Abstract
Background
Individuals with schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) exhibit impaired cognitive functioning in a pattern similar to that found in schizophrenia; less clear is the extent to which these individuals also share schizophrenia patients’ impairments in functional capacity and real-world functioning.
Method
We evaluated 46 SPD patients, as well as 38 individuals with avoidant personality disorder (AvPD) and 55 healthy controls (HC) on: cognitive functioning, real-world functioning (employment and residential status), and functional capacity (indexed by the UPSA, a performance-based skills assessment).
Results
We found that individuals with SPD exhibited worse performance on both the cognitive battery and the UPSA than the other groups; they were also less likely to be employed and to be living independently. Additionally, cognitive and UPSA performance in the SPD group was intercorrelated to a degree comparable to what has been found in schizophrenia, and this relationship was not present in the AvPD group. Finally, real-world functioning was related to UPSA performance for both patient groups.
Conclusions
SPD patients exhibit impaired real-world functioning suggesting that these deficits extend across the schizophrenia spectrum. In addition, there is supportive evidence for the validity and importance of performance-based measures such as the UPSA to predict everyday outcomes across the schizophrenia spectrum.
Keywords: schizophrenia spectrum, cognition, functional outcome, performance-based assessments, schizotypal personality disorder
Schizophrenia patients exhibit impairments in a wide range of domains of everyday functioning, including social and occupational functioning, residential maintenance, medication management, and basic self-care (Wiersma et al., 2000). These impairments begin before illness onset, occur even following successful treatment of clinical symptoms, and persist after psychotic symptoms remit (Harvey, 2012), suggesting that psychosis does not fully account for functional impairments in schizophrenia. While functional impairment has been demonstrated to be strongly associated with cognitive impairment in chronic schizophrenia, it is much less clear whether functional impairment accompanies the more modest cognitive impairment seen in schizophrenia spectrum conditions and whether functional disability is worsened by medication status, history of psychosis, and broad and severe cognitive deficits of schizophrenia. Functional outcomes of schizophrenia patients are closely linked to impairments in cognitive performance (Green, 1996; Green et al., 2011), although other features of the illness clearly contribute (Bowie et al., 2010; Bowie et al., 2008a). One critical test for the hypothesis that functional impairment is intrinsically related to cognitive impairment would be evaluation of the functional relevance of cognition in a non-psychotic schizophrenia spectrum disorder with significant cognitive impairment.
Patients with Schizotypal Personality Disorder (SPD), a schizophrenia spectrum disorder that is phenomenologically and biologically related to schizophrenia (Siever et al., 1993), have impairments in several areas of cognitive functioning, such as episodic memory (Cadenhead et al., 1999), inhibition (Moritz and Mass, 1997), abstraction (Voglmaier et al., 1997), sustained attention (Roitman et al., 1997), working memory (Mitropoulou et al., 2005; Roitman et al., 2000) and context processing (McClure et al., 2008). These deficits are qualitatively similar but less severe than those seen in schizophrenia. Working memory in particular seems to be specifically impaired in individuals with SPD, relative to their other cognitive deficits (Mitropoulou et al., 2005), and may account for the other cognitive deficits of this disorder.
Individuals with SPD have well-documented deficits in interpersonal functioning, many of which are necessary to qualify for the DSM-IV diagnosis (American Psychiatric Association. and American Psychiatric Association. Task Force on DSM-IV., 2000). However, although individuals with this disorder demonstrate cognitive deficits similar to what is seen in schizophrenia, much less is known about their everyday functioning in other domains such as independent living, educational attainment, and employment status. Recent research in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder has also focused on the identification of impairments in the functional skills that underlie everyday functioning, referred to as “functional capacity”. These skills have been found to be more proximal to real-world functioning than cognitive impairments and may mediate the correlation between cognition deficits and everyday disability. Performance-based assessments are routinely used as co-primary outcomes measures in treatment studies aimed at cognition in schizophrenia (Bowie et al., 2012) and have been the subject of systematic comparative studies to identify those measures most closely linked to neuropsychological performance (Green et al., 2011). Despite the qualitative similarity in cognitive performance of people with SPD to those with schizophrenia and the acknowledgment that SPD produces disability, there has never been a study of functional capacity performance in SPD and there has been remarkably little research on impairments in everyday functioning beyond acknowledgements of the social limitations that contribute to the diagnosis. Such a study could be important for pharmacological cognitive enhancement and cognitive remediation.
In this study we sought to systematically examine the everyday functioning of individuals with SPD, as well as two comparison samples: patients with Avoidant Personality Disorder (AvPD) and healthy controls (HC), by administering a clinical interview eliciting information about employment, education, and living situation. The AvPD patients were selected as a control for the possibility of nonspecific deficits on the part of SPD cases. We chose a personality-disordered comparison sample instead of a psychotic schizophrenia spectrum comparison group because we expected participants with SPD to be less impaired than schizophrenia patients, the assessment of whom would not provide any information about more moderate nonspecific impairments. Thus, we selected a comparison sample without psychosis in order to control for generalized impairments on the part of SPD patients that were less than those known to occur in schizophrenia but still relevant to a comparison with healthy people. We complemented this assessment with information from medical records, and informants when available, leading to a best-estimate milestone oriented assessment. In addition, we administered a battery of cognitive tests, as well as the UCSD Performance-Based Skills Assessment, a laboratory-based assessment of functional capacity that is gaining widespread popularity as a reliable and valid indicator of functional abilities that is markedly superior to patient self-report (Green et al., 2011; Leifker et al.; Sabbag et al., 2011). We hypothesized that individuals with SPD would demonstrate reductions in everyday functioning compared to comparison samples (healthy and personality disordered). We also hypothesized that impaired performance on both our cognitive battery and the UPSA. Most important, we anticipated finding that these impairments in cognition and functional capacity would be correlated with each other and possibly related to real-world functioning in a manner consistent with that seen in schizophrenia. Thus, this is a comprehensive study of cognition, functional capacity and everyday outcomes in a population that shares characteristics with schizophrenia but without secondary factors (e.g., medication) that might modify these relationships, but has never been studied in this regard.
Materials and methods
Participants
Participants were 46 individuals with DSM-IV Schizotypal Personality Disorder (SPD), 38 with Avoidant PD (AvPD), and 55 healthy controls (HC). All participants were recruited from the community and resided within the New York City metro area. Recruitment, diagnosis, and exclusion criteria for the SPD and HC participants in our program of research have been presented in previous publications (Mitropoulou et al., 2005), and participants with AvPD were recruited with similar strategies. Consensus diagnoses were reached in a meeting of all raters with an expert diagnostician after administration of structured psychiatric interviews. All participants were without abuse of illicit substances or alcohol within the past 6 months or a past history of substance dependence and had been free of psychotropic medication for at least 2 weeks. Participants with lifetime histories of psychosis or head trauma with significant neurological sequelae (unconscious >10 minutes) were also excluded from participation. Demographic characteristics as well as scores on everyday functioning variables are presented in Table 1. All participants signed informed consent in accordance with the IRB approvals of this study at both the James J. Peters VAMC and Mt. Sinai School of Medicine.
Table 1.
SPD Group N=46 | AvPD Group N=38 | HC Group N=55 | |
---|---|---|---|
Age (M(SD) in years) | 37.5 (11.8) | 31.2 (9.6) | 32.3 (9.8) |
Sex (% male) | 60.9% | 52.6% | 37% |
Living Independently (%) | 43.6% | 52.9% | 72.1% |
Employed (%) | 52.6% | 69.7% | 67.5% |
Earned bachelor’s degree or higher (%) | 39.4% | 64.5% | 83.8% |
Procedure
Following enrollment, participants met with a clinical psychologist who assessed current real-world functioning (living situation, social and educational history, employment status, wages earned) using a structured interview, which required detailed questions and substantive examples in answering questions. Participants who received clinical services at either of the research sites also released their medical records for confirmation of their self-report. Participants were administered the cognitive assessment battery and the UCSD Performance-Based Skills Assessment (UPSA) by a trained research technician within three weeks of enrollment and the everyday functioning assessment. This assessment technician was unaware of the results of the other assessments.
Cognitive Assessments
This cognitive assessment was developed on the basis of previous work in schizotypal personality disorder. As a result, it is focused on processing speed and working memory, two of the abilities previously found (Mitropoulou et al. 2005) to be most impaired in this condition. As this study was initiated prior to the finalization of the MATRICS Consensus cognitive battery (MCCB), we were not able to select a battery that was informed by these developments.
Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence
The WASI is a brief measure of overall intelligence (Wechsler, 1999). We administered three subtests of the 4-subtest version: Vocabulary, Block Design, and Matrix Reasoning. Dependent variables were age-adjusted scaled scores for these three subtests.
DOT Test
The Dot Test is an assessment of visuospatial working memory in wide research use (Silver et al., 2003). Subjects are presented a dot at a specific position on a standard size paper and then asked to reproduce it at the same location on a separate sheet after different periods of delay (no delay, 10, 20, or 30 sec delay). The distance error at the 30 sec delay minus the distance error at the immediate condition was the dependent variable of interest in the current study.
Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT)
The PASAT is a test of auditory verbal working memory that has been well described and validated in general (Gronwall, 1977; Stuss et al., 1988), and in this population (Mitropoulou et al., 2005). Subjects listen to a tape-recorded voice presenting a series of numbers (50 numbers at a rate of one digit per two seconds) and are asked to add each adjacent pair of numbers and verbalize the sum. The total number of correct responses is the dependent variable.
Maryland Letter-Number Span (LNS)
This test requires subjects to listen to a list of intermingled letters and numbers (Gold et al., 1997). At the end of the presentation of the stimuli, subjects are asked to reproduce the information with the numbers in ascending order and the letters in alphabetical order, with numbers coming first. The test is discontinued after 4 failures at a single trial length. The number of trials correctly reproduced served as the dependent variable.
Trail-Making Test (TMT)
TMT has two conditions that combine to assess verbal/spatial perception, executive functioning, and psychomotor speed (Reitan and Wolfson, 1994). In Part A the subject must connect numbers presented on a standard sheet of paper in ascending order (1–2–3). In Part B the subject must alternate connecting numbers and letters (for example, 1-A, 2-B, 3-C, etc.). The amount of time to complete part A and part B were the two dependent variables.
UCSD Performance-Based Skills Assessment (UPSA)
The UPSA is an office-based test to measure schizophrenia patients’ ability to perform day-to-day tasks (Patterson et al., 2001). This test was designed for outpatients and measures performance in a number of domains of everyday functioning through the use of props and standardized performance situations. In line with our previous research (Bowie et al.; Bowie et al., 2006; McClure et al., 2007) four domains of the UPSA were used. Comprehension/ Planning measures the patient’s ability to comprehend written material that describe recreational outings and then plan the activities and list appropriate items necessary to bring to the outings. In the Finance domain, the patient must count out given amounts from real currency, make change and fill out a check to pay a utility bill. The Communication domain involves a series of role-play situations that require the patient make emergency calls, call directory assistance to request a telephone number, call the number, and then reschedule a medical appointment. In the Transportation/ Mobility domain, patients use information from bus schedules and maps to determine appropriate fare, state telephone numbers to answer relevant questions, decide which map to use to get to a certain location, and determine the appropriate route and transfers to reach a destination. Note that we developed locally specific stimuli for transportation/mobility and comprehension/planning, as the initial version of the UPSA had items based on San Diego activities. We also excluded the household chores subtest, because the analogue kitchen required was not portable enough to be used at field sites. We then re-standardized the scores to a 100-point scale, like the original 5-subtest UPSA.
Analysis
For the cognitive assessments, we calculated z-scores for each variable in the entire subject sample and then created a composite score using the mean of the z-scores for each cognitive variable. Group differences on our cognitive composite, as well as on the UPSA, were assessed with a series of univariate analyses of variance (ANOVAs). To evaluate group differences on our real-world variables, we performed chi-square tests for independent samples on our two clinical groups to assess group differences on current employment status (employed versus unemployed) and current living situation (living independently or not independently), and t-tests for hourly compensation rate. We then sought to explore the relationship between our assessments of performance-based skills and cognition using a series of Pearson and point biserial correlations.
We performed a power analysis to determine whether the differences in sample size would affect our ability to detect significant correlations. For the sample, correlations of a size of r=.40 could be detected at p<.05 with .80 power, while in the AvPD group, the similarly detectable correlation was r=.43. Thus, there were no major differences in power for correlational analyses across the two samples of PD patients and in both samples we were adequately powered to detect moderate, although not small, effects.
Results
Mean scores and standard deviations for the cognitive composite and UPSA are presented in Table 2. As hypothesized, the SPD group performed significantly worse on the cognitive composite (F(2, 102)=10.39, p<.001) than both the AvPD (p=.006) and HC (p<.001) groups, who did not differ (p=.14).
Table 2.
SPD Group N=46 | AvPD Group N=38 | HC Group N=55 | |
---|---|---|---|
| |||
M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | |
UPSA Total | 78.3 (11.5) | 86.3 (8.5) | 89.3 (5.7) |
Comprehension | 20.3 (3.6) | 22.2 (2.8) | 22.9 (2.9) |
Finance | 17.9 (4.6) | 20.4 (2.7) | 20.5 (2.3) |
Communication | 19.0 (4.8) | 20.1 (4.0) | 22.2 (3.7) |
Transportation | 21.1 (5.2) | 23.5 (3.8) | 23.8 (2.7) |
Cognitive Composite (z-score) | −2.2 (4.4) | .39 (2.8) | 1.7 (3.7) |
Average hourly wage ($) | 14.2 (7.1) | 18.1 (10.1) | 28.3 (17.7) |
In addition, SPD participants demonstrated significantly worse performance on the UPSA total score (F(2, 133)=13.99, p=.001) than both other samples (p<.001). In fact, SPD participants performed worse than the HC group on all domain scores as well (all ps <.03). SPD participants also performed worse than the AvPD group on all domain scores (all ps <.001) other than Communication (p=.70).
Results of our assessments of real world functioning are presented in Table 2. SPD participants were less likely to be currently living independently (χ2 (2)=12.73, p=.002) and were also less likely to have earned a bachelor’s degree or higher (χ2 (2)=17.43, p=.002) than the other two groups. Although there was no statistically significant difference in the proportion of SPD participants who were currently employed (χ2 (2)= 3.11, p=.21), individuals with SPD who were employed earned a significantly lower hourly wage (F(2, 42)=5.38, p=.008), than HC (See Table 2). Interestingly, individuals with AvPD also earned significantly less than HC (p=.04), and the difference between the SPD and AvPD was not significant (p=.39).
The correlations between UPSA subscales and total scores were similar in both patient groups, suggesting good internal consistency of the assessments. We found that our performance-based measure of functional capacity was correlated with cognitive performance in our SPD group in a degree similar to what has been found in schizophrenia patients (See Table 3). The correlation between UPSA scores and NP performance was quite consistent with that seen in multiple prior studies of schizophrenia (Leifker et al.) and the correlation for the AvPD group was smaller. The difference between these two correlations approached statistical significance: p=.06. However, the correlations between NP performance and UPSA scores and real world employment and independent living were not statistically significant for the SPD group. The correlation with UPSA total scores and independent living was statistically significant for the AvPD sample, but the correlation with employment was (r=.41, p=.01). The correlation between work and UPSA scores was not significantly higher in the AvPD group compared to SPD (p=.09).
Table 3.
SPD Participants
| |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cognition | UPSA Total | Comp. | Finance | Comm. | Employment# | Independent Living # | |
UPSA Total | .56** | .09 | .21 | ||||
Comprehension | .23 | .48** | −.21 | .08 | |||
Finance | .17 | .67** | .05 | .06 | .24 | ||
Communication | .43* | .72** | .18 | .37* | −.10 | .37* | |
Transportation | .36* | .77** | .22 | .36* | .33* | .16 | −.09 |
AvPD Participants
| |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cognition | UPSA Total | Comp. | Finance | Comm. | Employment # | Independent Living # | |
UPSA Total | .27 | .41* | .22 | ||||
Comprehension | .31 | .52** | .36* | .28 | |||
Finance | .26 | .54** | .07 | .13 | .20 | ||
Communication | .12 | .65** | .05 | .14 | .27 | .22 | |
Transportation | .07 | .77** | .26 | .31 | .28 | .28 | .22 |
p<.01
p<.05
point-biserial
Discussion
This is the first study of everyday functioning, functional capacity, and NP deficits in a critically important schizophrenia-related condition. We found that the SPD group demonstrated impaired performance on assessments of functional capacity, as well as NP performance, and they also exhibited impairments in real-world functioning. They were less likely to be living independently or to have earned more than a HS diploma than AvPD participants or HC. In addition, the functional impairments found in this sample are comparable to previously reported results in schizophrenia patients. For example, Mausbach et al. (2011) reported that 53% of their sample of schizophrenia patients was living independently, 61% were unemployed, and 40% had earned a bachelor’s degree or higher; these rates are comparable to the current results for our SPD group. Additionally, our SPD group demonstrated UPSA scores comparable to previously reported results in schizophrenia (Leifker et al., 2010; UPSA mean = 72.2, SD = 18.3). Furthermore, the correlations in the SPD group between functional capacity and NP performance were essentially identical to that seen in studies of schizophrenia and bipolar illness. Thus, functional capacity deficits in previous studies of people with schizophrenia and their correlation with NP deficits are not an artifact of severe global cognitive deficits, the age of onset and course of symptoms, and medications. Functional capacity scores were not related to everyday functioning as seen in previous studies of people with schizophrenia (Bowie et al., 2008a; Gold et al., 2012), but in those studies everyday functioning was measured with structured rating scales and not with milestone achievements.
Overall, this study suggests that the UPSA is sensitive to functional capacity deficits in SPD and other personality disorders. Although interpersonal deficits in the SPD population are widely documented, further examination of other domains of functioning in this group is clearly warranted as a first step to identifying effective ways to improve real-world outcomes of all schizophrenia spectrum patients.
There are some limitations in this study. These samples are smaller than previous studies of schizophrenia, but these patients are also less common in psychiatric and research settings. This sample size precludes performing mediation analyses, which could provide a more definitive answer about the role of cognitive impairment and functional capacity in the prediction of real-world outcomes. Our power analysis suggests that we could not have detected significant small correlations, but at the same time, many of the group differences were in fact significant. However, the consistent differences between the subject samples on cognition, UPSA scores, and everyday functioning suggest that the study is not underpowered overall for examining group differences in performance-based assessments. Finally, as noted above, the assessment of everyday functioning was based on achievement of milestones and not on a comprehensive, interview-based assessment of multiple domains of everyday functioning. Those comprehensive interview based measures have been found to most strongly relate to performance on ability-based measures (Harvey et al., 2009; Harvey et al., 2011; Leifker et al.), although even in those studies the correlations between the UPSA and everyday functioning were not much larger than seen in the current study (see for example Bowie et al., 2006, & Bowie et al., 2008, where the partial correlations between UPSA total scores and clinically rated everyday functioning were .40 and .29 respectively compared to .21 in this study).
We (Harvey et al., 2011) have recently argued that performance-based assessments of functional capacity may reflect a central, measurable endophenotype in the schizophrenia spectrum. This argument is based on findings that in schizophrenia disability is consistent across different locations and cultures (Harvey et al., 2009; McIntosh et al., 2011), that it is familial (Kendler et al., 1995; McGrath et al., 2009), that cognitive deficits are intrinsically linked related to functional deficits (Harvey et al., 2011) and that cognitive deficits are heritable and related to genetic variation (Greenwood et al., 2007; Gur et al., 2007). These findings support parts of that argument. In a population related to schizophrenia in various ways, we find that functional capacity manifests similar relationships to NP deficits to those seen in schizophrenia and manifests some evidence of association with real-world disability. This population is unconfounded by other more molar causes of disability such as psychosis and the association between slightly attenuated functional capacity deficits and similarly attenuated NP deficits is essentially identical to that seen in people with schizophrenia. Thus, deficits in functional capacity in the schizophrenia spectrum extend beyond schizophrenia and support the argument that they may reflect cross-diagnosis, potentially genetically transmitted, processes that are associated with impairments in cognition and have the potential to reduce everyday functioning.
Future directions could include interventions aimed at cognitive enhancement in SPD. This could consist either of pharmacological or cognitive-remediation oriented cognitive enhancement therapy or skills-based training. Genetic studies of cognitive and functional endophenotypes currently conducted in psychotic individuals could be expanded to include cases from the schizophrenia spectrum as well. Finally, studies of disability in SPD could take a more fine-grained approach such as has been applied in schizophrenia, with structured interviews aimed at assessment of the components of disability rather than global achievement of milestones, which may be more broadly influenced by extrinsic factors (Rosenheck et al., 2006).
Acknowledgments
Funding body agreements and policies
This research was supported by NIMH Grant Number MH 56140 to Dr. Siever, NIMH Grant Number MH 78775 to Dr. Harvey, and by the VA VISN-3 MIRECC. This research was also supported by Grant Number MO1-RR-00071 from the National Center for Research Resources (NCRR), a component of the National Institutes of Health (NIH). Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of NCRR or NIH.
Acknowledgements
N/a.
Footnotes
Dr. Harvey wished to disclose that in the past year, he has served as a consultant for: Abbott Labs, Boehringer-Ingelheim, Genentech, Johnson and Johnson, Otsuka-America, Pharma-Neuroboost, Roche Pharma, Shire Pharma, Sunovion Pharma. All other authors have no disclosures.
Contributors
Dr. McClure oversaw data collection, analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript. Drs. Harvey and Siever designed the study; they also helped write and revise the manuscript. Drs. Bowie and Iacoviello assisted in overseeing the data collection and revised the manuscript.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- American Psychiatric Association., American Psychiatric Association. Task Force on DSM-IV. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders : DSM-IV-TR. 4. American Psychiatric Association; Washington, DC: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Bowie CR, Depp C, McGrath JA, Wolyniec P, Mausbach BT, Thornquist MH, Luke J, Patterson TL, Harvey PD, Pulver AE. Prediction of real-world functional disability in chronic mental disorders: a comparison of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(9):1116–1124. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09101406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowie CR, Leung WW, Reichenberg A, McClure MM, Patterson TL, Heaton RK, Harvey PD. Predicting schizophrenia patients’ real-world behavior with specific neuropsychological and functional capacity measures. Biol Psychiatry. 2008a;63(5):505–511. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowie CR, McGurk SR, Mausbach B, Patterson TL, Harvey PD. Combined Cognitive Remediation and Functional Skills Training for Schizophrenia: Effects on Cognition, Functional Competence, and Real-World Behavior. Am J Psychiatry. 2012 doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11091337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowie CR, Reichenberg A, McClure MM, Leung WL, Harvey PD. Age-associated differences in cognitive performance in older community dwelling schizophrenia patients: differential sensitivity of clinical neuropsychological and experimental information processing tests. Schizophr Res. 2008b;106(1):50–58. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.10.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowie CR, Reichenberg A, Patterson TL, Heaton RK, Harvey PD. Determinants of real-world functional performance in schizophrenia subjects: correlations with cognition, functional capacity, and symptoms. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:418–425. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cadenhead KS, Perry W, Shafer K, Braff DL. Cognitive functions in schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophr Res. 1999;37(2):123–132. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(98)00147-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold JM, Barch DM, Carter CS, Dakin S, Luck SJ, MacDonald AW, 3rd, Ragland JD, Ranganath C, Kovacs I, Silverstein SM, Strauss M. Clinical, functional, and intertask correlations of measures developed by the Cognitive Neuroscience Test Reliability and Clinical Applications for Schizophrenia Consortium. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38(1):144–152. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold JM, Carpenter C, Randolph C, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR. Auditory working memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54:159–165. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830140071013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green MF. What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153(3):321–330. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.3.321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green MF, Schooler NR, Kern RS, Frese FJ, Granberry W, Harvey PD, Karson CN, Peters N, Stewart M, Seidman LJ, Sonnenberg J, Stone WS, Walling D, Stover E, Marder SR. Evaluation of functionally meaningful measures for clinical trials of cognition enhancement in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(4):400–407. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10030414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenwood TA, Braff DL, Light GA, et al. Initial heritability analyses of endophenotypic measures for schizophrenia: the consortium on the genetics of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:1242–1250. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.11.1242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gronwall DM. Paced auditory serial-addition task: a measure of recovery from concussion. Percept Mot Skills. 1977;44(2):367–373. doi: 10.2466/pms.1977.44.2.367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gur RE, Nimgaonkar VL, Almasy L, Calkins ME, Ragland JD, Pogue-Geile MF, Kanes S, Blangero J, Gur RC. Neurocognitive endophenotypes in a multiplex multigenerational family study of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(5):813–819. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.5.813. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harvey PD. Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia: profile, course, and neurobiological determinants. Handb Clin Neurol. 2012;106:433–445. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-52002-9.00025-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harvey PD, Helldin L, Bowie CR, Heaton RK, Olsson AK, Hjarthag F, Norlander T, Patterson TL. Performance-Based Measurement of Functional Disability in Schizophrenia: A Cross-National Study in the United States and Sweden. Am J Psychiatry. 2009 doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09010106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harvey PD, McClure MM, Patterson TL, McGrath JAAEP, Bowie CR, Siever LJ. Impairment in Functional Capacity as an Endophenotype Candidate in Severe Mental Illness. Schizophr Bull. 2011 doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, McGuire M, Gruenberg AM, Walsh D. Schizotypal symptoms and signs in the Roscommon Family Study: Their factor structure and familial relationship with psychotic and affective disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52:296–303. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950160046009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leifker FR, Patterson TL, Bowie CR, Mausbach BT, Harvey PD. Psychometric properties of performance-based measurements of functional capacity: test-retest reliability, practice effects, and potential sensitivity to change. Schizophr Res. 119(1–3):246–252. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.03.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leifker FR, Patterson TL, Heaton RK, Harvey PD. Validating measures of real-world outcome: the results of the VALERO expert survey and RAND panel. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37(2):334–343. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClure MM, Barch DM, Flory JD, Harvey PD, Siever LJ. Context processing in schizotypal personality disorder: evidence of specificity of impairment to the schizophrenia spectrum. J Abnorm Psychol. 2008;117(2):342–354. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.2.342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClure MM, Bowie CR, Patterson TL, Heaton RK, Weaver C, Anderson H, Harvey PD. Correlations of functional capacity and neuropsychological performance in older patients with schizophrenia: evidence for specificity of relationships? Schizophr Res. 2007;89(1–3):330–338. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.07.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGrath JA, Avramopoulos D, Lasseter VK, Wolyniec PS, Fallin MD, Liang KY, Nestadt G, Thornquist MH, Luke JR, Chen PL, Valle D, Pulver AE. Familiality of Novel Factorial Dimensions of Schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009:591–600. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McIntosh BJ, Zhang XY, Kosten T, Tan SP, Xiu MH, Rakofsky J, Harvey PD. Performance-based assessment of functional skills in severe mental illness: results of a large-scale study in China. J Psychiatr Res. 2011;45(8):1089–1094. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.01.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitropoulou V, Harvey PD, Zegarelli G, New AS, Silverman JM, Siever LJ. Neuropsychological performance in schizotypal personality disorder: importance of working memory. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(10):1896–1903. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moritz S, Mass R. Reduced cognitive inhibition in schizotypy. Br J Clin Psychol. 1997;36 ( Pt 3):365–376. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1997.tb01244.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patterson TL, Goldman S, McKibbin CL, Hughs T, Jeste DV. UCSD Performance-Based Skills Assessment: development of a new measure of everyday functioning for severely mentally ill adults. Schizophr Bull. 2001;27:235–245. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reitan RM, Wolfson D. A selective and critical review of neuropsychological deficits and the frontal lobes. Neuropsychol Rev. 1994;4(3):161–198. doi: 10.1007/BF01874891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roitman SE, Cornblatt BA, Bergman A, Obuchowski M, Mitropoulou V, Keefe RS, Silverman JM, Siever LJ. Attentional functioning in schizotypal personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154(5):655–660. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.5.655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roitman SE, Mitropoulou V, Keefe RS, Silverman JM, Serby M, Harvey PD, Reynolds DA, Mohs RC, Siever LJ. Visuospatial working memory in schizotypal personality disorder patients. Schizophr Res. 2000;41:447–455. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(99)00085-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenheck R, Leslie D, Keefe R, McEvoy J, Swartz M, Perkins D, Stroup S, Hsiao JK, Lieberman J. Barriers to employment for people with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(3):411–417. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sabbag S, Twamley EM, Vella L, Heaton RK, Patterson TL, Harvey PD. Assessing everyday functioning in schizophrenia: not all informants seem equally informative. Schizophr Res. 2011;131(1–3):250–255. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siever LJ, Amin F, Coccaro EF, Trestman R, Silverman J, Horvath TB, Mahon TR, Knott P, Altstiel L, Davidson M, et al. CSF homovanillic acid in schizotypal personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150:149–151. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.1.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silver H, Feldman P, Bilker W, Gur RC. Working memory deficit as a core neuropsychological dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(10):1809–1816. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.10.1809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stuss DT, Guberman A, Nelson R, Larochelle S. The neuropsychology of paramedian thalamic infarction. Brain Cogn. 1988;8(3):348–378. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(88)90059-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voglmaier MM, Seidman LJ, Salisbury D, McCarley RW. Neuropsychological dysfunction in schizotypal personality disorder: a profile analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 1997;41(5):530–540. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(96)00056-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. PsychCorp; San Antonio, TX: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Wiersma D, Wanderling J, Dragomirecka E, Ganev K, Harrison G, An Der Heiden W, Nienhuis FJ, Walsh D. Social disability in schizophrenia: its development and prediction over 15 years in incidence cohorts in six European centres. Psychol Med. 2000;30:1155–1167. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799002627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]