Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Am J Primatol. 2013 Jan 11;75(3):238–251. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22117

Maternal effects on offspring mortality in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta)

Gregory E Blomquist
PMCID: PMC3580175  NIHMSID: NIHMS426891  PMID: 23315583

Abstract

The genetics of primate life histories are poorly understood, but quantitative genetic patterns in other mammals suggest phenotypic differences among individuals early in life can be strongly affected by interactions with mothers or other caretakers. I used generalized linear mixed model extensions of complex pedigree quantitative genetic techniques to explore regression coefficients and variance components for infant and juvenile mortality rates across pre-reproductive age classes in the semi-free ranging Cayo Santiago rhesus macaques. Using a large set of records (max. n=977 mothers, 6240 offspring), strong maternal effects can be identified early in development but they rapidly “burn off” as offspring age and mothers become less consistent buffers from increasingly prominent environmental variation. The different ways behavioral ecologists and animal breeders have defined and studied maternal effects can be subsumed, and even blended, within the quantitative genetic framework. Regression coefficients identify loss of the mother, maternal age, and offspring age within their birth cohort as having significant maternal effects on offspring mortality, while variance components for maternal identity record significant maternal influence in the first month of life.

Keywords: quantitative genetics, infant mortality, juveniles, reproductive ecology, Cayo Santiago

INTRODUCTION

Unless mating results in production of offspring who themselves survive infancy and the juvenile years and position themselves so as to reproduce, sex is only so much sound and undulation signifying nothing. [Hrdy, 1999, p. 81]

In response to Hrdy’s statement, one might ask: How much do primate mothers matter for the phenotypes of their offspring including mortality? While a long history of studying mother-infant interactions in primates implies mothers matter a great deal [e.g. Altmann, 1980; Goodall, 1986; Maestripieri, 2009], the answer to this question depends on a variety of conditions, such as which phenotypes one studies and at what ages—knowing that mothers interact more with their young offspring and mothers may affect some traits more than others. Furthermore, the answer must also be species- or even population-specific because evolved differences among species and their immediate local ecology can create larger or smaller opportunities for caretakers to affect offspring development [Charmantier & Garant, 2005; Dufour & Sauther, 2002]. In this paper, I offer a brief review of the theoretical background for describing how important mothers are for offspring mortality and then offer a case study with the free-ranging population of rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) on Cayo Santiago.

Imagine having the opportunity and resources to observe a primate population continuously for several decades. Over time, you amass a wealth of data on individual mothers and offspring. The influence of mothers on offspring mortality could be studied in two complementary ways. First, you could relate offspring loss to other measurable characteristics of mothers (e.g. maternal age, dominance rank, maternal style, milk composition) or the environment (e.g. food availability, population density, social group membership, group composition, predator abundance). This would identify trends (i.e. regression coefficients) for offspring mortality within the population, averaging over the individual mothers, to describe the dependence of mortality on these covariates. This influence of maternal phenotype on offspring phenotype is called a maternal effect [Wolf & Wade, 2009; Räsänen & Kruuk, 2007], and primatologists have been quite successful at finding them [reviewed in Maestripieri, 2009]. A familiar example seen in many primates would be primiparous mothers losing infants at higher rates than multiparae [e.g. Koyama et al., 1992; Bercovitch et al., 1998; Robbins et al., 2006].

A second approach would become available if you were able to observe individual mothers attempt to rear a series of offspring during their reproductive career. In this case, you might notice that some mothers tend to lose offspring while others are more successful at preventing their death. This implies there are stable differences among mothers in their influence on early life mortality. Those stable differences can be quantified with a variance component for mothers that accounts for some fraction of the total observable variance in offspring mortality. This fraction of total variance can be thought of as the repeatability of offspring mortality for mothers [Nakagawa & Schielzeth, 2010; Jones et al., 2010]. While useful in itself, the repeatability does little to identify what causes stable differences among mothers in risk to their offspring. It could be due to 1) genes mothers carry that influence her ability to provide care, 2) non-genetic factors, such as early life experiences of mothers, that influence the care they provide, 3) the genes mothers transmit to offspring in gametes that form half of an offspring genome and exert direct effects on offspring phenotypes like early life mortality, or 4) covariance between 1) and 3). In a large population, with several offspring measured per mother, mothers produced those offspring by mating with different identified fathers, and known pedigree relationships among mothers one can accurately estimate variance components for all four of these sources [Wilson et al., 2010; Lynch & Walsh, 1998; Cheverud & Moore, 1994]. In the absence of such a mating structure and rich pedigree information this is not possible, and it will not be attempted here. These steep requirements have prevented primatologists, and behavioral ecologists in general, from using this approach, though it is common in the animal breeding literature [Wilson & Réale, 2006]. Because it will likely become feasible to apply these techniques to wild and free-ranging primates in the near future [Kruuk et al., 2008; Morris et al., 2011], it is worth reviewing some of the theory behind the animal breeder’s model of maternal effects.

The breeder’s tradition of calculating variance components is an application of quantitative genetics. Quantitative genetics is a set of statistical techniques and related biological interpretations for the inheritance and evolution of phenotypes influenced by allelic variation at many loci scattered throughout the genome [Roff, 2007; Lynch & Walsh, 1998]. Phenotypes can be continuously distributed (e.g. birth weight) or even binary (e.g. survive/die) provided one assumes a continuous underlying genetic liability [Roff, 1997]. Crudely, these techniques take observable phenotypic measurements on sets of individuals of known genealogical relationship and decompose the total phenotypic variation (σp2) into several underlying components. For the model described in the previous paragraph, these are 1) maternal genetic variance (σm2), 2) maternal permanent environment variance (σc2), 3) additive genetic variance (σa2), and 4) covariance between additive and maternal genetic variance (σam), respectively. Any remaining variance is residual error (σr2). Total phenotypic variance is just the sum of these components (σp2=σa2+σm2+σc2+σam+σr2). These can be transformed into simpler ratios on a 0–1 scale like the narrow-sense heritability (h2=σa2σp2) or maternal gentic effect (m=σm2σp2). For example, Southey et al. [2003] analyzed lamb mortality from birth to weaning in domestic sheep (Ovis aries) and found the heritability was about 0.05 with maternal genetic effect about twice that, and they had a negative covariance. In addition to estimating variance components, current “animal model” techniques also allow for very flexible modeling of covariates such as maternal age, parity, or social dominance rank that might also affect offspring phenotypes and incorporate the kinds of maternal influences that behavioral ecologists are more familiar with [Kruuk, 2004]. For example, lamb mortality was affected by lamb sex, litter size, maternal age, and type of rearing environment [Southey et al., 2003]. The variance components essentially partition the remaining variation after these factors have been accounted for [Wilson, 2008].

Variance components are useful for exploring maternal effects for at least two reasons. First, any maternal variance component indicates how much variation in offspring phenotypes is due to maternal variation. In other words, how much consistent differences among mothers matter even if it is unknown what it is about them affects their offspring (e.g. placenta quality, gestation length, maternal style, body mass). Second, variance components appear in simple equations for describing microevolutionary change [Cheverud & Moore, 1994]. Intergenerational response (change in the mean,) is predicted by additive genetic variation and strength of selection (βP). Without maternal effects, this is simply the breeder’s equation (R=σa2βP) and response will always be in the same direction as the trait is under selection. A more complicated equation describes response when maternal effects are present, where additive genetics becomes the whole portion in brackets of R=[σa2+32σam+12σm2]βP. This is because a portion of offspring phenotypic variation is due to their own genes (σa2), some to genetic variation in the environment provided by mothers (σm2), and part to the covariance between the two (σam). It also means that offspring phenotypes can evolve strictly through changes in the environment provided by mothers (σa2=0, σm2 > 0). In the sheep example quoted above, small variances mean response will be slow. Furthermore, the negative covariance will also curtail response. This implies coadaptation between maternal and additive genetics such that improvement in the direct effect of offspring genes that reduces risk of mortality is offset by increases in risk via maternal effect genes, or vice versa [Cheverud & Wolf, 2009; Cheverud & Moore, 1994].

There are far-reaching implications of maternal effects that few evolutionary biologists appreciated until the 1990s [Mousseau & Fox, 1998; Rossiter, 1996]. Whether or how maternal effects are adaptive and for whom (mothers v. offspring) is a topic of current debate and research [Marshall & Uller, 2007; Jones, 2005]. Adaptive interpretations primarily emphasize the strategies of mothers to adjust offspring phenotypes, functioning as a form of phenotypic plasticity [Räsänen & Kruuk, 2007]. This means that these are shared phenotypes between the two parties being negotiated with mothers in a position to signal adaptive patterns of development. Mothers adjusting how offspring develop can have large-scale demographic results influencing population growth rates, dispersal patterns, and phenotypic divergence among populations or sexes [Badyaev et al., 2002; Badyaev, 2005; Mousseau & Fox, 1998]. Many anthropologists and human biologists have become interested in maternal effects under the guise of the developmental origin of health and disease [Kuzawa & Quinn, 2009; Wells, 2007], to understand variation in adult phenotypes from fetal and early post-natal experiences [Lummaa & Clutton-Brock, 2002].

In this paper, I pursue describing maternal influences on offspring mortality through both the behavioral ecologist (regression coefficients) and animal breeder (variance component) traditions. As noted above, only the later of these is novel. Attention to variance components in the literature is currently very uneven for different sorts of primate phenotypes. For morphological traits, there have been a good number of studies, particularly of the skull, that describe its genetic architecture and are now adding molecular details [e.g. Koh et al., 2010; Roseman et al., 2010; Visscher et al., 2008; Cheverud & Dittus, 1992; Cheverud, 1982]. Unfortunately, this is almost entirely about static adult morphology and has little to say about early life ontogeny when mothers are likely most influential. Life history and reproductive traits have had very little treatment, especially if humans are excluded, and all of it comes from captive colonies [Gagliardi et al., 2010; Martin et al., 2002; Ha et al., 2002; Jaquish et al., 1996; Williams-Blangero & Blangero, 1995]. Other captive studies of the genetics of early life mortality have focused on the effects of inbreeding [Van Coillie et al., 2008; Ralls & Ballou, 1982].

The major obstacle to quantitative genetic research in primates is the large samples of pedigreed individuals needed. This is especially true for life history traits that are expected to have low heritabilities and large residual effects [Price & Schluter, 1991]. Because of this, the Cayo Santiago rhesus macaques are a valuable study population [Rawlins & Kessler, 1986; Sade et al., 1985]. Cayo Santiago is a small, 15 ha island about 1 km off the coast of mainland Puerto Rico inhabited by a population of about 1000 rhesus macaques. These monkeys are descendants of 409 individuals trapped in India that were released on the island in 1938. Demographic data have been collected in a regular census taken on weekday mornings with frequent, but brief interruptions since 1959 [Sade et al., 1985]. Animals are also annually trapped for tattooing and tissue sampling. They are provisioned with commercial monkey chow, and the population has been managed by removal of individuals, but matings are unmanaged and animals live in naturally formed social groups.

Several authors have used Cayo Santiago for quantitative genetic research [e.g. Blomquist, 2009; Havill, 2003; Cheverud et al., 1990]. Previous work on the heritability of female lifetime fitness (n = 590) in the population produced some unexpected results suggesting weak maternal influences on early life mortality [cf. Blomquist, 2010]. The heritability of having ever reproduced was roughly quantified by coding 1=if a female ever reproduced and 0=she never reproduced and analyzing the variable as normal/Gaussian, as is typical of quantitative traits. This had a low heritability of 0.27 and no maternal effect. The low heritability is not surprising, but the lack of maternal effect is unexpected when maternal characteristics are predicted to make a large contribution to early life phenotypes including mortality. Moreover, the pedigree information for this population of monkeys is not rich enough to satisfactorily separate maternal effects from direct genetic effects (see below).

Here, I use a much larger dataset of male and female demographic records from Cayo Santiago and generalized linear mixed model techniques [Wilson et al., 2010; Bolker et al., 2009] to estimate variance components for the binary phenotype of offspring death. I adopt a simple approach of focusing on the repeatability of offspring death for mothers. A heritability of offspring mortality for some age classes as a trait of mothers [Pettay et al., 2005] is also presented but it must be cautiously interpreted as it does not follow the strict decomposition in the animal breeder model of maternal effects (described above) and instead represents a familial patterning among mothers in offspring mortality of ambiguous genetic origin. With this new method and larger dataset I explore the age-course of maternal effects across infant and juvenile periods, rather than treating it as a single pre-reproductive stage of life. Maternal effects of both the ecologist’s (regression coefficients) animal breeder’s (variance components) tradition are statistically significant and, for some offspring ages, show strong maternal influences on early life death.

METHODS

Data were extracted from the Cayo Santiago demographic database in 2005 [Blomquist, 2007]. I scored individual mortality data from birth to 900 days of life on 6240 offspring of 977 mothers. I used this maximum age because I was strictly interested in pre-reproductive mortality. This is the approximate age (2.46 years) females begin to mate successfully such that they give birth when they are three years old. In addition to the 0–900 day interval, I also scored a series of five 180-day (≈ 6 month) age classes (Figure 1). Equal length age classes were chosen to avoid any circularity in defining classes based on prior knowledge of death rates, but they were required to be long enough that there was appreciable mortality (≥2%) in all the age classes (precluding a longer series of shorter classes). Because mother-infant interactions are more intense in the first few weeks after birth and mortality in this period is common, I also explored dividing the first interval into an initial 0–30 day and subsequent 30–180 day age classes [Maestripieri & Hoffman, 2012]. Offspring were only included if they came from cohorts prior to or including 2002 to ensure they could have survived to 900 days by the time the data were extracted in early 2005. Furthermore, infant ages are only recorded precisely after 1962. Maternal ages need not be known as precisely and were accepted from cohorts back to 1956. Births at Cayo Santiago are strongly seasonal making such assignments to cohorts simple [Rawlins & Kessler, 1985]. Any twins were also excluded because of their high death rates and extreme rarity [Bercovitch et al., 2002; Geissmann, 1990]. A small number of unsexed individuals were also excluded.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Death rates in the analyzed dataset (dots) and for all sexed infants born in the same cohorts (circles).

Death was scored as a separate binary variable for each of the age classes with 1=death and 0=survived. All 6240 offspring were scored for each age class. Any individuals removed through colony management during an age class or prior to it were set to a missing value (NA) that is omitted from the analysis. Age class death rates can then be computed from the complement of a Kaplan & Meier [1958] survival rate that accounts for the censoring due to removals (deathrate=1survivedenterremoved). These death rates are also the mean phenotype of each age class. For example, if a monkey lived past 900 days it had a 0 for each age class. If a monkey died in the 180–360 age class it had a 1 for 180–360, 0 for any younger age class (e.g. 0–180), and missing value for all the later age classes (e.g. 360–540). Finally, to ensure repeatabilities were interpretable (see below) only mothers having two or more infants scored with non-missing values in an age class were retained in the analysis of that age class. These restrictions for constructing the dataset cause only a small downward bias in the death rates versus those seen in the full set of infants born in the 1962–2002 cohorts (Figure 1). Final numbers of offspring retained for the analysis were between 5859 and 3094 depending on the age class

I analyzed offspring death with univariate generalized linear mixed models (GLMM), running a single model for each age class to explore the age-course of effects predicting mortality (Equation 1). Offspring death was treated as a trait of the mothers with all mothers having multiple offspring scored if they were included for the model for an age class.

y=Xβ+inZiui+r (1)

In equation 1, is the vector of phenotypic measurements of offspring death, X is an incidence matrix for fixed effects with β as their vector of regression coefficient estimates, Zi is an incidence matrix for random effect i with ui as the vector of solutions for the random effect, and r is residual error [Kruuk, 2004; Lynch & Walsh, 1998]. All fixed effects in the model were covariates with regression coefficients estimated in. Offspring sex was dummy coded with 1=male. Offspring age in cohort is the number of days deviating from the median birth date within the offspring’s cohort. Positive values are birth dates after the cohort median. Mother’s age at birth was recorded in years and the average age at birth (8 years) subtracted to ease interpretation of regression coefficients and baseline mortality rates. A set of dummy variables also indicated whether mothers were primiparous, whether they had given birth in the previous cohort, whether they exited due to death or removal during the current offspring age class, or whether they had done so prior to it. These final two variables are collectively referred to as maternal presence. Maternal age, primiparity, previous reproductive state, and presence are clearly maternal effects of the kind often studied by behavioral ecologists. Offspring age in cohort might also be considered one as a consequence of maternal conception or gestation time.

For all age classes models were initially run with a pair or random effects: maternal identity and offspring birth cohort. These have variance components estimated (σi2, and σb2, respectively) and solution predicted (i, and b, respectively). Maternal identity solutions correspond to the variance in offspring mortality explained by stable differences among individual mothers, regardless of their cause. These are referred to as repeatability models as they partition variation in mortality a simple manner such that maternal repeatability can be calculated.

For age classes with high repeatabilities (σi2 > 0) I also explored an augmented model included three random effects and attempts to break the stable differences among which mothers into additive genetic and non-genetic (permanent environment) sources. Thus there are three variance components estimated (σa2, σpe2, and σb2, respectively) and solution predicted (a, pe, and b, respectively). In these models, σa2+σpe2 is approximately the individual variance and a+pe individual solutions.

The means of all random effects in repeatability or augmented models are zero. The maternal identity, permanent environment, and cohort effects have covariances given by σi2, Iσpe2 and Iσb2 where I is an identity matrix having the number of mothers or cohorts as its dimensions. The additive genetic covariance is σa2, where the elements of A are twice the kinship coefficients of a pair of individuals x and y [Axy=2Θxy, Lynch & Walsh, 1998]. Pedigree relationships are known from behavioral interactions of mothers and infants and a smaller number of paternities in the 1988–1998 birth cohorts determined from 10 microsatellites (Table 1). Further details of the paternity analysis can be found in Bercovitch et al. [2003]. The paternity of nearly all monkeys dying during their first year of life is unknown because they were never trapped to have tissue collected. This prevents analyzing early life mortality as a phenotype of the offspring because a maternal identity effect and direct genetic effect would be confounded for most all offspring. This explains why Blomquist [2010] found significant heritability and no maternal effect when running this type of model, and implies great caution is required interpreting his results.

Table 1.

Pedigree statistics for the mothers analyzed in each offspring age class.

0–900 0–180 0–30 30–180 180–360 360–540 540–720 720–900
phenotyped IDs 783 949 977 916 859 816 779 666
pedigree members 909 1100 1130 1070 1013 966 924 786
maximum generations 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 7
founders 73 78 79 77 72 68 65 63
maternities 836 1022 1051 993 941 898 859 723
paternities 175 221 224 218 217 213 204 142
maternal grandmothers 734 891 914 866 819 784 756 628
maternal grandfathers 64 88 90 87 87 84 80 49
paternal grandmothers 175 221 224 218 217 213 204 142
paternal grandfathers 64 88 90 87 87 84 80 49
maternal sibs 687 859 884 836 798 758 720 589
paternal sibs 129 169 172 166 166 162 153 97
maternal half-sibs 687 857 882 834 796 756 720 589
paternal half-sibs 129 167 170 164 164 160 153 97
full sibs 0 2 2 2 2 2 0 0
F > 0 17 26 26 25 25 25 22 13
F ≥ 1/16 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4
F ≥ 1/8 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4
F ≥ 1/4 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2
% largest family 84.598 82.545 82.389 82.991 84.205 85.300 86.364 85.623
% disconnected 3.080 1.909 1.681 2.243 1.876 1.863 2.165 2.799
offspring per mother 2.297 2.371 2.372 2.364 2.388 2.382 2.347 2.231
offpsring per father 1.989 2.105 2.113 2.076 2.087 2.068 2.040 1.797
maternal sibship (n > 1 ) 3.195 3.205 3.203 3.179 3.179 3.198 3.172 3.100
paternal sibship (n > 1 ) 3.071 3.189 3.185 3.132 3.132 3.115 3.122 2.853
Ā ij 0.005 0.005 0.005 0.005 0.005 0.006 0.006 0.006
% Aij > 0 10.621 10.828 10.824 11.042 11.641 11.876 11.980 10.138
% Aij ≥ 1/16 2.878 2.584 2.521 2.641 2.829 2.962 3.026 3.045
% Aij ≥ 1/8 1.641 1.440 1.402 1.474 1.577 1.652 1.689 1.758
% Aij ≥ 1/4 0.770 0.662 0.644 0.679 0.728 0.766 0.794 0.845

Treating offspring mortality as a trait of mothers requires no paternity information on offspring. In fact, offspring are only part of the pedigree if they survive to become mothers themselves or are genealogical links between mothers. This means dead offspring do not appear in the pedigree—they are simply repeat measurements of a phenotype assigned to their mother. As such, only paternities of the mothers remain in the pedigree. Errors in these should only lower heritability estimates [Pemberton, 2008]. However, infant stealing potentially complicates the assignment of offspring to mothers and introduces a potential bias to heritability and repeatability measures because stolen infants typically die within a few days unless their mothers retrieve them. However, the physical signs of pregnancy, typical possessiveness of rhesus mothers, and pursuit of stolen infants which should all be recorded through regular observation of the population suggest this is unlikely to be very common [Quiatt, 1979]. Furthermore, this would only induce a downward bias to repeatability by shuffling offspring among mothers unless there were mothers which frequently managed to steal infants, undetected by observers, which then died. Heritability would be affected in similar ways only if related mothers frequently managed to steal infants undetected.

A binomial distribution with logit link function was used to account for the binary nature of the offspring mortality data [Southey et al., 2003; Nakagawa & Schielzeth, 2010]. Modeling mortality with the logit link produces regression coefficients for the logarithm of the odds of death. These are more interpretable when transformed. The baseline probability of death is calculated from the intercept coefficient (β0) as e0β1+e0β. Other coefficients are easily interpreted as proportionate change in the probability of death for a unit change in the covariate as eβ0+β11+eβ0+β1e0β1+eoβ (e.g. probability of death increases 26.4 × baseline with maternal loss). I report these transformed regression coefficients giving the baseline mortality rate and proportionate changes in the mortality rate due to a unit increase in the covariate. Baseline mortality is for a female offspring, born on her cohort’s median date, to a 8-year old, multiparous female who did not give birth the previous year and was present throughout the offspring age class.

All data preparation and analysis was performed in R 2.15 [R Development Core Team, 2012] with the repeatability and augmented models analyzed via functions in the MCMCglmm package [Hadfield, 2010]. MCMCglmm relies on Bayesian Markov Chain Monte Carlo techniques [O’Hara et al., 2008] to provide estimates of variance components and regression coefficients by repeated sampling of a long-running simulation of the posterior distributions of model terms. Following Postma et al. [2011] and the MCMCglmm documentation [Ch. 8 of “CourseNotes” vignette; Hadfield, 2010], I used parameter expanded priors with σi2, σa2, σpe2, and σb2 set to 1 with a degree of belief parameter of 1. In these models, where each offspring’s death/survival is only observed once, σr2 cannot be estimated and was fixed at 10. The logit link-specific variance also contributes π2/3 to the total variance in these models. Estimated values of σi2, σa2, σpe2, and σb2 are dependent on the residual and link variance. Ratios of these variance components to the total are easier to interpret because they are independent of the fixed residual, though dependence on the link variance remains. These are the repeatability (σi2σa2+σpe2+σb2+10+π23 or σa2+σpe2σa2+σpe2+σb2+10+π23), heritability (σa2σa2+σpe2+σb2+10+π23), and cohort effect (σb2σa2+σpe2+σb2+10+π23).

The repeatability and cohort effect are the most important of these. The repeatability represents the fraction of variance on the logit scale accounted for by differences among mothers. If greater than 0, it indicates there are stable differences among mothers across the repeated measurements of offspring death in an age class. A similar interpretation applies to the cohort effect, that a fraction of total variance is explained by differences in risk of death among years. The heritability is difficult to interpret because, although offspring death is treated as a phenotype of mothers here, it is likely a complex mixture of direct genetic effects in offspring that influence risk of death (half of which come from mothers), direct genetic effects in the mother affecting her care of offspring (maternal genetic effects from the offspring’s perspective), and their covariance. I consider it to reflect familial similarity in early life mortality rates of unknown origin, and focus interpretation on the repeatability as a crude measure of maternal effects.

For each univariate model, I ran a single chain for 2005000 iterations and saved every 1000th sample after a 5000 iteration burn-in. This provides 2000 samples of the posterior distributions of the variance components, random effect solutions, and regression coefficients. Effective sample sizes for the non-fixed variance components were always greater than 1615. Autocorrelation of the retained samples was less than 0.065 in all cases and there were no trends in diagnostic plots of the chain over simulation time. Minimum effective size for the regression coefficients was 1296 and maximum autocorrelation was 0.074. Shape and location of these distributions of regression coefficients or variance components can be used to infer whether parameters are greater or less than 0. Credible intervals covering 95% of the posterior distribution are often used for these tests. These are appropriate for regression coefficients in this study but variance components and ratios have a lower boundary at 0 and thus cannot include it within their 95% interval. Instead, I simply visualize a ratio’s posterior distribution and modal value and interpret the resulting patterns. The mode is declared different from 0 if the lower tail of the visualized distribution does not approach 0 and the lower boundary of the 95% interval is greater than 0 at three digits past the decimal. Formal comparison of alternative models with different random effects using information criteria (e.g. DIC) is unreliable for GLMM [Wilson et al., 2010]. Ratios were not converted to the proportion scale [by equation 23 in Nakagawa & Schielzeth, 2010] because the resulting values are dependent on the mean which would complicate comparisons across age classes. Nevertheless, posterior distributions for repeatabilities on the proportion scale had an identical shape to those presented here. Regression coefficient dependence on fixing σr2=10 was removed through their multiplication by 11+10c2(where=16315π), which rescales σr2=0. Random effect solutions for cohort or individual mother effects were rescaled by 11+10c2 [Hadfield et al., 2010].

Research reported here relies on data collected over several decades by many observers under different funding auspices and IACUC approvals which should be consulted for further details. All research reported here, and occurring at Cayo Santiago today, adheres to the ASP’s Principles for the Ethical Treatment of Non-human Primates.

RESULTS & DISCUSSION

The regression coefficients and variance components record a number of statistically significant maternal effects on offspring mortality in rhesus macaques. In general, these are strongest on the youngest age classes. Among the regression coefficients, maternal age effects are significant in age classes prior to 180 days whether pooled or separate (Table 2). The linear effect of age is negative meaning offspring of older mothers are less likely to die, while the quadratic effect is positive meaning that offspring of very young and very old mothers are more likely to die (Figure 2). This recovers a pattern already known in offspring mortality at Cayo Santiago [Hoffman et al., 2010]. The maternal presence variables have by far the largest coefficients and index the dramatic risks posed to offspring by loss of the mother. The effect of losing a mother during an age class diminishes with offspring age, but it is statistically significant even in the final age class (720–900 days). By this final age class, offspring have been fully weaned for at least one year and spend very little time in contact or close proximity with their mothers except in stressful situations [Suomi, 2005]. Loss of the mother during an age class causes a 2.1–26.4× increase in the probability of offspring death, depending on the age class examined. For example, in the 720–900 day age class the mortality increases from a baseline of 0.007 to 0.020 (=0.007 × 2.808) when the mother is lost. In the 0–180 day class, this is a much greater change from 0.048 to 0.581. The absence of any maternal presence effect and change in sign of estimated coefficient in the 360–540 age class is atypical. This is likely due to the crudity of coding with 1=exit, which includes both maternal death or removal through population management. The ratio of the number of mother deaths to mother removals is lowest in this period. If removal of the mother is less damaging than death of the mother, then this decreased ratio of deaths to removals will diminish the maternal exit effect on offspring death. Separating death and removal into separate dummy variables is not possible because there are very few of each in most age classes.

Table 2.

Regression equations for death in each age class. β are posterior means and their 95% credible interval given on the logit scale. These are transformed into baseline death rate (intercept) and a proportionate change in probability of death due to a unit change in the variable (β′).

β β 95% CI β β 95% CI
0–900 days 0–180 days
intercept 0.149 −1.744 −2.010 −1.479* 0.048 −2.997 −3.285 −2.722*
male 1.129 0.144 0.013 0.273* 1.021 0.021 −0.127 0.172
age in cohort 0.998 −0.002 −0.004 0.000 0.996 −0.004 −0.007 −0.002*
primiparous 1.236 0.254 −0.036 0.541 1.288 0.267 −0.061 0.587
rep. previous 1.151 0.167 −0.037 0.373 1.178 0.172 −0.065 0.421
age 0.981 −0.023 −0.057 0.011 0.941 −0.063 −0.102 −0.026*
age2 1.006 0.007 0.002 0.011* 1.015 0.015 0.011 0.020*
exit during 2.691 1.340 1.160 1.531* 12.110 3.303 2.949 3.673*
exit prior
0–30 days 30–180 days
intercept 0.029 −3.514 −3.869 −3.169* 0.020 −3.881 −4.287 −3.505*
male 0.934 −0.071 −0.245 0.113 1.245 0.224 0.012 0.443*
age in cohort 0.994 −0.006 −0.009 −0.003* 0.999 −0.001 −0.005 0.002
primiparous 1.151 0.145 −0.260 0.522 1.549 0.449 −0.011 0.917
rep. previous 1.168 0.160 −0.124 0.443 1.182 0.171 −0.175 0.531
age 0.956 −0.046 −0.092 0.000 0.947 −0.055 −0.111 −0.003*
age2 1.015 0.015 0.010 0.021* 1.010 0.010 0.003 0.016*
exit during 26.372 4.682 4.004 5.401* 14.814 3.031 2.562 3.554*
exit prior 5.515 1.805 −0.123 3.527
180–360 days 360–540 days
intercept 0.033 −3.366 −3.706 −3.042* 0.025 −3.654 −4.119 −3.221*
male 1.091 0.090 −0.094 0.294 0.966 −0.036 −0.252 0.186
age in cohort 1.006 0.006 0.003 0.009* 0.993 −0.007 −0.011 −0.003*
primiparous 0.950 −0.053 −0.493 0.389 1.412 0.356 −0.161 0.851
rep. previous 1.171 0.164 −0.154 0.480 1.222 0.206 −0.142 0.560
age 0.963 −0.039 −0.087 0.009 1.032 0.033 −0.028 0.094
age2 1.006 0.006 −0.000 0.012 0.997 −0.004 −0.012 0.005
exit during 3.351 1.294 0.778 1.793* 0.659 −0.425 −1.101 0.233
exit prior 2.818 1.101 0.415 1.809* 0.889 −0.121 −0.944 0.622
540–720 days 720–900 days
intercept 0.024 −3.695 −4.265 −3.201* 0.007 −4.903 −5.984 −4.037*
male 0.803 −0.224 −0.526 0.069 1.058 0.057 −0.330 0.439
age in cohort 1.001 0.001 −0.004 0.006 0.995 −0.005 −0.012 0.001
primiparous 1.368 0.322 −0.306 0.965 1.174 0.162 −0.753 1.081
rep. previous 0.808 −0.218 −0.640 0.219 1.135 0.128 −0.420 0.709
age 1.034 0.035 −0.043 0.114 1.069 0.067 −0.032 0.166
age2 0.994 −0.006 −0.017 0.005 0.995 −0.005 −0.018 0.007
exit during 2.071 0.755 −0.042 1.494 2.808 1.046 0.168 1.858*
exit prior 1.142 0.136 −0.596 0.796 2.072 0.736 0.045 1.442*

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Predicted probability of death across maternal ages for the 0-30 age class (solid black line). The small primiparity effect is also illustrated with the dashed line covering ages of primiparae. The oscillating grey line and rug show the distribution of maternal ages in the sample.

Coefficients for offspring age within cohorts are significant for several age classes (Table 2). However, their sign, even when significant, is not consistent. In general, being born later within a cohort decreases risk of death. How advantages arise from being born later are unclear (e.g. greater gestation length). Reproduction in the previous year, primiparity, and offspring sex are not significant predictors of mortality (Table 2). The lack of sex effect may be surprising as male infants are often considered more susceptible to death [Bhaumik et al., 2004]. Given recent theoretical attention, one might speculate absence of sex effects might be related to sex ratio variation by maternal condition where mothers in better condition are more likely to have male infants which offsets their greater frailty [Schino, 2004; Bercovitch et al., 2000]. The lack of significant primiparity effect on offspring death at Cayo Santiago has already been documented in a smaller sample [Bercovitch et al., 1998], though primate primiparae are generally described as less competent mothers and expected to lose infants at higher rates [e.g., Altmann, 1980].

Ratios of the random effect variance components also show an initially strong effect of mothers on offspring death that quickly declines in later age classes (Table 3, Figure 3). Any interval including the first 30 days of life (0–30, 0–180, or 0–900) has a repeatability that is significantly greater than zero. This is largest when the age class is confined to 0–30 days (0.183). In all subsequent age classes, repeatabilities are essentially 0.

Table 3.

Variance component ratios and 95% credible intervals for the repeatability models in all age classes and augmented models in the three classes with repeatabilities greater than 0.

age class repeatabilty models augmented models
repeatability cohort effect pe effect heritability repeatability cohort effect
0-900 0.046: 0.008-0.084 0.098: 0.058-0.160 0.000: 0.000-0.054 0.041: 0.000-0.068 0.050: 0.020-0.090 0.099: 0.061-0.165
0-180 0.101: 0.047-0.162 0.036: 0.017-0.081 0.001: 0.000-0.124 0.000: 0.000-0.116 0.097: 0.052-0.164 0.037: 0.014-0.073
0-30 0.183: 0.116-0.260 0.066: 0.027-0.120 0.001: 0.000-0.190 0.083: 0.000-0.194 0.181: 0.115-0.265 0.065: 0.026-0.121
30-180 0.001: 0.000-0.112 0.021: 0.000-0.060
180-360 0.000: 0.000-0.084 0.033: 0.008-0.079
360-540 0.001: 0.000-0.149 0.134: 0.064-0.242
540-720 0.001: 0.000-0.197 0.080: 0.020-0.198
720-900 0.001: 0.000-0.247 0.322: 0.171-0.513

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Violin plots of posterior distributions for variance component ratios in the offspring age classes. Each posterior mode is indicated by the black dot corresponding to the repeatability models in Table 3.

In the augmented models, repeatabilites of offspring mortality are nearly identical to the repeatability models, but there is no statistical evidence of any familial signal. No heritability can be declared greater than 0. However, the posterior modes are a large portion of the repeatability for 0–900 and 0–30 days implying there might be some similarity among related mothers in the mortality of their offspring that would have been apparent in a larger sample with more precise pedigree information. Regardless, the interpretation of these heritabilities would be difficult. The repeatabilities could reflect maternal genetic variance for ability to prevent offspring mortality, persistent but non-genetic factors influencing maternal care, maternal contribution to the offspring genome that affects offspring mortality, their covariance, or some combination of these. Nevertheless, their rapid decline as offspring age is consistent with much of this effect being due to maternal ability to prevent mortality.

In later age classes, the lack of repeatability means mothers are no longer a reliable buffer from the environment or controller of this shared phenotype of offspring death. This should not be taken to mean mothers are no longer important in later age classes, but rather that the individual mothers become more variable in how they affect offspring as the offspring get older (i.e. increasing within mother variation). Indeed, the notable maternal presence covariate indicates that mothers, on average, are still influencing offspring mortality in these later age classes.

In contrast to the decline of the repeatability, birth cohort initially explains little variation in offspring death but rises dramatically as offspring age and may reflect seasonality of mortality risk. Cohort effects are greater than 0 in all age classes, except 30–180 (Table 3). The largest effects are in age classes 360–540 (0.135) and 720–900 (0.322, Figure 3). Redirection of maternal care away from the offspring to a newborn sibling [Devinney et al., 2001], likely exposes the yearling to annually varying conditions that mothers previously provided some buffer from.

The random effect solutions provide some illustrations of the patterns summarized in the variance components ratios. Quantitative analysis of random effect solutions (a.k.a. BLUPs) in ecological settings is problematic [Hadfield et al., 2010], so I restrict discussion to a few points on the cohort effects (b) and mother identity effect (i). Among the cohort solutions for the 0–30 day age class, the highest value is for 1963, indicating it was a catastrophic year of offspring death (Figure 4). This is explained by a brutal experiment described by Sade et al. [1985, p. 36] in their Demographic Observations

The pregnant females were taken to a small building in Punta Santiago, Puerto Rico, the point of land nearest Cayo Santiago. The infants were delivered by Cesarean section and asphyxiated followed by repeated attempts at resuscitation…Most of the infants, about 18 percent of the 1963 birth crop, died as a result of the experiment.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Random effect solutions for death in the 0-30 day age class scaled as death rates relative to the baseline as in Table 2 (0.03). Posterior modes of the cohort solutions (upper panel) show unusually high mortality in 1963 (filled dot). Posterior modes of solutions for individual mothers (lower panel) are given as rug along the axis and full posterior density for the extreme mothers with lowest (black) and highest mortality (grey).

More relevant to the description of maternal influence on offspring mortality are the maternal identity effects in the 0–30 day age class when repeatability is largest. The highest and lowest solutions respectively identify the mothers that were consistently the worst and best mothers for offspring mortality adjusting for all the other fixed and random effects in the model. Picking out these two mothers makes a useful contrast (Figure 4). The “worst” mother lived about 9 years and had 5 offspring, all of whom died within a few days of birth. The “best” mother lived 21 years giving birth to 14 infants, none of which died between 0 and 30 days.

CONCLUSIONS

How much do primate mothers matter for the phenotypes of their offspring like mortality? This application of generalized linear mixed model techniques generates novel results for answering this question and thinking about how primate mothers influence life histories. Mothers certainly do affect offspring mortality, but the variable maternal characteristics that either underlie the maternal age effect or the consistent differences among mothers described by the maternal variance component only have an effect on offspring survival prior to weaning, which is largely accomplished in the 180–360 age class at Cayo Santiago [Maestripieri & Hoffman, 2012]. Once in juvenile age classes, having a mother present still reduces offspring risk of death, but apparently any mother will do, at least as described by the characteristics assessed in this analysis. For yearlings and beyond, birth cohort becomes more important as mothers become less effective or less consistent buffers from the environment.

The two perspectives on maternal effects were complementary here, but they were essentially treated separately. An interesting question that blends these perspectives is whether there is heterogeneity among mothers in how they affect offspring mortality as mothers age. The regression coefficients only describe a population average trajectory of changing risk to offspring (e.g. Figure 2). The maternal variance component is about mothers being shifted above or below this average. However, it is quite possible, and evolutionary genetic models of senescence would predict, that there is variation among mothers in their rates of aging and thus whether the late life increasing risk to offspring occurs earlier or later. That is beyond what can be done with this data set, but this theoretical prediction has recently been explored with mixed support in some wild bird and mammal populations [Brommer et al., 2010; Wilson et al., 2007].

In this analysis, the maternal variance component described stable differences among mothers in how they made life safer or riskier for their infants. It does not indicate what might underlie those stable differences (e.g. behavior, hormones, morphology). But, stable differences in the sorts of maternal characteristics that might affect infants (such as maternal style, milk yield, or even body size) point to either maternal genetics or early life experiences of mothers determining these maternal effects on infant mortality [Lindström, 1999; Nickerson, 1995; Berman, 1986]. This conclusion must be tempered by the inability to separate maternal variance from offspring additive variance in this population. This severely limits interpretation of any genetic variance components and leaves the lingering question of how much of this significant repeatability of offspring mortality is due to direct effects in offspring themselves. Additional research in settings where paternity of offspring is known would help resolve this question.

A final concern with this analysis is what can be generalized to other primate or mammalian species. Cayo Santiago is a somewhat unusual population of rhesus macaques that lives at very high density, is provisioned, is closed to in- or out-migration, and has no predators. The early life strength and decline of maternal effects is a common pattern noted in reviews of mammalian maternal effects, mostly in livestock and rodents [Wilson & Réale, 2006; Cheverud, 1984]. That offspring mortality in the Cayo Santiago macaques matches this pattern is encouraging but not particularly surprising. Novel patterns might be found in primates with extensive allocare or paternal support [e.g. Aotus, Callithrix, Saguinus; Solomon & French, 1997]. Additional sources of mortality or food-stress might also be a source of divergent results, as shown in other taxa [Bethea et al., 2008; Thompson et al., 2009]. However, a recent meta-analysis of differences in variance components between favorable and unfavorable conditions in wild animal species showed that heritabilities increase in favorable conditions but there is no consistent pattern in maternal effects. This might be surprising because it would make sense that mothers matter more in unfavorable environments where there is so much more for mothers to buffer offspring from [Charmantier & Garant, 2005]. In summary, at present, there is little reason to expect dramatically different patterns elsewhere among primates though this should encourage, rather than deter, greater interest in maternal effects among primatologists.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Cayo Santiago is part of the Caribbean Primate Research Center (CPRC) which is supported by the University of Puerto Rico, Medical Sciences Campus and the National Institutes of Health. The genetic database from which paternity data were provided was originally created by J. Berard, F. Bercovitch, M. Kessler, M. Krawczak, P. Nürnberg, and J. Schmidtke. The National Science Foundation, Harry Frank Guggenheim Foundation, University of Berlin, Deutsche Forschungsmeinschaft, Medizinische Hochschule Hannover, NIH, and CPRC funded the creation of the genetic database. Additional funding for this research came from the University of Illinois Graduate College. M. Gerald, J. Cant, T. Kensler, B. Hallgrímsson, and J. Turnquist were all helpful resources while working with CPRC materials. D. Sade, R. Rawlins, J. Berard, and M. Gerald must be credited for the upkeep of the demographic records on Cayo Santiago and A. Figueroa, E. Davila, and E. Maldonado for their day-to-day maintenance. Comments from the review editor, D. G. Smith; symposium organizer, A. M. Dettmer Erard; and anonymous reviewers greatly improved this manuscript.

REFERENCES

  1. Altmann J. Baboon Mothers and Infants. Harvard University Press; Cambridge: 1980. [Google Scholar]
  2. Badyaev AV. Maternal inheritance and rapid evolution of sexual size dimorphism: passive effects or active strategies? American Naturalist. 2005;166:S17–S30. doi: 10.1086/444601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Badyaev AV, Hill GE, Whittingham LA. Population consequences of maternal effects: sex-bias in egg-laying order facilitates divergence in sexual dimorphism between bird populations. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2002;15:997–1003. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bercovitch FB, Lebron MR, Martinez HS, Kessler MJ. Primigravidity, body weight, and costs of rearing first offspring in rhesus macaques. American Journal of Primatology. 1998;46:135–144. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(1998)46:2<135::AID-AJP3>3.0.CO;2-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bercovitch FB, Widdig A, Berard JD, Nürnberg P, Kessler MJ, Schmidtke J, Trefilov A, Krawczak M. Multiple sirehood in free-ranging twin rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) American Journal of Primatology. 2002;57:31–34. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bercovitch FB, Widdig A, Nurnberg P. Maternal investment in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): reproductive costs and consequences of raising sons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2000;48:1–11. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bercovitch FB, Widdig A, Trefilov A, Kessler MJ, Berard JD, Schmidtke J, Nurnberg P, Krawczak M. A longitudinal study of age-specific reproductive output and body condition among male rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta. Naturwissenschaften. 2003;90:309–312. doi: 10.1007/s00114-003-0436-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Berman CM. Maternal lineages as tools for understanding infant social development and social structure. In: Rawlins RG, Kessler MJ, editors. The Cayo Santiago Macaques: History, Behavior, and Biology. State University of New York Press; Albany, NY: 1986. pp. 73–92. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bethea CL, Centeno ML, Cameron JL. Neurobiology of stress-induced reproductive dysfunction in female macaques. Molecular Neurobiology. 2008;38:199–230. doi: 10.1007/s12035-008-8042-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bhaumik U, Aitken I, Kawachi I, Ringer S, Orav J, Lieberman E. Narrowing of sex differences in infant mortality in Massachusetts. Journal of Perinatology. 2004;24:94–99. doi: 10.1038/sj.jp.7211021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Blomquist GE, University of Illinois . Ph.D. thesis. Urbana: 2007. Quantitative Genetics of Life History Microevolution in the Cayo Santiago Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta) [Google Scholar]
  12. Blomquist GE. Trade-off between age of first reproduction and survival in a female primate. Biology Letters. 2009;5:339–342. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Blomquist GE. Heritability of individual fitness in female macaques. Evolutionary Ecology. 2010;24:657–669. [Google Scholar]
  14. Bolker BM, Brooks ME, Clark CJ, Geange SW, Poulsen JR, Stevens MHH, Simone S, White J. Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 2009;24:127–135. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2008.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Brommer JE, Rattiste K, Wilson A. The rate of ageing in a long-lived bird is not heritable. Heredity. 2010;104:363–370. doi: 10.1038/hdy.2009.125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Charmantier A, Garant D. Environmental quality and evolutionary potential: lessons from wild populations. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B. 2005;272:1415–1425. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cheverud JM. Phenotypic, genetic, and environmental morphological integration in the cranium. Evolution. 1982;36:499–516. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1982.tb05070.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cheverud JM. Evolution by kin selection: A quantitative genetic model illustrated by maternal performance in mice. Evolution. 1984;38:766–777. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1984.tb00349.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cheverud JM, Dittus WPJ. Primate population studies at Polonnaruwa II. heritability of body measurements in a natural population of toque macaques. American Journal of Primatology. 1992;27:145–156. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350270209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cheverud JM, Falk D, Vannier M, Konigsberg L, Helmkamp RC, Hildebolt C. Heritability of brain size and surface features in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) Journal of Heredity. 1990;81:51–57. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.jhered.a110924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cheverud JM, Moore AJ. Quantitative genetics and the role of the environment provided by relatives in behavioral evolution. In: Boake CRB, editor. Quantitative Genetic Studies of Behavioral Evolution. University of Chicago Press; Chicago: 1994. pp. 67–100. [Google Scholar]
  22. Cheverud JM, Wolf JB. The genetics and evolutionary consequences of maternal effects. In: Maestripieri D, Mateo JM, editors. Maternal Effects in Mammals. University of Chicago Press; Chicago: 2009. pp. 11–37. [Google Scholar]
  23. Devinney BJ, Berman CM, Rasmussen KL. Changes in yearling rhesus monkeys’ relationships with their mothers after sibling birth. American Journal of Primatology. 2001;54:193–210. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dufour DL, Sauther ML. Comparative and Evolutionary Dimensions of the Energetics of Human Pregnancy and Lactation. American Journal of Human Biology. 2002;14:584–602. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.10071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gagliardi C, Falkenstein KP, Franke DE, Kubisch HM. Estimates of heritability for reproductive traits in captive rhesus macaque females. American Journal of Primatology. 2010;72:811–819. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Geissmann T. Twinning frequency in catarrhine primates. Human Evolution. 1990;5:387–396. [Google Scholar]
  27. Goodall J. The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behavior. Harvard University Press; Cambridge, MA: 1986. [Google Scholar]
  28. Ha JC, Ha RR, Almasy L, Dyke B. Genetics and caging type affect birth weight in captive pigtailed macaques (Macaca nemestrina) American Journal of Primatology. 2002;56:207–213. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hadfield JD. Mcmc methods for multi-response generalized linear mixed models: The MCMCglmm R package. Journal of Statistical Software. 2010;33:1–22. [Google Scholar]
  30. Hadfield JD, Wilson AJ, Garant D, Sheldon BC, Kruuk LEB. The misuse of BLUP in ecology and evolution. American Naturalist. 2010;175:116–125. doi: 10.1086/648604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Havill LM. Osteon remodeling dynamics in the Cayo Santiago Macaca mulatta: The effect of matriline. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2003;121:354–360. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.10208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hoffman CL, Ayala JE, Mas-Rivera A, Maestripieri D. Effects of reproductive condition and dominance rank on cortisol responsiveness to stress in free-ranging female rhesus macaques. American Journal of Primatology. 2010;72:559–565. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20793. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hrdy SB. Mother Nature: A History of Mothers, Infants, and Natural Selection. Pantheon Books; New York: 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Jaquish CE, Cheverud JM, Tardif SD. Genetic and environmental impacts on litter size and early infant survival in three species of callitrichids. Journal of Heredity. 1996;87:74–77. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.jhered.a022959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Jones JH. Fetal programming: adaptive life-history tactics or making the best of a bad start? American Journal of Human Biology. 2005;17:22–33. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.20099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Jones JH, Wilson ML, Murray C, Pusey A. Phenotypic quality influences fertility in Gombe chimpanzees. Journal of Animal Ecology. 2010;79:1262–1269. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2010.01687.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kaplan EL, Meier P. Nonparametric estimation from incomplete observations. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1958;53:457–481. [Google Scholar]
  38. Koh C, Bates E, Broughton E, Do NT, Fletcher Z, Mahaney MC, Hlusko LJ. Genetic integration of molar cusp size variation in baboons. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2010;142:246–260. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.21221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Koyama N, Takahata Y, Huffman MA, Norikoshi K, Suzuki H. Reproductive parameters of female Japanese macaques: Thirty years data from the Arashiyama troops, Japan. Primates. 1992;33:33–47. [Google Scholar]
  40. Kruuk LEB. Estimating genetic parameters in natural populations using the ‘animal model’. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B. 2004;359:873–890. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2003.1437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kruuk LEB, Slate J, Wilson AJ. New answers for old questions: The evolutionary quantitative genetics of wild animal populations. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 2008;39:525–548. [Google Scholar]
  42. Kuzawa CW, Quinn EA. Developmental origins of adult function and health: Evolutionary hypotheses. Annual Review of Anthropology. 2009;38:131–147. [Google Scholar]
  43. Lindström J. Early development and fitness in birds and mammals. Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 1999;9:343–348. doi: 10.1016/s0169-5347(99)01639-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lummaa V, Clutton-Brock T. Early development, survival and reproduction in humans. Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 2002;17:141–147. [Google Scholar]
  45. Lynch M, Walsh B. Genetics and Analysis of Quantitative Traits. Sinauer Associates, Inc.; Sunderland, MA: 1998. [Google Scholar]
  46. Maestripieri D. Maternal influences on offspring growth, reproduction, and behavior in primates. In: Maestripieri D, Mateo JM, editors. Maternal Effects in Mammals. University of Chicago Press; Chicago: 2009. pp. 256–291. [Google Scholar]
  47. Maestripieri D, Hoffman CL. Behavior and social dynamics of rhesus macaques on Cayo Santiago. In: Wang Q, editor. Bones, Genetics, and Behavior of Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta): Cayo Santiago and Beyond. Springer; New York: 2012. pp. 247–262. [Google Scholar]
  48. Marshall DJ, Uller T. When is a maternal effect adaptive? Oikos. 2007;116:1957–1963. [Google Scholar]
  49. Martin LJ, Mahaney MC, Bronikowski AM, Dee CK, Dyke B, Comuzzie AG. Lifespan in captive baboons is heritable. Mechanisms of Ageing and Development. 2002;123:1461–1467. doi: 10.1016/s0047-6374(02)00083-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Morris WF, Altmann J, Brockman DK, Cords M, Fedigan LM, Pusey AE, Stoinski TS, Bronikowski AM, Alberts SC, Strier KB. Low demographic variability in wild primate populations: Fitness impacts of variation, covariation, and serial correlation in vital rates. American Naturalist. 2011;177:E14–E28. doi: 10.1086/657443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Mousseau TA, Fox CW. The adaptive significance of maternal effects. Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 1998;13:403–407. doi: 10.1016/s0169-5347(98)01472-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H. Repeatability for Gaussian and non-Gaussian data: a practical guide for biologists. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society. 2010;85:935–956. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00141.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Nickerson SC. Milk production: Factors affecting milk composition. In: Harding F, editor. Milk Quality. Blackie Academic and Professional; London: 1995. pp. 3–24. [Google Scholar]
  54. O’Hara RB, Cano JM, Ovaskainen O, Teplitsky C, Ahlo JS. Bayesian approaches in evolutionary quantitative genetics. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2008;21:949–957. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01529.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Pemberton JM. Wild pedigrees: the way forward. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B. 2008;275:613–621. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.1531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Pettay JI, Kruuk LEB, Jokela J, Lummaa V. Heritability and genetic constraints of life-history trait evolution in preindustrial humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2005;102:2838–2843. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0406709102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Postma E, Heinrich F, Koller U, Sardell RJ, Reid JM, Arcese P, Keller LF. Disentangling the effect of genes, the environment and chance on sex ratio variation in a wild bird population. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B. 2011;278:2996–3002. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Price T, Schluter D. On the low heritability of life history traits. Evolution. 1991;45:853–861. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1991.tb04354.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Quiatt D. Aunts and mothers: Adaptive implications of allomaternal behavior of nonhuman primates. American Anthropologist. 1979;81:310–319. [Google Scholar]
  60. R Development Core Team . R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing; Vienna, Austria: 2012. [Google Scholar]
  61. Ralls K, Ballou J. Effects of inbreeding on infant mortality in captive primates. International Journal of Primatology. 1982;3:491–505. [Google Scholar]
  62. Räsänen K, Kruuk LEB. Maternal effects and evolution at ecological time scales. Functional Ecology. 2007;21:408–421. [Google Scholar]
  63. Rawlins RG, Kessler MJ. Climate and seasonal reproduction in the Cayo Santiago macaques. American Journal of Primatology. 1985;9:87–99. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350090203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Rawlins RG, Kessler MJ, editors. The Cayo Santiago Macaques: History, Behavior, and Biology. SUNY Press; Albany: 1986. [Google Scholar]
  65. Robbins AM, Robbins MM, Gerald-Steklis N, Steklis HD. Age-related patterns of reproductive success among female mountain gorillas. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2006;131:511–521. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.20474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Roff DA. Evolutionary Quantitative Genetics. Chapman and Hall; New York: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  67. Roff DA. A centennial celebration for quantitative genetics. Evolution. 2007;61:1017–1032. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00100.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Roseman CC, Willmore KE, Rogers J, Hildebolt C, Sadler BE, Richtsmeier JT, Cheverud JM. Genetic and environmental contributions to variation in baboon cranial morphology. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2010;143:1–12. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.21341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Rossiter MC. Incidence and consequences of inherited environmental effects. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 1996;27:451–476. [Google Scholar]
  70. Sade DS, Chepko-Sade BD, Schneider JM, Roberts SS, Richtsmeier JT. Basic Demographic Observations on Free-Ranging Rhesus Monkeys. Human Relations Area Files; New Haven, CT: 1985. [Google Scholar]
  71. Schino G. Birth sex ratio and social rank: consistency and variability within and between primate groups. Behavioral Ecology. 2004;15:850–856. [Google Scholar]
  72. Solomon NG, French JA, editors. Cooperative Breeding in Mammals. Cambridge University Press; New York: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  73. Southey BR, Rodriguez-Zas SL, Leymaster KA. Discrete time survival analysis of lamb mortality in a terminal sire composite population. Journal of Animal Science. 2003;81:1399–1405. doi: 10.2527/2003.8161399x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Suomi SJ. Mother-infant attachment, peer relationships, and the development of social networks in rhesus monkeys. Human Development. 2005;48:67–79. [Google Scholar]
  75. Thompson ME, Muller MN, Wrangham RW, Lwanga JS, Potts KB. Urinary C-peptide tracks seasonal and individual variation in energy balance in wild chimpanzees. Hormones and Behavior. 2009;55:299–305. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Van Coillie S, Galbusera P, Roeder AD, Schempp W, Stevens JMG, Leus K, Reinartz G, Pereboom Z. Molecular paternity determination in captive bonobos and the impact of inbreeding on infant mortality. Animal Conservation. 2008;11:306–312. [Google Scholar]
  77. Visscher PM, Hill WG, Wray NR. Heritability in the genomics era—concepts and misconceptions. Nature Reviews Genetics. 2008;9:255–266. doi: 10.1038/nrg2322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Wells JCK. The thrifty phenotype as an adaptive maternal effect. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society. 2007;82:143–172. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2006.00007.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Williams-Blangero S, Blangero J. Heritability of age of first birth in captive olive baboons. American Journal of Primatology. 1995;37:233–239. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350370305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Wilson AJ. Why h2 does not always equal VA/VP? Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2008;21:647–650. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01500.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Wilson AJ, Nussey DH, Pemberton JM, Pilkington JG, Morris A, Pelletier F, Clutton-Brock TH, Kruuk LEB. Evidence for a genetic basis of ageing in two wild vertebrate populations. Current Biology. 2007;17:2136–2142. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Wilson AJ, Réale D. Ontogeny of additive and maternal genetic effects: Lessons from domestic mammals. American Naturalist. 2006;167:E23–E38. doi: 10.1086/498138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Wilson AJ, Reale D, Clements MN, Morrissey MM, Postma E, Walling CA, Kruuk LEB, Nussey DH. An ecologist’s guide to the animal model. Journal of Animal Ecology. 2010;79:13–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2009.01639.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Wolf JB, Wade MJ. What are maternal effects (and what are they not)? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B. 2009;364:1107–1115. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES