Abstract
The Darwin–Bateman paradigm recognizes competition among males for access to multiple mates as the main driver of sexual selection. Increasingly, however, females are also being found to benefit from multiple mating so that polyandry can generate competition among females for access to multiple males, and impose sexual selection on female traits that influence their mating success. Polyandry can reduce a male's ability to monopolize females, and thus weaken male focused sexual selection. Perhaps the most important effect of polyandry on males arises because of sperm competition and cryptic female choice. Polyandry favours increased male ejaculate expenditure that can affect sexual selection on males by reducing their potential reproductive rate. Moreover, sexual selection after mating can ameliorate or exaggerate sexual selection before mating. Currently, estimates of sexual selection intensity rely heavily on measures of male mating success, but polyandry now raises serious questions over the validity of such approaches. Future work must take into account both pre- and post-copulatory episodes of selection. A change in focus from the products of sexual selection expected in males, to less obvious traits in females, such as sensory perception, is likely to reveal a greater role of sexual selection in female evolution.
Keywords: Bateman gradient, direct and indirect benefits, mating competition, sex roles, sperm competition, variation in reproductive success
1. Introduction
The products of sexual selection that function as ornaments or armaments are found predominantly, though not exclusively, in males [1]. The fundamental cause of this sexual difference is believed to lie in the greater expenditure on gametes made by males and females [2]. Because males often invest less in any given reproductive event, they should be able to mate more frequently than females so that their reproductive success is more variable, and dependent on their ability to compete successfully for access to multiple females [2]. Numerous quantitative measures of the opportunity for sexual selection have been proposed, including relative parental investment [3], the operational sex ratio (OSR) [4], potential reproductive rates (PRRs) [5] and the variance in mating success [6–8]. Intrinsic and extrinsic factors interact in often complex ways to ultimately determine the extent to which males and females limit each other's reproductive potential (figure 1). We agree with Parker & Birkhead [7], who argue that the slopes of the so-called ‘Bateman gradients’ offer key measures because they can, in theory, indicate directly the intensity with which sexual selection operates. However, we recognize that empirical measurement of Bateman gradients in natural populations of animals is not without its problems, and that caution is necessary in interpreting positive slopes as evidence of sexual selection when other measures of the opportunity for selection, or data on selection differentials for traits under selection, are unavailable [13]. Nonetheless, Bateman gradients have considerable heuristic value for the theme of this volume, because they address explicitly how polyandry will influence sexual selection.
Bateman gradients connect mating success, measured as number of mates, to reproductive success. When the slope of the Bateman gradient is steep, one can expect strong selection among individuals for access to as many mates as possible, because the fitness pay-off from multiple mates is high. When males compete more strongly for mating opportunities than do females, males should have a steeper gradient than females (figure 2a), as found for some, but not all, of Bateman's fruit flies [14,17]. Conversely, when females compete more strongly than males for mating opportunities, we expect the female gradient to be steeper (figure 2b), as found in the sex-role-reversed species of pipefish, Syngnathus typhle [18]. Yet, we might also expect both sexes to show steep gradients, such that both benefit from having multiple mates (figure 2c), or shallow gradients (figure 2d) with neither sex benefitting greatly. Such scenarios illustrate both the general utility of Bateman gradients in predicting mate competition, and why the labelling of competition as a ‘sex role’ obfuscates reality.
In their contribution, Parker & Birkhead [7] show theoretically how considering the costs and benefits of multiple mating can moderate Bateman gradients, and thus the intensity of sexual selection acting on males and females. Polyandry is shown to reduce the difference between male and female gradients, though not which is greater [7]. In our contribution, we draw upon the empirical literature to argue that polyandry can indeed have a major impact on the strength of sexual selection. The Darwin–Bateman paradigm in its original form argued that females gain little from mating with more than one male, predicting that females should have a Bateman gradient that tends to zero. We therefore open this review with a discussion of the selective pressures that can promote the evolution of polyandry and thereby show why female gradients can often be steeply positive (§2). We then discuss how polyandry can influence the strength of competition for access to multiple mates for both sexes (§3), the consequences of polyandry for sexual selection acting after copulation—sperm competition and cryptic female choice (§4), and how these mechanisms of post-copulatory sexual selection can impact net sexual selection operating on males and females (§5).
2. Why do females mate with more than one male?
Mating with more than one partner is common among animals. Bateman realized that this represents the principal way by which males increase their reproductive success [2] (figure 2a,c). It is now clear that female reproductive success can also depend on number of mates, because of direct and indirect benefits, and because of effects of genetic diversity within broods. Indeed, a recent study of the cricket Gryllus campestris reported that, in a natural population, the gradient of the relationship between number of mates and lifetime reproductive success was of equal magnitude and positive for both males and females [19] (cf. figure 2c).
There are many costs of mating in general, and with multiple partners in particular [20,21]. For example, mate searching is often associated with time, energy and increased predation risk, as is competition for mates, courtship and mating activities [22,23]. The risk of being infected by sexually transmitted diseases and costs related to immune function are likely to increase with number of mating partners [24,25], and mating can be costly for females if male-derived chemicals or physical damage reduces their lifespan, and hence potential lifetime reproductive success. Nonetheless, where the benefits of mating exceed these costs, so that females have positive Bateman gradients, selection is expected to favour the evolution of polyandry. Here, we discuss the benefits females can gain from mating polyandrously.
(a). Direct benefits
Jacanas provide a well-studied example of how females can increase their reproductive output from having more partners. Both in the wattled, Jacana jacana, and bronze-winged jacana, Metopidius indicus, a single female can mate with up to four males, laying a clutch of four eggs that each male will incubate [26,27]. Similarly, in the short-snouted pipefish, Nerophis ophidion, the female attaches a clutch of eggs to the male's body, which he broods until offspring hatch four to six weeks later [28]. Each male only carries eggs from one female at a time, and because the egg maturation rate of females is considerably higher than the brooding rate of males, each female can, on average, produce two full clutches, while a male broods one. Thus, for both the jacanas and pipefish, the male contributes substantially to offspring care, and although the eggs are large and expensive to produce, female PRR is higher than male PRR. Hence, a female benefits from mating with several males, but only competitively successful females manage to do so. Because females engage in intra-sexual contest competition for mating partners, this leads to sex-role reversal, sensu [16], and because large, dominant and highly ornamented females are more successful, it generates both intra- and inter-sexual selection for these traits. Because a larger number of mates translate directly into a higher reproductive success for the females, it also means that species such as these are likely to have Bateman gradients with steeper slopes for females than for males (cf. figure 2b). In N. ophidion, males father all the offspring they care for [29], despite the polyandry. By contrast, there is a high level of multiple paternity in jacana broods [26]. In bronze-winged jacanas, not all males in a harem get eggs to incubate, but females appear to ‘bribe’ such males to stay by allowing them copulations [27]. If males without a clutch still father some young, this should reduce the variance in reproductive success among males. Still, it is not entirely clear what females gain from having such excess males.
In most animals, females do not double their reproductive success when mating with two males instead of one, like the pipefish and jacana can. Still, they can benefit directly from polyandry in a number of ways. Females that mate with several partners can receive more paternal care and protection for themselves and their offspring [20]. Mating with several males can also increase egg production, owing to nutrients in the ejaculate or nuptial gift [30–32]. Mating with multiple males can secure against infertility, which is important in populations in which infertile males are common [33,34]. Polyandry can also improve female fertility, because mating with multiple males generates a greater supply of sperm [31,35]. This may be particularly important in externally fertilizing species, in which sperm numbers can be severely limiting [36]. Sperm numbers can be limiting for females also in internally fertilizing species, for example if insemination fails [34], or if the most popular males become sperm depleted [37] or allocate sperm strategically [38]. Ironically, while sperm depletion should be less of a problem for monogamous animals, polyandry might be both cause and solution to sperm limitation [39].
(b). Indirect benefits
Indirect benefits arise when certain genes or combinations of genes raise offspring fitness [20,40], and can be divided into effects that relate to viability or attractiveness [20]. There is ample evidence that mate choice can target both types of genetic quality, and a substantial body of research has investigated the various traits used to signal it [23,40].
Genetic compatibility refers to how the male and female genomes complement each other in ways that affect offspring performance (fertilization, growth, survival, etc.) [41–43]. Again, there is good evidence of mate choice that targets such non-additive genetic effects, or mate choice that targets both ‘good’ and compatible gene effects [42–46]. However, when the genetic quality of mates cannot be determined before mating, females may increase their chances of finding a compatible sire for their offspring by mating polyandrously [41,47]. Recent work suggests the same can be true for males that mate polygynously [48]. In fact, a recent meta-analysis suggests that post-copulatory mechanisms are more likely to generate ‘good’ or compatible genetic benefits than is pre-copulatory female choice based on male secondary sexual traits [39]. Consistent with this, several empirical and theoretical studies have shown that polyandry can evolve in response to genetic incompatibility [49–52].
When polyandry leads to sperm competition, a male's ability to gain successful fertilizations can be related to the relative number of sperm, their ability to reach and fertilize the eggs, displacement of other males' sperm and preventing or delaying female remating. When this competitive edge is heritable, it should result in competitively superior sons [53,54]. Likewise, if a male's fertilization potential is positively associated with his genetic quality more generally, polyandry could deliver viability benefits for offspring produced by superior sperm competitors [55,56]. Experimental evolution studies in house mice have demonstrated how polyandry leads to the evolution of increased early embryo viability [57], and comparative analyses suggest that this effect may be general across mammalian species [58]. In §5, we discuss how these forms of post-copulatory sexual selection can moderate sexual selection operating before mating.
(c). Genetic diversity and relatedness within broods
Genetic diversity among offspring is lower in broods produced by two parents than in broods produced by three or more contributors, whether it is due to polygyny, polyandry or both. In habitats that are unpredictable from one generation to the next [59,60], increased genetic diversity among offspring may increase the chances that at least some survive [59], in a similar manner to that argued for the evolution of sex [61]. Such effects of polyandry have, for example, been found in an experimental study of the pygmy grasshopper, Tetrix subulata [62], and manipulated polyandry has been shown to increase colony resistance to parasites in the bumblebee, Bombus terrestris [63]. Restricted circumstances are required for polyandry to evolve as a result of these effects however, such as small population size and low costs of mating with more than one male [59].
Genetic and phenotypic diversity of a brood can also affect competition among siblings. If full-sibs compete more intensely for limited resources than half-sibs, for example owing to more overlapping demands, survival rates should be higher among broods consisting of more diverse offspring [59], as recently shown in S. typhle [64] and in the ascidian Ciona intestinalis [61]. This effect is likely to be most important if the offspring of a brood are in close proximity, competing for resources before [64] or after birth [65].
Given that polyandry can offer females so many direct and indirect benefits, it is clear that our understanding of sexual selection generally, and our refinement of the Darwin–Bateman paradigm, particularly, needs to consider selection on, and arising from, multiple mating by females.
3. Polyandry and mating competition
(a). Competition among females for mating opportunities
Polyandry can generate competition among females for mating opportunities. As we have seen, in species where females obtain reproductive benefits from males, the female Bateman gradient will be positive, and we should expect to see competition among females for access to multiple males. The intensity of female competition for mates will depend largely on the benefits of polyandry and the costs of mating for males in terms of their time out [5] (or mating latency [66]) from the mating pool. A long time out decreases the PRR. In the bushcricket, Kawanaphila nartee, males transfer a large spermatophore at mating, consisting of an ampulla that contains the ejaculate and around it a protein secretion of the accessory glands. The latter serves both as paternity guard, protecting the ampulla from being eaten by the female before the ejaculate is transferred, and as paternal investment, because the proteins consumed by the female contribute directly to offspring production [67,68]. Early in their reproductive season, there is a shortage of pollen-rich flowers, which limits egg production. At this time, females harvest the nutritious spermatophores that males offer [69,70]. Because males also use pollen to produce spermatophores, this means that male PRR is low early in the season, with males taking several days following mating before they can collect enough pollen to be ready to mate again. Elevated female mating frequency coupled with long time out for males generates a female-biased OSR, which causes females to compete for the limited supply of males [71,72].
Female competition in K. nartee involves episodes of scramble competition with females racing to locate sexually signalling males, and contest competition with females struggling with already mounted females, attempting to usurp males from those who have arrived first. Gwynne & Bailey [73] found a selective advantage for females with larger auditory spiracles during scramble competition, as this gives females greater auditory sensitivity, allowing them to locate a sexually signalling male rapidly. Moreover, male K. nartee appears to promote female competition by producing very short bursts of song that are more difficult for females to locate, compared with the continuous song they produce under high nutrient availability, when males instead compete to attract a limited supply of sexually receptive females [73]. Interestingly, auditory spiracle size exhibits considerable sexual dimorphism in this species, expected of a trait subject to intense sexual selection. In this case, the female has the larger auditory spiracle and the greater auditory sensitivity [74,75].
Female competition for access to males has been widely documented where males or the resources they provide to females are in limited supply [76,77]. However, because of variation in male mate quality, female competition might also be expected when males are not the limiting sex [77]. When females exhibit strong mating preferences, more attractive males, or those of higher genetic quality, may be in greater demand than less attractive males. Under these conditions, females will compete for access to high quality males. For example, there are now a number of studies of birds and mammals in which males form communal display grounds, or leks, which have reported intense female competition over access to the most attractive males [78–80]. In part, this competition might be anticipated because attractive males in high demand from choosy females can become sperm depleted [81,82], selecting for females able to monopolize the attention of attractive males or to mate with these males before they become sperm depleted [77].
It is now clear that polyandry can influence female reproductive success, and that females can be subject to sexual selection. Indeed, there has been a general increase in research and debate on the role of sexual selection in the evolution of female reproductive biology [77,78,83]. Much of the current empirical work is focused on explaining the evolution of relatively rare examples of female ornaments or armaments [84–86], mainly because these kinds of traits are generally recognized to be the products of sexual selection acting on males. Importantly, however, the products of sexual selection via female competition may often be more subtle, as illustrated by K. nartee, with sexual selection favouring sensory adaptations in females that allow them to locate males quickly [73]. Similarly, in the common goby, Pomatoschistus microps, females respond to increased female competition by laying larger clutches of eggs [87], whereas in S. typhle, the presence of competitively superior, large females, causes small females to invest more in growth and less in current reproduction [88], thereby improving future competitive ability.
(b). Competition among males for mating opportunities
Female mating patterns have long been recognized as influencing the strength of male mating competition [4,40]. In species where females are continually receptive and highly polyandrous, males are often unable to monopolize females, resulting in comparatively low mating skews. In Soay sheep, for example, an increased availability of sexually receptive ewes results in a reduction in the strength of selection on male body and horn size, because when receptive females are in abundance, males are unable to use these traits to control access to multiple females [89]. The monopolizability of females appears to be a major factor influencing mammalian mating systems [90]. Likewise, in dung beetles, horns that are used in the monopolization of mates are less likely to evolve in species that occur in crowded communities, where males are unable to efficiently monopolize the continually receptive females [91].
By contrast, where females mate only once, male competition can be extreme, generating both protandry (where males emerge or reach sexual maturity before females) and extreme mating skew among males. For example, in heliconid butterflies, monandry has resulted in pupal mating [92], whereby adult males copulate with females before they emerge from their pupal case. Likewise, in solitary bees, males emerge often several days before females and competition for access to females as they emerge is intense, with the largest males achieving a disproportionate share of matings [93]. Protandry can also arise under female polyandry, if there is a benefit to the male of mating first, such as higher fecundity in young females [94] or first male paternity advantage, as in the bushcricket Requena verticalis [95]. In fact, male R. verticalis, which provide a costly nuptial gift, exhibit mate choice, selecting against older females that are less likely to be virgins [95].
4. Post-copulatory sexual selection acting on males
Sperm competition [96,97] and cryptic female choice [98] are probably the most widely appreciated consequences of polyandry for sexual selection acting on males. Polyandry can promote the spatial and temporal overlap of ejaculates from multiple males at the time of fertilization, so that sexual selection will favour adaptations in males that ensure their own sperm are successful in fertilizing available ova, and prevent rival males from gaining access to fertilization opportunities. Such adaptations might act via direct competitive interactions between males over paternity, or as signals, delivered during copulatory courtship [98] and used by females in selecting among sperm from prospective fathers. Naturally, post-copulatory sexual selection could act on females as well, but this is a subject that has rarely been addressed [99].
In depth reviews of the evolutionary consequences of post-mating sexual selection for male reproductive behaviour, morphology and physiology are available elsewhere [97,100,101]. Therefore, we provide an overview of these here and explore how increased male investment into adaptations that result from female polyandry can reduce the potential strength of sexual selection acting on males. In §5, we then focus on the potential interactions between pre- and post-mating sexual selection, how post-mating sexual selection might impact the net strength of sexual selection acting on males, and how male expenditure on traits subject to post-mating sexual selection might affect the operation of pre-mating sexual selection via its impact on male PRRs.
(a). Selection on male reproductive expenditure
A range of male reproductive traits that promote paternity have evolved in response to polyandrous mating by females. Morphological adaptations that enhance paternity are perhaps best documented in the insects, with sexual selection favouring adaptations in male genital morphology that displace or remove sperm of rival males from the site of fertilization, giving the last male to mate a fertilization advantage [102]. Polyandrous females frequently play an active role in the removal or displacement of sperm, responding cryptically to stimuli received from male genitalia during copulation [103,104]. Thus, across insect taxa, species where females are polyandrous tend to have more complex male genital morphology than do monandrous species [105]. Within polyandrous taxa, male and female genital morphology frequently exhibit strong signals of coevolutionary change [106–108].
The degree to which males can displace rival sperm can depend on the time they spend copulating with a given female [97,109]. In the yellow dung fly, Scatophaga stercoraria, males do not use their genitalia to remove rival ejaculates. Rather, rival sperm are flushed from the female's sperm store by their own ejaculates, such that the proportion of offspring sired by a copulating male rises with diminishing returns [110,111]. A male's fitness can depend critically on the time he spends copulating, the costs of prolonged copulation in terms of his ability to find and copulate with additional females, and in the case of yellow dung flies the time he spends guarding the female while she deposits her eggs.
A variety of forms of mate guarding have evolved in response to polyandry. Behavioural mate guarding is common across a variety of taxa, including insects [97], birds [101] and mammals [112]. However, mate guarding can also be achieved in ways that do not require the guarding male to be physically present. For example, male insects [97], reptiles [113,114] and mammals [115] deliver secretions to the female reproductive tract that harden to form mating plugs that serve as barriers to copulation by rival males. Among mammals, the protein secretions that form these plugs are under strong sexual selection that increases with the degree of polyandry [116–119]. In Drosophila, males transfer seminal fluid proteins within their ejaculates that suppress female sexual receptivity to remating and promote the immediate laying of eggs that will be fertilized by the copulating male [120]. These seminal fluid proteins are likewise characterized by a rapid evolutionary divergence [121] that is dependent on the degree of polyandry [122]. Tettigoniids also transfer compounds within the ejaculate that inhibit female sexual receptivity [69], and comparative analyses have found negative evolutionary covariation between ejaculate volume and the degree of polyandry [123]. Finally, in some insects, males transfer antiaphrodisiac pheromones to females, rendering them unattractive to other males. These pheromones show a faster rate of evolutionary divergence in clades where females are polyandrous [124].
Given that expenditure on mate guarding can have the effect of reducing a male's ability to mate with additional females [125,126], selection is expected to favour phenotypic plasticity in these components of pre- and post-copulatory expenditure to maximize the fitness returns of individual males. Indeed, we see considerable phenotypic plasticity within species, with males investing in mate guarding more heavily when the risk of sperm competition is high. For example, in mammals [127], birds [128], reptiles [129] and insects [130–133], males guard for longer, or with greater intensity, when the risk of female remating with rival males is elevated, whereas in Drosophila, males copulate for longer [134] and transfer greater quantities of receptivity inhibiting seminal fluid proteins when in the same situation [135,136]. In many taxa however, males are unable to control female mating frequencies, so that sperm competition conforms to a raffle in which a male's fertilization success depends critically on the number of sperm he has at the site of fertilization relative to his competitors [38].
Implicit in Bateman's [2] argument was the assumption that sperm were cheap, and that males were limited in their reproduction only by the number of females they could acquire. However, we now know that ejaculate production can represent a significant cost of reproduction for males [137,138]. Studies of insects [139], amphibians [140], reptiles [141], birds [142] and mammals [37] have all shown how males can become depleted of sperm and seminal fluid reserves with successive copulations, limiting the numbers of females with which they can mate. Moreover, there is growing evidence to suggest that males face a trade-off between the allocation of resources to ejaculate production and to pre-copulatory competition over access to females (table 1).
Table 1.
species | evidence | references |
---|---|---|
acanthocephalan worms | testes mass decreases with increasing sexual size dimorphism (a proxy for the intensity of male contest competition) | [143] |
insects | ||
Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni | juvenile hormone application increases eye span at the expense of testes mass | [144] |
Onthophagus nigriventris | ablation of developing horns in pre-pupae increases testes mass in adults | [145] |
Gnatocerus cornutus | selection for increased mandible size generates correlated reduction in testes mass | [146] |
Teleogryllus oceanicus | negative genetic correlation between courtship song structure and ejaculate quality | [147] |
Nauphoeta cinerea | competitive interactions between males decrease spermatophore size and sperm numbers | [148] |
Panorpa cognate | negative genetic correlation between attractiveness and ejaculate investment per mating | [149] |
Photinus greeni | negative phenotypic correlation between attractiveness and competitive fertilization success | [150] |
Hemideina crassidens | males with large weapons have relatively smaller testes and ejaculate volumes than males with small weapons | [151] |
amphibians | ||
Crinia georgiana | negative among population covariation between testes mass and forearm development | [152] |
fishes | ||
Oncorhynchus kisutch | breeding coloration negatively correlated with sperm motility | [153] |
Salvelinus alpinus | negative phenotypic correlation between red spawning coloration, dominance status and sperm density | [154,155] |
Poecilia reticulata | negative genetic correlation between ornamentation and ejaculate quality | [156] |
Pomatoschistus minutus | males lacking breeding coloration have testes 4.3 times larger, in absolute terms, than males with breeding coloration | [157] |
Thalassoma bifasciatum | males with high mating success divert resources towards mate guarding at the expense of gamete production | [81] |
birds | ||
Chlamydotis undulata | males with exaggerated courtship displays show rapid deterioration in spermatogenic function compared with males with less elaborate displays | [158] |
Gallus gallus domesticus | decline in ejaculate quality is associated with success in dominance interactions | [159] |
Malurus melanocephalus | negative phenotypic correlation between plumage ornamentation and ejaculated sperm numbers | [160] |
mammals | ||
pinnipeds | among harem breeders, testis mass decreases with degree of sexual size dimorphism (a proxy for the intensity of male contest competition) | [161] |
Homo sapiens | negative phenotypic correlation between voice attractiveness and ejaculated sperm numbers | [162] |
Mating costs of ejaculate production have recently been incorporated explicitly into Parker's game theoretic models of ejaculate expenditure [152]. Here, males are assumed to have a fixed energy budget for expenditure on reproduction, which can be spent either on acquiring mates (through mate searching, fighting for direct access to females or the resources they require, or courtship displays and ornamentation used to attract females and persuading them to copulate) or on the various ejaculate components that determine male fertilization success (sperm, seminal fluid proteins). Male fitness is assumed to be the product of the number of matings obtained and the fitness gain per mating. Across species, the models predict that males should increase their expenditure on the ejaculate, and decrease their expenditure on gaining additional matings as the risk (probability that a female will mate with more than one male) and intensity (the number of males with which a female mates) of sperm competition increases (figure 3). There is now considerable evidence to support this basic prediction. Thus, across species, increases in the degree of polyandry are associated with increases in testes mass [100,164]. Laboratory evolution studies of single species have also shown increases in male ejaculate expenditure in response to experimentally elevated rates of polyandry, in both insects [165,166] and mammals [167,168], and similar patterns of micro-evolutionary divergence are implicated from cross-population studies of mammals [169], frogs [170] and birds [171] that show covariation in testes size and the degree of polyandry.
As with mate guarding, theoretical models predict that males should adjust their expenditure on gaining fertilizations, depending on the current risks or intensity of sperm competition [38]. Specifically, males should increase their ejaculate expenditure when faced with a risk of sperm competition from rival males, but reduce their expenditure as the number of males competing for a given set of ova increases (sperm competition intensity), because the fitness pay-off per unit of ejaculate expenditure is expected to decline as the number of males competing increases [38]. Studies of mammals [172,173], fish [174,175], birds [176] and insects [177] have all reported increased numbers of sperm ejaculated when males perceive cues to the presence of rival males (sperm competition risk). Indeed, two recent meta-analyses of the literature have reported moderate sized and general effects of the presence of rivals on ejaculate expenditure [178,179]. General support for the prediction that males should decrease their ejaculate expenditure with increasing intensity is perhaps less strong; the general effect size found by Kelly & Jennions [178] was not statistically significant. However, lack of concordance among studies may lie in experimental design. Manipulating a male's perception of the presence of rival males (risk) is simpler than manipulating his perception of sperm competition intensity, because an increased number of males present at the time of copulation may convey cues of heighted risk rather than the number of males actually competing for fertilizations [180]. Studies that have manipulated cues to a female's past mating frequency have provided good support for the prediction that males decrease expenditure with sperm competition intensity [181].
An assumption underlying intensity models is that males adjust their ejaculate expenditure in response to variation in fitness pay-offs for their investment. From a male's perspective, a highly polyandrous female is assumed to be of reduced reproductive value compared with a less polyandrous female. Male responses to sperm competition intensity can, in this sense, be seen as cryptic male mate choice [48]. Male adjustments in ejaculate expenditure have been reported for a number of measures of female quality, including female age, size, fecundity and secondary sexual trait expression [182,183], with larger ejaculates being delivered to females of greater quality. Again, the general effect of female quality on ejaculate expenditure appears both statistically significant and moderate in size, although it varies across taxonomic groups and reproductive mode [178]. These studies show how individual males can exhibit fine-grained phenotypic plasticity in the allocation of their reproductive resources to different females, and that polyandry is a significant driver of male expenditure.
(b). Impact on male potential reproductive rate and the strength of sexual selection
Adaptations to sperm competition might be expected to affect the strength of sexual selection acting on males insofar as they will influence male fitness via the number of mates acquired, the Bateman gradient. Males that must copulate for extended periods to displace rival sperm or guard their mates from potential rivals must take ‘time out’ from mate searching in order to ensure they fertilize their current female's batch of ova. Likewise, allocation of resources to mating plugs or seminal fluids can be costly in terms of time required to replenish the proteins necessary for additional mating attempts. These energetic costs of sperm competition, which arise as an evolutionary consequence of polyandry, will reduce a male's PRR, the male Bateman gradient [7], and thus the potential strength of sexual selection both among and within species [184,185]. Among species, if males increase their expenditure on the ejaculate with increasing risk of female remating, male time out for gamete and seminal fluid replenishment is likely to increase. This would have the effect of ameliorating any male bias in the OSR, and reducing the opportunity for sexual selection acting via the number of mates males obtain. Within species, as the degree of polyandry increases, the intensity of sperm competition should increase, and as discussed in §4a, ejaculate expenditure is then expected to decrease [38,186]. A decreased expenditure on the ejaculate by males would have the counter effect of decreasing male time out, increasing the OSR bias towards males, and thus the opportunity for sexual selection to act via male mating success. Such effects are likely to be strong in species where ejaculate costs are very high. For example, in bushcrickets, males can expend up to 30 per cent or more of their body mass on a single ejaculate, which consists of sperm, seminal fluid proteins that inhibit female receptivity, and the edible mass of accessory gland proteins that females consume during the period in which the ejaculate is transferred from the spermatophore to the female's sperm storage organs [123,187,188]. The magnitude of male expenditure on these large ejaculates comes at a considerable cost, increasing male time out from the mating pool required to replenish spent resources [126]. As we have seen for K. nartee, male time out can greatly exceed female time out, generating a complete reversal in the direction of sexual competition, so that females compete for access to males [72,189].
An additional, and probably more widespread, effect of sperm competition on the intensity of sexual selection is expected to arise in species were males provide parental care. Trivers [3] originally recognized that when females are polyandrous, so that their broods contain offspring from multiple males, each male has a lower expected relatedness to the brood, reducing the benefits of paternal care. Subsequent theoretical treatments have concluded that lost paternity through sperm competition can select against paternal care [190–193]. Experimental studies of fish, insects, birds and mammals have revealed how males reduce their expenditure on paternal care when the risk of lost paternity through sperm competition is elevated, although such effects are not always found [194]. A male is expected to respond only to a risk of lost paternity if he can expect to gain greater paternity in future breeding attempts, a factor that might help explain the often reported variation in the effects of paternity on paternal care among species [195]. By contrast, although cause and effect are difficult to distinguish, a broad pattern of negative coevolutionary variation has been found between rates of extra-pair paternity and female dependency on paternal care (estimated from the reduction in female reproductive success when the male is removed) across bird species [196,197]. Simmons & Parker [184] argued that increased polyandry and sperm competition could influence the strength of sexual selection acting on males indirectly, because lost paternity would select for reduced male expenditure on paternal care that would in turn reduce a male's time out from seeking additional mates. Indeed, the theoretical analyses of Kokko & Jennions [195] show that sex roles are predicted by both polyandry and the strength of sexual selection acting on males and females, but not by the initial bias in parental investment into gamete production. Greater sexual selection and female multiple mating were shown to generate more female than male care.
In socially monogamous birds, attractive males who are successful in forming breeding pair bonds are often also successful in gaining extra-pair fertilizations [198–200]. In this manner, sperm competition or cryptic female choice increases the variance in male reproductive success and strengthens the intensity of sexual selection on males [201,202]. However, in polygamous mating systems, lost paternity can dampen the opportunity for sexual selection. In the sand goby, Pomatoschistus minutus, males build nests in which they tend batches of eggs from several females. Despite paternal care, sexual selection acts predominantly via male competition for females [203]. Parasitic fertilizations in nest owners' nests are common in this system [204]. Jones et al. [204] modelled the opportunity for sexual selection on males, based on the observed variance in male reproductive success estimated from fertilization success within and among nests. They found that the opportunity for sexual selection declined slightly with the proportion of males that took part in sneak matings, but was markedly reduced when parasitic fertilizations were performed by non-nesting males instead of nesting males (figure 4). These contrasting findings for socially monogamous birds and polygynous fishes illustrate how post-copulatory sexual selection can have either synergistic or antagonistic effects on the strength of pre-copulatory sexual selection, a theme we will address in detail in §5.
5. Interactions between pre- and post-copulatory sexual selection
Quantifying the form and intensity of sexual selection across episodes of male contest competition and female choice has rightly been highlighted as an important endeavour for our understanding of the net force of sexual selection, and thus its evolutionary potential [205]. For example, episodes of male contest competition and female choice can be reinforcing, enhancing the evolutionary potential of that selection. But equally, it can be opposing, so that female choice might ease selection acting via male contest competition. Typically, researchers have estimated the form and strength of selection only at single episodes, rendering our understanding of the evolutionary consequences of selection incomplete [205]. In reality, when females are polyandrous, there are potentially five episodes of sexual selection that need to be quantified; pre-copulatory sexual selection via mate choice and competition for direct access to mates or for resources that qualify an individual to mate [12], and post-copulatory sexual selection via sperm competition and cryptic female choice (figure 5). While there are studies that have examined both male contest competition and female choice operating on the same species [205,207], few studies have extended this into the post-copulatory arena.
Soay sheep, Ovis aries, illustrate the ameliorating effects of antagonistic pre- and post-copulatory sexual selection on male fitness. While males compete for access to females, females are highly polyandrous, copulating with as many as 10 different rams per day, which generates intense sperm competition [208]. Data from a free-living population on St Kilda show that body size and horn size independently contribute to male ability to acquire females, and that horn size and testes size independently affect siring success [89]. Importantly, however, intensity of sexual selection acting on male secondary sexual traits depends critically on the availability of ewes, or the OSR. When ewes were limited, pre-copulatory sexual selection was strongest, as expected. However, as the availability of ewes increased, the intensity of post-copulatory sexual selection on testes size increased, and the strength of pre-copulatory sexual selection on body and horn size declined [89]. Moreover, dominant rams that monopolize access to ewes have a high copulation frequency and rapidly become sperm depleted, suffering a reduction in paternity success under sperm competition. Thus, while the copulatory success of large dominant rams increases through the rutting season, their siring success declines, reducing the intensity of pre-copulatory sexual selection acting on male secondary sexual traits [37].
Effects of sperm depletion for males achieving high competitive mating success have also been reported in studies of insect mating systems. In a laboratory study of Gerris lacustris, large males were able to obtain a greater proportion of matings [209]. However, small males, when they did obtain a mating, copulated for longer and obtained a higher relative paternity, so that there was no net action of sexual selection on male size [209]. In fireflies, Photinus greeni, males with the most attractive bioluminescent displays had the lowest competitive fertilization success [150]. In this experiment, males only mated once. Thus, this probably reflects a trade-off between male expenditure on attractiveness versus competitive fertility rather than on sperm depletion, but the effect is the same, that males favoured in pre-copulatory sexual selection can be disfavoured when females mate polyandrously.
Polyandry may also magnify the effects of pre-copulatory sexual selection. In guppies, Poecilia reticulata, females show strong preference for males with large orange patches, male orangeness and female preference show additive genetic variation, and are genetically correlated across the sexes, such that within populations the trait and preference coevolve until checked by natural selection [210,211]. Females also exercise cryptic female choice, accepting greater numbers of sperm from colourful males [212]. Even when female control over insemination is eliminated by artificial insemination of equal numbers of sperm from two males, paternity is biased towards the sperm donor with greater orange coloration [213]. Thus, pre- and post-copulatory sexual selection appear to act synergistically in these fish. Synergistic effects might be expected where high-quality males have enough resources to allocate to both secondary sexual traits and ejaculate production.
Life-history theory is firmly grounded in the notion of trade-offs in resource allocation to fitness related traits [214]. When variance in the allocation of resources required for growth and reproduction exceeds variance in the acquisition of those resources, individuals who invest heavily in attracting females might be expected to have fewer resources available for investment into their ejaculates [215,216]. This is an underlying assumption in sperm competition theory for which there is some good evidence (table 1). However, when variance in acquisition exceeds variance in allocation, we can expect high quality individuals to be in a position to invest heavily in multiple fitness traits [215,216], such as secondary sexual traits and ejaculates [217]. Thus, in some cases, we might expect to see positive correlations between competitive fertilization success and mate acquisition. Indeed, colourful male guppies ejaculate faster swimming and more viable sperm [218], implying that some males can invest heavily in both attractiveness and fertility. Females might thereby ensure good-genes benefits for their offspring not just by pre-copulatory mate choice, but also by polyandry and the competitive fertilization success of good quality males [55]. Where male attractiveness and fertilization success are positively correlated, polyandry is expected to intensify pre-copulatory sexual selection acting via female choice.
The phenotypic expression of traits subject to intense sexual selection via female choice can evolve condition dependence, providing honest signals of the underlying genetic quality of potential mates [40,219,220]. Normally associated with secondary sexual traits, this argument can equally pertain to ejaculate traits that promote paternity [221]. Condition dependence in male fertilization success can thus be an important avenue for cryptic female choice. Evidence for the synergistic action of pre- and post-copulatory female choice comes from dung beetles, Onthophagus taurus, in which males court females with a heritable and condition-dependent drumming [222]. High condition males are more attractive, have larger testes and shorter sperm [221]. Unsurprisingly, males with larger testes have a competitive advantage in sperm competition [166]. Moreover, females selectively use the short sperm of high-condition males [223,224]. The consequence of these re-enforcing episodes of pre- and post-copulatory female choice is the production of offspring with enhanced viability [225,226].
Finally, there may be no correlation between pre- and post-copulatory episodes of sexual selection. In a recent study of D. melanogaster, fertilization success contributed nearly as strongly as mating success to a male's net lifetime reproductive success. However, males mating last have the fertilization advantage in this species, and when variance in mating order was removed fertilization success explained little of a male's lifetime fitness [227]. The lack of correlation between mating and fertilization success means that measures of mating success alone are inappropriate for estimating overall male success in sexual selection in this population. Such findings are not limited to laboratory studies.
Leks are thought to epitomize sexual selection acting on males, because females mate with only the most attractive male present, resulting in extreme mating skews [228–230], and thus intense sexual selection. However, off lek polyandry can greatly temper the strength of sexual selection. This was found in Houbara bustards, Chlamydotis undulata, which at first appear to show a classic lek-based mating system [231]. However, the reproductive skew is nullified via extreme female polyandry [232]. Off lek polyandry is not uncommon in lekking species [80,233,234], questioning the validity of mating success data collected at leks as estimates of the strength of sexual selection.
In general, Parker & Birkhead's [7] simple model predicts that polyandry and sperm competition should reduce the benefits of multiple mates for males, and thus the strength of sexual selection. Indeed, a recent study of feral fowl, Gallus gallus, found just such an effect [235]. Polyandry was found to increase the relative importance of post-copulatory episodes of sexual selection on male dominance, relative to pre-copulatory episodes, and simultaneously erode the opportunity for and strength of sexual selection.
The earlier-mentioned studies illustrate how estimates of selection based on mating success alone can provide a limited and sometimes incorrect view of the action of sexual selection. Sperm competition and cryptic female choice can dampen or negate any male advantages obtained via mating success, or can amplify selection, depending on the life history costs of competing for mates and sperm production. There is little reason to expect that there should be any generality here. A growing number of studies are available to show that competitive males can be limited in their ability to produce costly ejaculates and thus unable to capitalize on their mating success (table 1). On the other hand, there are also a growing number of studies that are pointing towards mechanisms capable of generating positive associations between male attractiveness and sperm quality [236–238]. Understanding the net action of sexual selection in a given mating system will require detailed understanding of both pre- and post-copulatory processes.
6. Concluding remarks
Our review of the literature illustrates the many ways in which polyandry can moderate sexual selection. In general, when females benefit from polyandrous mating behaviour, for any of the reasons listed in §2, we should expect the slope of the Bateman gradient (translating female mating success to reproductive success) to increase. And as soon as males, or males of high quality, become a limiting resource for females, we can expect females to compete among themselves for such mates. However, polyandry has multiple repercussions for both males and females, before and after mating, and it has several important feedback mechanisms that will impact the action of sexual selection. For example, theoretical models of post-copulatory sexual selection suggest that polyandry will favour the evolution of ejaculate expenditure at a cost of reduced expenditure on ornaments and/or armaments (figure 3). Moreover, polyandry that occurs due to parasitic fertilizations (sneaking, extra-pair fertilizations, cuckoldry, etc.) might be expected to increase sexual selection on males in some cases. However, whether it does or not depends entirely on who gains the parasitic fertilizations—males already successful in gaining multiple females or males that otherwise might not breed [204]. An aspect of post-copulatory sexual selection that we would like to see explored, is selection acting on females via male adjustments in their reproductive investment.
Although a sex difference in the slopes of Bateman gradients, often combined with sexual dimorphism, can be indicative of sexual selection in the more ornamented or armed sex, such sex differences are not required for sexual selection to be acting strongly—it only makes it easier to discover. Therefore, the special interest in sex difference in ornamentation [201], Bateman gradients [14] or variance in reproductive success [6], characteristic of much research in this area, carries the risk that we fail to spot less obvious cases of sexual selection. In many cases, both sexes can show steep Bateman gradients, and hence benefit from having multiple mates, or be under strong sexual selection for other reasons.
Historically, sexual selection research has focused strongly on the evolution of male secondary sexual traits, undoubtedly because of the early expectation that female reproductive success should be independent of the number of males mated [2]. However, we now know that polyandry can clearly influence female reproductive success, and that females, like males, are subject to sexual selection. With a general increase in research and debate on the role of sexual selection in the evolution of female reproductive biology [77,78,83], much work has focused on explaining the evolution of female ornaments or armaments [84–86], because such traits are generally recognized to be the products of sexual selection acting on males. Importantly, however, the products of sexual selection via female competition may be less obvious. We believe, therefore, that a shift in focus from the expected products to the process of sexual selection acting on females, will widen our eyes to a far greater role of female competition over access to mates than our current view affords. We also believe that future sexual selection research must consider explicitly the effects of polyandry on mating competition and mate choice for both sexes, and on the net action of sexual selection acting on males and females across both pre- and post-copulatory episodes. Such an approach is essential for a true appreciation of what is arguably the most important agent of evolutionary change.
Acknowledgements
C.K. was supported by the Swedish Research Council, and L.W.S. was supported by an Australian Professorial Fellowship from the ARC.
References
- 1.Darwin C. 1871. The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London, UK: John Murray [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bateman AJ. 1948. Intra-sexual selection in Drosophila. Heredity 2, 349–368 10.1038/hdy.1948.21 (doi:10.1038/hdy.1948.21) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Trivers RL. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. In sexual selection and the descent of man, 1871–1971 (ed. Campbell BG.), pp. 136–179 Chicago, IL: Aldine [Google Scholar]
- 4.Emlen ST, Oring LW. 1977. Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems. Science 197, 215–223 10.1126/science.327542 (doi:10.1126/science.327542) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Clutton-Brock TH, Parker GA. 1992. Potential reproductive rates and the operation of sexual selection. Q. Rev. Biol. 67, 437–456 10.1086/417793 (doi:10.1086/417793) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Shuster SM, Wade MJ. 2003. Mating systems and strategies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press [Google Scholar]
- 7.Parker GA, Birkhead TR. 2013. Polyandry: the history of a revolution. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 368, 20120335 10.1098/rstb.2012.0335 (doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0335) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Shuster SM, Briggs WR, Dennis PA. 2013. How multiple mating by females affects sexual selection. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 368, 20120046 10.1098/rstb.2012.0046 (doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0046) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Kvarnemo C, Ahnesjö I. 1996. The dynamics of operational sex ratios and competition for mates. Trends Ecol. Evol. 11, 404–408 10.1016/0169-5347(96)10056-2 (doi:10.1016/0169-5347(96)10056-2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ahnesjö I, Forsgren E, Kvarnemo C. 2008. Variation in sexual selection in fishes. In Fish behaviour (eds Magnhagen C, Braithwaite VA, Forsgren E, Kapoor BG.), pp. 303–335 Enfield, NH: Science Publishers [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kvarnemo C, Merilaita S. 2006. Mating distribution and its temporal dynamics affect operational sex ratio: a simulation study. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 89, 551–559 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2006.00694.x (doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.2006.00694.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ahnesjö I, Kvarnemo C, Merilaita S. 2001. Using potential reproductive rates to predict mating competition among individuals qualified to mate. Behav. Ecol. 12, 397–401 10.1093/beheco/12.4.397 (doi:10.1093/beheco/12.4.397) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gerlach NM, McGlothlin JW, Parker PG, Ketterson ED. 2012. Reinterpreting Bateman gradients: multiple mating and selection in both sexes of a songbird species. Behav. Ecol. 23, 1078–1088 10.1093/beheco/ars077 (doi:10.1093/beheco/ars077) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Arnold SJ, Duvall D. 1994. Animal mating systems: a synthesis based on selection theory. Am. Nat. 143, 317–348 10.1086/285606 (doi:10.1086/285606) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Andersson M, Iwasa Y. 1996. Sexual selection. Trends Ecol. Evol. 11, 53–58 10.1016/0169-5347(96)81042-1 (doi:10.1016/0169-5347(96)81042-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Vincent A, Ahnesjö I, Berglund A, Rosenqvist G. 1992. Pipefishes and seahorses: are they all sex role reversed? Trends Ecol. Evol. 7, 237–241 10.1016/0169-5347(92)90052-D (doi:10.1016/0169-5347(92)90052-D) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Snyder BF, Gowaty PA. 2007. A reappraisal of Bateman's classic study of intrasexual selection. Evolution 61, 2457–2468 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00212.x (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00212.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Jones AG, Rosenqvist G, Berglund A, Arnold SJ, Avise JC. 2000. The Bateman gradient and the cause of sexual selection in a sex-role-reversed pipefish. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 267, 677–680 10.1098/rspb.2000.1055 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1055) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Rodríguez-Muñoz R, Breitman A, Slate J, Walling CA, Tregenza T. 2010. Natural and sexual selection in a wild insect population. Science 328, 1269–1272 10.1126/science.1188102 (doi:10.1126/science.1188102) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Jennions MD, Petrie M. 2000. Why do females mate multiply? A review of the genetic benefits. Biol. Rev. 75, 21–64 10.1017/S0006323199005423 (doi:10.1017/S0006323199005423) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Lehtonen J, Jennions MD, Kokko H. 2012. The many costs of sex. Trends Ecol. Evol. 27, 172–178 10.1016/j.tree.2011.09.016 (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2011.09.016) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Magnhagen C. 1991. Predation risk as a cost of reproduction. Trends Ecol. Evol. 6, 183–186 10.1016/0169-5347(91)90210-O (doi:10.1016/0169-5347(91)90210-O) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Jennions MD, Petrie M. 1997. Variation in mate choice and mating preferences: a review of causes and consequences. Biol. Rev. 72, 283–327 10.1017/S0006323196005014 (doi:10.1017/S0006323196005014) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Altizer S, et al. 2003. Social organization and parasite risk in mammals: Integrating theory and empirical studies. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 34, 517–547 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.34.030102.151725 (doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.34.030102.151725) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Innocenti P, Morrow EH. 2009. Immunogenic males: a genome-wide analysis of reproduction and the cost of mating in Drosophila melanogaster females. J. Evol. Biol. 22, 964–973 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01708.x (doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01708.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Emlen ST, Wrege PH, Webster MS. 1998. Cuckoldry as a cost of polyandry in the sex-role-reversed wattled jacana, Jacana jacana. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265, 2359–2364 10.1098/rspb.1998.0584 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0584) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Butchart SHM. 1999. Sexual conflicts and copulation patterns in polyandrous bronze-winged jacanas (Metopidius indicus). Behaviour 136, 443–468 10.1163/156853999501414 (doi:10.1163/156853999501414) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Berglund A, Rosenqvist G. 2003. Sex role reversal in pipefish. Adv. Study Behav. 32, 131–167 10.1016/S0065-3454(03)01003-9 (doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(03)01003-9) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.McCoy EE, Jones AG, Avise JC. 2001. The genetic mating system and tests for cuckoldry in a pipefish species in which males fertilise eggs and brood offspring externally. Mol. Ecol. 10, 1793–1800 10.1046/j.0962-1083.2001.01320.x (doi:10.1046/j.0962-1083.2001.01320.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Gwynne DT. 1984. Courtship feeding increases female reproductive success in bush crickets. Nature 307, 361–363 10.1038/307361a0 (doi:10.1038/307361a0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Arnqvist G, Nilsson T. 2000. The evolution of polyandry: multiple mating and female fitness in insects. Anim. Behav. 60, 145–164 10.1006/anbe.2000.1446 (doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1446) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Simmons LW. 2005. The evolution of polyandry: sperm competition, sperm selection, and offspring viability. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 36, 125–146 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.36.102403.112501 (doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.36.102403.112501) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Wetton JH, Parkin DT. 1991. An association between fertility and cuckoldry in the house sparrow, Passer domesticus. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 245, 227–233 10.1098/rspb.1991.0114 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1991.0114) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Garcia-Gonzalez F. 2004. Infertile matings and sperm competition: the effect of ‘nonsperm representation’ on intraspecific variation in sperm precedence patterns. Am. Nat. 164, 457–472 10.1086/423987 (doi:10.1086/423987) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Hasson O, Stone L. 2009. Male infertility, female infertility and extrapair copulations. Biol. Rev. 84, 225–244 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2008.00068.x (doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2008.00068.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Levitan DR. 1998. Sperm limitation, gamete competition, and sexual selection in external fertilizers. In Sperm competition and sexual selection (eds Birkhead TR, Møller AP.), pp. 175–217 New York, NY: Academic Press [Google Scholar]
- 37.Preston BT, Stevenson IR, Pemberton JM, Wilson K. 2001. Dominant rams lose out by sperm depletion. Nature 409, 681–682 10.1038/35055617 (doi:10.1038/35055617) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Parker GA, Pizzari T. 2010. Sperm competition and ejaculate economics. Biol. Rev. 85, 897–934 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Slatyer RA, Mautz BS, Backwell PRY, Jennions MD. 2012. Estimating genetic benefits of polyandry from experimental studies: a meta-analysis. Biol. Rev. 87, 1–33 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2011.00182.x (doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2011.00182.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Andersson M. 1994. Sexual selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press [Google Scholar]
- 41.Zeh JA, Zeh DW. 1996. The evolution of polyandry. I. Intragenomic conflict and genetic incompatibility. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 263, 1711–1717 10.1098/rspb.1996.0250 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1996.0250) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Tregenza T, Wedell N. 2000. Genetic compatibility, mate choice and patterns of parentage: invited review. Mol. Ecol. 9, 1013–1027 10.1046/j.1365-294x.2000.00964.x (doi:10.1046/j.1365-294x.2000.00964.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Puurtinen M, Ketola T, Kotiaho JS. 2009. The good-genes and compatible-genes benefits of mate choice. Am. Nat. 174, 741–752 10.1086/606024 (doi:10.1086/606024) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Zeh JA, Zeh DW. 2001. Reproductive mode and the genetic benefits of polyandry. Anim. Behav. 61, 1051–1063 10.1006/anbe.2000.1705 (doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1705) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Roberts SC, Gosling LM. 2003. Genetic similarity and quality interact in mate choice decisions by female mice. Nat. Genet. 33, 103–106 10.1038/ng1231 (doi:10.1038/ng1231) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Mays HL, Hill GE. 2004. Choosing mates: good genes versus genes that are a good fit. Trends Ecol. Evol. 19, 554–559 10.1016/j.tree.2004.07.018 (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2004.07.018) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Zeh JA, Zeh DW. 1997. The evolution of polyandry. II. Post-copulatory defenses against genetic incompatibility. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 264, 69–75 10.1098/rspb.1997.0010 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1997.0010) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Edward DA, Chapman T. 2011. The evolution and significance of male mate choice. Trends Ecol. Evol. 26, 647–654 10.1016/j.tree.2011.07.012 (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2011.07.012) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Colegrave N, Kotiaho JS, Tomkins JL. 2002. Mate choice or polyandry: reconciling genetic compatibility and good genes sexual selection. Evol. Ecol. Res. 4, 911–917 [Google Scholar]
- 50.Price TAR, Hodgson DJ, Lewis Z, Hurst GDD, Wedell N. 2008. Selfish genetic elements promote polyandry in a fly. Science 322, 1241–1243 10.1126/science.1163766 (doi:10.1126/science.1163766) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Champion de Crespigny FE, Hurst LD, Wedell N. 2008. Do Wolbachia-associated incompatibilities promote polyandry? Evolution 62, 107–122 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00274.x (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00274.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Michalczyk Ł, Millard AL, Martin OY, Lumley AJ, Emerson BC, Chapman T, Gage MJG. 2011. Inbreeding promotes female promiscuity. Science 333, 1739–1742 10.1126/science.1207314 (doi:10.1126/science.1207314) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Keller L, Reeve HK. 1995. Why do females mate with multiple males: the sexually selected sperm hypothesis. Adv. Study Behav. 24, 291–315 10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60397-6 (doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60397-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Pizzari T, Birkhead TR. 2002. The sexually-selected sperm hypothesis: sex biased inheritance and sexual antagonism. Biol. Rev. 77, 183–209 10.1017/S1464793101005863 (doi:10.1017/S1464793101005863) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Yasui Y. 1997. A ‘good-sperm’ model can explain the evolution of costly multiple mating by females. Am. Nat. 149, 573–584 10.1086/286006 (doi:10.1086/286006) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Evans JP, Simmons LW. 2008. The genetic basis of traits regulating sperm competition and polyandry: can selection favour the evolution of good- and sexy-sperm? Genetica 134, 5–19 10.1007/s10709-007-9162-5 (doi:10.1007/s10709-007-9162-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Firman RC, Simmons LW. 2012. Male house mice evolving with post-copulatory sexual selection sire embryos with increased viability. Ecol. Lett. 15, 42–46 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01706.x (doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01706.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Stockley P. 2003. Female multiple mating behaviour, early reproductive failure and litter size variation in mammals. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 270, 271–278 10.1098/rspb.2002.2228 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2228) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Yasui Y. 1998. The ‘genetic benefits’ of female multiple mating reconsidered. Trends Ecol. Evol. 13, 246–250 10.1016/S0169-5347(98)01383-4 (doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(98)01383-4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Crean AJ, Marshall DJ. 2009. Coping with environmental uncertainty: dynamic bet hedging as a maternal effect. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 364, 1087–1096 10.1098/rstb.2008.0237 (doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0237) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Aguirre JD, Marshall DJ. 2012. Does genetic diversity reduce sibling competition? Evolution 66, 94–102 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2011.01413.x (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2011.01413.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Forsman A, Ahnesjö J, Caesar S. 2007. Fitness benefits of diverse offspring in pygmy grasshoppers. Evol. Ecol. Res. 9, 1305–1318 [Google Scholar]
- 63.Baer B, Schmid-Hempel P. 1999. Experimental variation in polyandry affects parasite loads and fitness in a bumble-bee. Nature 397, 151–154 10.1038/16451 (doi:10.1038/16451) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Sagebakken G, Ahnesjö I, Braga Goncalves I, Kvarnemo C. 2011. Multiply-mated males show higher embryo survival in a paternally caring fish. Behav. Ecol. 22, 625–629 10.1093/beheco/arr023 (doi:10.1093/beheco/arr023) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.McLeod L, Marshall DJ. 2009. Do genetic diversity effects drive the benefits associated with multiple mating? A test in a marine invertebrate. PLoS ONE 4, e6347 10.1371/journal.pone.0006347 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006347) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Gowaty PA, Hubbell SP. 2005. Chance, time allocation, and the evolution of adaptively flexible sex role behaviour. Integr. Comp. Behav. 45, 931–944 10.1093/icb/45.5.931 (doi:10.1093/icb/45.5.931) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Simmons LW. 1990. Nuptial feeding in tettigoniids: male costs and the rates of fecundity increase. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 27, 43–47 [Google Scholar]
- 68.Simmons LW, Gwynne DT. 1993. Reproductive investment in bush crickets: the allocation of male and female nutrients to offspring. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 252, 1–5 10.1098/rspb.1993.0038 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1993.0038) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Simmons LW, Gwynne DT. 1991. The refractory period of female katydids (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae): sexual conflict over the remating interval? Behav. Ecol. 2, 276–282 [Google Scholar]
- 70.Simmons LW. 1995. Relative parental expenditure, potential reproductive rates, and the control of sexual selection in katydids. Am. Nat. 145, 797–808 10.1086/285768 (doi:10.1086/285768) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Gwynne DT, Simmons LW. 1990. Experimental reversal of courtship roles in an insect. Nature 346, 172–174 10.1038/346172a0 (doi:10.1038/346172a0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Simmons LW, Bailey WJ. 1990. Resource influenced sex roles of zaprochiline tettigoniids (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). Evolution 44, 1853–1868 10.2307/2409513 (doi:10.2307/2409513) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Gwynne DT, Bailey WJ. 1999. Female–female competition in katydids: sexual selection for increased sensitivity to a male signal? Evolution 53, 546–551 10.2307/2640790 (doi:10.2307/2640790) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Bailey WJ, Romer H. 1991. Sexual differences in auditory sensitivity: mismatch of hearing threshold and call frequency in a tettigoniid (Orthotera, Tettigoniidae: Zaprochilinae). J. Comp. Phys. A 169, 349–353 [Google Scholar]
- 75.Bailey WJ, Simmons LW. 1991. Male–male behavior and sexual dimorphism of the ear of a zaprochiline tettigoniid (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). J. Insect Behav. 4, 51–65 10.1007/BF01092551 (doi:10.1007/BF01092551) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Forsgren E, Amundsen T, Borg ÅA, Bjelvenmark J. 2004. Unusually dynamic sex roles in a fish. Nature 429, 551–554 10.1038/nature02562 (doi:10.1038/nature02562) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Rosvall KA. 2011. Intrasexual competition in females: evidence for sexual selection? Behav. Ecol. 22, 1131–1140 10.1093/beheco/arr106 (doi:10.1093/beheco/arr106) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Stockley P, Bro-Jørgensen J. 2011. Female competition and its evolutionary consequences in mammals. Biol. Rev. 86, 341–366 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00149.x (doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00149.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Karvonen E, Rintamäki PT, Alatalo RV. 2000. Female–female aggression and female mate choice on black grouse leks. Anim. Behav. 59, 981–987 10.1006/anbe.1999.1379 (doi:10.1006/anbe.1999.1379) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Petrie M, Hall M, Halliday T, Budgey H, Pierpoint C. 1992. Multiple mating in a lekking bird: why do peahens mate with more than one male and with the same male more than once? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 31, 349–358 [Google Scholar]
- 81.Warner RR, Shapiro DY, Marconato A, Petersen CW. 1995. Sexual conflict: males with highest mating success convey the lowest fertilization benefits to females. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 262, 135–139 10.1098/rspb.1995.0187 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1995.0187) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Jones TM. 2001. A potential cost of monandry in the lekking sandfly Lutzomyia longipalpis. J. Insect Behav. 14, 385–399 10.1023/A:1011127514317 (doi:10.1023/A:1011127514317) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Clutton-Brock TH. 2009. Sexual selection in females. Anim. Behav. 77, 3–11 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.08.026 (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.08.026) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Heinsohn R, Legge S, Endler JA. 2005. Extreme reversed sexual dichromatism in a bird without sex role reversal. Science 309, 617–619 10.1126/science.1112774 (doi:10.1126/science.1112774) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Amundsen T. 2000. Why are female birds ornamented? Trends Ecol. Evol. 15, 149–155 10.1016/S0169-5347(99)01800-5 (doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(99)01800-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Watson NL, Simmons LW. 2010. Reproductive competition promotes the evolution of female weaponry. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 2035–2040 10.1098/rspb.2009.2335 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.2335) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Heubel KU, Lindström K, Kokko H. 2008. Females increase current reproductive effort when future access to males is uncertain. Biol. Lett. 4, 224–227 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0630 (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2007.0630) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Berglund A. 1991. Egg competition in a sex-role reversed pipefish: subdominant females trade reproduction for growth. Evolution 45, 770–774 10.2307/2409928 (doi:10.2307/2409928) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Preston BT, Stevenson IR, Pemberton JM, Coltman DW, Wilson K. 2003. Overt and covert competition in a promiscuous mammal: the importance of weaponry and testes size to male reproductive success. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 270, 633–640 10.1098/rspb.2002.2268 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2268) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Clutton-Brock T. 1989. Mammalian mating systems. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 236, 339–372 10.1098/rspb.1989.0027 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1989.0027) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Pomfret JC, Knell RJ. 2008. Crowding, sex ratio and horn evolution in a South African beetle community. Proc. R. Soc. B 275, 315–321 10.1098/rspb.2007.1498 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.1498) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Deinert EL. 2003. Mate location and competition for mates in a pupal mating butterfly. In Butterflies: ecology and evolution taking flight (eds Boggs CL, Watt WB, Ehrlich PR.), pp. 91–108 Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press [Google Scholar]
- 93.Paxton RJ. 2005. Male mating behaviour and mating systems of bees: an overview. Apidologie 36, 145–156 10.1051/apido:2005007 (doi:10.1051/apido:2005007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Wiklund C, Forsberg J. 1991. Sexual size dimorphism in relation to female polygamy and protandry in butterflies: a comparative study of Swedish Pieridae and Satyridae. Oikos 60, 373–381 10.2307/3545080 (doi:10.2307/3545080) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Simmons LW, Llorens T, Schinzig M, Hosken D, Craig M. 1994. Sperm competition selects for male mate choice and protandry in the bushcricket, Requena verticalis (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). Anim. Behav. 47, 117–122 10.1006/anbe.1994.1013 (doi:10.1006/anbe.1994.1013) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Parker GA. 1970. Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in the insects. Biol. Rev. 45, 525–567 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1970.tb01176.x (doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.1970.tb01176.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Simmons LW. 2001. Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in the insects. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press [Google Scholar]
- 98.Eberhard WG. 1996. Female control: sexual selection by cryptic female choice. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press [Google Scholar]
- 99.Partridge C, Ahnesjö I, Kvarnemo C, Mobley KB, Berglund A, Jones AG. 2009. The effect of perceived female parasite load on post-copulatory male choice in a sex-role-reversed pipefish. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 63, 345–354 10.1007/s00265-008-0668-3 (doi:10.1007/s00265-008-0668-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Birkhead TR, Møller AP. 1998. Sperm competition and sexual selection. New York, NY: Academic Press [Google Scholar]
- 101.Birkhead TR, Møller AP. 1992. Sperm competition in birds: evolutionary causes and consequences. New York, NY: Academic Press [Google Scholar]
- 102.Waage JK. 1979. Dual function of the damselfly penis: sperm removal and transfer. Science 203, 916–918 10.1126/science.203.4383.916 (doi:10.1126/science.203.4383.916) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Córdoba-Aguilar A. 1999. Male copulatory sensory stimulation induces female ejection of rival sperm in a damselfly. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 266, 779–784 10.1098/rspb.1999.0705 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0705) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Córdoba-Aguilar A. 2002. Sensory trap as the mechanism of sexual selection in a damselfly genitalic trait (Insecta: Calopterygidae). Am. Nat. 160, 594–601 10.1086/342819 (doi:10.1086/342819) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Arnqvist G. 1998. Comparative evidence for the evolution of genitalia by sexual selection. Nature 393, 784–786 10.1038/31689 (doi:10.1038/31689) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Simmons LW, Garcia-Gonzalez F. 2011. Experimental coevolution of male and female genital morphology. Nat. Commun. 2, 374 10.1038/ncomms1379 (doi:10.1038/ncomms1379) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Perry JC, Rowe L. 2012. Sexual conflict and antagonistic coevolution across water strider populations. Evolution 66, 544–557 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2011.01464.x (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2011.01464.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Evans JP, Gasparini C, Holwell GI, Ramnarine IW, Pitcher TE, Pilastro A. 2011. Intraspecific evidence from guppies for correlated patterns of male and female genital trait diversification. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 2611–2620 10.1098/rspb.2010.2453 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.2453) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Siva-Jothy MT. 1987. Variation in copulation duration and the resultant degree of sperm removal in Orthetrum cancellatum (L.) (Libellulidae: Odonata). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 20, 147–151 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2012.01741.x (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2012.01741.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Parker GA. 1970. Sperm competition and its evolutionary effect on copula duration in the fly Scatophaga stercoraria. J. Insect Phys. 16, 1301–1328 10.1016/0022-1910(70)90131-9 (doi:10.1016/0022-1910(70)90131-9) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Simmons LW, Parker GA, Stockley P. 1999. Sperm displacement in the yellow dung fly, Scatophaga stercoraria: an investigation of male and female processes. Am. Nat. 153, 302–314 10.1086/303171 (doi:10.1086/303171) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Møller AP, Birkhead TR. 1989. Copulation behaviour in mammals: evidence that sperm competition is widespread. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 38, 119–131 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1989.tb01569.x (doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1989.tb01569.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Devine MC. 1977. Copulatory plugs, restricted mating opportunities and reproductive competition among male garter snakes. Nature 267, 345–346 10.1038/267345a0 (doi:10.1038/267345a0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Shine R, Olsson MM, Mason RT. 2000. Chastity belts in gartersnakes: the functional significance of mating plugs. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 70, 377–390 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2000.tb01229.x (doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.2000.tb01229.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Ramm SA, Parker GA, Stockley P. 2005. Sperm competition and the evolution of male reproductive anatomy in rodents. Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 949–955 10.1098/rspb.2004.3048 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.3048) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Ramm SA, Oliver PL, Ponting CP, Stockley P, Emes RD. 2008. Sexual selection and the adaptive evolution of mammalian ejaculate proteins. Mol. Biol. Evol. 25, 207–219 10.1093/molbev/msm242 (doi:10.1093/molbev/msm242) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Ramm SA, McDonald L, Hurst JL, Beynon RJ, Stockley P. 2009. Comparative proteomics reveals evidence for evolutionary diversification of rodent seminal fluid and its functional significance in sperm competition. Mol. Biol. Evol. 26, 189–198 10.1093/molbev/msn237 (doi:10.1093/molbev/msn237) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Dean MD, Findlay GD, Hoopman MR, Wu CC, MacCoss MJ, Swanson WJ, Nachman MW. 2011. Identification of ejaculated proteins in the house mouse (Mus domesticus) via isotopic labeling. BMC Genomics 12, 306 10.1186/1471-2164-12-306 (doi:10.1186/1471-2164-12-306) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Dorus S, Evans PD, Wyckoff GJ, Choi SS, Lahn BT. 2004. Rate of molecular evolution of the seminal protein gene SEMG2 correlates with levels of female promiscuity. Nat. Genet. 36, 1326–1329 10.1038/ng1471 (doi:10.1038/ng1471) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Avila FW, Sirot LK, LaFlamme BA, Rubinstein CD, Wolfner MF. 2011. Insect seminal fluid proteins: identification and function. Ann. Rev. Entomol. 56, 21–40 10.1146/annurev-ento-120709-144823 (doi:10.1146/annurev-ento-120709-144823) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Haerty W, et al. 2007. Evolution in the fast lane: rapidly evolving sex-related genes in Drosophila. Genetics 177, 1321–1335 10.1534/genetics.107.078865 (doi:10.1534/genetics.107.078865) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Wagstaff BJ, Begun DJ. 2007. Adaptive evolution of recently duplicated accessory gland protein genes in desert drosophila. Genetics 177, 1023–1030 10.1534/genetics.107.077503 (doi:10.1534/genetics.107.077503) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Vahed K, Gilbert FS. 1996. Differences across taxa in nuptial gift size correlate with differences in sperm number and ejaculate volume in bushcrickets (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 263, 1257–1265 10.1098/rspb.1996.0185 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1996.0185) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Estrada C, Schulz S, Yildizhan S, Gilbert LE. 2011. Sexual selection drives the evolution of antiaphrodisiac pheromones in butterflies. Evolution 65, 2843–2854 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2011.01352.x (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2011.01352.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Dickinson JL. 1995. Trade-offs between post-copulatory riding and mate location in the blue milkweed beetle. Behav. Ecol. 6, 280–286 10.1093/beheco/6.3.280 (doi:10.1093/beheco/6.3.280) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Vahed K. 2007. Comparative evidence for a cost to males of manipulating females in bushcrickets. Behav. Ecol. 18, 499–506 10.1093/beheco/arm021 (doi:10.1093/beheco/arm021) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Schubert M, Schradin C, Rödel HG, Pillay N, Ribble DO. 2009. Male mate guarding in a socially monogamous mammal, the round-eared sengi: on costs and trade-offs. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 64, 257–264 10.1007/s00265-009-0842-2 (doi:10.1007/s00265-009-0842-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Komdeur J. 2001. Mate guarding in the Seychelles warbler is energetically costly and adjusted to paternity risk. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 268, 2103–2111 10.1098/rspb.2001.1750 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2001.1750) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Cuadrado M. 2000. Influence of female's sexual stage and number of available males on the intensity of guarding behavior by male common chameleons: a test of different predictions. Herpetologica 56, 387–393 [Google Scholar]
- 130.McMillan VE. 1991. Variable mate guarding behavior in the dragonfly Plathemis lydia (Odonata: Libellulidae). Anim. Behav. 41, 979–988 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80636-0 (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80636-0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Vepsäläinen K, Savolainen R. 1995. Operational sex ratios and mating conflict between the sexes in the water strider Gerris lacustris. Am. Nat. 146, 869–880 10.1086/285829 (doi:10.1086/285829) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 132.McLain DK. 1980. Female choice and the adaptive significance of prolonged copulation in Nezara viridula (Hemiptera: Pentatomaidae). Psyche 87, 325–336 10.1155/1980/70498 (doi:10.1155/1980/70498) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Hunt J, Simmons LW. 2002. Confidence of paternity and paternal care: covariation revealed through the experimental manipulation of the mating system in the beetle Onthophagus taurus. J. Evol. Biol. 15, 784–795 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2002.00442.x (doi:10.1046/j.1420-9101.2002.00442.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Bretman A, Fricke C, Chapman T. 2009. Plastic responses of male Drosophila melanogaster to the level of sperm competition increase male reproductive fitness. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 1705–1711 10.1098/rspb.2008.1878 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1878) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Sirot LK, Wolfner MF, Wigby S. 2011. Protein-specific manipulation of ejaculate composition in response to female mating status in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 9922–9926 10.1073/pnas.1100905108 (doi:10.1073/pnas.1100905108) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Wigby S, Sirot LK, Linklater JR, Buehner N, Calboli FCF, Bretman A, Wolfner MF, Chapman T. 2009. Seminal fluid protein allocation and male reproductive success. Curr. Biol. 19, 751–757 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.036 (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.036) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Dewsbury DA. 1982. Ejaculate cost and male choice. Am. Nat. 119, 601–610 10.1086/283938 (doi:10.1086/283938) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Hayward A, Gillooly JF. 2011. The cost of sex: quantifying energetic investment in gamete production by males and females. PLoS ONE 6, e16557 10.1371/journal.pone.0016557 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0016557) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Sirot LK, Buehner N, Fiumera AC, Wolfner MF. 2009. Seminal fluid protein depletion and replenishment in the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster: an ELISA-based method for tracking individual ejaculates. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 63, 1505–1513 10.1007/s00265-009-0806-6 (doi:10.1007/s00265-009-0806-6) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Doyle JM. 2011. Sperm depletion and a test of the phenotype-linked fertility hypothesis in gray treefrogs (Hyla versicolor). Can. J. Zool. 89, 853–858 10.1139/z11-060 (doi:10.1139/z11-060) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Olsson M, Madsen T, Shine R. 1997. Is sperm really so cheap? Costs of reproduction in male adders, Vipera berus. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 264, 455–459 10.1098/rspb.1997.0065 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1997.0065) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Birkhead TR, Fletcher F. 1995. Depletion determines sperm numbers in male zebra finches. Anim. Behav. 49, 451–456 10.1006/anbe.1995.0058 (doi:10.1006/anbe.1995.0058) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Poulin R, Morand S. 2000. Testes size, body size and male-male competition in acanthocephalan parasites. J. Zool. 250, 551–558 [Google Scholar]
- 144.Fry CL. 2006. Juvenile hormone mediates a trade-off between primary and secondary sexual traits in stalk-eyed flies. Evol. Dev. 8, 191–201 10.1111/j.1525-142X.2006.00089.x (doi:10.1111/j.1525-142X.2006.00089.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Simmons LW, Emlen DJ. 2006. Evolutionary trade-off between weapons and testes. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103, 16 349–16 351 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Yamane T, Okada K, Nakayama S, Miyatake T. 2010. Dispersal and ejaculatory strategies associated with exaggeration of weapon in an armed beetle. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 1705–1710 10.1098/rspb.2009.2017 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.2017) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Simmons LW, Tinghitella RM, Zuk M. 2010. Quantitative genetic variation in courtship song and its covariation with immune function and sperm quality in the field cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus. Behav. Ecol. 21, 1330–1336 10.1093/beheco/arq154 (doi:10.1093/beheco/arq154) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Montrose VT, Harris WE, Moore AJ, Moore PJ. 2008. Sperm competition within a dominance hierarchy: investment in social status versus investment in ejaculates. J. Evol. Biol. 21, 1290–1296 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01570.x (doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01570.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Engqvist L. 2011. Male attractiveness is negatively genetically associated with investment in copulations. Behav. Ecol. 22, 345–349 10.1093/beheco/arq211 (doi:10.1093/beheco/arq211) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Demary KC, Lewis SM. 2007. Male courtship attractiveness and paternity success in Photinus greeni fireflies. Evolution 61, 431–439 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00033.x (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00033.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Kelly CD. 2008. Sperm investment in relation to weapon size in a male trimorphic insect? Behav. Ecol. 19, 1018–1024 [Google Scholar]
- 152.Parker GA, Lessells CM, Simmons LW. In press Sperm competition games: a general model for pre-copulatory male-male competition. Evolution. 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2012.01741.x (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2012.01741.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Pitcher TE, Doucet SM, Beausoleil J-MJ, Hanley D. 2009. Secondary sexual characters and sperm traits in coho salmon Oncorhynchus kisutch. J. Fish Biol. 74, 1450–1461 10.1111/j.1095-8649.2009.02210.x (doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2009.02210.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Liljedal S, Folstad I, Skarstein F. 1999. Secondary sex traits, parasites, immunity and ejaculate quality in the Arctic charr. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 266, 1893–1898 10.1098/rspb.1999.0863 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0863) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Rudolfsen G, Figenschou L, Folstad I, Tveiten H, Figenschou M. 2006. Rapid adjustments of sperm characteristics in relation to social status. Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 325–332 10.1098/rspb.2005.3305 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3305) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Evans JP. 2010. Quantitative genetic evidence that males trade attractiveness for ejaculate quality in guppies. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 3195–3202 10.1098/rspb.2010.0826 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.0826) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Kvarnemo C, Manson W, Svensson O. 2010. Investment in testes, sperm-duct glands and lipid reserves differs between male morphs but not between early and late breeding season in Pomatoschistus minutus. J. Fish Biol. 76, 1609–1625 10.1111/j.1095-8649.2010.02587.x (doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2010.02587.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Preston BT, Jalme MS, Hingrat Y, Lacroix F, Sorci G. 2011. Sexually extravagant males age more rapidly. Ecol. Lett. 14, 1017–1024 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01668.x (doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01668.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Pizzari T, Cornwallis CK, Froman DP. 2007. Social competitiveness associated with rapid fluctuations in sperm quality in male fowl. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 853–860 10.1098/rspb.2006.0080 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.0080) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Rowe M, Swaddle JP, Pruett-Jones S, Webster MS. 2010. Plumage coloration, ejaculate quality and reproductive phenotype in the red-backed fairy-wren. Anim. Behav. 79, 1239–1246 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.02.020 (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.02.020) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Fitzpatrick JL, Almbro M, Gonzalez-Voyer A, Kolm N, Simmons LW. 2012. Male contest competition and the coevolution of weaponry and testes in pinnipeds. Evolution 66, 3595–3604 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2012.01713.x (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2012.01713.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Simmons LW, Peters M, Rhodes G. 2011. Low pitched voices are perceived as masculine and attractive but do they predict semen quality in men? PLoS ONE 6, e29271 10.1371/journal.pone.0029271 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0029271) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.Kokko H, Rankin DJ. 2006. Lonely hearts or sex in the city? Density-dependent effects in mating systems. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 361, 319–334 10.1098/rstb.2005.1784 (doi:10.1098/rstb.2005.1784) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164.Simmons LW, Fitzpatrick JL. 2012. Sperm wars and the evolution of male fertility. Reproduction 144, 519–534 10.1530/REP-12-0285 (doi:10.1530/REP-12-0285) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165.Hosken DJ, Ward PI. 2001. Experimental evidence for testis size evolution via sperm competition. Ecol. Lett. 4, 10–13 10.1046/j.1461-0248.2001.00198.x (doi:10.1046/j.1461-0248.2001.00198.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Simmons LW, García-González F. 2008. Evolutionary reduction in testes size and competitive fertilization success in response to the experimental removal of sexual selection in dung beetles. Evolution 62, 2580–2591 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00479.x (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00479.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Firman RC, Simmons LW. 2010. Experimental evolution of sperm quality via postcopulatory sexual selection in house mice. Evolution 64, 1245–1256 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Firman RC, Simmons LW. 2010. Experimental evolutin of sperm competitiveness in a mammal. BMC Evol. Biol. 11, 19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169.Firman RC, Simmons LW. 2008. The frequency of multiple paternity predicts variation in testes size among island populations of house mice. J. Evol. Biol. 21, 1524–1533 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01612.x (doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01612.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170.Dziminski MA, Roberts JD, Beveridge M, Simmons LW. 2010. Among-population covariation between sperm competition and ejaculate expenditure in frogs. Behav. Ecol. 21, 322–328 10.1093/beheco/arp191 (doi:10.1093/beheco/arp191) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 171.Brown CR, Brown MB. 2003. Testis size increases with colony size in cliff swallows. Behav. Ecol. 14, 569–575 10.1093/beheco/arg030 (doi:10.1093/beheco/arg030) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 172.Ramm SA, Stockley P. 2009. Adaptive plasticity of mammalian sperm production in response to social experience. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 745–751 10.1098/rspb.2008.1296 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1296) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173.delBarco-Trillo J, Ferkin MH. 2004. Male mammals respond to a risk of sperm competition conveyed by odours of conspecific males. Nature 431, 446–449 10.1038/nature02845 (doi:10.1038/nature02845) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 174.Pilastro A, Scaggiante M, Rasotto MB. 2002. Individual adjustement of sperm expenditure accords with sperm competition theory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, 9913–9915 10.1073/pnas.152133499 (doi:10.1073/pnas.152133499) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 175.Smith CC, Ryan MJ. 2011. Tactic-dependent plasticity in ejaculate traits in the swordtail Xiphophorus nigrensis. Biol. Lett. 7, 733–735 10.1098/rsbl.2011.0286 (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2011.0286) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 176.Pizzari T, Cornwallis CK, Løvlie H, Jakobsson S, Birkhead TR. 2003. Sophisticated sperm allocation in male fowl. Nature 426, 70–74 10.1038/nature02004 (doi:10.1038/nature02004) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 177.Gage MJG. 1991. Risk of sperm competition directly affects ejaculate size in the Mediterranean fruit fly. Anim. Behav. 42, 1036–1037 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80162-9 (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80162-9) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 178.Kelly CD, Jennions MD. 2011. Sexual selection and sperm quantity: meta-analyses of strategic ejaculation. Biol. Rev. 86, 863–884 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2011.00175.x (doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2011.00175.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 179.delBarco-Trillo J. 2011. Adjustment of sperm allocation under high risk of sperm competition across taxa: a meta-analysis. J. Evol. Biol. 24, 1706–1714 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02293.x (doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02293.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 180.Engqvist L, Reinhold K. 2005. Pitfalls in experiments testing predictions from sperm competition theory. J. Evol. Biol. 18, 116–123 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2004.00792.x (doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2004.00792.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 181.Thomas ML, Simmons LW. 2008. Male-derived cuticular hydrocarbons signal sperm competition intensity and affect ejaculate expenditure in crickets. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 383–388 10.1098/rspb.2008.1206 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1206) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 182.Gage AR, Barnard CJ. 1996. Male crickets increase sperm number in relation to competition and female size. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 38, 349–353 10.1007/s002650050251 (doi:10.1007/s002650050251) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 183.Cornwallis CK, Birkhead TR. 2007. Experimental evidence that female ornamentation increases the acquisition of sperm and signals fecundity. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 583–590 10.1098/rspb.2006.3757 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.3757) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 184.Simmons LW, Parker GA. 1996. Parental investment and the control of sexual selection: can sperm competition affect the direction of sexual competition? Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 263, 515–519 10.1098/rspb.1996.0078 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1996.0078) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 185.Lorch PD. 2002. Understanding reversals in the relative strength of sexual selection on males and females: a role for sperm competition? Am. Nat. 159, 645–657 10.1086/339992 (doi:10.1086/339992) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 186.Parker GA, Ball MA, Stockley P, Gage MJG. 1996. Sperm competition games: individual assessment of sperm competition intensity by group spawners. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 263, 1291–1297 10.1098/rspb.1996.0189 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1996.0189) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 187.Simmons LW. 1995. Courtship feeding in katydids (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae): investment in offspring and in obtaining fertilizations. Am. Nat. 146, 307–315 10.1086/285801 (doi:10.1086/285801) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 188.Gwynne DT. 1986. Courtship feeding in katydids (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae): investment in offspring or in obtaining fertilizations? Am. Nat. 128, 342–352 10.1086/284566 (doi:10.1086/284566) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 189.Simmons LW. 1992. Quantification of role reversal in relative parental investment in a bushcricket. Nature 358, 61–63 10.1038/358061a0 (doi:10.1038/358061a0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 190.Queller DC. 1997. Why do females care more than males? Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 264, 1555–1557 10.1098/rspb.1997.0216 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1997.0216) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 191.Whittingham LA, Taylor PD, Robertson RJ. 1992. Confidence of paternity and male parental care. Am. Nat. 139, 1115–1125 10.1086/285376 (doi:10.1086/285376) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 192.Xia X. 1992. Uncertainty of paternity can select against paternal care. Am. Nat. 139, 1126–1129 10.1086/285377 (doi:10.1086/285377) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 193.Wright J. 1998. Paternity and paternal care. In Sperm competition and sexual selection (eds Birkhead TR, Møller AP.), pp. 117–145 New York, NY: Academic Press [Google Scholar]
- 194.Alonzo SH. 2009. Social and coevolutionary feedbacks between mating and parental investment. Trends Ecol. Evol. 25, 99–108 10.1016/j.tree.2009.07.012 (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2009.07.012) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 195.Kokko H, Jennions MD. 2008. Parental investment, sexual selection and sex ratios. J. Evol. Biol. 21, 919–948 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01540.x (doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01540.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 196.Møller AP. 2000. Male parental care, female reproductive success and extra-pair paternity. Behav. Ecol. 11, 161–168 10.1093/beheco/11.2.161 (doi:10.1093/beheco/11.2.161) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 197.Griffith SC, Owens IPF, Thuman KA. 2002. Extra pair paternity in birds: a review of interspecific variation and adaptive function. Mol. Ecol. 11, 2195–2212 10.1046/j.1365-294X.2002.01613.x (doi:10.1046/j.1365-294X.2002.01613.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 198.Møller AP. 1988. Female choice selects for male sexual tail ornaments in the monogamous swallow. Nature 332, 640–642 10.1038/332640a0 (doi:10.1038/332640a0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 199.Gibbs HL, Weatherhead PJ, Boag PT, White BN, Tabak LM, Hoysak DJ. 1990. Realized reproductive success of polygynous red-winged blackbirds revealed by DNA markers. Science 250, 1394–1397 10.1126/science.250.4986.1394 (doi:10.1126/science.250.4986.1394) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 200.Sheldon BC, Ellegren H. 1999. Sexual selection resulting from extrapair paternity in collared flycatchers. Anim. Behav. 57, 285–298 10.1006/anbe.1998.0968 (doi:10.1006/anbe.1998.0968) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 201.Møller AP, Birkhead TR. 1994. The evolution of plumage brightness in birds is related to extrapair paternity. Evolution 48, 1089–1100 10.2307/2410369 (doi:10.2307/2410369) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 202.Møller AP. 1998. Sperm competition and sexual selection. In Sperm competition and sexual selection (eds Birkhead TR, Møller AP.), pp. 55–90 New York, NY: Academic Press [Google Scholar]
- 203.Kvarnemo C. 1994. Temperature differentially affects male and female reproductive rates in the sand goby: consequences for operational sex ratio. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 256, 151–156 10.1098/rspb.1994.0063 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1994.0063) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 204.Jones AG, Walker D, Kvarnemo C, Lindström K, Avise JC. 2001. How cuckoldry can decrease the opportunity for sexual selection: data and theory from a genetic parentage analysis of the sand goby, Pomatoschistus minutus. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 9151–9156 10.1073/pnas.171310198 (doi:10.1073/pnas.171310198) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 205.Hunt J, Breuker CJ, Sadowski JA, Moore AJ. 2009. Male–male competition, female mate choice and their interaction: determining total sexual selection. J. Evol. Biol. 22, 13–26 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01633.x (doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01633.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 206.Parker GA. 2006. Sexual conflict over mating and fertilization: an overview. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 361, 235–259 10.1098/rstb.2005.1785 (doi:10.1098/rstb.2005.1785) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 207.Forsgren E. 1997. Female sand gobies prefer good fathers over dominant males. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 264, 1283–1286 10.1098/rspb.1997.0177 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1997.0177) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 208.Pemberton JM, Coltman DW, Smith JA, Pilkington JG. 1999. Molecular analysis of a promiscuous, fluctuating mating system. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 68, 289–301 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1999.tb01170.x (doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1999.tb01170.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 209.Danielsson I. 2000. Antagonistic pre- and post-copulatory sexual selection on male body size in a water strider (Gerris lacustris). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 268, 77–81 10.1098/rspb.2000.1332 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1332) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 210.Endler JA. 1988. Sexual selection and predation risk in guppies. Nature 332, 593–594 10.1038/332593b0 (doi:10.1038/332593b0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 211.Houde AE. 1997. Sex, color and mate choice in guppies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press [Google Scholar]
- 212.Pilastro A, Simonata M, Bisazza A, Evans JP. 2004. Cryptic female preference for colorful males in guppies. Evolution 58, 665–669 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 213.Evans JP, Zane L, Francescato S, Pilastro A. 2003. Directional postcopulatory sexual selection revealed by artificial insemination. Nature 421, 360–363 10.1038/nature01367 (doi:10.1038/nature01367) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 214.Roff DA. 2002. Life history evolution. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates [Google Scholar]
- 215.van Noordwijk AJ, de Jong G. 1986. Acquisition and allocation of resources: their influence on variation in life history tactics. Am. Nat. 128, 137–142 10.1086/284547 (doi:10.1086/284547) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 216.Reznick D, Nunney L, Tessier A. 2000. Big houses, big cars, superfleas and the costs of reproduction. Trends Ecol. Evol. 15, 421–425 10.1016/S0169-5347(00)01941-8 (doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(00)01941-8) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 217.Malo AF, Roldan ERS, Garde J, Soler AJ, Gomendio M. 2005. Antlers honestly advertise sperm production and quality. Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 149–157 10.1098/rspb.2004.2933 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2933) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 218.Locatello L, Rasotto MB, Evans JP, Pilastro A. 2006. Colourful male guppies produce faster and more viable sperm. J. Evol. Biol. 19, 1595–1602 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01117.x (doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01117.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 219.Rowe L, Houle D. 1996. The lek paradox and the capture of genetic variance by condition dependent traits. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 263, 1415–1421 10.1098/rspb.1996.0207 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1996.0207) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 220.Wilkinson GS, Taper M. 1999. Evolution of genetic variation for condition-dependent traits in stalk-eyed flies. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 266, 1685–1690 10.1098/rspb.1999.0832 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0832) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 221.Simmons LW, Kotiaho JS. 2002. Evolution of ejaculates: patterns of phenotypic and genotypic variation and condition dependence in sperm competition traits. Evolution 56, 1622–1631 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 222.Kotiaho JS, Simmons LW, Tomkins JL. 2001. Towards a resolution of the lek paradox. Nature 410, 684–686 10.1038/35070557 (doi:10.1038/35070557) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 223.Simmons LW, Kotiaho JS. 2007. Quantitative genetic correlation between trait and preference supports a sexually selected sperm process. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 16 604–16 608 10.1073/pnas.0704871104 (doi:10.1073/pnas.0704871104) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 224.García-González F, Simmons LW. 2007. Shorter sperm confer higher competitive fertilization success. Evolution 61, 816–824 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00084.x (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00084.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 225.García-González F, Simmons LW. 2011. Good genes and sexual selection in dung beetles (Onthophagus taurus): genetic variance in egg-to-adult and adult viability. PLoS ONE 6, e16233 10.1371/journal.pone.0016233 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0016233) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 226.Simmons LW, Holley R. 2011. Offspring viability benefits but no apparent costs of mating with high quality males. Biol. Lett. 7, 419–421 10.1098/rsbl.2010.0976 (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2010.0976) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 227.Pischedda A, Rice WR. 2012. Partitioning sexual selection into its mating success and fertilization success components. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 2049–2053 10.1073/pnas.1110841109 (doi:10.1073/pnas.1110841109) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 228.Birkhead TR, Atkin L, Møller AP. 1987. Copulation behaviour of birds. Behaviour 101, 101–138 10.1163/156853987X00396 (doi:10.1163/156853987X00396) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 229.Alatalo RV, Burke T, Dann J, Hanotte O, Höglund J, Lundberg A, Moss R, Rintamäki PT. 1996. Paternity, copulation disturbance and female choice in lekking black grouse. Anim. Behav. 52, 861–873 10.1006/anbe.1996.0234 (doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0234) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 230.Kokko H, Sutherland WJ, Lindström J, Reynolds JD, Mackenzie A. 1998. Individual mating success, lek stability, and the neglected limitations of statistical power. Anim. Behav. 56, 755–762 10.1006/anbe.1998.0815 (doi:10.1006/anbe.1998.0815) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 231.Hingrat Y, Saint Jalme M, Chalah T, Orhant N, Lacroix F. 2008. Environmental and social constraints on breeding sites selection. Does the exploded-lek and hotspot model apply to the Houbara bustard Chlamydotis undulata undulata? J. Avian Biol. 39, 393–404 10.1111/j.0908-8857.2008.03994.x (doi:10.1111/j.0908-8857.2008.03994.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 232.Lesobre L, Lacroix F, Le Nuz E, Hingrat Y, Chalah T, Jaime MS. 2010. Absence of male reproductive skew, along with high frequency of polyandry and conspecific brood parasitism in the lekking Houbara bustard Chlamydotis undulata undulata. J. Avian Biol. 41, 117–127 10.1111/j.1600-048X.2009.04794.x (doi:10.1111/j.1600-048X.2009.04794.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 233.Lanctot RB, Scribner KT, Kempenaers B, Weatherhead PJ. 1997. Lekking without a paradox in the buff-breasted sandpiper. Am. Nat. 149, 1051–1070 10.1086/286038 (doi:10.1086/286038) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 234.Lank DB, Smith CM, Hanotte O, Ohtonen A, Bailey S, Burke T. 2002. High frequency of polyandry in a lek mating system. Behav. Ecol. 13, 209–215 10.1093/beheco/13.2.209 (doi:10.1093/beheco/13.2.209) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 235.Collet J, Richardson DS, Worley K, Pizzari T. 2012. Sexual selection and the differential effect of polyandry. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 8641–8645 10.1073/pnas.1200219109 (doi:10.1073/pnas.1200219109) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 236.Peters A, Denk AG, Delhey K, Kempenaers B. 2004. Carotenoid-based bill colour as an indicator of immunocompetence and sperm performance in male mallards. J. Evol. Biol. 17, 1111–1120 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2004.00743.x (doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2004.00743.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 237.Helfenstein F, Losdat S, Møller AP, Blout JD, Richner H. 2010. Sperm of colourful males are better protected against oxidative stress. Ecol. Lett. 13, 213–222 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2009.01419.x (doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2009.01419.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 238.Pike TW, Blount JD, Lindström J, Metcalf NB. 2010. Dietary carotenoid availability, sexual signalling and functional fertility in sticklebacks. Biol. Lett. 6, 191–193 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0815 (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2009.0815) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]