Abstract
Background
Pain drawings are a diagnostic adjunct to history taking, clinical examinations, and biomedical tests in evaluating pain. We hypothesized that somatoform-functional pain, is mirrored in distinctive graphic patterns of pain drawings. Our aim was to identify the most sensitive and specific graphic criteria as a tool to help identifying somatoform-functional pain.
Methods
We compared 62 patients with somatoform-functional pain with a control group of 49 patients with somatic-nociceptive pain type. All patients were asked to mark their pain on a pre-printed body diagram. An investigator, blinded with regard to the patients’ diagnoses, analyzed the drawings according to a set of numeric or binary criteria.
Results
We identified 13 drawing criteria pointing with significance to a somatoform-functional pain disorder (all p-values ≤ 0.001). The most specific and most sensitive criteria combination for detecting somatoform-functional pain included the total number of marks, the length of the longest mark, and the presence of symmetric patterns. The area under the ROC-curve was 96.3% for this criteria combination.
Conclusion
Pain drawings are an easy-to-administer supplementary technique which helps to identify somatoform-functional pain in comparison to somatic-nociceptive pain.
Keywords: Chronic pain; Functional pain syndromes; Pain drawing, Somatoform pain disorder
Background
Pain drawings (PDs) are widely used to record subjective pain symptoms. In addition to good history taking, physical examinations, and results of biomedical tests, they can support to differentiate several types of local pain syndromes, such as chronic low back pain [1,2], chronic shoulder pain [3], neurogenic pain [4], and headaches [5].
Instead of focusing on any particular anatomical site, we concentrated on a particular pain type: We examined patients whose complaints could not be explained by either a peripheral structural or a neuropathic lesion. In psychiatric terms, the latter complaints are traditionally referred to as ‘somatoform pain’, whereas somatic medicine prefers the term ‘functional pain’ (e.g. functional pain syndromes). In the following, we will use the term somatoform-functional pain to describe this entire group of pain disorders.
Our aim was to identify those particular graphic criteria which specifically help to differentiate between somatoform-functional pain and somatic-nociceptive pain. More specifically, we wanted to find out which combination of features in a drawing has the best predictive value (sensitivity and specificity) to identify somatoform-functional pain. Based on the arguments of Margolis et al., we strictly applied quantitative methods of picture analysis and avoided any qualitative or experience-based interpretations of pictures or signs [6].
Methods
Setting, design, and patients
We compared two groups of inpatients at a tertiary university hospital: one with somatoform-functional pain, the other with unequivocal somatic-nociceptive pain. We selected all patients solely according to these two pain types, but other basic characteristics were also noted.
Patients with somatoform-functional pain were recruited from the medical psychosomatic department. The defining eligibility criterion was a pain without an explanatory morphologic correlate. Such a correlate was excluded by standard clinical, serological and radiological methods.
Patients with somatic-nociceptive pain were recruited from the orthopedic department. Only patients with a clear peripheral correlate, verified by standard clinical and technical diagnostic methods (i.e. X-ray, MRI or CT), were included from this department. Patients with pain of neuropathic origin (e.g. diabetic neuropathy, spinal canal stenosis, discopathia), cancer-related pain and those with inflammatory pain (e.g., rheumatoid arthritis) were not included in this pilot study.
In both departments the treating specialists made diagnoses according to the usual Western standards and diagnostic classification systems (ICD-10, DSM-IV).
To assess depressive mood we used the depression subscale of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS-D) [7,8]. To limit potential confounding of somatic-nociceptive pain by overt psychological factors, we excluded patients from the orthopedic department who endorsed a comorbid psychiatric disorder.
The patients were individually instructed by the investigator to mark all painful body areas with a red pencil on a letter-sized body diagram. The used body diagram shows four views of the whole body: frontal view, rear view, right and left lateral views (size: 105 mm from neck to sole). Additionally, there are enlarged views of the head, the neck, and the distal extremities. Patients were not given any instructions on how to apply the pencil (e.g. hatched or solidly filled areas), nor were they advised about the use of particular signs (e.g. circles, crosses, or arrows).
The study protocol has been approved by the local ethical committee and all patients provided written informed consent.
Definition of pain drawing criteria
The evaluation of all PDs was carried out by only one investigator. He was blinded with regard to the pain diagnosis and the department’s origin of the patients. The inventory of the drawings is based on their graphic features only. We strictly renounced any interpretive criteria (e.g. “not corresponding to anatomic structures”). The criteria included objective aspects only, e.g. the form (lines, hatches, circles, and rectangles) and the orientation (e.g. horizontal lines, symmetrical distribution). We took the position with regard to the template border (i.e. lines following the contour of the body scheme, or marks exceeding the border of the body scheme) into account. In addition, we considered quantitative criteria (i.e. number of marked quadrants, number of pain regions, and number of different marks) as well as the size of the marks (i.e. longest mark). Overall, our evaluation included a comprehensive panel of 24 graphic criteria (cf. below).
Data analysis
We compared the pain groups in terms of health characteristics and pain drawing marks. In the latter comparison we distinguished between frequent marks, occurring in at least 25% of the patients in one of the pain groups, and rare marks. The frequency of category-type statements was expressed in percentages, whereas the mean ± standard deviation (SD) was used for continuous variables. P-values for differences of PDs in the two pain groups were computed by Mann–Whitney U test and Pearson chi-square test or Fisher’s exact test where appropriate. Significance level was set as 0.001 with Bonferroni corrections for 24 comparisons. Furthermore, two multivariate logistic regression analyses were performed for frequent marks with respect to control variables: One basic model (analysis 1) adjusted only for age and gender, a second (analysis 2) additionally for other characteristic group differences. Principally, all covariates were tested which might co-influence the drawings of the two different pain groups (e.g. age, gender, pain duration, pain severity, as well as mood). Ninety-five % confidence intervals were calculated for all odds ratios.
Finally, in order to identify the combination of criteria with the highest selectivity (i.e. sensitivity and specificity), we used receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves: In a first step, we identified the criterion with the highest selectivity out of all significant frequent criteria. In a second step, we identified those criteria which added the most to the selectivity of the first criterion. To achieve this, the first criterion was separately modeled (i.e. two variables per model) with each of the other frequent criteria by binary logistic regression. Pairs of criteria which both contributed to the model were combined with the natural logarithm (ln) of the equation of the model. For instance, if the odds ratio was 2 for criterion A and 3 for criterion B, the score would be ln2 times criterion A plus ln3 times criterion B. This method allows to quantify the additive predictive value of multiple criteria. The most sensitive and specific combination was again analyzed by ROC-curve analysis. The two best matching criteria of this second combination were afterwards modeled once more by binary logistic regression, this time with a third criterion, in order to finally identify the most appropriate triple-combination.
Results
Description of the patients
The flowchart (Figure 1) describes the selection of the 62 (56%) patients with somatoform-functional pain and the 49 (44%) patients with somatic-nociceptive pain. Of the orthopedic group, 6 patients were excluded because of neuropathic pain syndrome and 29 because of psychiatric comorbidity.
Among the patients from the medical-psychosomatic department, chronic functional somatic syndromes included fibromyalgia (n = 14), chronic tension headache (n = 4), chronic temporomandibular pain (n = 1), atypical facial pain (n = 1), chronic back pain (n = 16), functional abdominal pain or chronic pelvic pain (n = 8). Other patients suffered from inexplicable pain of the trunk (n = 8), pain-related functional hemi-syndromes (n = 6), inexplicable chronic postsurgical pain (n = 1), chronic shoulder/arm pain (6), and chronic cervico/thoracic pain (n = 11).
Among the patients from the orthopedic department, acute traumatic pain included traumas of the upper extremities or shoulders (n = 11), bone fractures or joint lesions of the lower extremities (n = 15), thoracic, vertebral or pelvic traumas (n = 7). Degenerative osteoarthritic pain included hip arthritis (n = 9), knee arthritis (n = 9), degenerative shoulder pain (n = 5), and degenerative low back pain (n = 7).
Sixteen patients with somatoform-functional and 13 patients with somatic-nociceptive pain had more than one diagnosis for their pain type.
Further patient characteristics are shown in Table 1 together with p-values for group differences.
Table 1.
Variable | Somatoform-functional pain (n = 62) | Somatic-nociceptive pain (n = 49) | P-value |
---|---|---|---|
Age (years) |
47±12 (20–75) |
55±17 (16–84) |
0.005 |
Female sex (%) |
62.9 |
34.7 |
0.003 |
Pain duration (months) |
99±115 (3–451) |
17±24 (0.1-100) |
<0.001 |
Pain level (NRS) |
6.4±2.2 (1–10) |
3.5±1.4 (1–7) |
<0.001 |
Depressive disorder (%) |
87.1 |
0 |
<0.001 |
Anxiety disorder (%)1) |
35.5 |
0 |
<0.001 |
Opioids (%) (includes Tramadol) |
30.2 |
6.1 |
<0.001 |
NSAIDs (%) (includes Paracetamol and Metamizol) |
50.0 |
93.3 |
0.001 |
Antidepressants (%) |
85.5 |
0 |
<0.001 |
Antiepileptics (%) | 24.2 | 0 | <0.001 |
1) includes PTSD.
NSAIDs = non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs; NRS = numerical rating scale.
Data are given as means ± SD with range in parentheses or percentage values. Analyses used Mann–Whitney U test and Pearson chi-square test or Fisher’s exact test (where appropriate).
Description of pain drawings
Thirteen of the 24 analyzed graphic criteria displayed differences of the p-values ≤ 0.001. Eleven of these criteria were frequent (= occurred >25% in one of the two pain groups), cf. Table 2.
Table 2.
Frequently1)occuring drawings marks | |||
---|---|---|---|
Pain drawing criteria | Somatoform-functional pain (n = 62) | Somatic-nociceptive pain (n = 49) | P-value |
1. Number of marked pain regions2) |
9.7±7.4 (1–33) |
1.6±1.0 (1–6) |
<0.001 |
2. Total number of marks |
13.5±11.8 (1–73) |
2.1±1.4 (1–8) |
<0.001 |
3. Number of different types of marks |
2.6±1.3 (1–6) |
1.3±0.6 (1–3) |
<0.001 |
4. Number of affected quadrants |
3.0±1.1 (1–4) |
1.6±0.9 (1–4) |
<0.001 |
5. Number of symmetric marks |
6.9±7.5 (0–30) |
0.4±0.9 (0–4) |
<0.001 |
6. Number of “over the border” marks |
1.4±2.5 (0–13) |
0.4±0.9 (0–5) |
0.010 |
7. Length of the longest mark, in mm |
37.2±26.0 (2–105) |
14.7±9.2 (3–40) |
<0.001 |
8. Symmetric patterns yes, % |
75.8 |
16.3 |
<0.001 |
9. Neck involved yes, % |
74.2 |
4.1 |
<0.001 |
10. Circle mark yes, % |
16.1 |
26.5 |
0.179 |
11. Point mark yes, % |
37.1 |
4.1 |
<0.001 |
12. Long lines yes3), % |
62.9 |
6.1 |
<0.001 |
13. Strict horizontal mark yes, % |
25.8 |
2.0 |
<0.001 |
14. Hatching mark yes, % |
38.7 |
14.3 |
0.004 |
Rarely1)occurring drawing mark | |||
15. Half-side pattern yes, % |
9.7 |
0 |
0.033 |
16. Sternocleidomastoid involved yes, % |
21.0 |
2.0 |
0.003 |
17. Periorbital pain yes, % |
11.3 |
0 |
0.017 |
18. X marks yes, % |
8.1 |
22.4 |
0.054 |
19. Potato mark yes, % |
24.2 |
18.4 |
0.459 |
20. Right angle yes, % |
14.5 |
0 |
0.004 |
21. Contour pain yes, % |
19.4 |
0 |
0.001 |
22. Towing bar yes, % |
8.1 |
0 |
0.065 |
23. Radiation hand yes, % |
19.4 |
0 |
0.001 |
24. Ear crest yes,% | 11.3 | 0 | 0.017 |
1) Marks referred to as “frequently occurring“ appeared in ≥ 25% of cases in one of the two groups. Marks referred to as “rarely occurring“ appeared in less than 25% of cases in both groups.
2) If the same pain region was repeatedly marked in the different views of the body diagram, it was counted once only under this item.
3) A mark was considered to be “long” if it exceeded the length of a forearm (≥ 18 mm) in the template.
In the first multivariate analysis, Analysis 1 adjusted for age and gender, 7 of the frequent criteria had an OR > 3 (range 3.72–53.95). In the second multivariate analysis, Analysis 2 adjusted for age, gender, pain duration, pain severity and mood, 10 criteria had an OR > 3 (range 3.49–140.4), cf. Table 3.
Table 3.
Pain drawing criteria |
Analysis 1 |
Analysis 2 |
||
---|---|---|---|---|
Adjusted for age and gender |
Adjusted for age, gender, pain duration, pain severity and mood |
|||
OR | 95% CI | OR | 95% CI | |
1. Number of marked pain regions |
2.39 |
1.59-3.60 |
4.64 |
1.64-13.1 |
2. Total number of marks |
1.84 |
1.37-2.48 |
4.42 |
1.61-12.13 |
3. Number of different types of marks |
3.93 |
2.08-7.44 |
3.65 |
1.27-10.54 |
4. Number of affected quadrants |
3.74 |
2.22-6.27 |
3.97 |
1.62-9.72 |
5. Number of symmetric marks |
2.66 |
1.61-4.40 |
3.49 |
1.48-8.26 |
6. Number of “over the border” marks |
1.50 |
1.03-2.18 |
1.72 |
0.95-3.10 |
7. Length of the longest mark, mm |
1.57 |
1.25-1.98 |
1.52 |
1.07-2.16 |
8. Symmetric patterns yes, % |
19.6 |
6.46-59.38 |
21.94 |
3.59-134.11 |
9. Neck involved yes, % |
53.95 |
11.29-257.78 |
140.4 |
7.80-2527.29 |
10. Circle mark yes, % |
0.56 |
0.21-1.55 |
0.31 |
0.05-1.77 |
11. Point mark yes, % |
12.32 |
2.55-59.53 |
9.46 |
0.76-118.02 |
12. Long lines yes, % |
22.99 |
6.12-86.39 |
16.94 |
2.22-129.38 |
13. Strict horizontal lines yes, % |
0.074 |
0.01-0.62 |
0.08 |
0.00-1.71 |
14. Hatching mark yes, % | 3.72 | 1.33-10.42 | 8.66 | 1.56-48.14 |
Explanations for models adjusted for age and gender:
For categorical variables (yes, no) odds ratios indicate the following: if e.g. criterion no.11 (point mark) was present, the odds of a patient suffering from somatoform pain was 12-fold higher than suffering from somatic pain.
For continuous variables odds ratios indicate the following: e.g. for one additional mark (e.g. criterion no. 2) the odds of the patient suffering from somatoform pain as opposed to somatic pain was increased by factor 1.84.
Criterion no.7 (longest mark) was measured in 5 mm steps: For every increase of the length of the longest mark by 5 mm, the odds of the patient suffering from somatoform as opposed to somatic pain was increased by factor 1.57.
Bold figures: pain drawing criteria with OR > 3.
Figure 2 gives a graphic index of the investigated PD marks. A selection of representative examples of somatic-nociceptive and somatoform-functional PDs is illustrated with Figures 3 and 4.
Most sensitive and specific combination
The most equilibrated criteria combination to differentiate between the two types of pain included the total number of marks, the length of the longest mark, and presence versus absence of symmetric patterns (statistic calculations are shown in Tables 4 and 5). This means that PDs with a greater number of marks, typically with symmetric patterns and long marks, are most likely of somatoform-functional origin. The area under the ROC-curve for this triple criteria combination was 96.3% (Figure 5).
Table 4.
Pain drawing criteria | OR/unit (95% CI) | Log likelihood | p-value |
---|---|---|---|
Total number of marks |
1.7 (1.3, 2.3) |
- 25 |
< 0.001 |
Length of the longest mark |
1.1 (1.0, 1.1) |
|
0.029 |
Symmetric patterns (yes/no) | 6.5 (1.6, 26.9) | 0.010 |
This tabulated group lists the three best pain drawing criteria (two continuous plus one binary) with the highest power to distinguish somatoform-functional from somatic-nociceptive pain, the criteria are included in a model as separate variables.
Table 5.
Formula to compute the score | Mean ± SD (Range) | OR/SD (95% CI) | Log likelihood | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
55 points multiplied with the total number of marks plus |
748 ± 700 (81 4397) |
1114 (62, 20’132) |
- 25 |
- 0.001 |
32.2 points multiplied with the length of the longest mark plus |
|
|
|
|
187 points if symmetric patterns are present |
This table shows the properties of the best pain drawing criteria after combining them to one single continuous variable. The combination to one variable is necessary to obtain one single receiver operating characteristic curve including the diagnostic properties of all three criteria.
Furthermore, the effective predicting score value of an individual drawing could be computed by using the following score formula: 55 × the total number of marks, plus 32.5 × for each five-mm of the longest mark, plus an additional amount of 187 if symmetric patterns are present. For instance, a pain drawing with 8 marks, in which the extension of the longest mark is 25 mm, including symmetric patterns, yields a score value of 788. With a score of ≥ 779.5, the positive predictive value for a somatoform-functional pain would be nearly 100% (95%CI: 91.8%, 100%). Sixty-nine percent of our patients with somatoform-functional pain scored ≥ 779.5 (69.3%, 95%CI: 56.3%, 80.4%). The cut-off of 457.5 represents the mathematically optimal combination of sensitivity (90.3%; 95%CI: 80.1%, 96.4%) and specificity (93.9%; 95%CI: 83.1%, 98.7%) to discern somatoform-functional from somatic-nociceptive pain. This cut-off would still yield a positive predictive value of 94.9% for the probability of somatoform-functional pain (95%CI: 85.8%, 98.9%). Conversely, a score < 457.5 would generate a probability of somatic-nociceptive pain of 88.5% (95%CI: 76.6%, 95.6%).
Discussion
This study is an approach to a better implementation of PDs as one tool in the diagnostic assessment of somatoform-functional pain syndromes. PDs of patients with somatoform-functional pain differ significantly with respect to a defined spectrum of graphic features. The frequency and the stereotypical appearance of these patterns procure a suitable tool for diagnostic purposes. The greatest power to identify somatoform-functional pain resulted in our study from the combination of the number of marks, length of the longest mark, and presence versus absence of symmetric patterns. The area under the ROC-curve for this triple criteria combination is more than 95% (Figure 5). Assuming that the percentage of our cohort of pain patients with somatoform-functional pain (56%) is representative for the pain population assessed in a tertiary pain center, 91.9% of these patients would be correctly classified by these three pain drawing criteria alone (95%CI: 85.2%, 96.2%). The prevalence of some drawing criteria was highly different between the two pain populations (e.g., neck involvement, symmetric patterns; Table 2). This difference mathematically explains why some of the odds ratios are unusually high (Table 3). Clearly, the absolute values of these odds ratios should not be overinterpreted but may underscore the observation that some of the pain drawing criteria are vastly different between the two groups of pain patients.
The two patient groups also differed vastly in terms of their basic characteristics (Table 1). This was expected because somatoform-functional pain shows known associations with for instance female gender and increased pain duration. In addition, more than 80% of our somatoform-functional pain patients suffered from clinical depression. It is generally known that somatoform-functional pain disorders are frequently associated with psychiatric disorders. Excluding patients with somatoform-functional pain with a concurrent psychiatric comorbidity would have yielded a clinically less representative sample. Nevertheless, their PDs should not be used as a psychodiagnostic test in disguise [9]. There are more useful tests available for specific psychodiagnostic screening purposes [10]. Our opinion is that the graphic patterns in somatoform-functional pain disorders illustrate several pain specific perceptional aspects. By definition, the indicated pain areas reflect, in contrast to the somatic-nociceptive pain, no injuries. The signs in somatorm-functional pain disorders may have diverse other causes. In the following, we will discuss some possible explanations illustrated with PDs.
Inexplicable functional pain syndromes are often associated with generalized hyperalgesia [11-13]. In such a case, even the perception of normal postural tone (Figure 4f), the force in muscle insertions (Figure 4a), or the weight exerted on joints (Figures 4b, 4c) is amplified and results in a painful sensation. This generalized hyperalgesia of the musculoskeletal system could also explain the high number as well as the symmetrical distribution of the patients’ complaints. Other graphic patterns are indicative of increased local muscle tension [14]. Obviously, the neck is an area of predilection for stress-associated muscular pain (Figure 2a, 2d): 75% of our patients suffering from somatoform-functional pain indicate in their PDs neck pain. Finally, another subtype of somatoform-functional pain can be found in patients suffering from severe posttraumatic stress disorder. Their initial physical pain related to the trauma often seems to leave an irreversible imprint on their body scheme which persists for years in some sort of “memory pain” [15,16] (Figure 4g: patient with whiplash-associated PTSD 10 years ago).
To conclude, we think that pain drawings of patients with somatoform-functional pain could hint at their cryptic and almost unknown pathophysiology. Generally, we recommend the PD-method as a helpful diagnostic adjunct in the assessment of any complex pain problems. Specially, the identification of patients with somatoform-functional pain is difficult and often related to frustrating diagnostic detours. An early identification of somatoform-functional pain is of clinical importance: Whereas treatment of somatic-nociceptive pain relies on analgesics (according to WHO criteria), the therapy of somatoform-functional pain considers dual antidepressants with analgesic properties alone or in combination with other modalities of multimodal pain treatment [17].
In this study we excluded orthopedic patients with a psychiatric comorbidity as well as pain patients with both somatoform-functional pain and somatic-nociceptive pain. When assessing PDs in clinical routine, however, one should keep in mind that the individual patient may also present with combinations of different pain types. Generally, we would not advise the use of PDs to rule out somatic-nociceptive pain, but recommend them as a positive indicator to identify somatoform-functional complaints.
We emphasize several limitations of our pilot study. Since we did not include any patients with neuropathic pain, tumor pain, or inflammatory rheumatic diseases, we cannot make any statement about the above mentioned criteria with regard to these patient groups. Because of more multilocular or widespread pain origins, these pain groups might show a closer overlap in their PDs with somatoform-functional pain syndromes than the classic orthopedic pain patients. For further differentiations more investigations are essential. Although we controlled for important demographic and clinical covariates, we are unable to exclude the possibility that factors other than the type of pain (e.g., the different clinical setting) might have contributed to differences in PD. Assessment of PD with only one rater does not allow to draw conclusions about the interrater reliability of PD.
Conclusion
To sum up, PDs are an easy-to-administer technique helping the clinician to detect somatoform-functional pain in comparison with somatic-nociceptive orthopedic pain. Clinical experience shows that many patients do appreciate this personalized and documentative style as one means of the diagnostic approach.
Abbrevations
(HADS-D): Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; (PDs): Pain drawings; (ROC): Receiver operating characteristic; (SD): Curves, standard deviation.
Competing interests
The authors have no competing interests to report. All authors declare no financial or other relationships that might lead to a competing interest.
Authors’ contributions
NE and MLG are responsible for the whole clinical concept and examinations, and RvK and RC for the study design and the statistical analyses. NE and RvK supervised the study on a medical-methodological level. NK, MLG and EM performed the clinical tests. All authors participated substantially in the acquisition or the analyses of the data. All co-authors were involved in drafting or revising the manuscript and have approved its final version.
Prior presentations
All authors confirm that the manuscript has not been published elsewhere.
Pre-publication history
The pre-publication history for this paper can be accessed here:
Contributor Information
Niklaus Egloff, Email: niklaus.egloff@insel.ch.
Rafael JA Cámara, Email: rafael@camara.li.
Roland von Känel, Email: roland.vonkaenel@insel.ch.
Nicole Klingler, Email: nicklingler@gmx.li.
Elizabeth Marti, Email: elizabeth.marti@spital.so.ch.
Marie-Louise Gander Ferrari, Email: marie-louise.gander@insel.ch.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Annette Kocher and Jean Pierre Geri for their editorial assistance. All authors declare no financial or other relationships that might lead to a conflict of interest.
References
- Mann NH, Brown MD, Hertz DB, Enger I, Tompkins J. Initial-impression diagnosis using Low-back pain patient pain drawing. Spine. 1993;18:41–53. doi: 10.1097/00007632-199301000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan CW, Goldman S, Ilstrup DM, Kunselman AR, O’Neill PI. The pain drawing and Waddel’s nonorganic physical signs in chronic Low-back pain. Spine. 1993;13:1717–1722. doi: 10.1097/00007632-199310000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toomingas A. Characteristics of pain drawings in the neck-shoulder region among the working population. Int Arch Occup Env Health. 1999;72:98–106. doi: 10.1007/s004200050344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bertilson B, Grunnesjö M, Johansson SE, Strender LE. Pain drawing in the assessment of neurogenic pain and dysfunction in the neck/shoulder region: inter-examiner reliability and concordance with clinical examination. Pain Med. 2007;8(2):134–146. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4637.2006.00145.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broadbent E, Niederhoffer K, Hague T, Corter A, Reynolds L. Headache sufferers‘drawings reflect distress, disability and illness. J Psychosom Res. 2009;66:465–470. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margolis RB, Tait RC, Krause SJ. A rating system for use with patient pain drawings. Pain. 1986;24:56–65. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(86)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bjelland I, Dahl AA, Haug TT, Neckelmann D. The validity of the hospital anxiety and depression scale. An updated literature review. J Psychosom Res. 2002;52(2):69–77. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(01)00296-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herrmann C, Kaminsky B, Rüger U, Kreuzer H. Praktikabilität und klinische relevanz eines routinemässigen psychologischen screenings von patienten internistischer allgemeinstationen. Psychother Psychosom Med Psychol. 1999;49:48–54. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenough CG, Fraser RD. Comparison of eight psychometric instruments in unselected patients with back pain. Spine. 1991;16(9):1068–1074. doi: 10.1097/00007632-199109000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carnes D, Ashby D, Underwood M. A systematic review of pain drawing literature- should pain drawings Be used for psychologic screening? Clin J Pain. 2006;22(5):449–457. doi: 10.1097/01.ajp.0000208245.41122.ac. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yunus MB. Central sensitivity syndromes: a New paradigm and group nosology for fibromyalgia and overlapping conditions, and the related issue of disease versus illness. Semin Arthritis Rheum. 2008;37:339–352. doi: 10.1016/j.semarthrit.2007.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Banic B, Petersen-Felix S, Andersen OK, Radanov BP, Villiger PM, Arendt-Nielsen L, Curatolo M. Evidence for spinal cord hypersensitivity in chronic pain after whiplash injury and in fibromyalgia. Pain. 2004;107:7–15. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2003.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egloff N, von Känel R. Kommentar zu faustregel 7. Praxis. 2010;99:26–28. doi: 10.1024/1661-8157/a000003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richter P, Hebgen R. Triggerpunkte und muskelfunktionskettenbuch, 2. Auflage. Stuttgart: Hypokrates Verlag; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Walter S, Leissner N, Jerg-Bretzke L, Hrabal V, Traue HC. Pain and emotional processing in psychological trauma. Psychiatr Danub. 2010;22(3):465–470. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egloff N, Hirschi A, von Känel R. Schmerzstörungen bei traumatisierten – neurophysiologische aspekte und klinische phänomenologie. Praxis. 2012;101(2):87–97. doi: 10.1024/1661-8157/a000816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clauw DJ. Fibromyalgia: an overview. Am J Med. 2009;122(12):3–13. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2009.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]