Skip to main content
British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology logoLink to British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology
. 2013 Feb 5;75(3):728–737. doi: 10.1111/bcp.12002

Cognitive effects of two nutraceuticals Ginseng and Bacopa benchmarked against modafinil: a review and comparison of effect sizes

Chris Neale 1, David Camfield 1, Jonathon Reay 2, Con Stough 1, Andrew Scholey 1
PMCID: PMC3575939  PMID: 23043278

Abstract

Over recent years there has been increasing research into both pharmaceutical and nutraceutical cognition enhancers. Here we aimed to calculate the effect sizes of positive cognitive effect of the pharmaceutical modafinil in order to benchmark the effect of two widely used nutraceuticals Ginseng and Bacopa (which have consistent acute and chronic cognitive effects, respectively). A search strategy was implemented to capture clinical studies into the neurocognitive effects of modafinil, Ginseng and Bacopa. Studies undertaken on healthy human subjects using a double‐blind, placebo‐controlled design were included. For each study where appropriate data were included, effect sizes (Cohen's d) were calculated for measures showing significant positive and negative effects of treatment over placebo. The highest effect sizes for cognitive outcomes were 0.77 for modafinil (visuospatial memory accuracy), 0.86 for Ginseng (simple reaction time) and 0.95 for Bacopa (delayed word recall). These data confirm that neurocognitive enhancement from well characterized nutraceuticals can produce cognition enhancing effects of similar magnitude to those from pharmaceutical interventions. Future research should compare these effects directly in clinical trials.

Keywords: Bacopa, cognitive enhancer, Ginseng, modafinil, nootropic

Introduction

Over recent years there has been increasing interest in the area of cognitive enhancement. This field has traditionally focused on the effects of so‐called nootropics in those with fragile cognition (e.g. those with age‐related cognitive decline, mild cognitive impairment or dementia). However in recent years the field of cognition enhancement has also embraced studies into cognition enhancement in young, cognitively intact individuals [1]. In the context of pharmaceutical cognition enhancers, methylphenidate and modafinil (Provigil) have taken centre stage [2]. Modafinil had been taken as a cognitive enhancer by around 10% of respondents to an online survey of the readership of the journal Nature [3], though it is worth noting that this study may have a high response bias from individuals using cognitive enhancers.

As well as pharmaceutical approaches to cognitive enhancement, there is growing interest in the possibility that certain nutraceuticals may enhance cognitive performance. Herbal extracts may contain multiple active components which, in concert, may influence numerous neuronal, metabolic and hormonal systems involved in behavioural processes [4]. Additionally the interactions between these actives may be synergistic, resulting in complex dose and time dependent effects. These factors are challenging for psychopharmacology, making certain positive effects fragile even where there is strict batch to batch consistency across studies, and rendering negative findings sometimes difficult to interpret. Nevertheless there is growing evidence that certain standardized natural products have reproducible neurocognitive effects in humans, possibly because of their inherent polyphamacological properties. Examples of the most promising nutraceuticals in this context include species of Salvia (sage) [58], Panax ginseng [b9b12] and Bacopa monniera [13,14,15,16,17]. For the purposes of this short review we chose to concentrate on Ginseng and Bacopa. The former has been shown to enhance robustly cognitive performance after acute dosing [b18], whilst Bacopa has been shown to have nootropic effects with chronic dosing [b16].

We are not aware of any study to date which has directly compared the cognition enhancing effects of a nutraceuticals and a pharmaceutical in a head to head trial in healthy volunteers. The purpose of this review is to examine the literature describing clinical trials of the neurocognitive effects of modafinil, Panax ginseng and Bacopa monniera in order to compare their effect sizes across different cognitive domains.

Methods

Search strategy

The electronic databases SCOPUS, PubMed and PsychInfo were accessed in early 2012 (April). Key word searches were conducted by combining ‘Ginseng’, ‘Bacopa’ or ‘modafinil’ with ‘cognition’, ‘memory’, ‘neuropsychological’, ‘neurocognitive’ and ‘executive function’. Articles that did not relate to human cognitive trials were excluded as were articles that were not in English. Accepted articles were those that had completed double‐blind, randomized, placebo‐controlled empirical investigations on healthy human subjects using cognitive function as a primary outcome. Additionally studies were included only if they described an intervention with at least one arm assessing one of Ginseng, Bacopa or modafinil. In the case of Ginseng and Bacopa, only studies using ‘pure’ extracts were accepted, i.e. there were no other supplements present within the target nutraceutical arm. Furthermore, all extracts must have been used in isolation and not contaminated by co‐use of other supplements as adjunct interventions.

Papers identified as meeting eligibility criteria were analyzed for emerging common cognitive domains between the intervention types. Applying these criteria resulted in eight relevant studies for modafinil, seven for Bacopa, and nine for Ginseng.

Effect size analysis

Effect sizes were calculated on statistically significant data in order to assess the magnitude of effects. Effect sizes were calculated using Cohen's d [b19]. The strength of clinical effects were defined as small: d = 0.2, medium: d = 0.5 and large: d = 0.8 effect sizes as defined by Cohen [b19]. Chronic trials utilizing a repeated measures design were analyzed at endpoint and not mid‐points. Effect sizes are presented in Tables 13. It should be noted that all treatment‐associated benefits (e.g. increased accuracy, shorter reaction times) are presented as positive effect sizes and impairments (e.g. more errors, slower reaction times) as negative effect sizes respectively.

Table 1.

Summary of human trials into the cognitive effects of modafinil

Author/Year n Dose Sample Trial design Outcome measures Results Cohen's d effect size
Turner et al. (2002)[23] 60 100 mg (n = 20) or 200 mg (n = 20) Placebo mean age 25.30 years, 100 mg mean age 24.35 years, 200 mg mean age 25.10 years Acute
DB, PC, PG
Decision making (gambling), RVIP, digit span, visual spatial working memory, pattern recognition, stop signal, stroop colour naming
Self rated mood and wakefulness.
Significant improvements from drug on digit span, Stroop colour naming, spatial planning, gambling deliberation and pattern recognition.
Suggestion of reduction in impulsive responsiveness.
DS FAS 0.12
DS BAS 0.09
 DS FS 0.12
 DS BS 0.09
 PRM 0.09
DMTS L 0.09
 NTOL 0.11
 DT 0.08
 N.B. Effect sizes calculated as average mean and SD of low and high dose combined in keeping with the analysis used by the authors
Randall et al. (2003) [30] 30 100 mg or 200 mg Placebo mean age 20.7 years, 100 mg mean age 20.3 years, 200 mg mean age 20.7 years Acute
DB, PC, PG
Motor screening, RVIP, DMTS, IED, SOC, trail making, Stroop, COWAT, clock drawing.
VAS mood, sleep and wakefulness
No significant cognitive effects
Significant improvements in self‐reported mood and wakefulness
No cognitive effects
Müller et al. (2004) [28] 16 200 mg 20–29‐year‐olds, mean age 24.1 years Acute
DB, PC, CO
Visual spatial working memory, trail‐making, state anxiety, numeric working memory Significant decreases in error rates at longest delay (8000 ms) in visual spatial task with modafinil, as well as faster reaction times in 1000 and 4000 ms delays.
No speed–accuracy interaction
VSM E 8000 ms 0.77
VSM RT 1000 ms 0.21
VSM RT 4000 ms 0.15
Baranski et al. (2004) [26] 18 4 mg kg−1 – average 300 mg 18–40 years, mean age 24.2 years Acute
DB, PC, CO
Choice reaction time, numeracy, DRN, visual comparison, logical reasoning, self rated mood and fatigue Improvements in reaction time and vigilance as well as fatigue and motivation. Effect sizes unavailable – raw data not present in research paper
Randall et al. (2004) [27] 45 100 mg or 200 mg 50–67 years, placebo mean age 55.7 years, 100 mg mean age 58.8 years, 200 mg mean age 58.2 years Acute
DB, PC, PG
Motor screening, RVIP, DMTS, IED, SOC, trail making, Stroop, COWAT, clock drawing, VAS mood, sleep and wakefulness No improvements in executive functioning (trail making, SOC)
Improvements in Stroop response time, more pronounced with 200 mg dose
No improvements in self‐reported mood and wakefulness
100 mg
Stroop RT 0.16
200 mg
Stroop RT 0.45
Stoops et al. (2005) [51] 6 100, 200, 400 mg 22–28 years, mean age
24 years
Acute
DB, PC, CO
Wakefulness and stress measures, mental arithmetic (×3 50 min blocks) Improved self‐reported stress and wakefulness measures.
Improved mental arithmetic performance
Effect sizes unavailable – raw data not present in research paper
Winder‐Rhodes et al. (2010) [25] 12 300 mg 18–39 years
Mean age
26.3 years
Acute
DB, PC, CO
Digit span, RVIP, SOC, Pattern recognition, stop‐signal Placebo and prazosin (used as another data point) decreased accuracy on SOC, whilst modafinil did not exert this negative effect. However, the effects of modafinil were not statistically significant No significant effects
Ghaharemi et al. (2011) [29] 19 200 mg Mean age 33.7 years Acute
DB, PC, CO
Part of a larger study using MD participants and fMRI
Abstract picture associative learning
No effect on performance of the task in the healthy control group Effect sizes unavailable – raw data not present in research paper

CO, crossover; COWAT, controlled oral word association test; DB, double‐blind; DMTS L, delayed matching to sample latency; DMTS, delayed matching to sample; DRN, detection of repeated numbers; DS BAS, digit span backward actual span; DS BS, digit span backward score; DS FAS, digit span forward actual span; DS FS, digit span forward score; DT, deliberation time (gamble); IED, intra/extra dimensional set shift; MD, methamphetamine‐dependent; PC, placebo‐controlled; PG, parallel groups; PRM, pattern recognition memory; RVIP, rapid visual information processing; S RT, Stroop reaction time; SOC, Stockings of Cambridge; VAS, visual analogue scale; VSM E, visual spatial memory error rates; VSM RT, visual spatial memory reaction time; VSM, visual spatial memory.

Table 3.

Summary of human trials into the cognitive effects of Bacopa monniera

Author/Year n Dose Sample Trial design Outcome measures Results Cohen's d effect size
Stough et al. (2001)[17] 46
23(B)
23(P)
KM
300 mg daily
BC: min 55%
18–60 years 12 week
DB, PC
IT, AVLT, RT, DS Improved IT & three AVLT subtests (learning rate, proactive interference and forgetting rate) at 12 weeks compared with PL. RT & DS did not improve IT: 0.25
AVLT1,: 0.27
AVLT2,: 0.52
AVLT3,: 1.01
Nathan et al. (2001) [51] 38
18 (B)
20 (P)
KM
300 mg single dose
BC: min 55%
18–60 years 0 and 2 h DB, PC AVLT; DS; DSST: SDMT: SCT; TMT; RT; IT No improvements made on all measures. NS
Roodenrys et al. (2002) [15] 76
37 (B)
39 (P)
KM
300 mg (subjects <90 kg)
400 mg (subjects >90 kg)
BC:55%
40–65 years 0 and 3 months DB, PC Memory questionnaire, story recall, general knowledge, WP, DS, VS, coding Improved recall of delayed WP task. No improvements in DS, VS, coding, general knowledge, memory questionnaire or story recall DWP : 0.23
Raghav et al. (2006) [52] 40
20 (B)
20 (P)
CDRI
250 mg daily
BC: min 55%
55+ years
AAMI ppts
0 and 12 weeks DB, PC WMS Differences between treatment groups were not statistically detailed N/S
Stough et al. (2008) [16] 62
33 (B)
29 (P)
KM
300 mg daily
BC: min 55%
18–60 years 0 and 90 days DB, PC CDR testing battery Improved two CDR subtests (RVIP and working memory); Remaining nine subtests showed no improvements WM: 0.47
RVIP: 0.30
Calabrese et al. (2008) [13] 44
24 (B)
24 (P)
MH
300 mg daily
BC: min 50%
65+ years 0 and 12 weeks DB, PC Primary outcome: AVLT
Secondary outcomes: Stroop, DAT, WAIS letter digit test
Significant AVLT subtest (delayed word recall) and Stroop improvements. No improvements on DAT, WAIS letter digit test AVLT4,: 0.36
Stroop: 0.32
Barbhaiya et al. (2008) [53] 44
23 (B)
21 (P)
BM
450 mg daily
BC: bacoside A3 >5% w/w
50–75 years
MMSE 24+
0, 12 and 24 weeks DB, PC
Treatment between 0–12 weeks
AVLT, WMS, WAIS, cognitive testing battery administered after 12 weeks treatment and 24 weeks follow‐up Comparisons not between treatment groups but change from baseline in each group No effect sizes available
Morgan & Stevens (2010) [14] 81
36 (B)
45 (P)
BM
300 mg daily
BC: 40–50%
55+ years 0 and 12 weeks
DB, PC
Multiple cognitive measures: AVLT, CFT, TMT, MAC‐Q Significant improvements on verbal learning, memory acquisition and delayed recall AVLT measures
No significant differences on CFT, TMT or MAC‐Q
AVLT4: 0.95
AVLT5: 0.53
AVLT6: 0.57

Note: 0 in trial design column refers to baseline testing, i.e. prior to ingestion of first tablet. AAMI, age associated memory impairment; AE, adverse events; AVLT, auditory verbal learning test; AVLT1AVLT learning rate; AVLT2, AVLT forgetting rate; AVLT3, AVLT pro‐active interference; AVLT4, AVLT delayed recall; AVLT5, AVLT total learning; AVLT6, AVLT retroactive interference; BC, bacoside content; BM, BacoMind; CDR, cognitive drug research; CDRI, Central Drug Research Institute, Lucknow, India; DAT, divided attention task; DB, double‐blind; DS, digit span; DSST, memory task and digit symbol substitution test; DWP, delayed word pairs; IT, inspection time; KM, KeenMind; MH, MediHerb; MMSE, Mini Mental State Examination; PC, placebo‐controlled; RT, reaction time; RVIP, rapid visual information processing; SCT, speed of comprehension test; SDMT, symbol digit modalities test; TMT, Reitan trail making test; VS, visual span; WAIS, Wechsler adult intelligence scale; WM, working memory (CDR component); WMS, Wechsler memory scale.

Results

Modafinil

Modafinil (C15H15NO2S [b20]) is a pharmaceutical drug used as a licensed treatment for excessive daytime sleepiness associated with narcolepsy or shift‐work [2]. The mechanisms responsible for its effects remain largely unknown. It appears to exert a wide range of effects including via modulating catecholamine activity [b21]. The human pharmacokinetic profile is known, with peak effects between 2–4 h after oral ingestion and a half‐life of 12–15 h [b22].

As noted in a review by Repantis et al. [2], as well as its use in the context of sleep disorders, there has been an increase in academics and office workers using modafinil as a cognitive enhancer. Research into the cognitive enhancing effects of modafinil generally falls into one of two types: studies in sleep deprived and non‐sleep deprived human subjects. These have typically compared doses of 100 mg and/or 200 mg doses against placebo. As modafinil is used as a treatment for excessive daytime sleepiness, there is a large body of research assessing cognitive effects in sleep‐deprived participant groups. However, the results from these studies are not comparable with the participant groups used to assess the cognitive effects of Bacopa or Ginseng so are not included here.

In non‐sleep deprived adults, modafinil is associated with improvements in accuracy of pattern recognition and the stop signal task following 100 mg and 200 mg [b23], with several studies showing improvements in digit span with the 100 mg dose alone [b23,b24]. Furthermore, modafinil improved accuracy of an executive planning task (Stocking of Cambridge) [b25]. Faster reaction times have also been shown across a range of tasks, notably the Stroop colour naming task of selective attention [23,24,26,27]. There are also numerous tasks that are unaffected by modafinil, regardless of dose, including trailmaking [b28], mathematical processing [b26], spatial working memory [b24], logical memory [b27], associative learning [b29] and verbal fluency [b30].

Effect sizes restricted to those domains significantly affected by modafinil are presented in Table 1 (note that all benefits are presented as positive effect sizes).

Panax ginseng

Ginseng refers to extracts from the Araliaceae family of plants. It is estimated that, in the US, Ginseng is the second most used psychoactive herbal product [b31]. The active components are believed to be the ginsenosides, of which over 30 have been isolated, though many exist in trace amounts [b32]. The chemical structure of these aglycone saponins can be used to classify the ginsenosides into three groups: the protopanaxadiol group (e.g. Rb1, Rb2, Rb3, Rc, Rd, Rg3, Rh2, Rs1), the protopanaxatriol group (e.g. Re, Rf, Rg1, Rg2, Rh1) and the oleanolic acid group (e.g. Ro) [b33].

Over the last decade or so a number of studies have revealed that single doses of Panax ginseng (also known as Asian ginseng) can modulate aspects of cognitive function [9] such as brain activity as measured by electroencephalography [b34] and peripheral blood glucose concentrations [b11,b35], in healthy young volunteers. Most of these studies have used a standardized extract (G115) which contains an invariant 4% ginsenosides.

With regards to cognitive function, a number of controlled studies have identified both positive and negative behavioural effects. The most consistent finding, however, is one of improved secondary memory (i.e. declarative memory involving recollection) performance following extract G115 alone [10,36,37] and in combination with both Ginkgo biloba [b37] and Paullinia cupana (guaraná) [b36]. In addition, Panax ginseng (G115) has been shown to enhance aspects of working memory [b38], to improve mental arithmetic performance (in a task that loads heavily on working memory resources) [b11,b12] and to speed attentional processes [b39] in healthy volunteers. These benefits to reaction time occurred without a concomitant cost to accuracy, precluding the possibility of a treatment related shift in the speed/accuracy trade‐off. One recent study has shown that acute administration of a standard extract of a different Ginseng species, Panax quinquefolius (American ginseng), which has a ginsenoside profile distinct from that of Panax ginseng, can also improve working memory performance [b38].

Despite growing evidence supporting the efficacy of Panax ginseng (G115) in modulating cognitive processes following a single dose, only three empirical studies have directly investigated the cognitive and mood effects following more extended Ginseng ingestion periods (with only two of these studies using the standardized G115 extract). Two early studies revealed improved speed of performing a mental arithmetic task following 12 weeks administration of Panax ginseng (200 mg G115 day–1) in young volunteers [b40]. The most recent, Reay et al. [b38,b41] found both positive and negative effects of 7 days dosing with G115. Whilst there were beneficial effects of the 400 mg dose on various measures of the 3‐back task there were also negative effects on reaction time limited to the 200 mg dose.

Effect sizes restricted to those domains significantly affected by Ginseng from selected publications are presented in Table 2.

Table 2.

Summaryof human trials into the cognitive effects of Panax ginseng

Author/Year n Dose Sample Trial design Outcome measures Results Cohen's d effect size
Kennedy et al. (2001) [10] 20 200, 400, 600 mg (G115) 20–27 years, mean age 21.3 years Acute
DB, PC, CO
CDR testing battery, self‐reported mood measures 400 mg lead to improvements on quality of memory and secondary memory at all time point post dose.
Significant negative effect on attention in the 200 and 600 mg doses
200 mg
IWR 4 h −0.17
DVFA 6 h 0.60
DVRT 4 h 0.50
DVRT 6 h 0.40
CRT 1 h 0.36
CRT 4 h 0.15
CRT 6 h 0.22
SMRT 1 h 0.17
SMRT 2.5 h 0.05
PR 1 h −0.01
PRRT 6H 0.19
400 mg
IWR 1 h 0.02
IWR 2.5 h −0.15
IWR 4 h −0.12
IWR 6 h 0.12
SRT 6 h 0.52
DVA 1 h 0.37
DVA 4 h 0.44
DVFA 4 h 0.20
DWRT 2.5 h 0.47
WR 2.5 h 0.08
WR 6 h −0.10
PR 1 h −0.01
600 mg
IWR 1 h 0.13
IWR 2.5 h −0.01
IWR 4 h 0.03
SRT 4 h 0.86
SRT 6 h 0.64
DVA 2.5 h −0.34
DVFA 4 h 0.17
DVRT 4 h 0.31
DVRT 6 h 0.62
WR 2.5 h −0.06
WR 6 h −0.28
Kennedy et al. (2002) [37] 20 400 mg (G115) Mean age 21.1 years Acute
DB, PC, CO
CDR testing battery, S3, S7, Bond–Lader Improvements in secondary memory (CDR score), speed of memory task performance and attentional accuracy.
No effects of G115 on other measures in the CDR battery
IWR 4 h 0.54
IWR 6 h 0.77
DVFA 2.5 h 0.37
DVFA 4 h 0.41
CRTA 1 h 0.16
CRTA 2.5 h 0.23
SMRT 2.5 h 0.39
NMC 2.5 h −0.24
NMC 6 h −0.30
DWRA 2.5 h −0.06
DWRA 6 h 0.63
WR 2.5 h 0.03
WR 4 h −0.08
Scholey et al. (2002) [18] 20 200, 400, 600 mg (G115) Mean age 19.9 years Acute
DB, PC, CO
S3, S7 No effects on S3
Fewer subtractions at all time points following 200 mg dose on S7
400 mg showed significant improvement in accuracy with reduced amount of errors
Effect sizes unavailable – raw data not present in research paper
Kennedy et al. (2004) [36] 28 200 mg (G115) Mean age 21.4 years Acute
DB, PC, CO
CDR testing battery, S3, S7, sentence verification, logical reasoning, self‐reported mood measures Improvement on speed of attention and speed of memory (CDR scores) DVRT 6 h 0.41
CRT 3 h 0.15
CRT 4 h 0.02
NWMRT 2.5 h 0.32
NWMRT 4 h 0.29
NWMRT 6 h 0.39
WRRT 1 h 0.01
WRRT 4 h 0.11
PRRT 4 h 0.02
Reay et al. (2005) [11] 30 200 and 400 mg separate doses Mean age 22.6 years Acute
DB, PC, CO
Mental fatigue (self‐rated), RVIP, S3, S7.
RVIP, S3, S7 are administered as a 10 min ‘cognitive demand battery’ and is repeated at hourly intervals at baseline and up to 6 h post‐dose
More significant effects using the 200 mg dose. Significant reduction of mental fatigue scores as well as increased performance in S7 in all except one time point (5 h). Significant positive effects shown in RVIP and S3.
400 mg dose showed significant increase in mental fatigue at 3 h post dose as well as modest improvements on RVIP and S3 error rates
200 mg
MF 2 h 0.20
MF 3 h 0.59
MF 4 h 0.31
MF 5 h 0.90
MF 6 h 1.03
RVIP RT 5 h 0.53
RVIP 6 h −0.11
S3 4 h 0.27
S3 E 4 h 0.17
S3 E 5 h 0.39
S7 1 h 0.34
S7 2 h 0.36
S7 3 h 0.45
S7 4 h 0.35
S7 6 h 0.39
400 mg
MF 3 h 0.61
RVIP RT 6 h 0.29
RVIP 5 h −0.07
S3 6 h −0.09
S3 E 2 h 0.17
Sünram‐Lea et al. (2005) [39] 30 200 mg 18–25 years, mean age 20 years Acute
DB, PC, CO
CDR testing battery, self‐report mood measures Improvements on ‘speed of attention’ component of CDR battery from the choice reaction time task.
No other measure was significant
CRT 0.33
Reay et al. (2006) [12] 27 200 mg Mean age
21.89 years
Acute
DB, PC, CO
Mental fatigue (self‐rated), RVIP, S3, and S7.
RVIP, S3, S7 are administered as a 10 min ‘cognitive demand battery’ and is repeated at hourly intervals at baseline and up to 6 h post dose.
Mixed effects on mental fatigue
Significant improvements on RVIP performance and reduced false alarms.
Significant improvements on serial 3 s at 4 and 6 h, but significant reduction in performance at 3 h
MF 5 h 1.08
MF 6 h 1.40
RVIP 4 h 0.24
RVIP FA 6 h 0.21
S3 3 h 0.28
S3 4 h 0.22
S3 6 h 0.28
S3E 6 h 0.03
Reay et al. (2009) [41] 30 200 and 400 mg Mean age 22.87 years DB,PC,CO Acute and sub‐chronic (8 days) testing sessions.
Multiple tests including corsi‐blocks, 1/2/3/4 back and random number generation
Reduced reaction time on 3‐back reaction time at 400 mg, but increases in reaction time with 200 mg
Increased 3‐back sensitivity index scores with 400 mg, but decreased with 200 mg
Improved self‐reported mood levels
Day 1
200 mg
3BRT 4 h −0.25
400 mg
3BRT 2.5 h 0.81
3BSI 1 h 0.40
3BSI 4 h 0.30
Day 8
200 mg
3BRT 1 h −0.10
3BRT 2.5 h −0.48
3BSI 4 h −0.43
400 mg
3BSI 1 h 0.04
3BSI 4 h 0.16

3BRT, 3‐back reaction time; 3BSI, 3‐back sensitivity index; CO, crossover; CRT, choice reaction time; CRTA, choice reaction time accuracy; DB, double‐blind; DVA, digit vigilance accuracy; DVFA, digit vigilance false alarms; DVRT, digit vigilance reaction time; DWRA, delayed word recall percentage accuracy; IWR, immediate word recall (% accuracy); MF, mental fatigue; NMC, numeric working memory percentage change; NWMRT, numeric working memory reaction time; PC, placebo‐controlled; PR, picture recognition; PRRT, picture recognition reaction time; RT, reaction time; RVIP FA, rapid visual information processing false alarms; RVIP RT, rapid visual information processing reaction Time; RVIP, rapid visual information processing; S3 E, serial threes errors; S3, serial threes correct; S7 E, serial sevens errors; S7, serial sevens correct; SMRT, spatial memory reaction time; SMRT, spatial memory reaction time; SRT, simple reaction time; WR, word recognition; WRRT, word recognition reaction time.

Bacopa monniera

Bacopa monniera (BM), known as Bacopa or Brahmi, is a herb from the Scrophulariaceae family of plants which has been used for centuries in Ayurvedic medicine. BM has been shown to contain a complex mixture of constituents including alkaloids, saponins and flavonoids. The key constituents of Bacopa are thought to be the triterpenoid saponins, bacosides A and B [b42]. These bacosides usually co‐occur, differing only their optical rotation, with the presence of bacoside B believed to be an artefact generated during the isolation of bacoside A [b43]. Animal studies have shown BM to be an antioxidant [b44], memory enhancer [b45], antidepressant [b46] and to reduce the concentrations of beta‐amyloid in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease [b47].

Human studies reveal consistent cognitive enhancement as a result of BM administration across young, old and impaired adult populations [b48]. Unlike modafinil and Ginseng, Bacopa may not acutely improve cognitive functioning (although there are as yet unpublished reports of acute effects during more effortful cognitive processing). To date publications are restricted to effects which are evident only after chronic interventions (typically 12 weeks of a 300 mg daily dose), with no significant improvements occurring after a 5 week intervention [b17] or acutely after 2 h [b49]. The most robust effects of BM are on memory performance, including positive effects on learning and consolidation of target stimuli [b17], delayed recall [b13], total learning [b14], visual retention of information [b15] and working memory [b16]. There is also evidence that BM can improve speed of information processing in both the inspection time task and rapid visual information processing [b16,b17]. Using BM in an older population group (55 years and over) has shown improvements in executive functioning on the Stroop task and the mental control subtest of the Wechsler memory scale [b13,b14]. There are also tasks which appear to be unaffected by BM administration including working memory speed [b17], reaction time [13,16,17] and divided attention [b13]. Effect sizes for significant findings restricted to those domains significantly affected by Bacopa are presented in Table 3.

Effect sizes

With regards to modafinil, our analysis revealed small effect sizes on reaction time and small to large effect sizes on response accuracy during visual‐spatial working memory, as well as a dose‐dependent increase in effect size for stroop reaction times (Table 1). There appeared to be no cognitive costs (i.e. cognitive impairments in other domains) associated with these effects, with effect sizes ranging from d = 0.083 to d = 0.774 (for accuracy of visual spatial memory).

There was large variation in the effect sizes for Ginseng (Table 2). The largest effect size, d = 1.396, was for amelioration of self‐rated mental fatigue during heavily loaded cognitive processing. Regarding cognitive processing the largest effect size was for simple reaction time (d = 0.860). Interestingly another measure, reaction time on the 3‐back working memory task, showed a large positive effect size (d = 0.806) following acute Ginseng administration and also had the largest impairment d = −0.481 [b41] following dosing for 8 days. This raises the possibility of neuroadaptations to the neural substrates influenced by acute Ginseng administration with longer term dosing resulting in opposite effects to acute dosing.

Chronic BM interventions generally produced the most consistent and largest effect sizes. These ranged from small to medium effect sizes for measures of attention and information processing tasks such as RVIP, Stroop and inspection time. Larger effect sizes were evident for auditory verbal learning tasks where the effect sizes ranged from d = 0.230 for delayed word pair memory to d = 0.950 for delayed word recall (AVLT4) and d = 1.01 for protection from pro‐active interference during delayed memory (AVLT3).

Conclusions

All three substances reviewed here exerted overwhelmingly positive effects on neurocognitive function across different cognitive domains. However, it must be made clear that this review has only looked at the statistically significant results and did not include the non‐significant results from studies into the neurocognitive effects of these substances. Modafinil had the strongest effects on speed of information processing and executive functioning. Ginseng exerts acute positive effects on secondary memory and more heavily cognitively loaded working memory tasks. Bacopa administration appears to predominantly enhance learning and memory, with effects restricted to chronic administration. The differential effects on cognitive domains presumably reflects different mechanisms of action of each substance. Modafinil has multiple effects on neurotransmitter systems including region specific increases in adrenergic, histaminergic and glutaminergic activity and decreased GABAergic activity [b50]. The mechanisms for BM are unknown, but there is evidence that it has pro‐cholinergic effects as suggested by the effects on inspection time and rapid visual information processing [b17,b51] as well as anti‐oxidant and anti‐inflammatory properties. As might be expected from an extract with multiple components Ginseng has been reported to have multiple properties relevant to neurocognitive function. These include glucoregulation, modulation of cholinergic and dopaminergic activity as well as increasing nitric oxide synthesis [b19]. Whilst the purpose of this review was to compare nutraceuticals and pharmaceuticals, it is worth noting that the majority of the BM research presented here relates to chronic administration whilst the modafinil and Ginseng research is predominantly in the context of acute administration. However, the purpose of this review is to assess substances that enhance cognition.

With regards to modafinil, the results here show the cognitive enhancing effects of the substance on non‐sleep deprived subjects, rather than during the treatment of excessive tiredness in narcolepsy or after sleep deprivation. It is also worth noting that many research papers using non‐sleep deprived individuals have not adequately reported the cognitive results so we were unable to compute effect sizes for these studies (although this factor is by no means limited to modafinil research). Regarding the effects of Ginseng and Bacopa, research into herbal medicines brings its own difficulties. For example human studies into BM have used different products across trials. Whilst the manufacturers of all BM products included in our review claim standardization, compositions of individual treatments have not been compared. However, all extracts used in studies to date are reported to have standardized bacoside content to levels between 50–55%. All Ginseng studies included in Table 2 used the standardized extract G115.

This review is limited by the number of studies currently available. As the number of double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, randomized trials is rising, it is likely that future reviews will be able to compare particular groups of individuals such as those with specific cognitive dysfunction or particular age groups. At present, the number of available studies is too low to make such direct comparisons. Future research studies may wish to compare directly the differences of these substances in the same cohort. We are also aware that some of the research into nutraceuticals is not widely available through popular search engines such as those used for this review.

In conclusion the nutraceuticals Ginseng and Bacopa produced effect sizes for cognitive enhancement which were comparable with those seen for modafinil, albeit in different cognitive domains. Future studies should directly compare the cognitive effects of these agents in direct, head to head clinical trials. Furthermore, presentation and statistical analysis of results in certain research papers have made calculating effect sizes difficult in some instances. This is an issue that will need to be addressed in future studies into both pharmaceutical or nutraceutical research aiming to establish any cognitive enhancing effects of said interventions.

Competing Interests

All authors have completed the Unified Competing Interest form at http://www.icmje.org/coi_disclosure.pdf (available on request from the corresponding author) and declare no support from any organization for the submitted work, AS and CS have received funding from Soho Flordis International (SFI) in the previous 3 years and there are no other relationships or activities that could appear to have influenced the submitted work.

References

  • 1.Sahakian B, Morein‐Zamir S. Professor's little helper. Nature. 2007;450:1157–1159. doi: 10.1038/4501157a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Repantis D, Schlattmann P, Laisney O, Heuser I. Modafinil and methylphenidate for neuroenhancement in healthy individuals: a systematic review. Pharmacol Res. 2010;62:187–206. doi: 10.1016/j.phrs.2010.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Maher B. Poll results: look who's doping. Nature. 2008;452:674–675. doi: 10.1038/452674a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Scholey A, Kennedy D, Wesnes K. The psychopharmacology of herbal extracts: issues and challenges. Psychopharmacology. 2005;179:705–707. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2062-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kennedy DO, Dodd FL, Robertson BC, Okello EJ, Reay JL, Scholey AB, Haskell CF. Monoterpenoid extract of sage (Salvia lavandulaefolia) with cholinesterase inhibiting properties improves cognitive performance and mood in healthy adults. J Psychopharmacol. 2011;25:1088–1100. doi: 10.1177/0269881110385594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Scholey AB, Tildesley NTJ, Ballard CG, Wesnes KA, Tasker A, Perry EK, Kennedy DO. An extract of Salvia (sage) with anticholinesterase properties improves memory and attention in healthy older volunteers. Psychopharmacology. 2008;198:127–139. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1101-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Tildesley N, Kennedy DO, Perry EK, Ballard CG, Savelev S, Wesnes KA, Scholey AB. Salvia lavandulaefolia (Spanish Sage) enhances memory in healthy young volunteers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2003;75:669–674. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(03)00122-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Tildesley NTJ, Kennedy DO, Perry EK, Ballard C, Wesnes KA, Scholey A. Positive modulation of mood and cognitive performance following administration of acute doses of Salvia lavandulaefolia essential oil to healthy young volunteers. Physiol Behav. 2005;83:699–709. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kennedy DO, Scholey AB. Ginseng: potential for the enhancement of cognitive performance and mood. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2003;75:687–700. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(03)00126-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Kennedy DO, Scholey AB, Wesnes KA. Dose dependent changes in cognitive performance and mood following acute administration of Ginseng to healthy young volunteers. Nutr Neurosci. 2001;4:295–310. doi: 10.1080/1028415x.2001.11747370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Reay JL, Kennedy DO, Scholey AB. Single doses of Panax ginseng (G115) reduce blood glucose levels and improve cognitive performance during sustained mental activity. J Psychopharmacol. 2005;19:357–365. doi: 10.1177/0269881105053286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Reay JL, Kennedy DO, Scholey AB. Effects of Panax ginseng, consumed with and without glucose, on blood glucose levels and cognitive performance during sustained ‘mentally demanding’ tasks. J Psychopharmacol. 2006;20:771–781. doi: 10.1177/0269881106061516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Calabrese C, Gregory WL, Leo M, Kraemer D, Bone K, Oken B. Effects of a standardized Bacopa monnieri extract on cognitive performance, anxiety, and depression in the elderly: a randomized, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled trial. J Altern Complement Med. 2008;14:707–713. doi: 10.1089/acm.2008.0018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Morgan A, Stevens J. Does Bacopa monnieri improve memory performance in older persons? Results of a randomized, placebo‐controlled, double‐blind trial. J Altern Complement Med. 2010;16:753–759. doi: 10.1089/acm.2009.0342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Roodenrys S, Booth D, Bulzomi S, Phipps A, Micallef C, Dip G, Smoker J. Chronic effects of Brahmi (Bacopa monnieri) on human memory. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27:279–281. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00419-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Stough C, Downey LA, Lloyd J, Silber B, Redman S, Hutchison C. Examining the nootropic effects of a special extract of Bacopa monniera on human cognitive functioning: 90 days double‐blind placebo‐controlled randomized trial. Phytother Res. 2008;22:1629–1634. doi: 10.1002/ptr.2537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Stough C, Lloyd J, Clarke J, Downey LA, Hutchison CW, Rodgers T, Nathan PJ. The chronic effects of an extract of Bacopa monniera (Brahmi) on cognitive function in healthy human subjects. Psychopharmacology. 2001;156:481–484. doi: 10.1007/s002130100815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Scholey AB, Kennedy DO. Acute, dose‐dependent cognitive effects of Ginkgo bilobaPanax ginseng and their combination in healthy young volunteers: differential interactions with cognitive demand. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2002;17:35–44. doi: 10.1002/hup.352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cohen J. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates; 1988. Statistical Power Analyses for the Behavioural Sciences. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.In Y, Tomoo K, Ishida T, Sakamoto Y. Crystal and molecular structure of an (S)‐()‐enantiomer of modafinil, a novel wake‐promoting agent. Chem Pharm Bull (Tokyo) 2004;52:1186–1189. doi: 10.1248/cpb.52.1186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Minzenberg MJ, Carter CS. Modafinil: a review of neurochemical actions and effects on cognition. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:1477–1502. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Wong YN, Simcoe D, Hartman LN, Laughton WB, King SP, McCormick GC, Grebow PE. A double‐blind, placebo‐controlled, ascending‐dose evaluation of the pharmacokinetics and tolerability of modafinil tablets in healthy male volunteers. J Clin Pharmacol. 1999;39:30–40. doi: 10.1177/00912709922007534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Turner DC, Robbins TW, Clark L, Aron AR, Dowson J, Sahakian BJ. Cognitive enhancing effects of modafinil in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology. 2003;165:260–269. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1250-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Randall DC, Viswanath A, Bharania P, Elsabagh SM, Hartley DE, Shneerson JM, File SE. Does modafinil enhance cognitive performance in young volunteers who are not sleep‐deprived? J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2005;25:175–179. doi: 10.1097/01.jcp.0000155816.21467.25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Winder‐Rhodes SE, Chamberlain SR, Idris MI, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ, Müller U. Effects of modafinil and prazosin on cognitive and physiological functions in healthy volunteers. J Psychopharmacol. 2010;24:1649–1657. doi: 10.1177/0269881109105899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Baranski JV, Pigeau R, Dinich P, Jacobs I. Effects of modafinil on cognitive and meta‐cognitive performance. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2004;19:323–332. doi: 10.1002/hup.596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Randall DC, Fleck NL, Shneerson JM, File SE. The cognitive‐enhancing properties of modafinil are limited in non‐sleep‐deprived middle‐aged volunteers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2004;77:547–555. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2003.12.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Müller U, Steffenhagen N, Regenthal R, Bublak P. Effects of modafinil on working memory processes in humans. Psychopharmacology. 2004;177:161–169. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1926-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Ghahremani DG, Tabibnia G, Monterosso J, Hellemann G, Poldrack RA, London ED. Effect of modafinil on learning and task‐related brain activity in methamphetamine‐dependent and healthy individuals. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:950–959. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Randall DC, Shneerson JM, Plaha KK, File SE. Modafinil affects mood, but not cognitive function, in healthy young volunteers. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2003;18:163–173. doi: 10.1002/hup.456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Barnes P, Powell‐Griner E, McFann K, Nahin R. 2002. Advance Data Report #343. Complementary and alternative medicine use among adults: United States.: National Center for Completmentary and Alternative Medicine. [PubMed]
  • 32.Tachikawa E, Kudo K, Harada K, Kashimoto T, Miyate Y, Kakizaki A, Takahashi E. Effects of ginseng saponins on responses induced by various receptor stimuli. Eur J Pharmacol. 1999;369:23–32. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(99)00043-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Gillis CN. Panax ginseng pharmacology: a nitric oxide link? Biochem Pharmacol. 1997;54:1–8. doi: 10.1016/s0006-2952(97)00193-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kennedy DO, Scholey AB, Drewery L, Marsh VR, Moore B, Ashton H. Electroencephalograph effects of single doses of Ginkgo biloba and Panax ginseng in healthy young volunteers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2003;75:701–709. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(03)00120-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Reay JL, Kennedy DO, Scholey AB. The glycaemic effects of single doses of Panax ginseng in young healthy volunteers. Br J Nutr. 2006;96:639–642. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Kennedy DO, Haskell CF, Wesnes KA, Scholey AB. Improved cognitive performance in human volunteers following administration of guarana (Paullinia cupana) extract: comparison and interaction with Panax ginseng. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2004;79:401–411. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2004.07.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Kennedy DO, Scholey AB, Wesnes KA. Modulation of cognition and mood following administration of single doses of Ginkgo biloba, ginseng, and a ginkgo/ginseng combination to healthy young adults. Physiol Behav. 2002;75:739–751. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00665-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Reay JL, Scholey AB, Kennedy DO. Panax ginseng (G115) improves aspects of working memory performance and subjective ratings of calmness in healthy young adults. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2010;25:462–471. doi: 10.1002/hup.1138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Sünram‐Lea SI, Birchall RJ, Wesnes KA, Petrini O. The effect of acute administration of 400 mg of Panax ginseng on cognitive performance and mood in healthy young volunteers. Curr Top Nutraceu Res. 2005;3:65–74. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.D'Angelo L, Grimaldi R, Caravaggi M. A double‐blind, placebo‐controlled clinical study on the effect of a standardized ginseng extract on psychomotor performance in healthy volunteers. J Ethnopharmacol. 1986;16:15–22. doi: 10.1016/0378-8741(86)90063-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Reay JL, Scholey AB, Milne A, Fenwick J, Kennedy DO. Panax ginseng has no effect on indices of glucose regulation following acute or chronic ingestion in healthy volunteers. Br J Nutr. 2009;101:1673–1678. doi: 10.1017/S0007114508123418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Das A, Shanker G, Nath C, Pal R, Singh S, Singh HK. A comparative study in rodents of standardized extracts of Bacopa monniera and Ginkgo biloba: anticholinesterase and cognitive enhancing activities. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2002;73:893–900. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(02)00940-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Russo A, Borrelli F. Bacopa monniera, a reputed nootropic plant: an overview. Phytomedicine. 2005;12:305–317. doi: 10.1016/j.phymed.2003.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bhattacharya SK, Bhattacharya A, Kumar A, Ghosal S. Antioxidant activity of Bacopa monniera in rat frontal cortex, striatum and hippocampus. Phytother Res. 2000;14:174–179. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-1573(200005)14:3<174::aid-ptr624>3.0.co;2-o. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Hota SK, Barhwal K, Baitharu I, Prasad D, Singh SB, Ilavazhagan G. Bacopa monniera leaf extract ameliorates hypobaric hypoxia induced spatial memory impairment. Neurobiol Dis. 2009;34:23–39. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2008.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Sairam K, Dorababu M, Goel RK, Bhattacharya SK. Antidepressant activity of standardized extract of Bacopa monniera in experimental models of depression in rats. Phytomedicine. 2002;9:207–211. doi: 10.1078/0944-7113-00116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Holcomb LA, Dhanasekaran M, Hitt AR, Young KA, Riggs M, Manyam BV. Bacopa monniera extract reduces amyloid levels in PSAPP mice. J Alzheimers Dis. 2006;9:243–251. doi: 10.3233/jad-2006-9303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Pase MP, Kean J, Sarris J, Neale C, Scholey AB, Stough C. The cognitive‐enhancing effects of Bacopa monnieri: a systematic review of randomized, controlled human clinical trials. J Altern Complement Med. 2012;8:647–652. doi: 10.1089/acm.2011.0367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Nathan PJ, Clarke J, Lloyd J, Hutchison CW, Downey L, Stough C. The acute effects of an extract of Bacopa monniera (Brahmi) on cognitive function in healthy normal subjects. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2001;6:345–351. doi: 10.1002/hup.306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Ballon JS, Feifel D. A systematic review of modafinil: potential clinical uses and mechanisms of action. J Clin Psychiatry. 2006;67:554–566. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v67n0406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Nathan PJ, Stough C. Inspection time: a neuropsychophysiological test for measuring the functional integrity of the cholinergic system. Med Hypotheses. 2001;57:759–760. doi: 10.1054/mehy.2001.1484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Stoops WW, Lile JA, Fillmore MT, Glaser PEA, Rush CR. Reinforcing effects of modafinil: influence of dose and behavioral demands following drug administration. Psychopharmacology. 2005;182:186–193. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0044-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Raghav S, Singh H, Dalal P, Srivastava J, Asthana O. Randomized controlled trial of standardized Bacopa monniera extract in age‐associated memory impairment. Indian Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;48:238. doi: 10.4103/0019-5545.31555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Barbhaiya HC, Desai RP, Saxena VS, Pravina K, Wasim P, Geetharani P, Allan JJ, Venkateshwarlu K, Amit A. Efficacy and tolerability of BacoMind® on memory improvement in elderly participants – a double blind placebo controlled study. Pharmacology and Toxicology. 2008;3:425–434. [Google Scholar]

Articles from British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology are provided here courtesy of British Pharmacological Society

RESOURCES