Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2012 Jun 13;32(24):8149–8157. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0485-12.2012

Differentiating Hemispheric Contributions to Syntax and Semantics in Patients with Left-Hemisphere Lesions

Paul Wright 1, Emmanuel A Stamatakis 2, Lorraine K Tyler 1,
PMCID: PMC3575031  EMSID: EMS51869  PMID: 22699896

Abstract

Understanding the relationship between brain and cognition critically depends on data from brain-damaged patients since these provide major constraints on identifying the essential components of brain–behavior systems. Here we relate structural and functional fMRI data with behavioral data in 21 human patients with chronic left hemisphere (LH) lesions and a range of language impairments to investigate the controversial issue of the role of the hemispheres in different language functions. We address this issue within a dual neurocognitive model of spoken language comprehension in which core linguistic functions, e.g., syntax, depend critically upon an intact left frontotemporal system, whereas more general communicative abilities, e.g., semantics, are supported by a bilateral frontotemporal system and may recover from LH damage through normal or enhanced activity in the intact right hemisphere. The fMRI study used a word-monitoring task that differentiated syntactic and semantic aspects of sentence comprehension. We distinguished overlapping interactions between structure, neural activity, and performance using joint independent components analysis, identifying two structural–functional networks, each with a distinct relationship with performance. Syntactic performance correlated with tissue integrity and activity in a left frontotemporal network. Semantic performance correlated with activity in right superior/middle temporal gyri regardless of tissue integrity. Right temporal activity did not differ between patients and controls, suggesting that the semantic network is degenerately organized, with regions in both hemispheres able to perform similar computations. Our findings support the dual neurocognitive model of spoken language comprehension and emphasize the importance of linguistic specificity in investigations of language recovery in patients.

Introduction

Although some cognitive functions are impaired following brain damage, others are preserved. Understanding the nature of these preservations and declines is critical for elucidating the relationship between cognition and brain and for developing effective interventions. Conservation of cognitive functions can be achieved by means of functional reorganization, in which regions not typically involved in a specific function are recruited, or by residual functionality of the damaged normal system (Rosen et al., 2000; Zahn et al., 2004; Crinion and Price, 2005; Saur et al., 2006). For language function, it remains unclear which linguistic components can be preserved following left hemisphere (LH) language system damage and which neural mechanisms underlie their preservation. Here we investigate the differential contributions of left-lateralized and bilateral language systems to syntactic and semantic aspects of spoken language comprehension in patients with LH damage using a multivariate method combining structure, function, and performance (Calhoun et al., 2006).

We frame our research within a neurocognitive model of spoken language comprehension (Bozic et al., 2010) that differentiates between core linguistic functions, such as syntax, which depend upon the integrity of a LH frontotemporal (FT) network (Tyler et al., 2011), and broader communicative semantic/pragmatic interpretation supported by a bilateral FT system. Semantic and pragmatic inference may be preserved in patients with LH damage and syntactic deficits (Ostrin and Tyler, 1995; Hagoort, 1997; Tyler and Marslen-Wilson, 2008), raising the possibility that the right hemisphere (RH) residue of the bilateral system may suffice to preserve performance due to degenerate organization (Friston and Price, 2003). This mechanism relies on normally bilateral activity, as distinct from adaptive recruitment of novel RH regions (Saur et al., 2006).

To test this hypothesis, we collected fMRI data while patients listened to spoken sentences in which we manipulated syntactic and semantic information, and we measured patients' comprehension abilities using various tasks. Syntactic information activates left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and posterior middle temporal gyrus (MTG) (Caplan et al., 1996; Tyler et al., 2011). Damage to either region, or the connectivity between them, disrupts syntactic performance (Papoutsi et al., 2011) leaving semantic processing, associated with bilateral MTG, relatively unimpaired. Given the interdependencies between syntax and semantics (Marslen-Wilson and Tyler, 1980) and the two processes' instantiation in partially overlapping neural networks involving left MTG, univariate analyses may not distinguish them, nor can they describe the three-way relationship between performance, tissue integrity, and neural activity that may reflect functional reorganization.

We therefore used joint independent component analysis (jICA) (Calhoun et al., 2006) to combine structural and functional MR images and to differentiate spatially distinct structural–functional networks, each with a variable level of expression across patients. We identified networks relevant to syntax after accounting for regions involved in semantics, and vice versa, by correlating components with performance in two tasks. We predicted that preserved syntax in the patients would be associated with activity and tissue integrity in the LH frontotemporal network (Tyler et al., 2010b, 2011), whereas semantics—normally supported bilaterally—may correlate with normal or enhanced activity in right MTG.

Materials and Methods

Participants

We recruited 21 chronic brain-damaged patients with LH damage [mean (SD) age, 57.4 (12.5) years; 6 female] who were at least 1.4 years postinjury [mean (SD), 6.5 (7.5) years], were native British English speakers, and were right-handed before their stroke. We excluded anyone who could not give informed consent, could not follow instructions, had magnetic resonance contraindications, or had multiple brain injuries. Patients' lesions involved a wide range of LH areas including the ventral frontal lobe (inferior frontal gyrus, ventral precentral gyrus, and occasionally middle frontal gyrus), the majority of the temporal lobe (especially the superior and middle temporal gyri) and perisylvian parietal regions (angular and supramarginal gyri; Fig. 1). Patients' lesions arose from stroke (n = 18) or surgery (n = 3). Patients varied in their ability to produce language: some patients were fluent while others were only able to produce single words. All patients were able to give informed consent and to understand task instructions but their language comprehension abilities varied (see Results, Behavioral results, below).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Lesion frequency map. Voxel color indicates the number of patients with damage (maximum 12 out of 21). Left, Surface rendering on left hemisphere. Middle and right, Sagittal sections at the indicated MNI x-coordinate.

We tested a group of 21 healthy controls, age-matched to the patient group [mean (SD) age, 60.0 (11.5) years; controls vs patients: p = 0.48; 9 female]. All control participants were right-handed native speakers of British English with no history of neurological or psychiatric illness and no evidence of severe hearing impairment or cognitive impairment. The research was approved by the East of England Research Ethics Committees.

Behavioral tasks

fMRI on-line word monitoring task.

Participants performed a word-monitoring task in the scanner, which patients can perform without difficulty (Tyler, 1992). The task minimizes working memory demands and has been used extensively in studies of impaired and healthy populations (Blank et al., 1981; Friederici, 1985; Tyler, 1992; Ostrin and Tyler, 1995). The procedure used in the current study was previously described by Tyler et al. (2010a,b). Participants listened to spoken sequences, each of which was preceded by a visually presented target word that they were asked to respond to with a button press. Target words were presented throughout the entire trial to minimize working memory demands.

Three types of spoken stimuli were presented, in which we varied the availability of syntactic and semantic information to distinguish their respective contributions to sentence processing (Table 1). Normal Prose (NP) sentences were both grammatical and semantically and pragmatically plausible. Anomalous Prose (AP) sentences were syntactically correct, with their grammatical structure matched to the NP sentences, but lacked coherent sentential/pragmatic meaning. Random Word Order (RWO) consisted of strings of words with no grammatical or sentential meaning. Half the RWO strings were derived from NP sentences and half from AP sentences.

Table 1.

Examples of the prose stimuli used in the word monitoring task

Prose type Target word position Spoken sequence (target word in capitals)
Normal prose Early Jane didn't enjoy herself very much. Her NECK was stiff because she had a bad cold and she couldn't lift anything properly.
Late I saw Bob in the library yesterday. He was trying to find the name of the TREE he planted last year.
Anomalous prose Early Stephen didn't catch himself very much. Her TOOTH was driven because he had a weak nail and she couldn't heat anyone properly.
Late He set Richard up the sleep yesterday. She was writing to use the college of a FISH she opened last week.
Random word order Early Very Stephen catch much himself didn't. Her NOSE because properly had anyone couldn't he and nail weak a heat driven was.
Late The set he yesterday sleep Richard up. Use was college a to writing she of ROAD last opened she week.

We measured the processing of sentential information by manipulating the position of the target word in the sentence. In NP and AP, target words occurring later in the sentence elicit faster response times (RTs) than earlier targets, but in RWO, target word position does not affect RT (Marslen-Wilson and Tyler, 1980; Tyler et al., 2010a). Faster monitoring latencies to later compared with earlier words [the word position effect (WPE)] in NP reflects the on-line construction of a syntactically structured, meaningful representation spanning the sentence, whereas the WPE in AP is underpinned primarily by syntactic processing, without the contribution of sentential or pragmatic meaning (Marslen-Wilson and Tyler, 1975, 1980). The absence of a WPE in RWO confirms this interpretation and rules out effects based solely on anticipation. We calculated the WPE for each condition using the formula WPE = (early RT − late RT)/mean RT. Single-trial RTs were inverse transformed before averaging to reduce the influence of outliers (Ratcliff, 1993).

The task was presented in the scanner using a blocked design because patients may have difficulty rapidly switching between tasks. Presentation was divided into two scans, with a short break in between, with equal numbers of each trial type in each scan. Each scan comprised 15 trials of NP (165 s), followed by 12 trials of silent baseline (132 s), 15 trials of RWO (165 s), 12 trials of acoustic baseline (132 s, not used in current analysis), and 15 trials of AP (165 s). Stimulus presentation was controlled by a PC running in-house software, via pneumatic insert earphones (Etymotic Research) and an LCD projector viewed with a mirror inside the MRI head coil. Participants responded using an MRI-compatible button box.

Sentence–picture matching task (out of scanner).

We obtained additional measures of language impairment using a sentence–picture matching task, which is frequently used to assess syntactic and semantic abilities in impaired populations (Caramazza and Zurif, 1976; Ostrin and Tyler, 1995; Tyler et al., 2002, 2009, 2011; Wilson et al., 2010). Patients heard a sentence and were asked to match it to one of three line drawings. Each sentence described an activity involving an agent and a patient (e.g., “The boy chases the horse”) and was semantically reversible, in that either entity could perform the action (e.g., “The horse chases the boy”). Of the three line-drawings presented to the patient, one correctly depicted the scene described in the sentence (e.g., a boy chasing a horse), a second depicted a similar scene but with the agent and patient reversed (a reverse role distractor, e.g., a horse chasing a boy) and the third involved a change in the action/verb (a lexical distractor, e.g., a boy riding a horse). Patients with syntactic impairments make reverse role errors with few lexical distractor errors (Black et al., 1991), whereas patients with semantic impairment show the opposite pattern.

Image acquisition

MR images were acquired using a 3T Tim Trio MRI scanner (Siemens Medical Solutions) at the MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK, as reported previously (Tyler et al., 2010a,b). Functional scanning used sparse sampling with 2 s scans interspersed with 9 s of silence to prevent scanner noise interfering with spoken stimuli (Hall et al., 1999). Target cues were presented 1 s after scan onset, and auditory stimuli were presented 1.1 s later, 100 ms after the onset of silence. The variable duration between the end of the spoken stimulus and the following scan ensured sampling of a range of time points around the time of the likely peak hemodynamic response. Functional imaging parameters were as follows: 32 oblique axial slices angled away from the eyes, each 3 mm thick with interslice gap of 0.75 mm; in-plane resolution of 3 mm; FOV of 192 × 192 mm; total TR = 11 s (2 s acquisition + 9 s silence); TE = 30 ms; and flip angle = 78°. Structural imaging parameters were as follows: T1-weighted MPRAGE sequence, 1 mm isotropic resolution in the sagittal plane, TR = 2250 ms, TI = 900 ms, TE = 2.99 ms, and flip angle = 9°.

Imaging processing

Functional and structural images were preprocessed using SPM5 (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, University College London, London, UK). Functional images were realigned to correct for head movement and coregistered with the structural images. Structural images were normalized to Montreal Neurological Institute space using unified segmentation-normalization with high warping regularization to prevent distortion of lesions (Crinion et al., 2007). Unified normalization failed in one patient, so cost function masking was used for normalization (Brett et al., 2001). Images from controls were also normalized using unified normalization, but default warping regularization was used. Normalization parameters from structural images were applied to functional images, which were then smoothed using a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm FWHM.

Functional images for each participant were entered into a general linear model (GLM) with regressors describing the onset of stimuli in each condition (NP, AP, RWO, and acoustic baseline) convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function, the six movement parameters from realignment as uninteresting variables, and a high-pass filter with cutoff period of 660 s. The duration of the stimuli in the model was set to zero, since with our TR of 11 s, the hemodynamic response to each stimulus was sampled almost exclusively by the single following scan. The silent baseline was modeled implicitly. The general linear model produced a contrast image for each condition, describing the effect size for each stimulus type above the silent baseline at each voxel. Contrast image voxels in damaged tissue were identified automatically [as described in Additional imaging analyses, below, and in Stamatakis and Tyler (2005)] and were set to zero on a patient-by-patient basis [as described previously by Tyler et al. (2010b)].

Finally, T1-weighted structural images were processed for analysis of tissue integrity. We used T1-weighted image intensity, rather than segmented gray- or white-matter images, because the location and extent of damage in stroke and resection do not respect tissue class and the T1 intensity provides a measure of damage due to either ischemia or resection in both gray and white matter. Moreover, this method previously demonstrated robust associations between damage and highly specific cognitive measures (Bright et al., 2005; Tyler et al., 2005a,b, 2009, 2010b). Structural images were deskulled using the standard SPM brain mask and then smoothed using a Gaussian kernel of 10 mm FWHM. A larger smoothing kernel was used for structural images than functional images to reflect the greater spatial extent of the expected effects. Because jICA produces separate images for each data type, the images are not required to have the same resolution or smoothing parameters.

Joint independent components analysis

We used a multivariate analysis to discriminate structural–functional networks involved in syntactic and semantic processing. The jICA method, implemented in the Fusion toolbox (Calhoun et al., 2006), allows us to describe tissue integrity and activity relating to behavioral performance on one aspect of language function (e.g., semantics) while accounting separately for variability in tissue integrity and activity related to performance on another (e.g., syntax). The jICA approach combines data from the functional and structural images, and then reduces the combined data to a set of independent components. Each component comprises maps of tissue integrity and activity—these are correlated across subjects but may be anatomically distinct—and a vector of loading values describing the component's level of expression across patients. Patients with high tissue integrity and/or activity values in the areas described in the respective maps have high loading values and vice versa. The loading values were interpreted post hoc by testing their correlations with performance. Our post hoc analyses test the relative variability in the loading values within each component since the interpretation of the absolute magnitude of the loading values is not straightforward.

We performed jICA analysis using the Fusion ICA Toolbox (FIT v2.0b, The MIND Research Network, http://icatb.sourceforge.net) with the following parameters. We entered three features into the analysis: tissue integrity, activity for NP-silence (semantics + syntax), and activity for AP-silence (syntax only). Since our focus was on sentential semantics and syntax, we did not include the RWO condition, which comprised word strings with neither grammar nor meaning. Tissue integrity was represented by smoothed, deskulled structural images (see Imaging processing, above) and activity represented by the contrast images (weighted betas) generated by the GLM.

Univariate analyses with structural images typically include global signal as a confound (Tyler et al., 2005a), but this was not possible within the jICA framework. We therefore tested the effect of including global signal by running a univariate, voxelwise correlation between structural images and WPE AP both with and without global signal as a covariate of no interest in the model. We used WPE AP because this score correlated with tissue integrity in the jICA results. Both analyses produced a cluster centered on the left IFG (LIFG), with the cluster without global signal correction contained entirely within the cluster with global signal correction. We therefore concluded that omitting global signal correction gave similar but more conservative results for our set of structural images, making it unlikely that the jICA results would be driven by intersubject variability in global signal. We also tested the effect of image inhomogeneity by running a separate jICA using bias-corrected images produced by the SPM5 segmentation algorithm (Ashburner and Friston, 2005). The results of the two analyses were highly similar, with virtually indistinguishable patterns in the feature images and highly correlated loading values between corresponding components (r > 0.98). We report results using non-bias-corrected images.

We set the number of jICA components to seven, which was one-third of the sample size (n = 21). This heuristic was previously used to set the number of components when the minimum description length algorithm failed to converge (Specht et al., 2009), as was the case for our data. ICA was performed using the Infomax algorithm. The output feature images were scaled to z scores. Images were thresholded at z = 3.09 (p < 0.001) and a cluster size of 47 voxels (1.27 cm3). Cluster size threshold was set to the average needed to achieve p < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons, in the standard random effects analyses for NP-silence and AP-silence in the 21 patients (see Additional imaging analyses, below).

We identified functionally relevant components by testing the correlation of their loading values with semantic and syntactic performance scores. We tested correlations with WPE for NP and AP, and with both reverse role and lexical errors from sentence picture matching. We did not include WPE for RWO for the same reasons we did not include activity for RWO. The significance of each Pearson's correlation was calculated as one-tailed, since our a priori prediction was that improved performance would be associated with greater tissue integrity/activity. Because our data comprised patients with heterogeneous lesions, jICA was liable to produce components dominated by one or two patients whose lesion and/or activity varied a great deal from the group. The loading values from these components are unsuitable for correlations because their distributions are highly skewed. Moreover, a significant correlation would not be interpretable, as it would be driven by one or two patients, rather than a consistent trend across the group. We therefore removed outlying loading values before testing correlations with performance. Outliers were defined as data points that were outside the interquartile range by >1.5 interquartile ranges (Tukey, 1977). Previous work using jICA selected only components with effects predominantly in the brain rather than in ventricles or around the edge of the brain. The latter were assumed to describe movement or pulsation artifacts (Specht et al., 2009). In our analysis, we found no components with effects predominantly outside the brain, so none were excluded under these criteria.

Additional imaging analyses

Independent to the main analysis, we defined damaged voxels in each patient by using a lesion-detection algorithm as previously published (Stamatakis and Tyler, 2005). Briefly, T1-weighted structural images from each LH-damaged participant were entered into individual two-sample t tests with images from a group of age-matched controls. Voxels with a significant drop in T1-weighted image intensity were defined as damaged. The threshold for significance was manually adjusted, with the statistical image overlaid on the patient's structural image, to minimize classification of healthy tissue or CSF in enlarged ventricles as lesion. The resulting lesion images were used to mask contrast images (as described in Imaging processing, above) and were combined to give a lesion frequency map for the group (Fig. 1) as previously described (Tyler et al., 2010b, 2011). The tissue integrity data used in the jICA was independent of automatic lesion identification.

We compared functional activation in controls and patients using group-level (random effects) one- and two-sample t tests in SPM. One-sample t tests were used to describe activity significantly greater than silent baseline in each group by comparing contrast images for each group with zero. Two-sample t tests were used to identify regions with significantly greater activity in controls than patients, and vice versa. In the two-sample t tests, the two sets of contrast images were defined as independent groups with unequal variance; global mean scaling was not used.

Results

Behavioral results

Patients showed a wide range of syntactic abilities, with some very impaired and others performing similarly to controls. On sentence–picture matching, patients' reverse role error rate ranged from 0 to 47% (mean, 17%; SD, 16%), extending well outside the range for controls (0–9%; mean, 2.9%; SD, 2.6%). In contrast, they made few lexical errors (0–6%; mean, 1.9%; SD, 2.2%) and controls made none. On the word-monitoring task performed in the scanner, patients' overall ability to perform the task was as good as controls. Patients generated similar numbers of missing or inappropriate (e.g., anticipatory) responses as controls (percent missing/inappropriate responses for patients: mean, 5%; SD, 6%; controls: mean, 3%; SD 3%; t test, patients vs controls: p > 0.05). Patients and controls also showed the typical reaction time advantage to NP over AP, indicating their overall ability to comprehend normal speech (RT difference AP − NP for patients: mean, 91 ms; SD, 54 ms; controls: mean, 70 ms; SD, 35 ms; t test, patients vs controls: p > 0.05). Patients' range of performance on syntax (WPE AP) was numerically, but not significantly, lower than controls: patients, −0.17–0.32 (mean, 0.09; SD, 0.13); controls, −0.09–0.38 (mean, 0.14; SD, 0.12). Both groups showed a similar range of performance on semantics (WPE NP): patients, 0.08–0.58 (mean, 0.33; SD, 0.13) and controls, −0.05–0.62 (mean, 0.29; SD, 0.17). (Note: sentence–picture matching scores were not available for the scanned controls, so scores are taken from a separate group, also age-matched to the patients. These scores are used in this section only to illustrate the normal range, and do not appear in any further analyses.)

Selection of components relevant to performance

We used jICA to combine structural and functional images from 21 patients and then decompose these data into seven independent components. From these, we selected functionally relevant components on the basis of their correlation with performance, using the word-position effect for normal prose (WPE NP) as a measure of successful processing of sentential semantics, WPE for anomalous prose (WPE AP) as a measure of successful syntactic processing, and reverse-role errors in the sentence–picture matching task as a measure of syntactic comprehension impairment. Correlations with performance were significant in two of the seven components (we report one-tailed significance, since we predict that larger component loading should only correlate with better performance). Component 1 had loading values that correlated positively with WPE NP (r = 0.412, p < 0.05) but not with either the WPE AP (r = −0.329, p > 0.05) or the reverse-role errors in sentence–picture matching (r = −0.320, p > 0.05). Because this component only correlated with performance for the NP sentences that carried both semantic and syntactic information, and not with AP sentences that contained only syntactic information, it was designated the semantics component. Component 2 had loading values that correlated positively with WPE AP (r = 0.580, p < 0.01) and negatively with reverse role errors in sentence–picture matching (r = −0.774, p < 0.001) and was designated the syntax component. The syntax component did not correlate with WPE NP (r = 0.048, p > 0.05). Figure 2 illustrates the dissociation of the two components by plotting the relationship between each component's loading values and both corresponding and noncorresponding performance measures. Each score correlated only with loading values for its corresponding ICA component. None of the reverse correlations were significant (|r| < 0.39, p > 0.05, one-tailed).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

The syntax and semantics components show dissociated relationships with performance. A, WPE NP correlated positively with loading on the semantics component (blue) but showed no relationship with the syntax component (red). B, WPE AP correlated positively with loading on the syntax component only (red) and showed a reversed but nonsignificant relationship with the semantics component (blue). C, Reverse-role error rate from the sentence–picture matching task correlated negatively with loading on the syntax component only (red, larger loading value = fewer errors); the relationship with the semantics component was in the same direction but was not significant. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; n.s., not significant; a.u., arbitrary units.

The syntax component

Preserved syntax was associated with intact structure and function in a left frontotemporal network, consistent with previous studies (Caplan et al., 1996; Tyler et al., 2010b, 2011). Significant tissue integrity effects formed a single, long cluster extending from the gray matter of LIFG, through the white matter of the superior longitudinal and arcuate fasciculi, to the white matter underlying the planum temporale (Table 2; Fig. 3, green). Consistent with the structural data, the functional images in the syntax component described activity in LIFG and in left posterior superior and middle temporal gyri (LpSTG/MTG; Table 2; Fig. 3, blue and red). Effects in LIFG were seen only for AP, the condition that loaded most strongly on syntactic processing. Effects in LpSTG/MTG were seen for both NP and AP. Taken as a whole, these findings are consistent with studies showing that syntactic comprehension depends critically upon the integrity of a left frontotemporal network, particularly the LIFG and left posterior temporal lobe (Dronkers et al., 2004; Caplan et al., 2008; Tyler and Marslen-Wilson, 2008; Tyler et al., 2010a; Papoutsi et al., 2011).

Table 2.

Brain regions involved in syntax component

Region BA Cluster size (cm3) Peak voxel
x y z z score
Normal prose
    LpSTG/MTG 21, 22 8.2 −60 −24 9 8.65
    Left medial SFG 10 1.7 −3 66 18 5.56
    Medial precuneus 7, 19 1.6 3 −78 39 5.22
Anomalous prose
    LIFG 47, 45 1.7 −51 18 −3 4.87
    LpSTG/MTG 21, 22 1.4 −60 −27 12 4.19
Tissue integrity
    LIFG, insula, SLF, AF 44, 45, 47 53.2 −26 10 18 4.97

Peak voxel coordinates (x, y, z) in Montreal Neurological Institute space (in mm). BA, Brodmann area; SFG, superior frontal gyrus; SLF, superior longitudinal fasciculus; AF, arcuate fasciculus.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Syntax component. Syntactic performance correlated with tissue integrity and activity in a left frontotemporal network. A, Surface view showing tissue integrity (TI) in LIFG (green) activity to AP in LIFG and LSTG (red) and activity to NP in LpSTG/MTG (blue). Central legend shows colors of the feature images and their overlap in A and B. B, sagittal sections (at the indicated MNI x-coordinate) showing tissue integrity (green) in white matter in the superior longitudinal and arcuate fasciculi, extending from LIFG to the planum temporale. All effects shown at voxel-level p < 0.001 (z > 3.09) and cluster size > 1.27 cm3 (approximately p < 0.05 corrected). C, Higher loading values from the syntax component correlated with larger WPE AP (red) but not for WPE NP (blue). D, Higher loading values correlated with fewer reverse role errors on sentence-picture matching (RR, red) but not with lexical errors (Lex, blue). *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; n.s., not significant; a.u., arbitrary units.

The semantics component

Performance on sentential semantics (i.e., WPE NP but not WPE AP) correlated with activity in right pSTG/MTG (RpSTG/MTG) for NP and, to a lesser extent, AP (Table 3, Fig. 4). Activity for NP included a small cluster in right temporal pole. The location of activity in RpSTG/MTG was consistent across both prose types (Fig. 4); however, the significance and extent of activity was greater for NP than AP (NP: peak z = 10.18, size = 16.1 cm3; AP: peak z = 5.04, size = 3.4 cm3). Unlike the syntax component, the semantics component showed no significant relationship with tissue integrity, indicating that support of semantics by activity in RpSTG/MTG was not related to damage to a particular region in LH. Although jICA combines tissue integrity and activity such that they show the same pattern of expression over subjects in a given component, their patterns of expression over the brain remain independent. This allows jICA to explain variance that is correlated across subjects but spatially independent between image types, which in this case included a pattern of subject-wise variance in activity in RpSTG/MTG that was not expressed in tissue integrity in any region.

Table 3.

Brain regions involved in semantic component

Region BA Cluster size (cm3) Peak voxel
x y z z score
Normal prose
    RpSTG/MTG 21, 22 16.1 63 −21 12 10.18
    Right temporal pole/IFG 38, 47 1.3 54 18 −6 5.56
Anomalous prose
    CSF 4.7 0 −36 3 7.23
    RpSTG/MTG 21, 22 3.4 63 −21 12 5.04
Tissue integrity
    No significant clusters

Peak voxel coordinates (x, y, z) in Montreal Neurological Institute space (in mm). BA, Brodmann area.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Semantics component. Semantic performance correlated with activity in right temporal regions. A, Surface view showing activity to NP (blue) and AP (red) or both NP and AP (magenta) in RpSTG/MTG, and a small cluster in right IFG (BA 47). Central legend shows colors of the feature images and their overlap in A and B. B, Sagittal sections (at the indicated MNI x-coordinate) illustrating the extent of temporal activity. All effects shown at voxel-level p < 0.001 (z > 3.09) and cluster size >1.25 cm3 (approximately p < 0.05 corrected). TI, Tissue integrity. C, Higher loading values from the semantic component correlated with larger WPE NP (blue) but not for WPE AP (red). *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; n.s., not significant; a.u., arbitrary units.

Investigating the mechanism underlying right hemisphere support of performance

Semantic performance depended upon activity in RpSTG/MTG in the context of LH damage. Since semantics/pragmatics typically involves bilateral pSTG/MTG regions (Tyler et al., 2010a), the most straightforward explanation is that function was preserved because of degeneracy in the normal system, i.e., residual function in the intact part of the normally bilateral system was sufficient to support semantic processing. Alternatively, activity within the bilateral network may have shifted to become more right-lateralized as a response to LH damage, on the hypothesis that right hemisphere (RH) regions may be recruited to perform either new computations or play an enhanced role in existing computations. At first glance, the jICA results do not support the recruitment hypothesis, since this would predict a negative correlation between activity in RH and tissue integrity in LH, which we did not observe.

However, since these effects may have been too small to reach statistical threshold, and since recruitment of contralesional regions is a critical issue in poststroke recovery, we ran follow-up analyses looking for any increase in RH activity in patients with LH damage, over normal levels of activity in controls. If activity did not differ between groups, then we could rule out the adaptive recruitment hypothesis in favor of the degeneracy hypothesis. We contrasted whole-brain activity during NP in our 21 LH-damaged patients with a group of 21 age-matched controls. In one-sample t tests, both groups activated bilateral pSTG/MTG during NP, but in the patients, activity on the left appeared to be weaker than controls (Table 4, Fig. 5A,B). Confirming this, two-sample t tests contrasting the two groups showed significantly lower activity for patients than controls in LSTG at the voxel-level threshold of p < 0.001, extending to MTG at p < 0.01 (Table 4, Fig. 5C), but no significant differences in RpSTG/MTG, even at a liberal voxel-level threshold of p < 0.01 without correction for cluster size. The differences in LSTG reflected reduction of activity by underlying damage in LpSTG/MTG. The absence of differences in RpSTG/MTG, even at a low threshold, argues against adaptive recruitment of RH, and suggests that RpSTG/MTG can support semantics in patients with left-hemisphere damage because the normally bilateral semantic network is a degenerate system, such that RH is sufficient for function after damage to LH (Friston and Price, 2003).

Table 4.

Brain regions identified by univariate fMRI analyses in patients and controls

Region BA Cluster size Peak voxel
x y z z score
Patients, NP-silence, p < 0.001
    RSTG, MTG, IFG 22, 21, 44 56.2 51 −12 −3 6.57
    R occipital 18, 19, 37 9.2 36 −87 −6 5.57
    L + R SMA 6, 24, 32 21.5 9 12 39 5.54
    L STG, MTG 22, 21 37.9 −42 −33 12 5.40
    L occipital 19, 18, 37 11.6 −39 −90 0 4.79
    R precentral gyrus, MFG 6, 4, 3 5.9 54 0 45 4.47
    L precentral gyrus 6 1.6 −48 3 39 4.36
    L cerebellum 1.5 −12 −60 −15 4.24
Controls, NP-silence, p < 0.001
    R STG, MTG, IFG, precentral 22, 21, 38, 44, 45, 47, 4, 6 79.3 51 −21 −6 > 8.00
    L STG, MTG, IFG, precentral, occipital, fusiform 22, 21, 38, 44, 45, 47, 6, 4–1, 18, 19, 37 162.7 −63 −24 9 6.95
    R occipital, fusiform, cerebellum 19, 37, 18 26.7 45 −75 −9 5.58
Controls > patients, NP-silence, p < 0.001
    L STG 22 4.2 −51 −18 9 4.14
Controls > patients, NP-silence, p < 0.01
    L STG, MTG 22, 21 19.8 −57 −42 6 4.40

Cluster size in cubic centimeters. Peak voxel coordinates (x, y, z) in Montreal Neurological Institute space (in mm). BA, Brodmann area; SMA, supplementary motor area; MFG, middle frontal gyrus. Note: for controls > patients, NP-silence, the cluster at p < 0.01 includes a peak voxel that was not present at p < 0.001 since at the higher threshold this voxel was part of a separate cluster that failed to meet cluster size threshold.

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Univariate fMRI analyses in patients and controls. A, B, Both patients (A) and controls (B) activated bilateral superior and middle temporal gyri for NP-baseline, in addition to occipital and precentral regions involved in viewing a visual target and making a button response. For the patients as a group, activity in LH appeared weaker than RH, reflecting underlying damage. C, Activity in LpSTG/MTG was significantly lower in patients than controls, but there were no differences in RH. No regions were more active in patients than controls. Voxel-level thresholds are indicated for each map. Cluster-level threshold for all contrasts was set to p < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons.

Discussion

We used multivariate analysis to identify distinct structural–functional networks supporting syntactic and semantic aspects of speech comprehension in patients with left hemisphere damage. First, we found that syntactic performance depends on tissue integrity in a network comprising LIFG and LpMTG. Second, we identified a region in RpSTG/MTG supporting semantic processing, which emerged when using jICA to account for variance in structure and function related to syntax and semantics separately. Post hoc analyses showed that activity in RpSTG/MTG was not enhanced in patients as a consequence of their LH damage, but was at similar levels to controls, and thus reflected remaining function in the intact residue of a normally bilateral network (Bozic et al., 2010). These results shed light on the distinct neural mechanisms by which syntax and semantics may be preserved following brain injury. These findings, moreover, support a dual neurobiological model for language, with a left-lateralized system supporting core linguistic functions such as syntax (Caplan et al., 1996; Friederici et al., 2003; Dronkers et al., 2004; Snijders et al., 2009; Pallier et al., 2011), and a bilateral system supporting the semantic and pragmatic interpretation of spoken language (Dobel et al., 2001; Bookheimer, 2002; Friederici et al., 2003; Indefrey and Cutler, 2004; Jung-Beeman, 2005; Humphries et al., 2006; Bozic et al., 2010; Rodd et al., 2010).

Performance on two separate tests of syntactic comprehension correlated with tissue integrity in left IFG, superior longitudinal and arcuate fasciculi and pSTG/MTG, as well as activity during syntactic processing (AP) in LIFG and left pSTG/MTG. This combination of structural and functional effects strengthens the claim that syntax depends not only on the LIFG, but also on the pSTG/MTG and white matter pathways connecting the two regions (Papoutsi et al., 2011; Rolheiser et al., 2011).

Syntactic performance correlated with activity in LIFG only during AP and with activity in LpSTG/MTG during both AP and NP. In AP, word-monitoring performance depends on syntax in the absence of sentential semantics, whereas in NP, both syntactic and semantic sentential information are available. Therefore, word monitoring during AP loads upon syntax, whereas during NP semantic sentential information also supports word monitoring and the loading upon syntax is reduced (Tyler et al., 2010b). The observed pattern of activity implicates LIFG in syntactic computations, but suggests that LpSTG/MTG is involved in the normal processes of both syntactic and semantic analysis processes in spoken language. In the case of LSTG/MTG, however, syntactic processing is coupled with activity in LIFG, whereas semantic processing does not require the involvement of the LIFG, except perhaps in cases of high cognitive demands involving processes such as selection (Thompson-Schill et al., 1999; Badre et al., 2005). This relationship between LIFG and LpSTG/MTG complements previous findings that syntactic performance in patients with LH damage depends on effective connectivity between LIFG and LpSTG/MTG, with the LIFG driving performance (Papoutsi et al., 2011). Papoutsi et al. (2011) used a different syntactic manipulation from that used in the current study and did not require listeners to perform an explicit task, thus minimizing cognitive load, but found the same left frontotemporal network as reported here and previously (Snijders et al., 2009). While the exact nature of the relationship between the LIFG and LpSTG/MTG in syntactic processing is unclear, one possibility is that interactivity between LIFG and MTG may be necessary for the sustained maintenance and updating of syntactic representations (Snijders et al., 2009). Whatever the nature of this functional relationship, it is clear from the current findings that although they may be distinct regions, the interaction between LIFG and LMTG is essential for syntactic processing.

Activity in RpSTG/MTG correlated with behavioral performance on stimuli that loaded on sentential semantics (WPE for NP, but not AP). Although normal prose typically elicits bilateral temporal activity (Fig. 4B), the jICA analysis separated activity in the two hemispheres into two components with distinct patterns of variability across patients. Activity in LpSTG/MTG covaried with LH damage, whereas activity in RpSTG/MTG was unrelated to damage: there were no significant voxels in the tissue integrity feature in this component and activity in RpSTG/MTG did not differ between patients and controls. This suggests that the latter component therefore reflects the residue of the bilateral semantic network. The ability of a subset of the network to support performance demonstrates that semantic processing is instantiated in a degenerate fashion, with regions in both hemispheres capable of performing similar computations (Friston and Price, 2003; Price and Crinion, 2005). This finding extends previous work relating semantic comprehension in patients with LH stroke to activity in regions that were normally active in controls, without differences in lateralization of activity between patients and controls (Zahn et al., 2004). Our analysis additionally locates activity supporting semantics to a specific node of the normal semantic network and dissociates preserved semantics from impaired syntax in the same group of patients.

The jICA analysis did not reveal any contribution of frontal regions in semantic processing. Activity and tissue integrity in LIFG only correlated with performance on syntax. Although some studies do report LIFG involvement (particularly pars orbitalis/BA 47) (Binder et al., 2009) in semantics, these studies typically involve tasks that involve making explicit decisions about semantic attributes or involve processes of semantic selection/competition or controlled retrieval (Dapretto and Bookheimer, 1999; Kuperberg et al., 2000; Homae et al., 2002; for full list, see Binder et al., 2009). In contrast, the task we used here—the word-monitoring task—on NP sentences involved responding to high-frequency, concrete words without an explicit semantic decision. Moreover, the word-monitoring task reflects the ability of a listener to activate the semantic and syntactic properties of individual words and integrate them into a coherent and structured representation. These processes have been argued to be automatic and obligatory (Marslen-Wilson and Tyler, 1980), and therefore poor candidates for LIFG involvement. When sentences do not carry the normal combination of semantic/pragmatic and syntactic information, as is the case with Anomalous Prose, then performance on the word-monitoring task reflects the listener's ability to integrate only the syntactic properties of each word into the developing sentential representation, a process that reveals processes of syntactic integration in the frontotemporal network.

Overall, our findings support a dual neurocognitive model for speech comprehension previously described in healthy controls (Bozic et al., 2010) and extend the evidence for this model by demonstrating distinct bilateral and left-lateralized systems in patients with LH damage. In the dual model, bilateral perisylvian regions provide the foundation for interpretation and processing of speech. Within this context, left-lateralized perisylvian regions support core linguistic processing of morpho-syntax and sentential syntax. The dual model is based in part on the observation that agrammatic patients often have preserved semantics (Hagoort et al., 2003; Crinion and Price, 2005; Tyler and Marslen-Wilson, 2008) and predicts RH support of semantics in the context of LH damage that impairs syntax. The current findings confirm this prediction and add to existing neuropsychological findings using a novel analysis that integrates performance, tissue integrity, and neural activity to show evidence of two functionally dissociable neural systems in the same group of patients.

The dual model, supported by the current findings, suggests refinement of a recently proposed hierarchical model of mechanisms for recovery from aphasia in patients with LH damage (Heiss and Thiel, 2006). In this model, the best recovery is afforded by full or partial recuperation of the damaged left hemisphere. With increasing LH damage, RH regions may support function, but with less efficiency than their LH homologues. Our findings show that this model applies differentially to the LH core linguistic network and the bilateral semantic network. Syntactic processing is a strong case of the hierarchical model: performance depends entirely upon an intact LH, with RH recruitment unable to support recovery after damage to LH (Tyler et al., 2010b, 2011). In contrast, semantic processing appears to be supported efficiently by RpSTG/MTG even when the LpSTG/MTG is damaged. This qualification of the hierarchical model underlines the importance of using linguistically specific imaging paradigms when investigating the role of RH in supporting language recovery. A full model for language recovery must account separately for left-lateralized core linguistic processes, such as syntax, and bilaterally supported processes, such as sentential semantics.

In conclusion, using a novel multivariate analysis, we differentiated distinct neural systems supporting syntax and semantics, and each system was associated with a distinct mechanism of recovery from LH damage. Syntax depends critically on spared activity and tissue in LIFG and posterior LpSTG/MTG and the white matter connecting them. In contrast, semantics may be supported by activity in RpSTG/MTG, regardless of LH damage, as a result of bilateral, degenerate representation of semantic processes in the intact brain. These results support a neurocognitive framework with distinct systems supporting core linguistic and general communicative processes, and emphasize the importance of linguistic specificity in imaging investigations of language recovery in patients.

Footnotes

This work was supported by a Medical Research Council (UK) programme grant (Grant number G0500842 to L.K.T.) and a Stephen Erskine Fellowship, Queens' College, Cambridge to E.A.S. We thank the patients for their cooperation.

References

  1. Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Unified segmentation. Neuroimage. 2005;26:839–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Badre D, Poldrack RA, Paré-Blagoev EJ, Insler RZ, Wagner AD. Dissociable controlled retrieval and generalized selection mechanisms in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2005;47:907–918. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.07.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Binder JR, Desai RH, Graves WW, Conant LL. Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:2767–2796. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Black M, Nickels L, Byng S. Patterns of sentence processing deficit: processing simple sentences can be a complex matter. J Neurolinguist. 1991;6:79–101. [Google Scholar]
  5. Blank MA, Pisoni DB, McClaskey CL. Effects of target monitoring on understanding fluent speech. Percept Psychophys. 1981;29:383–388. doi: 10.3758/bf03207348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bookheimer S. Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2002;25:151–188. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bozic M, Tyler LK, Ives DT, Randall B, Marslen-Wilson WD. Bihemispheric foundations for human speech comprehension. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:17439–17444. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000531107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brett M, Leff AP, Rorden C, Ashburner J. Spatial normalization of brain images with focal lesions using cost function masking. Neuroimage. 2001;14:486–500. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bright P, Moss HE, Stamatakis EA, Tyler LK. The anatomy of object processing: the role of anteromedial temporal cortex. Q J Exp Psychol B. 2005;58:361–377. doi: 10.1080/02724990544000013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Calhoun VD, Adali T, Giuliani NR, Pekar JJ, Kiehl KA, Pearlson GD. Method for multimodal analysis of independent source differences in schizophrenia: combining gray matter structural and auditory oddball functional data. Hum Brain Mapp. 2006;27:47–62. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Caplan D, Hildebrandt N, Makris N. Location of lesions in stroke patients with deficits in syntactic processing in sentence comprehension. Brain. 1996;119:933–949. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.3.933. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Caplan D, Stanczak L, Waters G. Syntactic and thematic constraint effects on blood oxygenation level dependent signal correlates of comprehension of relative clauses. J Cogn Neurosci. 2008;20:643–656. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Caramazza A, Zurif EB. Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension-evidence from aphasia. Brain Lang. 1976;3:572–582. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(76)90048-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Crinion J, Price CJ. Right anterior superior temporal activation predicts auditory sentence comprehension following aphasic stroke. Brain. 2005;128:2858–2871. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Crinion J, Ashburner J, Leff A, Brett M, Price C, Friston K. Spatial normalization of lesioned brains: performance evaluation and impact on fMRI analyses. Neuroimage. 2007;37:866–875. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dapretto M, Bookheimer SY. Form and content: dissociating syntax and semantics in sentence comprehension. Neuron. 1999;24:427–432. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80855-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dobel C, Pulvermüller F, Härle M, Cohen R, Köbbel P, Schönle PW, Rockstroh B. Syntactic and semantic processing in the healthy and aphasic human brain. Exp Brain Res. 2001;140:77–85. doi: 10.1007/s002210100794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dronkers NF, Wilkins DP, Van Valin RD, Jr, Redfern BB, Jaeger JJ. Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension. Cognition. 2004;92:145–177. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Friederici AD. Levels of processing and vocabulary types: evidence from on-line processing in normals and agrammatics. Cognition. 1985;19:133–166. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90016-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Friederici AD, Rüschemeyer SA, Hahne A, Fiebach CJ. The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: localizing syntactic and semantic processes. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13:170–177. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.2.170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Friston KJ, Price CJ. Degeneracy and redundancy in cognitive anatomy. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003;7:151–152. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00054-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hagoort P. Semantic priming in Broca's aphasics at a short SOA: no support for an automatic access deficit. Brain Lang. 1997;56:287–300. doi: 10.1006/brln.1997.1849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hagoort P, Wassenaar M, Brown C. Real-time semantic compensation in patients with agrammatic comprehension: electrophysiological evidence for multiple-route plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100:4340–4345. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0230613100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hall DA, Haggard MP, Akeroyd MA, Palmer AR, Summerfield AQ, Elliott MR, Gurney EM, Bowtell RW. “Sparse” temporal sampling in auditory fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp. 1999;7:213–223. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)7:3&#x0003c;213::AID-HBM5&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-N. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Heiss WD, Thiel A. A proposed regional hierarchy in recovery of post-stroke aphasia. Brain Lang. 2006;98:118–123. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Homae F, Hashimoto R, Nakajima K, Miyashita Y, Sakai KL. From perception to sentence comprehension: the convergence auditory and visual information of language in the left inferior frontal cortex. Neuroimage. 2002;16:883–900. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Humphries C, Binder JR, Medler DA, Liebenthal E. Syntactic and semantic modulation of neural activity during auditory sentence comprehension. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18:665–679. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.4.665. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Indefrey P, Cutler A. Pre-lexical and lexical processing in listening. In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. The cognitive neurosciences. 3rd edition. Cambridge, MA: MIT; 2004. pp. 759–774. [Google Scholar]
  29. Jung-Beeman M. Bilateral brain processes for comprehending natural language. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:512–518. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kuperberg GR, McGuire PK, Bullmore ET, Brammer MJ, Rabe-Hesketh S, Wright IC, Lythgoe DJ, Williams SC, David AS. Common and distinct neural substrates for pragmatic, semantic, and syntactic processing of spoken sentences: an fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000;12:321–341. doi: 10.1162/089892900562138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Marslen-Wilson W, Tyler LK. Processing structure of sentence perception. Nature. 1975;257:784–786. doi: 10.1038/257784a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Marslen-Wilson W, Tyler LK. The temporal structure of spoken language understanding. Cognition. 1980;8:1–71. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(80)90015-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Ostrin RK, Tyler LK. Dissociations of lexical function: semantics, syntax and morphology. Cogn Neuropsychol. 1995;12:345–389. [Google Scholar]
  34. Pallier C, Devauchelle AD, Dehaene S. Cortical representation of the constituent structure of sentences. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:2522–2527. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018711108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Papoutsi M, Stamatakis EA, Griffiths J, Marslen-Wilson WD, Tyler LK. Is left fronto-temporal connectivity essential for syntax? Effective connectivity, tractography and performance in left-hemisphere damaged patients. Neuroimage. 2011;58:656–664. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Price CJ, Crinion J. The latest on functional imaging studies of aphasic stroke. Curr Opin Neurol. 2005;18:429–434. doi: 10.1097/01.wco.0000168081.76859.c1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Ratcliff R. Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychol Bull. 1993;114:510–532. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.3.510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Rodd JM, Longe OA, Randall B, Tyler LK. The functional organisation of the fronto-temporal language system: evidence from syntactic and semantic ambiguity. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:1324–1335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.12.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Rolheiser T, Stamatakis EA, Tyler LK. Dynamic processing in the human language system: synergy between the arcuate fascicle and extreme capsule. J Neurosci. 2011;31:16949–16957. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2725-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Rosen HJ, Petersen SE, Linenweber MR, Snyder AZ, White DA, Chapman L, Dromerick AW, Fiez JA, Corbetta MD. Neural correlates of recovery from aphasia after damage to left inferior frontal cortex. Neurology. 2000;55:1883–1894. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.12.1883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Saur D, Lange R, Baumgaertner A, Schraknepper V, Willmes K, Rijntjes M, Weiller C. Dynamics of language reorganization after stroke. Brain. 2006;129:1371–1384. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Snijders TM, Vosse T, Kempen G, Van Berkum JJ, Petersson KM, Hagoort P. Retrieval and unification of syntactic structure in sentence comprehension: an fMRI study using word-category ambiguity. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:1493–1503. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Specht K, Zahn R, Willmes K, Weis S, Holtel C, Krause BJ, Herzog H, Huber W. Joint independent component analysis of structural and functional images reveals complex patterns of functional reorganisation in stroke aphasia. Neuroimage. 2009;47:2057–2063. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Stamatakis EA, Tyler LK. Identifying lesions on structural brain images-validation of the method and application to neuropsychological patients. Brain Lang. 2005;94:167–177. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Taylor KI, Stamatakis EA, Tyler LK. Crossmodal integration of object features: voxel-based correlations in brain-damaged patients. Brain. 2009;132:671–683. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Thompson-Schill SL, Aguirre GK, D'Esposito M, Farah MJ. A neural basis for category and modality specificity of semantic knowledge. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37:671–676. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00126-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Tukey . Exploratory data analysis. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley Publishing; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  48. Tyler LK. Spoken language comprehension: an experimental approach to disordered and normal processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  49. Tyler LK, Marslen-Wilson W. Fronto-temporal brain systems supporting spoken language comprehension. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2008;363:1037–1054. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Tyler LK, deMornay-Davies P, Anokhina R, Longworth C, Randall B, Marslen-Wilson WD. Dissociations in processing past tense morphology: neuropathology and behavioural studies. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14:79–94. doi: 10.1162/089892902317205348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Tyler LK, Marslen-Wilson W, Stamatakis EA. Dissociating neuro-cognitive component processes: voxel-based correlational methodology. Neuropsychologia. 2005a;43:771–778. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.07.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Tyler LK, Marslen-Wilson WD, Stamatakis EA. Differentiating lexical form, meaning, and structure in the neural language system. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005b;102:8375–8380. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0408213102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Tyler LK, Shafto MA, Randall B, Wright P, Marslen-Wilson WD, Stamatakis EA. Preserving syntactic processing across the adult life span: the modulation of the frontotemporal language system in the context of age-related atrophy. Cereb Cortex. 2010a;20:352–364. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Tyler LK, Wright P, Randall B, Marslen-Wilson WD, Stamatakis EA. Reorganization of syntactic processing following LH brain damage: does right-hemisphere activity preserve function? Brain. 2010b;133:3396–3408. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Tyler LK, Marslen-Wilson WD, Randall B, Wright P, Devereux BJ, Zhuang J, Papoutsi M, Stamatakis EA. Left inferior frontal cortex and syntax: function, structure and behaviour in patients with left hemisphere damage. Brain. 2011;134:415–431. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Wilson SM, Dronkers NF, Ogar JM, Jang J, Growdon ME, Agosta F, Henry ML, Miller BL, Gorno-Tempini ML. Neural correlates of syntactic processing in the nonfluent variant of primary progressive aphasia. J Neurosci. 2010;30:16845–16854. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2547-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Zahn R, Drews E, Specht K, Kemeny S, Reith W, Willmes K, Schwarz M, Huber W. Recovery of semantic word processing in global aphasia: a functional MRI study. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2004;18:322–336. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.10.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES