Abstract
Biological parents pass on genotypes to their children, as well as provide home environments that correlate with their genotypes; thus, the association between the home environment and children's temperament can be genetically (i.e. passive gene-environment correlation) or environmentally mediated. Furthermore, family environments may suppress or facilitate the heritability of children's temperament (i.e. gene-environment interaction). The sample comprised 807 twin pairs (M age = 7.93 years) from the longitudinal Wisconsin Twin Project. Important passive gene-environment correlations emerged, such that home environments were less chaotic for children with high Effortful Control, and this association was genetically mediated. Children with high Extraversion/Surgency experienced more chaotic home environments, and this correlation was also genetically mediated. In addition, heritability of children's temperament was moderated by home environments, such that Effortful Control and Extraversion/Surgency were more heritable in chaotic homes, and Negative Affectivity was more heritable under crowded or unsafe home conditions. Modeling multiple types of gene-environment interplay uncovered the complex role of genetic factors and the hidden importance of the family environment for children's temperament and development more generally.
The interplay of genes and environment on children's development is complex and dynamic. Accurately depicting these relations, both conceptually and quantitatively, is essential to elucidating influences on individual differences in development generally, and differences in risk for psychopathology more specifically. The complexities of the interplay extend across multiple environmental levels from the cellular to the macroenvironment (Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, 1994; Wachs, 2010). Here, we simultaneously examine both linear and nonlinear contributions of the family environment to children's temperament in middle childhood. In doing so, we elucidate aspects of the environment that suppress or facilitate the development of temperament. With biometric modeling, we identify passive gene-environment correlation (rGE), such that the relation between the home environment and children's temperament is genetically mediated. We also identify a type of gene by environment interaction (GxE); more specifically, we show that the family environment moderates the heritability of temperament.
The Heritability of Temperament
Temperament constitutes early emerging affective and motivational components of behavior that changes over time and impacts many facets of development. Its associations with later social relationships, academic competence, and psychopathology mark temperament as an important construct. Despite its importance in understanding child development across multiple domains, much remains to be understood about the genetic and environmental underpinnings of temperament. Definitions of temperament, including Rothbart and Bates' (1998, p.109) “constitutionally based individual differences in emotional, motor, and attentional reactivity and self-regulation,” imply that temperament has a strong genetic component. Twin studies that have actually tested the heritability (the proportion of phenotypic variation due to genetic variation) of temperament primarily via parent report have supported a genetic basis to most dimensions (e.g. Gagne & Goldsmith, 2011, Goldsmith, Buss, & Lemery, 1997; Hur, 2009; Lemery, Doelger & Goldsmith, 2008; Rhee, Cosgrove, Schmitz, Haberstick, Corley & Hewitt, 2007; Saudino, 2005; Schmitz, Saudino, Plomin, Fulker, & DeFries, 1996).
Classic quantitative biometric modeling of twins is based on the comparison of similarities between monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) cotwins (Neale & Cardon, 1992). MZ cotwins growing up in the same home share 100% of their genomic DNA and their common environment; any differences between them can be attributed to the nonshared environment. DZ cotwins share 50% of their genomic DNA, so differences between them are due to both genes and nonshared environments (E). Thus, MZ twins should be approximately twice as similar as DZ twins if additive genetic effects (A) contribute to individual differences. If the common environment (C) is important; however, MZ twins are less than twice as similar as DZ twins because common environmental influences, by definition, act on both types of twins in similar ways. In this way, the classic ACE model quantifies heritable and environmental contributions to variance in traits.
Heritability estimates resulting from these biometric models vary across dimensions of temperament and perhaps environmental circumstance (Lemery-Chalfant, 2010; Saudino, 2005). Heritability estimates range broadly from about .20 to about .80 for childhood temperament (Saudino, 2005); Rothbart and colleagues' (2001) higher order factors of temperament are similarly heritable: .58 for Effortful Control, .42 for Negative Affectivity, and .41 for Extraversion/Surgency (Goldsmith et al., 1997). These heritability estimates are subject to random sampling error, influences of assessment instruments, population-specific distributions of genotypes and environments, variations in mating patterns, developmental perturbations, and variations in the degree to which other basic assumptions of the underlying quantitative genetic model are met.
The association between temperament and symptoms of mood and behavioral disorders in children are largely accounted for by shared genetic factors (Lemery-Chalfant, Clifford & Swann, in press). Negative correlations between effortful control at five years of age and internalizing and externalizing symptoms at eight years of age were genetically mediated (Lemery-Chalfant, Doelger & Goldsmith, 2008). Thus, the genetic influence on children's symptoms of psychopathology may partially operate by protecting children from disorder. Negative affectivity is positively associated with symptoms, with a common genetic factor accounting for the association between negative affectivity in 7–12 year olds and both attention problems and aggressive behaviors two years later (Gjone & Stevenson, 1997). A similar pattern was reported with preadolescents; genetic covariance between negative emotionality and oppositional defiant and conduct disorder symptoms represented 30–50% of the variance in symptoms, respectively, although nonshared environment also contributed to the covariances (Singh & Waldman, 2010). With an adoption design, the association between negative emotionality and internalizing-externalizing symptoms was genetically mediated (Schmitz & Saudino, 2003). Thus, genetic influences on temperament likely also impact child psychopathology.
Contrasting Views of the Importance of the Common Family Environment
Classic biometric modeling of independent ACE contributions to trait variance has generally not supported a significant role of the common environment for temperament and related constructs (e.g. Kendler et al., 1996; McCall, 1991; Mullineaux, Deater-Deckard, Petrill, Thompson, & DeThorne, 2009; Plomin, Defries, McClearn, & McGuffin, 2001; Saudino, 2005; Saudino & Cherny, 2001; Slutske et al., 1997), with the reduced AE model often best representing the variance. As a result, several theorists have stressed that family environments within the normal range are not important for child development (Bouchard, 2004; Harris, 1998; Rowe, 1994; Scarr, 1992). Instead, shared genes account for similarities between individuals and account for approximately 50% of the variance, and nonshared environments account for differences between individuals and the remaining variance. Turkheimer (2000) labeled the lack of systematic, common environment effects the “gloomy prospect” and suggested that important environmental contributions to behavior and development are random and unsystematic and thus not conducive to study.
Substantial non-behavioral genetic research shows consistent associations between the family environment and children's temperament. Aspects of the home environment, such as level of chaos and quality of the physical environment, are consistently associated with child outcomes when not investigated using genetically-informed designs, making nonsignificant findings within the field of behavior genetics suspect (e.g. Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2000; Pettit, Bates, & Dodge, 1997). The point of reconciliation would be that “home” environment is not typically experienced as “common” (Goldsmith, 1988).
The conceptual framework of facilitative versus determinative environments provides an interpretative context. A facilitative (permissive) environment places few constraints on the child's development. A determinative environment, on the other hand, either constrains the genetically influenced pathways of development or leverages them beyond their expected expression (Baumrind, 1993; Bradley, 2006; Novak & Peleaz, 2003; Sroufe, 1997). For example, a chaotic, unstructured home typically does not support development of foundational skills, such as self-regulation and self-efficacy. Similarly, Wachs (1988) suggested that the physical environment plays a role in the expression of temperament such that the right conditions must be present for the expression of any given dimension of temperament. Thus, environmental affordances affect the expression of heritable traits, which is perhaps an obvious point but one that is worth emphasizing in this context.
Two aspects of the home environment are the focus of our investigation: (a) general levels of home chaos, such as high levels of noise, confusion, and disorganization; and (b) the physical home, including the layout and size of the living environment, the safety, and the availability of basic necessities and materials to encourage learning and growth.
Chaotic homes, which provide little structure or routine, are associated with children's poor self-regulatory and cognitive skills, aggression, impulsivity, and internalizing and externalizing problems (e.g. Dumas, Nissley, Nordstrom, Smith, & Levine, 2005; Evans, Wells, & Moch, 2003; Valiente, Lemery-Chalfant, & Reiser, 2007; Wachs, 2000). Children raised in highly chaotic homes demonstrate increased behavioral problems over and above the effects of bad parenting and are more likely to engage in externalizing behaviors (Coldwell, Pike, & Dunn, 2006; Prevatt, 2003). Home chaos also mediates the relation between socioeconomic status (SES) and components of socioemotional development, including self regulation (Evans, Gonnella, Marcynyszyn, Gentile & Salpekar, 2005).
Relations between the physical home environment and temperament have also been reported, although the literature is more limited. Elements in the home contributing to the ambiance have been associated with children's temperament, outcomes in psychopathology, and development in general (Bradley, 1993; Evans, 2006; Matheny, Wilson, & Thoben, 1987; Wachs, 1988). Children's development of fine and gross-motor skills is linked with the number of toys in the home (Abbott & Bartlett, 2001). Children in overcrowded homes receive less parental attention (Liddell & Kruger, 1989), and score higher on neuroticism (Matheny & Phillips, 2001; Murray, 1974) and externalizing behaviors (Supplee, Unikel & Shaw, 2007).
Integrating Contrasting Literatures by Modeling Gene-Environment Interplay
Many developmental psychologists traditionally adhered to the idea that chaotic and overcrowded and unsafe environments crucially impact maladaptive child outcomes, given that children are less able to develop optimally in the face of constant disruptions and confusion. In contrast, behavior geneticists have shown that correlations between measured family environments and child traits are often genetically mediated, through the process of rGE, that is, genetic differences are associated with exposure to different environments. In fact, many well known “environmental” variables demonstrate moderate heritability (Kendler & Baker, 2007; Plomin et al., 2001). Passive rGE occurs when heritable traits of the parents influence the family environment, such that biological parents pass on genotypes to their children, as well as an environment that correlates with the genotype. For example, parents with poor self regulation skills pass on a genotype that does not support the development of adaptive self regulation in their children, and these same parents likely provide a more chaotic home environment with fewer routines and little predictability. The resulting association between the home environment and children's temperament is ambiguous in terms of whether it is genetically (passive rGE) or environmentally mediated.
These two literatures can be integrated by modeling more complex nonlinear relations between measured aspects of the common environment and genetic influences on temperamental traits. A new generation of quantitative biometric modeling is available that estimates GxE and rGE as well as the main effects of genes and environments that have been studied using the classic ACE models. Specifically, Purcell (2002) expanded the classic models to incorporate both family-level and individual-level moderation of the ACE pathways, with the estimation of rGE also possible with an individual-level moderator. Price and Jaffee (2008) then proposed a model that allows for the estimation of passive rGE with a family-level moderator in the case where significant moderation of heritability is present.
Although these models have not yet been used with temperament, rGE were found between adolescent attitudes about their relationship with their parents (measured at the individual level) and their negativity and positivity personality factors (Krueger, South, Johnson, & Iacono, 2008), which often are interpreted in hedonic temperamental terms. Simultaneously, parent-child relationships significantly moderated the heritability of personality, such that heritability was higher for positivity when regard for parents was higher, and heritability was higher for negativity when regard and conflict were higher. Similarly, rGE were reported between income and adult internalizing problems, interpreted as social selection, or active rGE (South & Krueger, 2011). Income simultaneously moderated the heritability of internalizing problem behaviors, with heritability higher with higher income, interpreted as social causation.
The Present Study
We aimed to uncover the role of measured aspects of the common family environment on children's temperament using the twin design to integrate the developmental psychology literature that emphasizes the importance of the home environment, with the behavior genetic literature that does not support an important role of the common family environment on children's traits and development. Using contemporary biometric modeling, we aimed to elucidate passive rGE, such that the correlatons between measured aspects of the home environment and children's temperament are genetically mediated, and GxE, or family environment moderation of the heritability of temperament. Temperament was assessed with both mother and father report of Effortful Control, Negative Affectivity, and Surgency, and measured aspects of the home environment included mother report of home chaos, and experimenter observation of the physical home environment. We hypothesized that higher chaos and poorer quality physical environments would be both correlated with, and moderate the heritability of children's temperament, based on the facilitative versus determinative environments conceptual framework.
Methods
Participants
The sample consisted of 807 twin pairs participating in the longitudinal Wisconsin Twin Project, with 51% boys and mean age of 7.93 years (SD=0.87). There were 301 MZ and 263 same sex DZ, and 243 opposite sex DZ twin pairs. Methods of recruitment from state birth records have previously been described in depth (see Lemery-Chalfant, Goldsmith, Schmidt, Arneson, & Van Hulle, 2006). Ethnicity was 88.5% Caucasian, 4.1% African American, and 5.8% other or mixed. Parent education ranged from no formal education to a graduate degree, with a majority of parents having completed some or all of college (M mothers = 14.91 years; M fathers = 14.45 years of schooling). Family income ranged from unemployed to $200,000+ a year with the average family earning $51,000–$70,000.
Procedure
Mothers completed demographic information and the zygosity questionnaire via a phone interview. Around three to four months later (M=3.72, SD=4.77), mothers participated in another phone interview during which they completed Rothbart's Children's Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ) and the Confusion, Hubbub, and Order Scale (CHAOS). At this time, the father also completed the CBQ over the telephone. In addition, families participated in a four-hour home visit, which involved a number of assessments. Child testers completed the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME) during and immediately after the home visit.
Measures
Parents provided demographic information and zygosity was determined through agreement between parent and observer ratings. Mothers completed the Zygosity Questionnaire for Young Twins (Goldsmith, 1991), which measures physical similarities. The agreement of this questionnaire with genotyping is greater than 95% (Forget-Dubois et al., 2003; Price, Freeman, Craig, Petrill, Ebersole & Plomin, 2000). Observers also completed questions concerning zygosity after a home-visit. For six of the twin pairs in this sample, parents and observers did not agree and these pairs were omitted from genetic analyses.
Socio-economic status (SES)
A mean composite was formed from standardized years of mother education, father education, and total family income (r's = .45–.55, p<.0001).
Children's Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ; Rothbart et al., 2001)
The CBQ consisted of 180 questions scored on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from “extremely untrue of your child” to “extremely true of your child” over the past six months. Sample questions include “My child is full of energy, even in the evening,” “My child will move from one task to another without completing any of them,” and “My child is afraid of loud noises.” Internal consistency for the CBQ scales ranged from .69 (Sadness) to .89 (Shyness), averaging .73 across all scales used to form the superordinate factors used in model fitting. Preliminary analyses showed that mother-report and father-report CBQ were moderately correlated (r's=.41–.65, p<.0001), thus, mean scale composites across reporter were formed. Based on Rothbart et al. (2001), scales were further composited into three higher order factors – Effortful Control, Negative Affectivity, and Extraversion/Surgency. Effortful control is a mean composite of Inhibitory Control and Attentional Focusing (r = .72, p<.0001), Negative Affectivity is a mean composite of Distress to Novelty, Distress to Limitations, Sadness, and lack of Soothability (r's = .24–.59, p<.0001), and Extraversion/Surgency is a mean composite of Activity Level, lack of Shyness, Impulsivity, Smiling and Laughter, and Approach (r's = .15–.66, p<.0001).
Confusion, Hubbub, and Order Scale (CHAOS; Matheny, Wachs, Ludwig, & Phillips, 1995)
This mother report questionnaire consisted of 15 true/false statements asking participants to think about how each item described their home. Sample questions include, “It's a real `zoo' at our home,” “Our home is a good place to relax (reverse scored),” “First thing in the day, we have a regular routine at home (reverse scored),” and “We almost always seem to be rushed.” The global mean was computed such that higher scores indicated higher chaos, with an internal consistency of .80.
Living Environment Observation Scale (LEOS)
Adapted from Caldwell and Bradley's (1984) Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME), observers completed this scale immediately after visiting the home, rating nine dimensions of the home on a scale of one to three, where 1=no/minimal evidence, 2=moderate evidence, and 3=substantial evidence. The nine dimensions were: structural safety of the home, home décor, child-friendly home, adequate living space for number of individuals in the home, interpersonal space, overall organization, cleanliness, outside play environment, and condition of street where the twins live. A sample item (Overall Organization) includes “This item reflects the overall physical organization of the house: (1) Home is cluttered making it difficult to walk around objects, unable to find a clear space to do assessments activities, (2) Home is generally clean though floors may need to be vacuumed or washed; noticeable dust on furniture, and (3) Home is clean and appears to have been cleaned recently or on a regular basis.” The global mean was computed such that higher scores indicated a higher quality physical environment, with an internal consistency of .84. The LEOS was transformed for negative skewness (−2.079 to −1.260; SE = .066) and kurtosis (4.650 to .430; SE = .132) by taking an inverse transformation (1/x).
Although the LEOS and CHAOS were correlated (r = −.22, p <.001), they were analyzed separately because they measure fundamentally different aspects of the home environment.
Statistical Approach
Two independent samples were formed for use with statistical tests that assume independent observations. The Twin 1 subsample was created by randomly selecting one twin from each pair. The Twin 2 subsample included the co-twins of the Twin 1 subsample. Correlations and t-tests were tested separately with the Twin 1 and Twin 2 subsamples. Average correlations (using Fisher's Z transformation) across the two samples are reported. We also report twin intraclass correlations.
Age, age-squared, and sex residualized scores were used for twin modeling to account for any age and sex effects on ACE estimates, as is standard practice to reduce potential biases when it is infeasible to incorporate additional covariates (McGue & Bouchard, 1984). Using structural equation modeling, full univariate biometric models decompose the variance into three components: additive genetic (A; the sum of the average effects of individual genes across the genotype), common environment (C; aspects of the environment that make twins growing up in the same home similar to one another), and non-shared environment (E; aspects of the environment that make twins dissimilar to one another as well as measurement error). C is completely shared between co-twins growing up in the same home, and E is completely independent across individuals. The latent A influence is correlated between cotwins and equals 1.00 for MZ twins, because they share 100% of their genomic DNA, and equals 0.50 for DZ twins, because they share on average 50% of their genomic DNA. Reduced models of the form AE, CE, or E are compared to the full ACE model. E can never be dropped because it also contains measurement error. MZ twins are more similar to each other than DZ twins for heritable traits. If shared environment is significant, however, MZ twins are less than twice as similar as DZ twins because these influences act on both types of twins in similar ways.
Price and Jaffee (2008) introduce a classical twin ACE model that estimates passive rGE in the presence of significant family-level moderation of heritability (GxE). The existence of GxE allows the main effect of the measured family environment to be distinguished from passive rGE; thus, this model can only be used when it is first established that there is significant moderation of the latent factor A by the measured family environment. Power to distinguish between a main effect of the family environment and passive rGE increases as the magnitude of A and moderation on A increases.
Specifically, the variance in the phenotype is partitioned into uncorrelated within and between family variables. Between family differences in the phenotype are then predicted by a measured aspect of the family environment, a measure that differs between, but not within families. The within family phenotypic variance is not affected by the measured family environment; with passive rGE operating the same way for both MZ and DZ cotwins. The measured environment is modeled as a random effect and correlations with the latent factor A are estimated to index passive rGE (a linear function of the measured environment), while simultaneously estimating the measured environment's main effect on the phenotype (constant with respect to the measured environment).
A nonsignificant chi-square difference test and a stronger negative Akaike's Information Criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1987) indicate more parsimonious fit of a reduced model over the full model. The statistical program Mx (Neale, Boker, Xie & Maes, 2003) was used to fit the models.
Results
Descriptive Statistics and First Order Associations
The phenotypic correlations, means and standard deviations for all variables are given in Table 1. Effortful Control was moderately negatively correlated with the other two factors, whereas Extraversion/Surgency and Negative Affectivity were largely independent. CHAOS and LEOS largely tapped independent aspects of the home environment. The CBQ factors were low to moderately correlated with CHAOS and LEOS in expected directions. Girls were higher on Effortful Control, t(393) = 5.40, p<.01; M girls = 4.78; M boys = 4.48, and boys were higher on Extraversion/Surgency, t(389) = -6.70, p<.01; M boys = 4.89; M girls = 4.65. Table 1 also gives twin intraclass correlations. Twin correlations support high heritability of these temperament factors, with MZ cotwins more similar than DZ cotwins. In addition, twin correlations computed separately for male MZ, female MZ, same-sex male DZ, same-sex female DZ, and opposite sex DZ pairs support nonsignificant sex differences.
Table 1.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | MZm | MZf | DZm | DZf | DZos | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. CBQ – Effortful Control | .72 | .65 | .13 | .06 | .02 | |||||
2. CBQ – Negative Affectivity | −.494* | .83 | .79 | .43 | .44 | .43 | ||||
3. CBQ – Extraversion/Surgency | −.378* | .115* | .69 | .62 | −.12 | −.11 | −.03 | |||
4. CHAOS | −.323* | .273* | .184* | |||||||
5. LEOS | .173* | −.136* | −.100* | −.224* | ||||||
Mean | 4.630 | 4.138 | 4.770 | 4.16 | 1.462 | |||||
Range | 1.86–6.70 | 1.62–6.29 | 2.63–6.58 | 0–13 | 1.12–1.75 | |||||
Standard Deviation | .766 | .587 | .510 | 3.112 | .155 |
Note. MZm = monozygotic male; MZf = monozygotic female; DZm = dizygotic male; DZf = dizygotic female; DZos = dizygotic opposite sex twin pairs. CBQ = Children's Behavior Questionnaire; CHAOS = Confusion, Hubbub, and Order Scale; LEOS = Living Environment Observation Scale (higher scores indicate less crowded and more safe conditions). Correlations are mean correlations of Twin 1 and Twin 2 subsamples.
p<.01.
We next fit saturated models to the temperament composites to test assumptions of the twin design and provide a more formal test of sex differences.
For Effortful Control, we fit the saturated model where means, variances, and covariances were estimated separately across the sex and zygosity groups, yielding a fit of −2LL(1545) = 3274.07, AIC = 184.07. Next, a series of nested models were fit. First, we determined that means could not be equated a) between co-twins within zygosity group (χ2Δ(3) = 13.46, p = 0.004, AIC = 7.46), b) between MZ and DZ zygosity groups (χ2Δ(5) = 24.97, p <.001, AIC = 14.97), and c) between sexes (χ2Δ(1) = 24.38, p <.001, AIC = 22.38). However, variances and covariances could be equated across sex and zygosity groups. Specifically, we found that variances could be equated a) between co-twins within zygosity group (χ2Δ(5) = 13.84, p = 0.02, AIC = 3.84), b) between MZ and DZ zygosity groups (χ2Δ(6) = 15.33, p = 0.02, AIC = 3.33), and c) between sexes (χ2Δ(4) = 9.19, p = 0.06, AIC = 1.19). Furthermore, covariances could be equated across sexes (χ2Δ(3) = 2.42, p = 0.49, AIC = −3.58). Because variances and covariances were equivalent across zygosity groups, there is no evidence of sibling interaction effects with the Effortful Control factor (Neale & Cardon, 1992). Competition or contrast effects are distinguished from genetic effects because they result in higher total phenotypic variance in DZ than in MZ twins.
For Negative Affectivity, we fit the saturated model where means, variances, and covariances were estimated separately across the sex and zygosity groups, yielding a fit of −2LL(1545) = 2327.93, AIC = −762.08. We then fit a series of nested models. First, we determined that means could be equated a) between co-twins within zygosity group (χ2Δ(3) = 1.15, p = 0.77, AIC = −4.85), b) between MZ and DZ zygosity groups (χ2Δ(5) = 12.35, p = 0.03, AIC = 2.35), and c) between sexes (χ2Δ(1) = 0.29, p = 0.59, AIC = −1.72). Next, we found that variances could also be equated a) between co-twins within zygosity group (χ2Δ(5) = 11.67, p = 0.04, AIC = 1.67), b) between MZ and DZ zygosity groups (χ2Δ(6) = 7.30, p = 0.29, AIC = −4.70), and c) between sexes (χ2Δ(4) = 5.24, p = 0.26, AIC = −2.76). Furthermore, covariances could be equated across sexes (χ2Δ(3) = 1.73, p = 0.63, AIC = −4.27).
Similarly, for Extraversion/Surgency, we fit the saturated model where means, variances, and covariances were estimated separately across the sex and zygosity groups, yielding a fit of −2LL(1545) = 2047.93, AIC = −1042.07. Next, we systematically equated means, variances, and covariances across groups. We determined that means could be equated a) between co-twins within zygosity group (χ2Δ(3) = 4.14, p = 0.25, AIC = −1.86), and b) between MZ and DZ zygosity groups (χ2Δ(5) = 7.09, p = 0.21, AIC = −2.91). However, means could not be equated between sexes (χ2Δ(1) = 54.42, p <.001, AIC = 52.42). Next, we found that variances could also be equated a) between co-twins within zygosity group (χ2Δ(5) = 4.22, p = 0.52, AIC = −5.78), b) between MZ and DZ zygosity groups (χ2Δ(6) = 9.85, p = 0.13, AIC = −2.16), and c) between sexes (χ2Δ(4) = 13.20, p = 0.01, AIC = 5.20). Furthermore, covariances could be equated across sexes (χ2Δ(3) = 1.76, p = 0.62, AIC = −4.24).
In summary, we found some mean differences across groups for Effortful Control and Extraversion/Surgency, although all variances and covariances were equivalent across groups, indicating that it is not necessary to model competition or contrast effects. Before fitting the following biometric models, we first regressed out the influence of sex, age, and age squared, as is standard practice when multiple covariates cannot be incorporated into the model.
Classic Biometric ACE Modeling of Temperament
We began with the ACE full model (which allows for common environmental influences), then systematically dropped A and C to test the significance of each influence; E is always included because it includes measurement error. Table 2 summarizes the fit statistics for each model, as well as the ACE estimates for the full models and the (bolded) most parsimonious reduced model. For all three temperament factors, the best fitting model was the AE, with 49–79% of the variance due to heritability. Thus, classic twin modeling focused on independent main effects of genetic and environmental influences indicated that all three components of temperament were moderately to highly heritable, with no significant effects of the common environment.
Table 2.
Temperament Dimension | Model | −2LL | df | AIC | Δdf | Δ χ 2 | p | h2 | c2 | e2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Effortful Control | ACE | 1804.69 | 2244 | −2679.31 | - | - | - | .54 | .00 | .46 |
AE | 1804.69 | 2243 | −2679.31 | 1 | 0.00 | .99 | .54 | - | .46 | |
CE | 1856.16 | 2243 | −2629.84 | 1 | 51.47 | <.01 | - | .20 | .80 | |
E | 1887.27 | 2244 | −2600.73 | 2 | 82.59 | <.01 | - | - | 1.00 | |
Negative Affectivity | ACE | 877.02 | 2242 | −3606.98 | - | - | - | .79 | .00 | .21 |
AE | 877.02 | 2243 | −3608.98 | 1 | 0.00 | .99 | .79 | - | .21 | |
CE | 984.00 | 2243 | −3502.00 | 1 | 106.99 | <.01 | - | .52 | .48 | |
E | 1220.94 | 2244 | −3267.06 | 2 | 343.92 | <.01 | - | - | 1.00 | |
Extraversion/Surgency | ACE | 713.03 | 2242 | −3770.97 | - | - | - | .49 | .00 | .51 |
AE | 713.03 | 2243 | −3772.97 | 1 | 0.00 | .99 | .49 | - | .51 | |
CE | 762.03 | 2243 | −3723.97 | 1 | 50.00 | <.01 | - | .15 | .85 | |
E | 778.01 | 2244 | −3710.00 | 2 | 64.98 | <.01 | - | - | 1.00 |
Note. A = additive genetic; C = shared environment; E = nonshared environment; −2LL = −2 times the log likelihood; df = degrees of freedom; AIC = Akaike's Information Criterion; Δdf = change in degrees of freedom; Δχ2 = change in chi-square value from the best fitting full model to reduced model; p = probability; h2 = additive genetic; c2 = common environment; and e2 = nonshared environment. The most parsimonious model is in bold.
Contemporary Biometric Modeling of Gene-Environment Interplay on Temperament
Based on Price and Jaffee (2008; see Method), we began by testing whether or not heritability was moderated by our measured aspects of the family environment (GxE). If GxE was significant, we then modeled rGE. We began with the full moderated ACE model (i.e., the measured family environment is estimated to moderate the A, C, and E parameters), then dropped nonsignificant parameters to reach the most parsimonious model. For each of the three temperament factors, C was nonsignficant and was dropped from the model. Only AE models are presented for simplicity.
Effortful Control
A model allowing CHAOS moderation of the A and E paths on Effortful Control fit significantly better than the AE model with no moderation (see Table 3). Across levels of CHAOS, the estimates of A and E changed such that estimates of A increased from 76% (minus 1 SD) to 82% (plus 1 SD), indicating that children's Effortful Control was modestly more heritable under chaotic home conditions. The overall variance in Effortful Control was also higher under chaotic home conditions (.361 at plus 1 SD; .289 at minus 1 SD for the between-family variable). Passive rGE was also significant, rGE = −.69, supporting genetic mediation of the covariance between CHAOS and children's Effortful Control. A model allowing LEOS moderation of the A and E paths on Effortful Control was not more parsimonious than the classic AE model (Table 3); thus, estimates of A (60%), and E (40%) remained constant across levels of LEOS, and the model estimating rGE was not fit.
Table 3.
Temperament Dimension | Model with Moderator | −2LL | df | AIC | Δdf | Δ χ 2 | p | rGE |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Effortful Control | CHAOS | |||||||
AE full moderation, with rGE | 1730.90 | 2240 | −2749.10 | - | - | - | −.69 | |
AE full moderation, no rGE | 1780.84 | 2241 | −2701.55 | 1 | 49.55 | <.01 | - | |
No moderation, no rGE | 1804.69 | 2243 | −2681.31 | 2 | 73.79 | <.01 | - | |
LEOS | ||||||||
AE full moderation, no rGE | 1239.91 | 1971 | −2702.09 | - | - | - | - | |
No moderation, no rGE | 1243.45 | 1973 | −2702.55 | 2 | 3.54 | .17 | - | |
| ||||||||
Negative Affectivity | CHAOS | |||||||
AE full moderation, no rGE | 876.39 | 2241 | −3605.62 | - | - | - | - | |
No moderation, no rGE | 877.02 | 2243 | −3608.98 | 2 | 0.63 | .73 | - | |
LEOS | ||||||||
AE full moderation, with rGE | 371.65 | 1970 | −3568.35 | - | - | - | .003 | |
AE full moderation, no rGE | 371.65 | 1971 | −3570.35 | 1 | 0.00 | .99 | - | |
No moderation, no rGE | 380.25 | 1973 | −3565.75 | 2 | 8.61 | .04 | - | |
| ||||||||
Extraversion/Surgency | CHAOS | |||||||
AE full moderation, with rGE | 603.37 | 2240 | −3876.63 | - | - | - | .74 | |
AE full moderation, no rGE | 684.92 | 2241 | −3797.08 | 1 | 81.55 | <.01 | - | |
No moderation, no rGE | 713.03 | 2243 | −3772.97 | 2 | 109.66 | <.01 | - | |
LEOS | ||||||||
AE full moderation, no rGE | 270.99 | 1971 | −3671.01 | 1 | - | - | - | |
No moderation, no rGE | 255.22 | 1973 | −3690.78 | 2 | −15.77 | .99 | - |
Note. CHAOS = Confusion, Hubbub, and Order Scale; LEOS = Living Environment Observation Scale; A = additive genetic; E = nonshared environment; rGE = gene-environment correlation; −2LL = −2 times the log likelihood (fit statistic); df = degrees of freedom; AIC = Akaike's Information Criterion (fit index); Δdf = change in degrees of freedom; Δχ2 = change in chi-square value from the full model to reduced model; p = probability. The best fitting model is in bold.
Negative Affectivity
Similarly, a model estimating CHAOS moderation of the A and E paths on Negative Affectivity was not more parsimonious than the classic AE model (see Table 3), thus, estimates of A (79%) and E (21%) remained constant across levels of home chaos.
However, a model allowing LEOS moderation of the A and E paths on Negative Affectivity fit significantly better than the AE model without moderation (Table 3). Next, a model that simultaneously estimated rGE was fit, although rGE was estimated at r = .003, and was not significant so was thus dropped. Estimates of A varied from 78% (plus 1 SD) to 85% (minus 1 SD), indicating that heritability increased under crowded or unsafe home conditions (i.e., lower LEOS). The overall variance in Negative Affectivity was also higher under crowded or unsafe home conditions (.302 at minus 1 SD; .239 at the mean for the between-family variable).
Extraversion/Surgency
A model allowing CHAOS moderation of the A and E paths on Extraversion/Surgency fit significantly better than the AE model without moderation (see Table 3). Across levels of CHAOS, the estimates of A and E changed, such that estimates of A increased modestly from 81% (minus 1 SD) to 84% (plus 1 SD), indicating that children's Extraversion/Surgency was more heritable under chaotic home conditions. Whereas heritability increased, the overall variance in Extraversion/Surgency minimally changed (.124 at minus 1 SD; .160 at plus 1 SD for the between-family variable). Passive gene-environment correlation was also signficant, rGE = .74, supporting genetic mediation of the covariance between CHAOS and children's Extraversion/Surgency. A model estimating LEOS moderation of the A and E paths on Extraversion/Surgency was not more parsimonious than the AE model without moderation (see Table 3), thus, estimates of A (49%) and E (51%) remained constant across levels of the physical home environment.
Discussion
Modeling multiple types of gene-environment interplay uncovered the complex role of genetic factors and the hidden importance of the family environment for children's temperament and development more generally. We considered the genetic and environmental architecture of temperament in middle childhood by simultaneously modeling main effects, GxE, and rGE when possible using a new quantitative genetic model (Price & Jaffee, 2008, see Method). This new model allows for the simultaneous estimation of GxE and rGE using family-level measures of the environment, such as chaos and the physical environment in the home. Previous models (e.g., Purcell, 2002), allow for the simultaneous estimation of GxE and rGE only with individual-level measures of the environment, such as parent-child conflict and regard measured independently for cotwins. This model has recently been criticized because other, simpler models (i.e., common factor or correlated factor models) may be more parsimonious (Rathouz, Van Hulle, Rodgers, Waldman & Lahey, 2008).
Given the widespread importance of family-level aspects of the environment for child development, the Price and Jaffee (2008) model fills a critical need. It is essential to model GxE and rGE as well as main genetic and environmental effects, given increasing evidence in the literature that all of these processes are relevant for child development.
Passive Gene-Environment Correlation, Children's Temperament, and the Home
We demonstrated important passive rGE, such that home environments were less chaotic for children with high Effortful Control, and this association was genetically mediated. Furthermore, children with high Extraversion/Surgency experienced more chaotic home environments, and this correlation also was genetically mediated. This finding suggests that some of the same genes in parents that contribute to levels of chaos in the home are passed down to children and contribute to their temperament.
Previous literature, largely focused on adolescent adjustment, also documents the important role of genetic mediation of covariation between putative measures of the environment and traits. For example, the correlation between negative relationships of mothers with their adolescents was .59, with genetic factors explaining 69% of this covariation (Reiss & Neiderhiser, 2000). Shared genetic liability explained part of the co-occurrence of negative life events and depression as well (Kendler & Karkowski-Shuman, 1997, in adults; Thapar, Harold & McGuffin, 1998, in adolescents). Thus, rGE plays a central role in associations between adolescent experiences and adjustment.
Although these findings highlight the importance of rGE, shared genetic variance does not preclude an environmental influence on the trait (Waldman, 2007). Marital stability, corporal punishment, and physical maltreatment have important environmental influences on children's behavioral problems when controlling for genetic associations (D'Onofrio et al., 2005; Jaffee, Caspi, Moffitt & Taylor, 2004).
Our findings bolster the emerging literature showing that “environmental” measures of the home share genetic variance with cognitive abilities, personality and temperament. Specifically, in adults, genetic influences on cognitive abilities accounted for a significant portion of the heritability of education and occupational status (Lichtenstein & Pedersen, 1997), and genetic influences on extraversion, openness, and neuroticism accounted for all of the genetic influences on stressful life events (Saudino, Pedersen, Lichtenstein, McClearn, & Plomin, 1997). In toddlers, the heritability of the HOME measure of the home environment was accounted for by cognitive abilities and temperament (Saudino & Plomin, 1997).
Unfortunately, we were unable to test for the presence of rGE between the LEOS observational assessment of the physical home context and Effortful Control and Extraversion/Surgency because the Price and Jaffee (2008) model requires significant moderation of the additive genetic path before rGE can be estimated. The rGE between physical home and Negative Affectivity was nonsignificant, suggesting that genes that influence children's Negative Affectivity do not influence the physical home environment.
Moderation of the Heritability of Temperament by the Home Environment
We found evidence that the heritability of children's temperament was moderated by home environments, such that Effortful Control and Extraversion/Surgency were more heritable in chaotic homes, and Negative Affectivity was more heritable under crowded or unsafe home conditions. Note that these modest increases in heritability were not due to overall decreases in the phenotypic variance, in fact, the between family variance in Effortful Control and Negative Affectivity were higher in adverse home conditions, whereas the variance did not change across the distribution of home environments for Extraversion/Surgency.
At this point in this relatively new literature, it is difficult to form directional hypotheses concerning moderation of heritability. Johnson (2007) hypothesized that adaptive behaviors are more heritable under positive environments, and risky behaviors are more heritable under adverse environments. However, it is challenging to cleanly distinguish adaptive and risky behaviors, as they vary depending on the broader context and the outcome. Temperamental shyness, for example, increases risk of later anxiety disorders, but is protective against later externalizing problems (e.g. Rubin, Coplan, & Bowker, 2009; Schwartz, Snidman, & Kagan, 1996). Similarly, high empathy, prosociality, and strong inhibitory processes are generally adaptive features of temperament and childhood personality; however, if these processes are over-developed, they may contribute to risk for mood disorders in adolescent girls (Zahn-Waxler, Shirtcliff, & Marceau, 2008). The opposite argument also seems likely: where the importance of genetic factors for individual differences is lower in portions of the population exposed to adversities that impact the disorder or trait, e.g., post traumatic stress disorder in war veterans (Button, Scourfield, Martin, Purcell, & McGuffin, 2005; Wichers et al., 2002). A broader statement is perhaps more accurate: social control constrains heritability (Rutter, 2006; Shanahan & Hofer, 2005).
The literature supports the idea that social control constrains heritability, whereas the environment is permissive and heritability is higher under conditions of low social constraint. Individual differences in disinhibition (i.e. partying, drinking and multiple sex partners) were not heritable for those with a religious upbringing (Boomsma, deGeus, van Baal & Koopmans, 1999). There are also numerous examples of heritability varying by cohort, with lower social controls allowing for the expression of heritable individual differences. The heritability of smoking in females rose over time as it became more socially acceptable, with no change in men (Kendler, Thornton & Pedersen, 2000). Similarly, when overall mean height increased in Finland from 1928 to 1957 (attributed to improved nutrition), heritability increased (Silventoinen, Kaprio, Lahelma & Koskenvuo, 2000). Perhaps structured, secure and safe home environments exert social control over children's temperament and thus decrease heritability. It would be informative to determine how stable these effects are across various environments and developmental periods.
Multiple Pathways of Temperament Development and Risk for Child Psychopathology
Developmental processes that contribute to temperament and child psychopathology include a) direct effect of genes and environments, b) environmental moderation of genetic effects, and c) indirect effects of genes through environments and social conditions.
The direct effect of genes, termed the reactive pathway by Reiss and Leve (2007), suggests that genes and environments make independent contributions to children's outcomes. This main effects model can be expanded by considering GxE. Multiple types of moderational models have been proposed (Shanahan & Hofer, 2005), including diathesis stress (does an adverse environment act on a vulnerable, often genetic substrate?), social context as enhancement (does a positive environment act on a positive or adaptive genetic substrate?), social context as compensation (does a positive environment suppress the expression of a vulnerable genetic diathesis?), and social context as social control (do social norms or institutions limit choices, preventing or minimizing the expression of the diathesis?). The more recent biological sensitivity to context/differential susceptibility model also falls under this category (Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg & van Ijzendoorn, 2011), as it predicts a cross-over interaction (are some individuals genetically or temperamentally more sensitive to both adverse and protective environments?). Illustrating this approach, difficult temperament may both impede healthy development in adverse environments and facilitate positive development in supportive contexts (Pluess & Belsky, 2010).
These models can be examined using molecular, as well as quantitative genetic approaches, e.g. children carrying the short verson of the tandem repeat in the promoter of the serotonin transporter gene paired with mothers with low social support were more likely to be behaviorally inhibited. Moffitt, Caspi, and Rutter (2006) and Rutter, Moffitt, and Caspi (2006) provide thorough reviews of GxE findings involving measured genes interacting with measured environments to predict problem behaviors and psychopathologies.
The literature utilizing these interactive models is expanding and evidence of GxE is accumulating. One limitation is that these models do not explicitly address rGE, and in fact, the extent to which the genetic diathesis and the measured environment are correlated can introduce bias to the estimates (Aiken & West, 1991). In terms of quantitative genetic models, parsing out the main effect of the measured environment can affect estimates of moderation of the ACE variance components if the moderator shares genetic variance with the trait of interest (rGE).
The third model, the social mediation pathway model (Reiss & Leve, 2007) is relatively new to the literature. In this case, poor home environments mediate the genetic influence on child adjustment through rGE. For example, infant behavioral inhibition evokes parental insensitivity, which then potentiates maladaptive parent-child interactions over time, exacerbating the child's fear of novelty toward clinical levels of anxiety. In support of this pathway, previous literature has shown that children's heritable traits evoke parent-child mutuality (Deater-Deckard & Petrill, 2004; Deater-Deckard & O'Connor, 2000), parental emotional over-involvement (Moberg, Lichtenstein, Forsman & Larsson, 2011), hostility (Boivin et al., 2005) and discipline (Riggins-Caspers, Cadoret, Knutson & Langbehn, 2003).
Current findings of large rGE between children's temperament and chaos in the home support the importance of considering the social mediation pathway model in addition to the more commonly used GxE moderational models. Effortful Control, for example may protect children from internalizing and externalizing disorders (Eisenberg et al., 2009) by decreasing chaos in the home. Testing these competing causal models of the etiology of psychopathology would greatly advance the literature.
Relevance of Gene-Environment Interplay for Preventive Interventions
Testing the reactive and the social-mediation pathways are important for elucidating proximal processes of child development and risk for psychopathology. Infant difficult temperament, for example, evokes adverse responses from caregivers (Boivin et al., 2005); however, a parenting intervention may extinguish these negative genetic influences, such that parents learn to respond to their infant's irritability and unadaptability with patience and consistency. As discussed above, protective environments can exert social control over a heritable risk for depression (Shanahan & Hofer, 2005).
On the other hand, the presence of a significant rGE does not always support the social mediation pathway. Chaotic home environments may not have a causal impact on children's adjustment; rather, the association may simply reflect genetic risk that parents transmit to their children. In this case, interventions targeting the home environment may be ineffective in reducing risk for psychopathology. Testing these competing process models would be informative for designing effective interventions.
Limitations to the Study
The study faced several limitations. First, generalizability to other populations is limited since the sample was predominantly composed of Caucasian families. Home environments vary by ethnic and cultural groups, which may lead to different patterns of genetic and environmental influence on children's temperament. For example, ethnic differences in children's IQ scores were significantly associated with the HOME (Brooks-Gunn, Klebanov & Duncan, 1996). Different cultural values and beliefs lead to different levels and types of affordances available in the home, such as parental responsiveness and the availability of learning materials (e.g., Bradley, 2006; Bradley & Corwyn, 2005).
Also, participants in this study were twins, so there is a question as to whether findings from a study of temperament in twin children can be generalized to single born children. Moilanen et al. (1999) found nonsignificant trends suggesting lower rates of emotional and behavioral problems in twins, whereas others have reported no differences in depression scores for twins and singletons (Angold, Erkanli, Silberg, Eaves & Costello, 2002). Studies with larger, population-based samples usually find no differences between twins and singletons in internalizing and externalizing problems (Lytton & Gallagher, 2002). Specific to temperament, there were no differences between twin and singleton infant's parent-reported temperament (Goldsmith et al., 1999). Thus, most literature supports the generalizability of twin data to singletons for temperament and symptoms of psychopathology.
The equal environments assumption (EEA), or the assumption that the environment influencing the behavior being studied is no more similar for MZ twins than DZ twins, is central to the classic twin study. If MZ twins have more similar trait relevant environments than DZ twins, then estimates of heritability would be too large. This assumption has been tested with large samples of twins. Comparing models of mother report of child and adolescent mood and behavioral disorders controlling for environmental similarity in twin pairs to models that did not control for environmental similarity, Cronk, Slutske, Madden, Bucholz, Reich and Heath (2002) reported that estimates were not greatly affected, supporting the EEA. Twin children's similarity in personality was unrelated to their similarity of childhood experience, which also supports the EEA (Borkenau, Riemann, Angleitner & Spinath, 2002). Thus, investigators continue to test the EEA, and the validity of this assumption is supported for personality and mood and behavior problems.
Third, independent mother and father reports were composited to assess children's temperament and although the physical home environment was assessed through trained observation after a home visit, mothers were relied upon to report on levels of chaos in the home. The question, of course, is the extent to which parent report assesses temperament with optimal validity and the extent to which shared rater variance accounts for associations between temperament and home chaos (Saudino, 2005). Parent report measures are inexpensive, easy to administer, and parents are more knowledgeable and familiar with their children in multiple contexts than other caregivers, such as teachers or other relatives (Rothbart & Bates, 2006). Importantly, the CBQ has been used in conjunction with the Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery (Lab-TAB; Goldsmith, Reilly, Lemery, Longley, & Prescott, 1993) demonstrating its validity (Gagne, Van Hulle, Aksan, Essex & Goldsmith, 2011).
Fourth, the Price and Jaffee model (2008) is limited in that it cannot estimate rGE in the absence of GxE. The existence of GxE allows the main effect of the measured family environment to be distinguished from passive rGE, with the measured environment's main effect on the phenotype constant with respect to the measured environment, and rGE modeled as a linear function of the measured environment. Thus, there are likely additional rGEs that could not be detected with this model.
Future Directions
In addition to testing the reactive and social mediation process models described above, other lines of inquiry would be useful. First, our study focused on the three main temperament factors found in childhood. Future studies should consider temperament at the lower-order scale level, as the genetic architecture of temperament, using the classic ACE model, has sometimes been found to vary by scale (Goldsmith, Lemery, Buss & Campos, 1999). Second, it will be important to consider age differences in the genetic and environmental underpinnings of temperament, preferably with a longitudinal design. Genes and environments probably have age-dependent influences on temperament, and documenting these differences will inform understanding of the course of development.
Conclusion
We considered the genetic and environmental architecture of temperament in middle childhood, simultaneously modeling main effects, GxE, and rGE using a new quantitative genetic model (Price & Jaffee, 2008). Children with high Effortful Control had less chaotic home environments, and children with high Extraversion/Surgency had more chaotic home environments—and these associations were mediated genetically through the process of passive gene-environment correlation. At the same time, Effortful Control and Extraversion/Surgency were modestly more heritable in chaotic homes, and Negative Affectivity was modestly more heritable under crowded or unsafe home conditions. It may be that structured, secure and safe home environments exert social control over children's temperament and thus decrease heritability. Gene-environment interplay on children's temperament should continue to be examined to uncover permissive and deterministic influences across a spectrum of environments.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health grant R01-MH59785 to Goldsmith and Lemery-Chalfant. Infrastructure support was provided by the Waisman Center via a core grant from NICHD (P30-HD03352). Special thanks to the staff and students at the Wisconsin Twin Project for collecting the data, and the participating families who generously shared their experiences.
References
- Abbott A, Bartlett D. Infant motor development and equipment use in the home. Child, Care, and Development. 2001;27:295–306. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2214.2001.00186.x. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2214.2001.00186.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aiken LS, West SG. Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Sage; Newbury Park, CA: 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Akaike H. Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika. 1987;52:317–332. [Google Scholar]
- Angold A, Erkanli A, Silberg J, Eaves L, Costello EJ. Depression scale scores in 8–17-year-olds: Effects of age and gender. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2002;43:1052–1063. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baumrind D. The average respectable environment is not good enough: A response to Scarr. Child Development. 1993;64:1299–1317. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02954.x. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boivin M, Perusse D, Dionne G, Saysset V, Tremblay RE. The genetic-environmental etiology of parents' perceptions and self-assessed behaviours toward their 5-month-old infants in a large twin and singleton sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2005;46:612–630. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00375.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boomsma DI, de Geus EJC, van Baal GCM, Koopmans JR. A religious upbringing reduces the influence of genetic factors on disinhibition: Evidence for interaction between genotype and environment on personality. Twin Research. 1999;2:115–125. doi: 10.1375/136905299320565988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borkenau P, Riemann R, Angleitner A, Spinath FM. Similarity of childhood experiences and personality resemblance in monozygotic and dizygotic twins: A test of the equal environments assumption. Personality and Individual Differences. 2002;33:261–269. [Google Scholar]
- Bouchard T., Jr. Genetic influence on human psychological traits: a survey. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2004;13:148–151. [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH. Children's home environments, health, behavior, and intervention efforts: A review using the home inventory as a maker measure. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs. 1993;119:439–480. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH. Home environment. In: Watt N, Ayoub C, Bradley RH, Puma J, LaBoeuf W, editors. The Crisis in Youth Mental Health, vol. 4: Early intervention programs and policies. Greenwood Publishing Group; Westport, CN: 2006. pp. 89–120. [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH, Corwyn R. Caring for children around the world: A view from HOME. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2005;29:468–478. [Google Scholar]
- Bronfenbrenner U, Ceci S. Nature-nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological Review. 1994;101:568–586. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.101.4.568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks-Gunn J, Klebanov PK, Duncan GJ. Ethnic differences in children's intelligence test scores: Role of economic deprivation, home environment, and maternal characteristics. Child Development. 1996;67:396–408. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Button TMM, Scourfield J, Martin N, Purcell S, McGuffin P. Family dysfunction interacts with genes in the causation of antisocial symptoms. Behavior Genetics. 2005;35:115–120. doi: 10.1007/s10519-004-0826-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caldwell BM, Bradley RH. Home observation for measurement of the environment. University of Arkansas Press; Little Rock: 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Coldwell J, Pike A, Dunn J. Household chaos-links with parenting and child behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:1116–1122. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01655.x. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01655.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cronk NJ, Slutske WS, Madden PAF, Bucholz KK, Reich W, Heath AC. Emotional and behavioral problems among female twins: An evaluation of the equal environments assumption. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002;41:829–837. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200207000-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deater-Deckard K, O'Connor TG. Parent-child mutuality in early childhood: Two behavioral genetic studies. Developmental Psychology. 2000;36:561–570. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.36.5.561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deater-Deckard K, Petrill SA. Parent-child dyadic mutuality and child behavior problems: An investigation of gene-environment processes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45:1171–1179. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00309.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D'Onofrio BM, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, Slutske WS, Martin NG. A genetically informed study of marital instability and its association with offspring psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2005;114:570–586. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dumas J, Nissley J, Nordstrom A, Smith E, Prinz R, Levine D. Home Chaos: Sociodemographic, Parenting, Interactional, and Child Correlates. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2005;34:93–104. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3401_9. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3401_9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg N, Valiente C, Spinrad TL, Cumberland A, Losoya SH. Longitudinal relations of children's effortful control, impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45:988–1008. doi: 10.1037/a0016213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis BJ, Boyce WT, Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH. Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary—neurodevelopmental theory. Developmental Psychopathology. 2011;23:7–28. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans G. Child development and the physical environment. Annual Review of Psychology. 2006;57:423–451. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190057. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans G, Wells N, Moch A. Housing and mental health: A review of the evidence and a methodological and conceptual critique. Journal of Social Issues. 2003;59:475–500. doi: 10.1111/1540-4560.00074. [Google Scholar]
- Evans G, Gonnella C, Marcynyszyn L, Gentile L, Salpekar N. The role of chaos in poverty and children's socioemotinoal adjustment. Psychological Science. 2005;16:560–565. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01575.x. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01575.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forget-Dubois N, Pérusse D, Turecki G, Girard A, Billette J-M, Rouleau G, et al. Diagnosing zygosity in infant twins: Physical similarity, genotyping, and chorionicity. Twin Research. 2003;6:479–485. doi: 10.1375/136905203322686464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gagne JR, Goldsmith HH. A longitudinal analysis of anger and inhibitory control in twins from 12 to 36 months of age. Developmental Science. 2011;14:112–124. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00969.x. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00969.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gagne JR, Van Hulle CA, Aksan N, Essex MJ, Goldsmith HH. Deriving childhood temperament measures from emotion-eliciting behavioral episodes: Scale construction and initial validation. Psychological Assessment. 2011;23:337–353. doi: 10.1037/a0021746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gjone H, Stevenson J. A longitudinal twin study of temperament and behavior problems: Common genetic or environmental influences? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997;36:1448–1456. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199710000-00028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldsmith HH. Human developmental behavioral genetics: Mapping the effects of genes and environments. Annals of Child Development. 1988;5:187–227. [Google Scholar]
- Goldsmith HH. A zygosity questionnaire for young twins: A research note. Behavior Genetics. 1991;21:257–269. doi: 10.1007/BF01065819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldsmith HH, Buss KA, Lemery KS. Toddler and childhood temperament: Expanded content, stronger genetic evidence, new evidence for the importance of environment. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33:91–905. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.33.6.891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldsmith HH, Lemery KS, Buss KA, Campos JJ. Genetic analyses of focal aspects of infant temperament. Developmental Psychology. 1999;35:972–985. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldsmith HH, Reilly J, Lemery KS, Longley S, Prescott A. Preliminary manual for the Preschool Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery. Technical Report Version 1.0 Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin—Madison; Madison, WI: 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Harris JR. The nurture assumption: why children turn out the way they do. Free Press; New York, NY: 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Hur Y. Genetic and environmental contributions to childhood temperament in South Korean Twins. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2009;12:549–554. doi: 10.1375/twin.12.6.549. doi:10.1375/twin.12.6.549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaffee SR, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Taylor A. Physical maltreatment victim to antisocial child: Evidence of an environmentally mediated process. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113:44–55. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson W. Genetic and environmental influences on behavior: Capturing all the interplay. Psychological Review. 2007;114:423–440. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.423. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, Baker JH. Genetic influences on measures of the environment: a systematic review. Psychological Medicine. 2007;37:615–626. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706009524. doi:10.1017/S0033291706009524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, Karkowski-Shuman L. Stressful life events and genetic liability to major depression: genetic control of exposure to the environment. Psychological Medicine. 1997;27:539–547. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797004716. doi:10.1017/S0033291797004716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, Neale MC, Prescott CA, Kessler RC, Heath AC, Corey LA, Eaves LJ. Childhood parental loss and alcoholism in women: A causal analysis using a twin-family design. Psychological Medicine. 1996;26:79–95. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700033730. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700033730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, Thornton LM, Pederson NL. Tobacco consumption in Swedish twins reared apart and reared together. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:886–892. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.9.886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger R, South S, Johnson W, Iacono W. The heritability of personality is not always 50%: Gene-environment interactions and correlations between personality and parenting. Journal of Personality. 2008;76(6):1485–1521. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00529.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lemery-Chalfant K. How genes and environments work together to promote resilience. In: Reich J, Zautra AJ, Hall JS, editors. Handbook of adult resilience. Guilford: 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Lemery-Chalfant K, Doelger L, Goldsmith HH. Genetic relations between effortful and attentional control and symptoms of psychopathology in middle childhood. Infant and Child Development. 2008;17:365–385. doi: 10.1002/icd.581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lemery-Chalfant K, Goldsmith HH, Schmidt NL, Arneson CL, Van Hulle CA. Wisconsin Twin Project: Current directions and findings. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2006;9:1030–1037. doi: 10.1375/183242706779462363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lemery-Chalfant K, Clifford S, Swann G. Temperament and child psychopathology: A behavior genetic perspective. In: Kim's Y, Saudino K, Ganiban J, editors. Behavior genetics of temperament and personality. (Behavior genetics book series). in press. [Google Scholar]
- Leventhal T, Brooks-Gunn J. The neighborhoods they live in: The effects of neighborhood residence on child and adolescent outcomes. Psychological Bulletin. 2000;126(2):309–337. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lichtenstein P, Pedersen NL. Does genetic variance for cognitive abilities account for genetic variance in educational achievement and occupational status? A study of twins reared apart and twins reared together. Biodemography and Social Biology. 1997;44(1–2):77–90. doi: 10.1080/19485565.1997.9988935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liddell C, Kruger P. Activity and social behaviour in a crowded South African township nursery—a follow-up study on the effects of crowding at home. Merrill-Parmer Quarterly. 1989;35:209–226. [Google Scholar]
- Lytton H, Gallagher L. Parenting twins and the genetics of parenting. In: Bornstein MH, editor. Handbook of parenting: Vol. 1 Children and parenting. 2nd edition Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Mahwah, NJ: 2002. pp. 227–253. [Google Scholar]
- Matheny A, Wilson R, Thoben A. Home and mother: relations with infant temperament. American Psychological Association, Inc. 1987;23(3):323–331. [Google Scholar]
- Matheny A, Phillips K. Temperament and context: Correlates of home environment with temperament continuity and change, newborn to 30 months. In: Wachs TD, Kohnstamm GA, editors. Temperament in context. Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 2001. pp. 81–101. [Google Scholar]
- Matheny A, Wachs T, Ludwig J, Phillips K. Bring order out of chaos: Psychometric characteristics of the confusion, hubbub, and order scale. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 1995;16:429–444. [Google Scholar]
- McCall R. So many interactions, so little evidence: Why? In: Wachs T, Plomin R, editors. Conceptualization and measurement of organism environment interaction. American Psychological Association; Washington, DC: 1991. pp. 142–161. [Google Scholar]
- McGue M, Bouchard TJ. Adjustment of twin data for the effects of age and sex. Behavior Genetics. 1984;14:325–343. doi: 10.1007/BF01080045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moberg T, Lichtenstein P, Forsman M, Larsson H. Internalizing behavior in adolescent girls affects parental emotional overinvolvement: A cross-lagged twin study. Behavior Genetics. 2011;41:223–233. doi: 10.1007/s10519-010-9383-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moffitt T, Caspi A, Rutter M. Measured gene- environment interactions in psychopathology: Concepts, research strategies, and implications for research, intervention, and public understanding of genetics. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2006;1:5–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00002.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moilanen I, Linna SL, Ebeling H, Kumpulainen K, Tamminen T, Piha J, Almqvist F. Are twins' behavioral/emotional problems different from singletons'? European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1999;8:IV/62–IV/67. doi: 10.1007/pl00010702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mullineaux P, Deater-Deckard K, Petrill S, Thompson L, DeThorne L. Temperament in middle childhood: A behavioral genetic analysis of fathers' and mothers' reports. Journal of Research in Personality. 2009;43:737–746. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.04.008. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2009.04.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray R. The influence of crowding on children's behavior. In: Canter D, Lee T, editors. Psychology and the Built Environment. Wiley; London: 1974. pp. 112–117. [Google Scholar]
- Neale MC, Cardon LR. Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. the Netherlands: Kluwer Academic; Dordrecht: 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Neale MC, Boker SM, Xie G, Maes HH. Mx: Statistical Modeling. 6th ed. Department of Psychiatry; Richmond, VA: 2003. VCU Box 900126. 23298. [Google Scholar]
- Novak G, Pelaez M. Child and adolescent development: A behavioral systems approach. Sage; Thousand Oaks, CA: 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Pettit G, Bates J, Dodge K. Supportive parenting, ecological context, and children's adjustment: A seven-year longitudinal study. Child Development. 1997;68:908–923. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01970.x. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01970.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plomin R, DeFries JC, McClearn GE, McGuffin P. Behavioral genetics. Worth; New York: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Pluess M, Belsky J. Differential susceptibility to parenting and quality child care. Developmental Psychology. 2010;46:379–390. doi: 10.1037/a0015203. doi: 10.1037/a0015203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prevatt F. The contribution of parenting practices in a risk and resiliency model of children's adjustment. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2003;21:469–480. doi: 10.1348/026151003322535174. [Google Scholar]
- Price TS, Freeman B, Craig IW, Petrill SA, Ebersole L, Plomin R. Infant zygosity can be assigned by parental report questionnaire data. Twin Research. 2000;3:129–133. doi: 10.1375/136905200320565391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price T, Jaffee S. Effects of the Family Environment: Gene-Environment Interaction and Passive Gene-Environment Correlation. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44:305–315. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.305. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Purcell S. Variance Components Models for Gene-Environment Interaction in Twin Analysis. Twin Research. 2002;5:554–571. doi: 10.1375/136905202762342026. doi:10.1375/136905202762342026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rathouz P, Van Hulle C, Rodgers J, Waldman I, Lahey B. Specification, testing, and interpretation of gene-by-measured-environment interaction models in the presence of gene-environment correlation. Behavior Genetics. 2008;38:301–315. doi: 10.1007/s10519-008-9193-4. doi: 10.1007/s10519-008-9193-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reiss D, Leve LD. Genetic expression outside the skin: clues to mechanisms of genotype x environment interaction. Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19:1005–1027. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reiss D, Neiderhiser JM. The interplay of genetic influences and social processes in developmental theory: Specific mechanisms are coming into view. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12:357–374. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400003060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rhee SH, Cosgrove VE, Schmitz S, Haberstick BC, Corley RC, Hewitt JK. Early childhood temperament and the covariation between internalizing and externalizing behavior in school-aged children. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2007;10:33–44. doi: 10.1375/twin.10.1.33. doi:10.1375/twin.10.1.33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riggins-Caspers KM, Cadoret RJ, Knutson JF, Langbehn D. Biology-environment interaction and evocative biology-environment correlation: Contributions of harsh discipline and parental psychopathology to problem adolescent behaviors. Behavior Genetics. 2003;33:205–220. doi: 10.1023/a:1023434206261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Hershey KL, Fisher P. Investigations of temperament at 3–7 years: The Children's Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development. 2001;72:1394–1408. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00355. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rothbart MK, Bates JE. Temperament. In: Damon W, Eisenberg N, editors. Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development. 5th ed. Wiley; New York: 1998. pp. 105–176. [Google Scholar]
- Rothbart MK, Bates JE. Temperament. In: Damon W, Eisenberg N, editors. Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development. 6th ed. Wiley; New York: 2006. pp. 99–166. [Google Scholar]
- Rowe David. The limits of family influence. Guilford; New York, NY: 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Rubin K, Coplan R, Bowker J. Social withdrawal in childhood. Annual Review of Psychology. 2009;60:141–171. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163642. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M. Genes and Behavior: Nature-nurture interplay explained. Blackwell Publishing; Oxford: 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M, Moffitt T, Caspi A. Gene–environment interplay and psychopathology: Multiple varieties but real effects. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:226–261. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01557.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saudino K. Behavior genetics and child temperament. Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics. 2005;26:214–223. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200506000-00010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saudino KJ, Cherny SS. Parent ratings of temperament in twins. In: Emde RN, Hewitt JK, editors. The transition from infancy to early childhood: Genetic and environmental influences in the MacArthur Longitudinal Twin Study. Oxford University Press; New York: 2001. pp. 73–88. [Google Scholar]
- Saudino KJ, Pedersen NL, Lichtenstein P, McClearn GE, Plomin R. Can personality explain genetic influences on life events? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997;72(1):196–206. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.72.1.196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saudino KJ, Plomin R. Cognitive and temperamental mediators of genetic contributions to the home environment during infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 1997;43:1–23. [Google Scholar]
- Scarr S. Developmental theories for the 1990s: Development and individual differences. Child Development. 1992;63:1–19. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb03591.x. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmitz S, Fulker DW, Plomin R, Zahn-Waxler C, Emde RN, DeFries JC. Temperament and problem behavior during early childhood. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1999;23:333–355. [Google Scholar]
- Schmitz S, Saudino KJ. Links between temperament and behavior problems in early adolescence. In: Petrill SA, Plomin R, DeFries JC, Hewitt JK, editors. Nature, nurture, and the transition to early adolescence. Oxford Press; New York: 2003. pp. 185–198. [Google Scholar]
- Schmitz S, Saudino KJ, Plomin R, Fulker DW, DeFries JC. Genetic and environmental influences on temperament in middle childhood: Analyses of teacher and tester ratings. Child Development. 1996;67:409–422. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz C, Snidmna N, Kagan J. Early childhood temperament as determinant of externalizing behavior in adolescence. Development and psychopathology. 1996;8:527–537. [Google Scholar]
- Shanahan MJ, Hofer SM. Social context in gene–environment interactions: Retrospect and prospect. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2005;60(B):65–76. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.special_issue_1.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singh A, Waldman I. The etiology of associations between negative emotionality and childhood externalizing disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2010;119:376–388. doi: 10.1037/a0019342. doi:10.1037/a0019342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slutske WC, Heath AC, Dinwiddie SH, Madden PAR, Bucholz KK, Dunne MP, Statham DJ, Martin NG. Modeling genetic and environmental influences in the etiology of conduct disorder: A study of 2.682 adult twin pairs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1997;106:266–279. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.106.2.266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silventoinen K, Kaprio J, Lahelma E, Koskenvuo M. Relative effect of genetic and environmental factors on body height: Differences across birth cohorts among Finnish men and women. Am J Public Health. 2000;90(4):627–630. doi: 10.2105/ajph.90.4.627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- South SC, Krueger RF. Socioeconomic status and health problems moderate genetic and environmental influences on internalizing pathology. Psychological Medicine. 2011;41:107–117. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710000279. doi:10.1017/S0033291710000279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sroufe LA. Psychopathology as an outcome of development. Development and Psychopathology. 1997;9:251–268. doi: 10.1017/s0954579497002046. doi:10.1017/S0954579497002046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Supplee L, Unikel E, Shaw D. Physical environmental adversity and the protective role of maternal monitoring in relation to early child conduct problems. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2007;28:166–183. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2006.12.001. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2006.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar A, Harold G, McGuffin P. Life eventsand depressive symptoms in childhood–shared genes or shared adversity: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1998;39:1153–1158. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turkheimer E. Three laws of behavior genetics and what they mean. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2000;9(5):160–164. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00084. [Google Scholar]
- Valiente C, Lemery-Chalfant K, Reiser M. Pathways to problem behaviors: chaotic homes, parent and child effortful control, and parenting. Social Development. 2007;16:249–267. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00383.x. [Google Scholar]
- Wachs TD. Relevance of physical environment influences for toddler temperament. Infant Behavior & Development. 1988;11:431–445. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(88)90004-5. [Google Scholar]
- Wachs TD. Necessary but not sufficient: the respective roles of single and multiple influences on individual development. American Psychological Association; Washington, DC: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Wachs TD. Viewing microsystem chaos through a Bronfenbrenner bioecological lens. 2010 ISSN: 9781433805653. [Google Scholar]
- Waldman ID. Gene-environment interactions reexamined: Does mother's marital stability interact with the dopamine receptor D2 gene in the etiology of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19:1117–1128. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wichers M, Purcell S, Danckaerts M, Derom C, Derom R, Vlietinck R, van Os J. Prenatal life and post-natal psychopathology: Evidence for negative gene–birth weight interaction. Psychological Medicine. 2002;32:1165–1174. doi: 10.1017/s0033291702006372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zahn-Waxler C, Shirtcliff EA, Marceau K. Disorders of childhood and adolescence; Gender and psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2008;4:275–303. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]