gyrus revealed by current source density analysis. Hippocampus 8, 217–230 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ylinen A., Bragin A., Nádasdy Z., Jandó G., Szabó I., Sik A., et al. (1995). Sharp wave-associated high-frequency oscillation (200 Hz) in the intact hippocampus: network and intracellular mechanisms. J. Neurosci. 15, 30–46 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]