Skip to main content
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences logoLink to Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
. 2013 Jan 7;280(1750):20122363. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2363

Rain reverses diel activity rhythms in an estuarine teleost

Nicholas L Payne 1,, Dylan E van der Meulen 1,2, Ruan Gannon 1, Jayson M Semmens 3, Iain M Suthers 1, Charles A Gray 2, Matthew D Taylor 4
PMCID: PMC3574447  PMID: 23173211

Abstract

Activity rhythms are ubiquitous in nature, and generally synchronized with the day–night cycle. Several taxa have been shown to switch between nocturnal and diurnal activity in response to environmental variability, and these relatively uncommon switches provide a basis for greater understanding of the mechanisms and adaptive significance of circadian (approx. 24 h) rhythms. Plasticity of activity rhythms has been identified in association with a variety of factors, from changes in predation pressure to an altered nutritional or social status. Here, we report a switch in activity rhythm that is associated with rainfall. Outside periods of rain, the estuarine-associated teleost Acanthopagrus australis was most active and in shallower depths during the day, but this activity and depth pattern was reversed in the days following rain, with diurnality restored as estuarine conductivity and turbidity levels returned to pre-rain levels. Although representing the first example of a rain-induced reversal of activity rhythm in an aquatic animal of which we are aware, our results are consistent with established models on the trade-offs between predation risk and foraging efficiency.

Keywords: accelerometry, chronobiology, circadian, rhythms, telemetry, zeitgeber

1. Introduction

Animals should partition energy expenditure into those periods that maximize their foraging or reproductive success. This prediction underpins the biological significance of activity rhythms, which are ubiquitous in nature, and most often identified at the diel (‘circadian’) scale [1,2]. There is mounting evidence that synchronicity between endogenous circadian rhythms and extrinsic cues leads to increased fitness [13], and, given their ubiquity, the general consensus is that circadian rhythms provide an adaptive advantage (for reviews see [1,3]). For example, reproductive success in cyanobacteria [2,4,5], fecundity of pitcher-plant mosquitoes [6], survival rates of Drosophila melanogaster [7] and longevity of golden hamsters Mesocricetus auratus [8] have all been shown to increase when the rhythmicity of exogenous cues match that of internal circadian ones. It is therefore not surprising that significant endeavour has been directed at identifying examples of plasticity in activity rhythms, and in determining the drivers of this plasticity.

While there is a tendency to classify an individual species's activity as either diurnal, nocturnal or crepuscular, examples of activity rhythm plasticity can be drawn from a broad range of taxa, and this plasticity appears to be driven by a variety of factors. The activity rhythms of both golden spiny mice Acomys russatus and golden hamsters Mesocricetus auratus are nocturnal in laboratory settings, but diurnal in the wild [9,10]. Honeybees display socially mediated switches between rhythmic and arrhythmic behaviour [11], and reversals of diel activity in fish have been associated with changes in water temperature [12], nutritional status [13] and habitat type [14]. However, the influence of rainfall on activity rhythms is yet to be evaluated.

Rainfall can have a drastic impact on aquatic environments, often leading to rapid changes in salinity, turbidity, dissolved oxygen and temperature, as well as imposing mechanical challenges as a result of increased water velocities. It seems intuitive that these changes ought to alter the conditions that are optimal for foraging in some aquatic taxa, and that this could drive a departure from normal activity rhythms. Probably due to the logistical challenges associated with observing free-ranging aquatic animals, the influence of rainfall on activity rhythms is generally unknown, and the significance of this knowledge gap is highlighted by the changes to rainfall regimes predicted in coming decades [15].

Biotelemetry is a rapidly evolving discipline that has facilitated the measurement of physiological and behavioural information in a suite of free-ranging aquatic animals (for reviews see [16,1719]). Here, we used accelerometry transmitters to evaluate activity rhythms in the estuarine-associated yellow-fin bream Acanthopagrus australis (Sparidae). Specifically, we tested the predictions that free-ranging A. australis would display circadian rhythms in locomotor activity, and that either the strength or direction of these rhythms would be altered by rain events.

2. Material and methods

(a). Species, study site and accelerometry transmitters

Six Acanthopagrus australis (338–687 g) were captured via hook and line approximately 15 km upstream from the mouth of the Georges River estuary in NSW, Australia (33.977° S, 151.036° E) in late May 2011. Accelerometry transmitters (Vemco, Halifax, Nova Scotia; model V9AP-2L, 69 kHz, 3.3 g in water, 66 mm length) were fitted internally (for surgery technique see [20]), with the longest dimension of the transmitters parallel to fish length within the body cavity, such that the z-axis represented surge (forwards–backwards) of the body, and the x- and y-axes represent dorsoventral and lateral acceleration, respectively (after [21]). We anticipated minimal effects of tagging on fish behaviour, and that these would be limited to the first 4 days post-tagging (as has previously been found for A. australis [22]). Acceleration was sampled at 10 Hz for 33 s only on the x- and y-axes, with a measurement range of ±29.4 m s−2. Acceleration was calculated as a root mean square value from each axis, and these data were transmitted on a pseudorandom schedule every 240 s (on average). Depth was transmitted every second duty cycle, such that the tags transmitted data alternating between acceleration and depth every 120 s, on average. An array of 34 Vemco VR2W acoustic receivers was deployed throughout the estuary, with an approximate spacing of 1.3 km between receivers, and these recorded the transmitted acceleration and depth data when transmitters were within range.

(b). Hydrological variables

A series of five Odyssey (Dataflow Systems Pty Ltd) loggers were deployed approximately equidistantly throughout the length of the estuary, mid-water, which recorded conductivity and temperature every hour. We considered it likely that the foraging ecology of bream would be influenced by water clarity following rain, so a Sea-Bird CTD profiler (SBE 19-plus V2; Sea-Bird Electronics, www.seabird.com) was deployed during a significant rainfall event that occurred in March 2012. Although this event did not coincide with the fish monitoring period, the event was of similar magnitude to one that occurred in July 2011 (rainfall recorded by the Australian Bureau of Meteorology at nearby Bankstown was 82 mm and 94 mm for 21–23 July 2011 and 8–10 Mar 2012, respectively; www.bom.gov.au). The Sea-Bird measured turbidity (NTU) throughout the water column adjacent to each of the 34 receivers, with each Sea-Bird cast occurring within 2 h of high tide on the day of the highest rainfall (8 March), on the following day (9 March), 5 days after rain (13 March) and 8 days after rain (16 March).

(c). Data analysis

Mean values of acceleration and depth per hour were calculated for each individual across the entire monitoring period (25 May to 25 September 2011). Displacement (km) was calculated as the difference in mean hourly position from the overall mean position for each fish (with position defined as the distance of the receiver that recorded a transmission from the estuary mouth). In this way, if the mean overall position of an individual was 15 km from the estuary mouth, a mean hourly position of 16 km would represent a displacement of +1 km. As a measure of the influence of rainfall on estuarine conductivity, hourly data from one Odyssey logger (located approx. 10 km upstream from the estuary mouth) were subjected to a 25-point running mean, in order to remove tidal fluctuations from hourly conductivity. These data were then used as a proxy of freshwater discharge.

As a preliminary examination of bream rhythmicity, a fast Fourier transform was applied to grand mean hourly acceleration values (the mean of average hourly values across the six individuals) across the entire monitoring period, and the resulting spectral density was plotted. Rhythmicity in acceleration (hereafter referred to as activity) data is indicated by peaks in spectral density. A series of linear mixed models were used to examine the influence of time of day (day or night, DN; 07.00–1800 or 18.00–07.00, respectively; sunrise and sunset throughout the monitoring period ranged from 06.00–07.00 and 17.00–18.00, respectively), conductivity and displacement on the activity and depth of A. australis, with individual fish as a random effect. We were interested in whether diel activity patterns varied with location and after rainfall, so included interactions between DN and both displacement and conductivity on the two response variables (activity and depth). We included a first-order correlation structure in our models to account for serial autocorrelation (a violation of the assumption of independence in model-fitting), and response variables were log-transformed to achieve a Gaussian distribution. Predictors were centred following Aitken & West [23] and assessed on the basis of Akaike's information criterion (AIC), with terms eliminated from the model if their removal increased the AIC (ΔAIC) by less than two [24]. Significant interaction terms were interpreted using ‘simple slopes’ parameter estimates after Aitken & West [23].

A significant rain event occurred approximately halfway through monitoring, so we examined temporal shifts in our response variables throughout this section of the time-series by stratifying it into four sections: the 7 days immediately pre-rain (period 1, 14–20 July 2011), immediately post-rain (period 2, 21–27 July 2011), and the two subsequent weeks (period 3, 28 July 2011–3 August 11; period 4, 4–10 August 2011). The choice of 7-day periods was based on the approximate time taken for turbidity to return to background levels after a rain event of similar magnitude (8–10 March; see §2b). Separate mixed models were run for each period, and given that our subjective temporal stratification would lead to relative within-period invariance in conductivity and displacement, we considered inclusion of interactions (as in the full model) inappropriate, so simplified the model to include just DN and the random effect (individual ID).

3. Results

A total of 55 123 depth data and 55 061 activity data were obtained across the six individuals, over a period of 124 days (table S1). Calculating hourly means for each fish resulted in 6622 h with mean activity and corresponding depth data. Bream activity was strongly circadian throughout the monitoring period, with a dominant peak in spectral density occurring at approximately 24 h (figure 1). All terms contributed to the model with lowest AIC for both activity and depth, and interaction terms were significant for both response variables (table 1), with the DN × conductivity term contributing to greater parsimony than the DN × displacement term for models of activity and depth (table 1). Activity increased with conductivity during the day but decreased during the night, such that bream were more diurnally active throughout the upper conductivity values and more nocturnal during the lower conductivity values experienced (figure 2a; figure 3a). Depth decreased with increasing conductivity during daylight hours, but increased with conductivity during the night (figure 2b; figure 3b).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Spectral density of Acanthopagrus australis activity.

Table 1.

Summary of linear mixed-effects models of activity and depth explained by time of day (day/night), relative estuary position (displacement) and conductivity, with individual bream ID as the random effect. Shown are the changes in Akaike's information criterion (ΔAIC) associated with removing interaction terms from the full model. Bold indicates significance level of p < 0.05.

β s.e. t p ΔAIC
activity
intercept −0.252 0.020 −12.590 <0.0001
day/night 0.030 0.005 5.603 <0.0001
conductivity −0.050 0.012 −3.985 <0.0001
displacement 0.206 0.019 11.032 <0.0001
day/night × conductivity 0.040 0.008 4.803 <0.0001 21.0
day/night × displacement 0.032 0.012 2.635 0.008 4.9
depth
intercept 0.449 0.045 9.914 <0.0001
day/night −0.013 0.006 −2.207 0.027
conductivity −0.020 0.021 −0.924 0.355
displacement 0.014 0.032 0.453 0.650
day/night × conductivity −0.038 0.009 −4.302 <0.0001 16.5
day/night × displacement −0.034 0.013 −2.659 0.008 4.4

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Simple slope estimates for interactions between time of day (day, grey lines; night, black lines) and conductivity for Acanthopagrus australis activity and depth. The entire range of x-values are presented, and the y-axis range is mean ± 2 s.d. for both acceleration and depth.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Relationships between conductivity (solid line) and activity and depth (broken lines), presented as a ratio between day and night. Ratios were calculated as the grand mean activity or depth values (n = 6 bream) during each day divided by those of the corresponding night, across the entire monitoring period. Ratios above unity (horizontal line) indicate greater daytime activity or depth, and below the line suggest greater night-time values. Shaded columns indicate periods 1 to 4 (from left to right, respectively).

During the week immediately subsequent to the July rain event (period 2), activity was significantly greater during the night than during the day, but the opposite activity pattern was seen for periods 1, 3 and 4 (figure 3a; table 2). A similar trend was observed for depth, with greater daytime depth occurring during period 2, and greater night-time depth during periods 1, 3 and 4 (figure 3b; table 2). The magnitude of the parameter estimates for period 4 was more than double those of other periods for both activity and depth, suggesting a strengthening of diel activity and depth pattern for this period (table 2). Visual inspection of displacement throughout the monitoring period suggested a generally positive relationship between displacement and conductivity (downstream movement following rain), although this was highly variable across individuals (figure 4).

Table 2.

Summary of linear mixed-effects models (individual bream ID random) of activity and depth explained by time of day (day/night) for the month around the July rain event. Periods 1–4 represent data from the 7 days prior to the rain event, immediately after the rain event, and from the subsequent two weeks, respectively. Parameter estimates (β) with positive sign indicate higher daytime values. Bold indicates significance level of p<0.05.

β s.e. d.f. t p
activity
period 1 0.028 0.007 463 3.85 <0.05
period 2 −0.021 0.009 430 −2.23 <0.05
period 3 0.027 0.009 457 3.10 <0.05
period 4 0.079 0.011 309 7.17 <0.05
depth
period 1 −0.016 0.006 463 −2.480 <0.05
period 2 0.025 0.011 430 2.292 <0.05
period 3 −0.034 0.011 457 −3.261 <0.05
period 4 −0.078 0.012 309 −6.751 <0.05

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Time-series of conductivity (grey line) and displacement (daily mean±s.e., n = 6 Acanthopagrus australis) throughout the monitoring period.

Rainfall preceded a drastic change in turbidity, with a significant increase seen for the upper half of the estuary on the day of heaviest rain (figure 5b), with this increased turbidity spreading to the lower reaches on the following day (figure 5c). Turbidity levels had decreased significantly 5 days after rainfall, although levels in the upper half of the estuary remained somewhat higher than those prior to rainfall (figure 5d versus 5a). Eight days post-rain, turbidity levels had reached levels comparable with those prior to rain (figure 5e versus 5a). Water temperature was relatively invariant throughout the monitoring period, with rain events associated with changes on the order of 1°C or less throughout the estuary.

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Change in turbidity subsequent to a rainfall event in the Georges River during March 2012. Sampling days are indicated as (a) prior to the rain event (10 February 2012), (b) on the day of heaviest rain (8 March 2012), (c) on the following day, and then (d) 5 and (e) 8 days after the rain event, respectively. Shown are mean turbidity values for each 0.5 m bin throughout the water column for each sample, and these are extrapolated across the x-axis for each sampling day. Distances from the estuary mouth are shown on the x-axis.

4. Discussion

Here, we provide the first example of a rain-induced switch in activity rhythm for an aquatic animal. Bream were more active and inhabited shallower depths during the day under normal conditions, but became nocturnal following rain, with the timing of a return to diurnal behaviour matching that of the return to normal estuarine conductivity and turbidity. Switches between diurnal and nocturnal or arrhythmic behaviour have been observed in various mammals [9,10,25] and insects [11], with predation risk and social structure the most commonly identified drivers of the shift. In fishes, variations in social rank [26], energetic status [13,27] and water temperature [12] have been associated with shifts between diurnal and nocturnal activity (for a review see [28]); however, we are not aware of any other studies that have documented a shift in activity rhythms associated with rainfall.

(a). What is the proximate driver of the shift in activity?

Rainfall is often associated with a change in location for estuarine fishes [2932], and this confounds a mechanistic understanding of a change in behaviour. It is possible that the switch from diurnal to nocturnal activity was driven by hydrological parameters (e.g. turbidity, salinity, etc.); however, it could also be due to a difference in habitats and/or prey assemblages along the estuary (there was a general downstream movement of bream following rain). Indeed, habitat type has previously been identified as the driver of activity rhythm plasticity in fish [14]. It seems intuitive that exposure to an unfamiliar habitat would reduce boldness during the day (when visual predators such as bream are more effective), and it is often suggested that a switch to nocturnal foraging represents a predator-avoidance mechanism (see [28] for a review of this topic). In this study, however, while time of day interacted with both conductivity and displacement, the day/night × conductivity term contributed to greater parsimony than that of displacement, suggesting a change in hydrology is more important for altering activity patterns than a change in location. Where hydrological change is the proximal mechanism, the switch to nocturnal activity here is consistent with theoretical models on the trade-offs between foraging efficiency and predation risk [33].

An optimal foraging mode should minimize the ratio between instantaneous mortality rates and net food intake [3438], and there are several examples where changes in metabolic demand drive a shift in activity pattern. Atlantic salmon switch from nocturnal to diurnal foraging with increasing water temperature (and therefore metabolic rate [12]), with the switch dependent on food availability [34]. Despite increased foraging efficiency during the day, well-nourished European minnows are predominantly nocturnal, but expose themselves to greater predation risks by becoming increasingly diurnal as their energetic reserves decline [13]. Many euryhaline fish experience elevated metabolic rates when exposed to salinity fluctuations in a laboratory setting [39,40], and mechanical challenges imposed by increased water velocities following rainfall are likely to further increase energetic demands of fish in an estuarine environment. Given that both penaeid prawns (a locally abundant prey item) and the potential predator Argyrosomus japonicus are generally nocturnal in eastern Australia [4143], it is certainly feasible that diurnal activity represents a selection for low-efficiency foraging and low predation risk. With an increased metabolic burden imposed by rain, however, bream may be unable to acquire sufficient energy during daylight, and may be forced to forage under conditions of higher prey abundance and increased predation risk (i.e. at night). The problem of diurnal food acquisition is likely to be exacerbated by the foraging impairment imposed by the drastic increase in turbidity following rain, with turbidity being shown to reduce both the incidence and efficiency of foraging in several fish species [44,45].

(b). Significance of the change in conditions for optimal foraging

Switches between diurnal and nocturnal activity (such as that described here) are generally considered to mask endogenous circadian rhythms that are entrained to environmental cues such as the diel cycle [46]. These circadian rhythms are involved in the regulation of several fundamental processes, such as gene expression, metabolism and reproduction [1,3]. Thus, departures from synchronous activity and circadian rhythms should provide the evidence of strong selective pressures, and provide some basis for evaluating the adaptive significance of circadian rhythms [1,3]. Whether the proximal mechanism for the switch in bream activity is a change in conductivity, turbidity, habitat or a combination of these, rainfall facilitated a wholesale shift in the conditions that are optimal for partitioning of bream activity. Further, while a return to diurnal activity 1 week after rain does not necessarily suggest greater foraging success outside rain events, it could be expected that a depletion of energy reserves as a result of suboptimal nocturnal foraging be succeeded by an increase in foraging intensity following a return to optimal foraging conditions. Our data are consistent with this prediction, with a greater disparity between day and night activity (and depth) occurring during period 4 than during other periods. There is abundant anecdotal evidence that suggests catchability of A. australis is low immediately following a rain event, but abnormally high during subsequent days.

While the activity reversal seen for A. australis is significant for understanding the mechanisms that entrain animal activity to environmental cues, a shift in foraging strategy associated with rainfall also has implications with respect to a changing climate. The frequency and magnitude of rain events are predicted to change dramatically in coming decades [15], and with increasing anthropogenic control of freshwater discharge into river systems worldwide, understanding the fitness costs associated with a reversal of foraging mode will be critical for predicting population viability, particularly for species that are already facing significant pressure from recreational and commercial fishing, such as A. australis.

Acknowledgements

We thank all volunteers for assistance with fieldwork, particularly Alex Pursche, Gwenael Cadiou, Cian Foster-Thorpe, Abbo Van-Neer and Teagan Marzullo. Financial support was provided by an Australian Research Council Linkage Grant (LP100100367) with the NSW Recreational Fishing Trust. Animal use was facilitated by a UNSW ACEC permit (11/30A).

References

  • 1.Sharma VK. 2003. Adaptive significance of circadian clocks. Chronobiol. Int. 20, 901–919 10.1081/cbi-120026099 (doi:10.1081/cbi-120026099) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hut RA, Beersma DGM. 2011. Evolution of time-keeping mechanisms: early emergence and adaptation to photoperiod. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 366, 2141–2154 10.1098/rstb.2010.0409 (doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0409) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Yerushalmi S, Green RM. 2009. Evidence for the adaptive significance of circadian rhythms. Ecol. Lett. 12, 970–981 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2009.01343.x (doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2009.01343.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Ouyang Y, Andersson CR, Kondo T, Golden S, Johnson CH. 1998. Resonating circadian clocks enhance fitness in cyanobacteria. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 8660–8664 10.1073/pnas.95.15.8660 (doi:10.1073/pnas.95.15.8660) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Woelfle MA, Yan OY, Phanvijhitsiri K, Johnson CH. 2004. The adaptive value of circadian clocks: an experimental assessment in cyanobacteria. Curr. Biol. 14, 1481–1486 10.1016/j.cub.2004.08.023 (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2004.08.023) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Emerson KJ, Bradshaw WE, Holzapfel CM. 2008. Concordance of the circadian clock with the environment is necessary to maximize fitness in natural populations. Evolution 62, 979–983 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00324.x (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00324.x) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Pittendrigh CS, Minis DH. 1972. Circadian systems: longevity as a function of circadian resonance in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 69, 1537–1539 10.1073/pnas.69.6.1537 (doi:10.1073/pnas.69.6.1537) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hurd MW, Ralph MR. 1998. The significance of circadian organization for longevity in the golden hamster. J. Biol. Rhythms 13, 430–436 10.1177/074873098129000255 (doi:10.1177/074873098129000255) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gattermann R, et al. 2008. Golden hamsters are nocturnal in captivity but diurnal in nature. Biol. Lett. 4, 253–255 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0066 (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2008.0066) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Levy O, Dayan T, Kronfeld-Schor N. 2007. The relationship between the golden spiny mouse circadian system and its diurnal activity: an experimental field enclosures and laboratory study. Chronobiol. Int. 24, 599–613 10.1080/07420520701534640 (doi:10.1080/07420520701534640) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bloch G, Robinson GE. 2001. Chronobiology: reversal of honeybee behavioural rhythms. Nature 410, 1048. 10.1038/35074183 (doi:10.1038/35074183) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Fraser NHC, Metcalfe NB, Thorpe JE. 1993. Temperature-dependent switch between diurnal and nocturnal foraging in salmon. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 252, 135–139 10.1098/rspb.1993.0057 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1993.0057) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Metcalfe NB, Steele GI. 2001. Changing nutritional status causes a shift in the balance of nocturnal to diurnal activity in European Minnows. Funct. Ecol. 15, 304–309 10.1046/j.1365-2435.2001.00527.x (doi:10.1046/j.1365-2435.2001.00527.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Fox RJ, Bellwood DR. 2011. Unconstrained by the clock? Plasticity of diel activity rhythm in a tropical reef fish, Siganus lineatus. Funct. Ecol. 25, 1096–1105 10.1111/j.1365-2435.2011.01874.x (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2435.2011.01874.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Allan RP, Soden BJ. 2008. Atmospheric warming and the amplification of precipitation extremes. Science 321, 1481–1484 10.1126/science.1160787 (doi:10.1126/science.1160787) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Cooke SJ, et al. 2004. Biotelemetry: a mechanistic approach to ecology. Trends Ecol. Evol. 19, 334–343 10.1016/j.tree.2004.04.003 (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2004.04.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Heupel MR, Semmens JM, Hobday AJ. 2006. Automated acoustic tracking of aquatic animals: scales, design and deployment of listening station arrays. Mar. Freshw. Res. 57, 1–13 10.1071/MF05091 (doi:10.1071/MF05091) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Halsey LG, Shepard ELC, Wilson RP. 2011. Assessing the development and application of the accelerometry technique for estimating energy expenditure. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Mol. Integr. Physiol. 158, 305–314 10.1016/j.cbpa.2010.09.002 (doi:10.1016/j.cbpa.2010.09.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.How JR, de Lestang S. 2012. Acoustic tracking: issues affecting design, analysis and interpretation of data from movement studies. Mar. Freshw. Res. 63, 312–324 10.1071/mf11194 (doi:10.1071/mf11194) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Walsh CT, Reinfelds IV, Gray CA, West RJ, van der Meulen DE, Craig JR. 2012. Seasonal residency and movement patterns of two co-occurring catadromous percichthyids within a south-eastern Australian river. Ecol. Freshw. Fish. 21, 145–159 10.1111/j.1600-0633.2011.00534.x (doi:10.1111/j.1600-0633.2011.00534.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Payne NL, Gillanders BM, Seymour RS, Webber DM, Snelling EP, Semmens JM. 2011. Accelerometry estimates field metabolic rate in giant Australian cuttlefish Sepia apama during breeding. J. Anim. Ecol. 80, 422–430 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2010.01758.x (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2656.2010.01758.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Butcher PA, Broadhurst MK, Orchard BA, Ellis MT. 2010. Using biotelemetry to assess the mortality and behaviour of yellowfin bream (Acanthopagrus australis) released with ingested hooks. ICES J. Mar. Sci. 67, 1175–1184 10.1093/icesjms/fsq028 (doi:10.1093/icesjms/fsq028) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Aitken LS, West SG. 1991. Multiple regression: testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications Inc [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Burnham KP, Anderson DR. 2002. Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach, 2nd edn New York, NY: Springer [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Fenn MGP, Macdonald DW. 1995. Use of middens by red foxes: risk reverses rhythms of rats. J. Mammal 76, 130–136 10.2307/1382321 (doi:10.2307/1382321) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Alanara A, Burns MD, Metcalfe NB. 2001. Intraspecific resource partitioning in brown trout: the temporal distribution of foraging is determined by social rank. J. Anim. Ecol. 70, 980–986 10.1046/j.0021-8790.2001.00550.x (doi:10.1046/j.0021-8790.2001.00550.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Metcalfe NB, Fraser NHC, Burns MD. 1998. State-dependent shifts between nocturnal and diurnal activity in salmon. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265, 1503–1507 10.1098/rspb.1998.0464 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0464) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Reebs SG. 2002. Plasticity of diel and circadian activity rhythms in fishes. Rev. Fish. Biol. Fish. 12, 349–371 10.1023/a:1025371804611 (doi:10.1023/a:1025371804611) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Sakabe R, Lyle JM. 2010. The influence of tidal cycles and freshwater inflow on the distribution and movement of an estuarine resident fish Acanthopagrus butcheri. J. Fish. Biol. 77, 643–660 10.1111/j.1095-8649.2010.02703.x (doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2010.02703.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Childs AR, et al. 2008. Do environmental factors influence the movement of estuarine fish? A case study using acoustic telemetry. Estuar. Coast. Shelf Sci. 78, 227–236 10.1016/j.ecss.2007.12.003 (doi:10.1016/j.ecss.2007.12.003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Taylor BR, Macinnis C, Floyd TA. 2010. Influence of rainfall and beaver dams on upstream movement of spawning Atlantic salmon in a restored brook in Nova Scotia, Canada. River Res. Appl. 26, 183–193 10.1002/rra.1252 (doi:10.1002/rra.1252) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hindell JS, Jenkins GP, Womersley B. 2008. Habitat utilisation and movement of black bream Acanthopagrus butcheri (Sparidae) in an Australian estuary. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 366, 219–229 10.3354/meps07519 (doi:10.3354/meps07519) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Railsback SF, Harvey BC, Hayse JW, LaGory KE. 2005. Tests of theory for diel variation in salmonid feeding activity and habitat use. Ecology 86, 947–959 10.1890/04-1178 (doi:10.1890/04-1178) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Metcalfe NB, Fraser NHC, Burns MD. 1999. Food availability and the nocturnal vs. diurnal foraging trade-off in juvenile salmon. J. Anim. Ecol. 68, 371–381 10.1046/j.1365-2656.1999.00289.x (doi:10.1046/j.1365-2656.1999.00289.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Gilliam JF, Fraser DF. 1987. Habitat selection under predation hazard: test of a model with foraging minnows. Ecology 68, 1856–1862 10.2307/1939877 (doi:10.2307/1939877) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Ochwada F, Loneragan NR, Gray CA, Suthers IM, Taylor MD. 2009. Complexity affects habitat preference and predation mortality in postlarval Penaeus plebejus: implications for stock enhancement. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 380, 161–171 10.3354/meps07936 (doi:10.3354/meps07936) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Ochwada-Doyle F, Gray CA, Loneragan NR, Taylor MD. 2010. Using experimental ecology to understand stock enhancement: comparisons of habitat-related predation on wild and hatchery-reared Penaeus plebejus Hess. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 390, 65–71 10.1016/j.jembe.2010.04.003 (doi:10.1016/j.jembe.2010.04.003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Taylor MD, Fairfax AV, Suthers IM. In press. The race for space: using acoustic telemetry to understand density-dependent emigration and habitat selection in a released predatory fish. Rev. Fish. Sci. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Dalla Via J, Villani P, Gasteiger E, Niederstatter H. 1998. Oxygen consumption in sea bass fingerling Dicentrarchus labrax exposed to acute salinity and temperature changes: metabolic basis for maximum stocking density estimations. Aquaculture 169, 303–313 10.1016/s0044-8486(98)00375-5 (doi:10.1016/s0044-8486(98)00375-5) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Sangiao-Alvarellos S, Arjona FJ, del Rio MPM, Miguez JM, Mancera JM, Soengas JL. 2005. Time course of osmoregulatory and metabolic changes during osmotic acclimation in Sparus auratus. J. Exp. Biol. 208, 4291–4304 10.1242/jeb.01900 (doi:10.1242/jeb.01900) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Taylor MD, Laffan SD, Fielder DS, Suthers IM. 2006. Key habitat and home range of mulloway Argyrosomus japonicus in a south-east Australian estuary: finding the estuarine niche to optimise stocking. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 328, 237–247 10.3354/meps328237 (doi:10.3354/meps328237) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Taylor MD, Ko A. 2011. Monitoring acoustically tagged king prawns Penaeus (Melicertus) plebejus in an estuarine lagoon. Mar. Biol. 158, 835–844 10.1007/s00227-010-1610-6 (doi:10.1007/s00227-010-1610-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Wassenberg TJ, Hill BJ. 1994. Laboratory study of the effect of light on the emergence behaviour of 8 species of commercially important adult penaeid prawns. Mar. Freshw. Res. 45, 43–50 10.1071/MF9940043 (doi:10.1071/MF9940043) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Gray SM, Sabbah S, Hawryshyn CW. 2011. Experimentally increased turbidity causes behavioural shifts in Lake Malawi cichlids. Ecol. Freshw. Fish. 20, 529–536 10.1111/j.1600-0633.2011.00501.x (doi:10.1111/j.1600-0633.2011.00501.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Huenemann TW, Dibble ED, Fleming JP. 2012. Influence of turbidity on the foraging of largemouth bass. Trans. Am. Fish Soc. 141, 107–111 10.1080/00028487.2011.651554 (doi:10.1080/00028487.2011.651554) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Mrosovsky N. 1999. Masking: history, definitions, and measurement. Chronobiol. Int. 16, 415–429 10.3109/07420529908998717 (doi:10.3109/07420529908998717) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES