Abstract
Impulse control disorders (ICDs), including disordered gambling, can occur in a significant number of patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD) receiving dopaminergic therapy. The neurobiology underlying susceptibility to such problems is unclear, but risk likely results from an interaction between dopaminergic medication and a pre-existing trait vulnerability. Impulse control and addictive disorders form part of a broader psychopathological spectrum of disorders, which share a common underlying genetic vulnerability, referred to as externalizing. The broad externalizing risk factor is a continuously varying trait reflecting vulnerability to various impulse control problems, manifested at the overt level by disinhibitory symptoms and at the personality level by antecedent traits such as impulsivity and novelty/sensation seeking. Trait “disinhibition” is thus a core endophenotype of ICDs, and a key target for neurobiological investigation. The ventral striatal dopamine system has been hypothesized to underlie individual variation in behavioral disinhibition. Here, we examined whether individual differences in ventral striatal dopamine synthesis capacity predicted individual variation in disinhibitory temperament traits in individuals with PD. Eighteen early-stage male PD patients underwent 6-[18F]Fluoro-l-DOPA (FDOPA) positron emission tomography scanning to measure striatal dopamine synthesis capacity, and completed a measure of disinhibited personality. Consistent with our predictions, we found that levels of ventral, but not dorsal, striatal dopamine synthesis capacity predicted disinhibited personality, particularly a propensity for financial extravagance. Our results are consistent with recent preclinical models of vulnerability to behavioral disinhibition and addiction proneness, and provide novel insights into the neurobiology of potential vulnerability to impulse control problems in PD and other disorders.
Keywords: dopa decarboxylase, dopamine, disordered gambling, externalizing, impulse control disorders, impulsivity, reward, ventral striatum
Introduction
Several addictive and impulse control disorders (ICDs) have been associated with Parkinson’s disease (PD) and its treatment with dopaminergic medication, including disordered gambling (Gallagher et al., 2007), substance dependence (Bienfait et al., 2010), and the addiction-like excessive use of dopaminergic medications, or DA dysregulation syndrome (Lawrence et al., 2003). The prevalence of ICDs in medicated PD patients was estimated at ∼14% in a large (n > 3000) multicentre study, with >25% of affected individuals having multiple ICDs (Weintraub et al., 2010). The development of ICDs in PD likely results from an interaction between dopaminergic medication and an underlying vulnerability, rather than from PD itself, since: (a) only a (substantial) minority of medicated PD patients develop ICDs (Weintraub et al., 2010); (b) ICDs are no more frequent in patients with de novo PD than in the general population (Weintraub et al., 2013); (c) ICDs can develop in non-PD individuals treated with dopaminergic medication (O’Sullivan et al., 2010; Voon et al., 2011a); and (d) a family history of gambling problems is a risk factor for the development of dopaminergic medication-linked ICDs (Weintraub et al., 2010; Voon et al., 2011a).
This precursive ICD vulnerability likely reflects variation in pre-existing temperament/personality (Dagher and Robbins, 2009), in particular, variation in the broad temperament dimension of trait disinhibition (vs. constraint), encompassing impulsivity, novelty/sensation seeking, non-planning, low self-control, and related constructs (Markon et al., 2005). In PD, just as in non-PD populations (Sher and Trull, 1994; MacLaren et al., 2011), phenotypic associations between trait disinhibition and substance use and ICDs (and their comorbidity) have been repeatedly demonstrated (Evans et al., 2005; Voon et al., 2011b).
These findings suggest etiologic similarities between ICDs and addictions in PD and the broader domain of externalizing (EXT) psychopathology in non-PD populations (Krueger et al., 2007). Patterns of phenotypic, environmental, and genetic relations among DSM-defined substance misuse and antisocial disorders are best accounted for by models positing a shared broad EXT factor or vulnerability (Krueger et al., 2007). I.e., putatively distinct disorders are better understood as variants within a broader EXT spectrum of disorders, and the reason they systematically co-occur is because they share a common underlying genetic vulnerability (Krueger et al., 2007). This broad EXT vulnerability is a continuously distributed dimension of risk (Krueger et al., 2007). Hierarchical models of the EXT spectrum (Krueger et al., 2007) posit a general factor linking all EXT syndromes, as well as distinct etiological factors that differentiate among distinct EXT syndromes. Disordered gambling, frequently comorbid with substance abuse and antisocial personality disorder (Kessler et al., 2008) is also considered one variant of EXT (Oleski et al., 2011; Blanco et al., 2012; Forbush and Watson, 2013; but see Slutske et al., 2013).
A shared genetic diathesis underlies the phenotypic associations between EXT disorders and disinhibited personality traits, including novelty seeking (NS; Young et al., 2000; Agrawal et al., 2004). Furthermore, prospective studies suggest that high levels of these traits in childhood and adolescence predate and predict the emergence of EXT psychopathology in adulthood (Sher et al., 2000; Slutske et al., 2012). I.e., the temperamental antecedent of disinhibition provides the core endophenotype of EXT disorders (Clark, 2005), mediating their systematic co-variation (Khan et al., 2005). Hence, understanding the genetics and neurobiology of disinhibitory personality traits is critical to understanding EXT psychopathology, including its manifestation in the context of PD and its treatment with DA replacement therapies.
The DA system has frequently been hypothesized to underlie individual variation in trait disinhibition. According to one prominent model (Pickering and Gray, 1999), individual differences along this temperament dimension are argued to reflect variation in the reactivity of a neural behavioral activation system (BAS), centered on the ventral striatum (VS), and its dopaminergic irrigation, activated by cues for reward. Similarly, according to Cloninger (1987), the disinhibitory temperament trait of NS reflects genetically determined variation in dopaminergically mediated BAS reactivity. When activated, the BAS can be characterized as an impulsive “go” system that activates on-going appetitive behavior (Pine et al., 2010).
Research is accumulating to suggest that variation in the DA synthesis pathway in particular plays a key role in the etiology of EXT. DA synthesis occurs within DA neurons. Tyrosine is transported into the cell via amino acid carriers in the blood-brain barrier and cell membranes. Once in the intracellular space it is hydroxylated to l-3,4-dihydroxiphenylalanine (l-DOPA) by tyrosine hydroxylase (TH). l-DOPA is then decarboxylated by aromatic l-amino acid decarboxylase [AADC; also known as dopa decarboxylase (DDC)] to DA (Elsworth and Roth, 2009). Variants [single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs)] in the DDC gene have been associated with nicotine (Ma et al., 2005; Yu et al., 2006; Zhang et al., 2006), alcohol (Agrawal et al., 2011; Kristjansson et al., 2012) and illicit drug (Hack et al., 2011) misuse, and most recently, disordered gambling (Lind et al., 2012). Importantly, Derringer et al. (2010) found that a combination of multiple SNPs in the DDC gene predicted individual variation in sensation seeking traits, suggesting that genetic variation in DA synthesis is related to broad EXT risk.
Positron Emission Tomography (PET) can be used to study the activity of AADC/DDC in pre-synaptic DA terminals in the living brain. The PET tracer 6-[18F]fluoro-l-DOPA (FDOPA), a radioactive analog of l-DOPA, the precursor of DA, is taken up by pre-synaptic dopaminergic neurons and is metabolized by AADC/DDC to 18F-DA, which is trapped and stored within vesicles in the nerve terminals (Kumakura and Cumming, 2009). FDOPA uptake, quantified as the influx constant KI, can be used as a measure of AADC/DDC activity and vesicular storage capacity (Brown et al., 1999). High values for FDOPA KI are observed in areas of dense DA nerve terminal innervation, such as the striatum, and FDOPA has been extensively used to probe the functional integrity of striatal dopaminergic neurons in PD (Brooks, 2010) where uptake correlates with the number of surviving nigrostriatal cell numbers (Snow et al., 1993). Notably, FDOPA studies in non-PD populations have shown relatively increased striatal KI values in alcohol (Tiihonen et al., 1998) and nicotine (Salokangas et al., 2000) dependent individuals, although those early studies did not measure disinhibitory personality traits, and could not resolve dorsal and ventral striatal regions. Further, Laakso et al. (2005) found increased striatal FDOPA uptake in A1+ allele carriers of the Taq1A polymorphism of the TTC12-ANKK1-DRD2 gene cluster, variation in which has been linked with EXT risk (Ducci et al., 2011).
Collectively, the research outlined above suggests that individual variation in DA synthesis capacity, particularly in the VS, underpins individual variation in trait disinhibition. Here, we tested the hypothesis that increased FDOPA uptake in VS, which is relatively spared in PD (Kish et al., 1988), would predict increased trait disinhibition or general EXT risk, in the context of PD.
Materials and Methods
Patients
We studied 18 men with early-stage (symptom duration <2 years) idiopathic PD based on UK PD Society Brain Bank diagnostic criteria. Table 1 summarizes their clinical characteristics. Disease severity was rated with the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) motor subscale while in an “off” condition withdrawn from medication before PET. Assessment of trait disinhibition, as well as PET scanning, was performed in a practically defined off state. Patients with pronounced tremor that would have produced difficulty with PET imaging, as well as patients with comorbid psychiatric or systemic physical illness were excluded. This cohort of 18 patients was drawn from a larger group of 186 PD patients that formed the multicentre REAL-PET investigation enrolled population. Drug escalation, tablet frequency, dose, and UPDRS motor response to medication have been described previously (Whone et al., 2003b). At the time of PET scanning, patients had only been started on dopaminergic medication within the previous 4–12 weeks, and were receiving low doses of either l-DOPA (between 300 mg to a maximum daily dose of ∼400 mg at time of PET) or a DA agonist (between 3 mg of ropinirole to a maximum daily dose of ∼8 mg at time of PET). Patients were administered the 30-item Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS), validated for use in PD (Ertan et al., 2005).
Table 1.
Parkinson’s disease patients (n = 18) | |
---|---|
Gender (male/female) | 18/0 |
Age (years, mean ± SD, range) | 64 ± 7, range 47–76 |
UPDRS motor in “off” (mean ± SD) | 14.6 ± 6.5 |
GDS Depression (mean ± SD) | 9.8 ± 8.9 |
The study was limited to men for several reasons. Firstly, ICDs and addictions in PD are more prevalent in males than females (Weintraub et al., 2010), as they are in non-PD populations (Eaton et al., 2012). Secondly, there are sex differences in DA synthesis capacity (Laakso et al., 2002). Thirdly, genetic factors for NS are excellent markers for EXT tendencies in males, but not females (Agrawal et al., 2004; Pitzer et al., 2007), although it is unclear whether this sex difference is an artifact of the manner in which males and females interpret the NS construct or whether there are true differences in the magnitude of genetic influence across males and females. It will be important in future studies to include females with PD.
Permission to undertake the study was granted by the Ethics Committee of Hammersmith, Queen Charlotte’s & Chelsea, and Acton Hospital Trust and all participants gave written informed consent following a full explanation of the procedure, in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki. The Administration of Radioactive Substances Advisory Committee (ARSAC) of the UK approved radioisotope use.
Measurement of trait disinhibition (vs. constraint)
Our measure of trait disinhibition (vs. constraint) was based on NS from Cloninger’s Tri-dimensional Personality Questionnaire (TPQ; Cloninger, 1987). The version of the TPQ used here was a 100-item, self-administered, true-false instrument. The questionnaire is scored so that higher scores reflect greater NS.
As originally constructed (Cloninger, 1987) TPQ-NS comprised four narrow facet-level scales: Exploratory Excitability vs. Stoic Rigidity (NS1), Impulsiveness vs. Reflection (NS2), Extravagance vs. Reserve (NS3), and Disorderliness vs. Regimentation (NS4). When Ando et al. (2004), however, examined the genetic and environmental factor structure of NS, factor analysis of the genetic inter-correlations yielded factors that did not fully resemble the phenotypic structure of NS as proposed by Cloninger (1987). NS was revised (r-NS) to consist of Impulsiveness vs. Reflection (NS2), Extravagance vs. Reserve (NS3) and Disorderliness vs. Regimentation (NS4), excluding Exploratory Excitability vs. Stoic Rigidity (NS1). Further, Flory et al. (2006), using factor analysis in a large normative sample of middle-aged adults, found Impulsiveness vs. Reflection (NS2) and Extravagance vs. Reserve (NS3) to have high loadings on a “non-planning impulsivity” factor, together with the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS), whereas Exploratory Excitability vs. Stoic Rigidity (NS1) loaded on a distinct “Openness to Experience” factor, and Disorderliness vs. Regimentation (NS4) failed to load strongly on any single factor. Hence, in the current study, we focused on those r-NS facets most strongly linked to trait disinhibition: Impulsiveness (vs. Reflection; NS2; eight items) and Extravagance (vs. Reserve; NS3; seven items). Sample items include “I often react so strongly to unexpected news that I say or do things that I regret,” “I often have to change my decisions because I had a wrong hunch or mistaken first impression” (Impulsivity, NS2) and “I often spend money until I run out of cash or get into debt from using too much credit,” “Because I so often spend too much money on impulse, it is hard for me to save money – even for special plans like a vacation” (Extravagance, NS3).
PET scanning protocol
Patients stopped medication at least 12 h prior to scanning. All participants underwent three-dimensional FDOPA PET using an ECAT EXACT HR++ (CTI/Siemens 966) camera, which covers an axial field of view of 23.4 cm and provides 95 transaxial planes. The tomograph has a spatial resolution of 4.8 + 0.2 mm FWHM (transaxial, 1 cm off axis) and 5.6 + 0.5 mm (axial, on axis) after image reconstruction (Spinks et al., 2000). A transmission scan, which corrects for attenuation of emitted radiation by skull and tissues, was acquired using a single rotating photon point source of 150 MBq of 137Cs. Thirty seconds after the start of the emission scan, 110 MBq of FDOPA in 10 ml normal saline was infused intravenously over 30 s. Three-dimensional sinograms of emission data were then acquired over 94 min as 26 time frames. Patients were placed in the scanner, orientated parallel to the orbito-metal line and head positioning was monitored throughout the scan.
Image quantification
Parametric images of specific FDOPA uptake (KI maps) were created at a voxel level for the whole brain using the Patlak graphical approach (Patlak and Blasberg, 1985) with a cerebellar cortex reference input function (Moore et al., 2003). Qualitative summated images created from the dynamic FDOPA time series by integrating all 26 frames of the dynamic image were also produced and then transformed into standard stereotaxic [Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI)] space using an FDOPA template created in-house from a healthy volunteer database. These so-called ADD images contain both tracer delivery and specific uptake information and provide adequate anatomical detail to allow stereotaxic manipulations. Subsequently, the KI maps were also individually normalized to MNI stereotaxic space by applying the transformation parameters defined during the normalization of their respective ADD image. This spatial transformation of parametric images made it possible to perform a region of interest (ROI) analysis as described below.
Region of interest analysis
An ROI analysis was performed using a fixed object map template after stereotaxic transformation of all images into standard (MNI) space. This approach effectively normalizes brain position and shape and so avoids variability of regions due to head size and position and subjective free hand definition. Standard ROI object maps that outlined the left and right hemisphere VS, caudate, and putamen were defined on the MNI single-subject ROI in stereotaxic space (Moore et al., 2003, 2008). The standard object map was placed onto the transformed KI maps, and values for specific FDOPA uptake were obtained for each region (see Figure 1 for ROIs). When performing our ROI analysis a manual correction for head movement was employed as described previously (Whone et al., 2003a).
To limit the number of comparisons, and because of our specific a priori hypothesis, KI values (units: ml g−1 min−1) for the caudate and putamen ROIs were averaged to form left and right dorsal striatum (DS) ROIs. Exploratory data analysis revealed that KI and NS data were normally distributed, and so we examined the relation between them using Pearson’s correlation coefficient. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.01.
Results
Mean ± SD scores in our PD sample for NS2 (Impulsivity) and NS3 (Extravagance) were 3.1 ± 1.6 and 3.1 ± 1.7, respectively. Whilst population norms for individual NS facets are unavailable, the total NS score (NS1 + NS2 + NS3 + NS4) of the current PD sample (13.6 ± 4) was comparable to that published for age-matched healthy male controls (n > 1000; mean age 67 ± 8; total NS = 11.6 ± 5; Stallings et al., 1996). Mean ± SD FDOPA KI values for left and right VS and left and right DS are reported in Table 2.
Table 2.
Region | FDOPA KI values (mean ± SD) |
---|---|
Left ventral striatum | 0.0127 ± 0.0015 |
Right ventral striatum | 0.0120 ± 0.0014 |
Left dorsal striatum | 0.0115 ± 0.0018 |
Right dorsal striatum | 0.0108 ± 0.0015 |
In line with our a priori hypothesis, we found a significant correlation between NS3 (Extravagance) scores and left VS FDOPA KI values [r (16) = 0.61, p = 0.008, 95% CI 0.20 – 0.83; Figure 2]. This relationship was unchanged when controlling for both age and GDS depression scores (r = 0.63, p = 0.009). There was a similar trend in the right VS that failed to reach our pre-specified level of statistical significance (r = 0.53, p = 0.026). (An additional analysis revealed that the slightly greater correlation between left VS FDOPA KI and NS3 was not a result of asymmetry of Parkinsonian symptoms). Furthermore, neither left (r = −0.13, p = 0.60) nor right (r = −0.22, p = 0.38) VS FDOPA KI values correlated with UPDRS motor scores. There was no significant relation between DS FDOPA KI values and NS3 scores, although the correlation with right DS values trended toward significance (left, r = 0.27, p = 0.27; right, r = 0.41, p = 0.09). Importantly, however, the correlation between left VS FDOPA KI values and NS3 scores was significantly greater than that between either left [t = 2.59, p (1-tail) = 0.01] or right [t = 1.46, p (1-tail) = 0.08] DS FDOPA KI values and NS3 scores.
Against our hypothesis, however, there was no significant correlation between NS2 (Impulsivity) scores and VS FDOPA KI values (left, r = 0.08, p = 0.77; right, r = 0.30, p = 0.22). Nor was there a significant correlation between NS2 scores and DS KI values (left, r = −0.12, p = 0.65; right, r = 0.22, p = 0.37). The correlation between left VS FDOPA KI values and NS3 (Extravagance) scores was significantly greater than that between left VS FDOPA KI values and NS2 (Impulsivity) scores [t = 2.67, p (1-tail) = 0.008].
Discussion
Our findings provide new insights into the neurobiology of potential vulnerability to impulse control problems in PD. Consistent with our hypothesis, we found that variation in trait disinhibition (vs. constraint), a continuously varying endophenotype for EXT or impulse-control psychopathology, was associated with levels of DA synthesis capacity in the VS. Individuals with higher levels of trait disinhibition, in particular, tendencies to financial irresponsibility and extravagance, had greater DA synthesis capacity, as indexed by FDOPA KI values in the left (and to a lesser extent, right) ventral, but not dorsal, striatum.
Although ICDs in PD have to date been treated as categorical (present/absent) disorders, it is now widely accepted that such disorders do not delineate highly discrete and easily distinguished categories. Rather, they delineate continuous, normally distributed underlying propensities to experience psychopathology, with personality traits linked to behavioral disinhibition forming the core of an EXT spectrum (Clark, 2005; Krueger et al., 2007). I.e., psychopathology represents the extremes of continuously distributed temperament traits. This general temperamental propensity to EXT, rather than its manifestations in particular disorders is therefore a key focus for etiological investigations.
Whilst a recent study linked genetic variation in DDC activity to an EXT endophenotype (sensation seeking) in healthy individuals (Derringer et al., 2010), and sensation seeking has been linked to increased ventral striatal BOLD-fMRI response during reward anticipation (Bjork et al., 2008; see also Yau et al., 2012), ours is the first demonstration that variation in trait disinhibition is linked to variation in DDC activity in the VS in PD, an important brain region underlying inter-individual variation in behavioral disinhibition in preclinical models (Dalley et al., 2011).
We found that only the r-NS subscale NS3 (Extravagance vs. Reserve) was related to ventral striatal DA synthesis capacity. There was no such relation with the NS2 subscale (Impulsivity vs. Reflection), despite the relatively high correlation between NS2 and NS3 (r = 0.45). Both subscales showed a similar range of variation in our PD sample, suggesting this is not the reason for a lack of correlation between NS2 scores and VS FDOPA KI values. NS3 appears more trait-like (evidenced by much higher test-rest correlation) than NS2 (Takeuchi et al., 2011), and appears to index those traits (irresponsibility, inability to approach life planfully) most strongly linked to general EXT risk (Krueger and South, 2009). It is particularly notable that only NS3 correlates with impaired decision-making on the Iowa Gambling Task (Álvarez-Moya et al., 2011), which has been linked to increased VS FDOPA KI values (Schlagenhauf et al., 2012).
On first glance, our results appear inconsistent with an earlier study that failed to find a correlation between trait NS and VS FDOPA KI values in unmedicated male PD patients (Kaasinen et al., 2001). However, that study used the total NS score from the original NS scales described by Cloninger (1987), and it is now clear (Ando et al., 2004) that the genetic and environmental factor structure of NS does not resemble the phenotypic structure of NS as originally proposed, making interpretation of that previous null result difficult.
FDOPA is not a specific ligand for DA neurons but rather is trapped by all neurons that contain DDC (Brown et al., 1999). Hence, it is a marker for all tissues that take up and store monoamines, including serotonin (5-HT) as well as DA neurons (Tison et al., 1991). 5-HT has also been implicated in various aspects of impulsivity (Winstanley et al., 2005; Miyazaki et al., 2011). However, post hoc analysis of a midbrain raphe ROI (Moore et al., 2008), a structure in which FDOPA is a validated marker of serotonergic function in PD (Pavese et al., 2012), in our sample, found no significant correlation between FDOPA KI values in the raphe and either NS2 (Impulsivity) or NS3 (Extravagance) scores (r = 0.11, p = 0.67 and r = 0.32, p = 0.2, respectively). Further, in MPTP treated monkeys, Karimi et al. (2013) found a near perfect correlation (n = 16, r2 = 0.95) between striatal FDOPA KI values obtained in vivo with PET and post-mortem striatal DA levels (measured using high performance liquid chromatography with electrochemical detection). It is likely, therefore, that individual differences in trait disinhibition are primarily related to individual differences in DA synthesis capacity.
This interpretation is consistent with several other recent findings on the neurobiology of impulse-control and addiction vulnerability. D2 autoreceptors located on DA nerve terminals exert a negative feedback regulation that reduces DA synthesis, DA neuron firing, and DA release (Wolf and Roth, 1990; Zhu et al., 1992). Bello et al. (2011) generated mice deficient in such D2 autoreceptors. These “autoDrd2KO” mice displayed elevated striatal DA synthesis and strikingly, demonstrated increased sensitivity to the rewarding and psychomotor stimulant properties of cocaine, together with a greatly exaggerated motivation to work for food reward. I.e., heightened DA synthesis in the autoDrd2KO mice resulted in an addiction-prone, disinhibited phenotype. In humans, striatal DA synthesis capacity measured with [β-11C]DOPA PET is negatively correlated with striatal D2 receptor density measured with [11C]raclopride PET (Ito et al., 2011). Given that many pre-synaptic D2 receptors in the striatum are DA autoreceptors (Sesack et al., 1994), increased DA synthesis capacity in humans may similarly reflect reduced D2 autoreceptor function. Notably, in animal models, variation in trait impulsivity, measured by premature responding on a 5-choice serial reaction time test of visual attention, has been linked to relatively reduced D2 receptor levels in the VS, functioning as autoreceptors (Besson et al., 2010).
Consistent with this, in humans, Buckholtz et al. (2010) (see also Zald et al., 2008) recently found that trait non-planning impulsivity was inversely correlated with D2/D3 autoreceptor availability in the substantia nigra/ventral tegmental area, measured using [18F]fallypride PET. Furthermore, Van Leare et al. (2009) found a selective negative correlation between cannabinoid CB1 receptor density, measured using [18F]MK-9470 PET, in left hemisphere limbic regions and NS3 (Extravagance) scores in healthy volunteers. Since CB1 receptor activation causes inhibition of DA synthesis (Moranta et al., 2004), reduced density of CB1 receptors would presumably result in higher levels of DA synthesis, and the negative correlation between CB1 receptor density and NS3 is consistent with the positive correlation between ventral striatal DA synthesis capacity and NS3 seen here.
Furthermore, Bello et al. (2011) found that autoDrd2KO mice showed exaggerated phasic DA release, resulting from a larger releasable pool of DA generated by the lack of DA synthesis inhibition in DA terminals. Similarly, in both monkeys (Doudet and Holden, 2003) and human individuals with PD (Piccini et al., 2003), the magnitude of amphetamine-induced striatal DA release as measured by [11C]-raclopride PET (an indirect measure of phasic DA release, Grace, 2008), is positively correlated with DA synthesis capacity measured using FDOPA PET. In humans, Buckholtz et al. (2010) found that trait disinhibition predicted greater amph-induced DA release (together with stronger drug-primed wanting) and the relation between midbrain autoreceptor availability and trait impulsivity was mediated via this enhanced amphetamine-induced striatal DA release.
The influence of variation in DA synthesis capacity on EXT tendency might result from DA’s established role in reward processing, particularly the attribution of incentive salience (Berridge, 2007). Incentive salience is a motivational component of reward, one that transforms sensory information about rewards and reward cues into attractive, “wanted” incentives, motivating pursuit (Berridge, 2007). Notably, in animals, there is considerable individual variability in incentive salience attribution, and rats with a strong propensity to attribute incentive salience to reward cues also show heightened behavioral disinhibition, risk-taking, and an increased tendency to seek drugs like cocaine (Flagel et al., 2010). Available data suggest that animals prone to attribute incentive salience to reward cues have a more active DA system than those who do not (Marinelli and White, 2000; Tomie et al., 2000). In healthy volunteers VS FDopa KI values have been found to positively correlate with BOLD-fMRI activity to reward cues in brain regions linked to incentive salience attribution (Siessmeier et al., 2006), and ventral striatal BOLD-fMRI response to reward cues in humans is increased as a function of trait non-planning impulsivity and is linked to genetic variation in the D2 (auto)-receptor (Forbes et al., 2009).
Of particular relevance to the development of extreme impulse control problems following treatment with dopaminergic medication in PD, rats with heightened incentive salience attribution show a greater propensity for dopaminergic drug-induced sensitization – a form of neuroplasticity hypothesized to play a key role in addiction (Flagel et al., 2008). In particular, the incentive-sensitization theory (Robinson and Berridge, 2008) posits that excessive drug use arises from the excessive attribution of incentive salience to drug rewards and their cues, due to progressive neuroadaptations in DA projections to ventral striatal motivation circuitry. Strikingly, PD individuals with DA dysregulation syndrome were shown to exhibit potentiated l-DOPA-induced ventral striatal DA release, which correlated with exaggerated drug-primed l-DOPA wanting, together with enhanced responding to monetary reward (Evans et al., 2006). Also, PD (and non-PD) individuals with disordered gambling showed impaired DA D2 autoreceptor activity (Ray et al., 2012) and exaggerated VS DA release to gambling and other reward cues (O’Sullivan et al., 2011; see also Steeves et al., 2009; Joutsa et al., 2012a), consistent with incentive-sensitization.
It is possible that, rather than reflecting an underlying neurobiological risk factor for ICDs in PD, increased VS FDOPA uptake could be the result of disease-related and/or drug-induced neuroplastic changes leading to disease- and/or drug-induced disinhibition. In the current sample of early-stage PD patients, who had only recently begun DA replacement therapy, however, mean ventral striatal FDOPA KI values were very similar to those of healthy controls (Whone et al., 2004) and were unrelated to severity of PD. In animal models, there is some evidence that repeated treatments with DA agonist drugs can enhance basal DA synthesis in the VS (Rowlett et al., 1997) and frontal cortex (Chernoloz et al., 2012). However, other studies show persistent inhibition of DA synthesis following repeated treatment with DA agonists (Imperato et al., 1996). A study of detoxified alcoholics found no differences in VS DA synthesis capacity measured with FDOPA PET (Heinz et al., 2005). Very recently, Joutsa et al. (2012b) found increased medial orbitofrontal, but not ventral striatal, FDOPA uptake in 10 individuals with manifest ICDs in PD. Since disinhibitory traits were not measured in the patients or controls of that study, however, it may be that several of the controls had high EXT risk, explaining the lack of difference in VS FDOPA uptake between ICD cases and controls. Frontal, but not striatal, DA synthesis is also increased following stress (Reinhard et al., 1982), suggesting that enhanced frontal, but not ventral striatal, FDOPA uptake in ICD cases may reflect state, as opposed to trait, effects.
Furthermore, Giladi et al. (2007) and Bastiaens et al. (2013) found that the appearance of heightened impulse control problems in affected PD individuals began only after a median interval of ∼2 years of treatment, and our patients had only recently been started on dopaminergic medication, within the previous 4–12 weeks, and were on only low doses of either l-DOPA or DA agonist. In addition, Voon et al. (2007) found that NS scores were not influenced by dopaminergic therapy. Importantly, numerous studies have found a shared genetic diathesis to underlie associations between EXT disorders and traits related to disinhibition, including NS (Young et al., 2000; Agrawal et al., 2004). Most critically, Derringer et al. (2010) found that genetic variation in DDC activity predicted variation in trait disinhibition, suggesting that variation in DDC activity reflects a trait EXT vulnerability, rather than a disease or medication-related effect (see also Engeln et al., 2012). Likewise, Flagel et al. (2010), found that, in rats, variation in incentive salience attribution and behavioral disinhibition are genetically influenced, correlated traits that depend on ventral striatal DA for their expression (Saunders and Robinson, 2012).
Hence, we think it highly unlikely that our findings of a positive correlation between levels of self-reported disinhibition and VS DA synthesis capacity are a result of PD and its treatment with dopaminergic medication. Rather, we think they reflect genetically influenced, pre-existing neurobiological individual differences in VS DA synthesis capacity in PD patients, underpinning variation in trait disinhibition or EXT propensity, which in turn is associated with future ICD morbidity risk (as PD advances and dopaminergic drug therapy increases), resulting from an increased susceptibility to DAergic-drug-induced incentive-sensitization (Boileau et al., 2006). Longitudinal studies will be required to establish the validity of this account.
In conclusion, we have shown that temperamental vulnerability to impulse control or EXT problems such as disordered gambling in male PD patients is related to relatively greater DA synthesis capacity in the ventral, but not dorsal, striatum. Our results are consistent with preclinical models of EXT risk, and may prove informative in understanding the psychological and neurobiological mechanisms whereby individual differences in temperament contribute to the development of impulse control and addictive pathologies in the context of the treatment of PD and other neurological disorders.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
Support for this work was provided by Parkinson’s UK (grant ref. G0603). We are most grateful to the technical PET staff for their assistance, to Dr N. Pavese for assistance with Figure 1 and to all the patients who participated in this study.
References
- Agrawal A., Jacobson K. C., Prescott C. A., Kendler K. S. (2004). A twin study of personality and illicit drug use and abuse/dependence. Twin Res. 7, 72–81 10.1375/136905204323016159 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agrawal A., Lynskey M. T., Todorov A. A., Schrage A. J., Littlefield A. K., Grant J. D., et al. (2011). A candidate gene association study of alcohol consumption in young women. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 35, 1–9 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01316.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Álvarez-Moya E. M., Ochoa C., Jiménez-Murcia S., Aymamí M. N., Gómez-Peña M., Fernández-Aranda F., et al. (2011). Effect of executive functioning, decision-making and self-reported impulsivity on the treatment outcome of pathologic gambling. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 36, 165–175 10.1503/jpn.090095 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ando J., Suzuki A., Yamagata S., Kijima N., Maekawa H., Ono Y., et al. (2004). Genetic and environmental structure of Cloninger’s temperament and character dimensions. J. Pers. Disord. 18, 379–393 10.1521/pedi.2004.18.4.379 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastiaens J., Dorfman B. J., Christos B. J., Nirenberg M. J. (2013). Prospective cohort study of impulse control disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1002/mds.25291 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bello E. P., Mateo Y., Gelman D. M., Noaín D., Shin J. H., Low M. J., et al. (2011). Cocaine supersensitivity and enhanced motivation for reward in mice lacking dopamine D2 autoreceptors. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1033–1038 10.1038/nn.2862 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge K. C. (2007). The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: the case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 191, 391–431 10.1007/s00213-006-0578-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Besson M., Belin D., McNamara R., Theobold D. E. H., Castel A., Beckett V. L., et al. (2010). Dissociable control of impulsivity in rats by dopamine D2/3 receptors in the core and shell subregions of the nucleus accumbens. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 560–569 10.1038/npp.2009.162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bienfait K. L., Menza M., Mark M. H., Dobkin R. D. (2010). Impulsive smoking in a patient with Parkinson’s disease treated with dopamine agonists. J. Clin. Neurosci. 17, 539–540 10.1016/j.jocn.2009.09.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bjork J. M., Knutson B., Hommer D. W. (2008). Incentive-elicited striatal activation in adolescent children of alcoholics. Addiction 103, 1308–1319 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02250.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blanco C., Myers J., Kendler K. S. (2012). Gambling, disordered gambling and their association with major depression and substance abuse: a web-based cohort and twin-sibling study. Psychol. Med. 42, 497–508 10.1017/S0033291711001401 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boileau I., Dagher A., Leyton M., Gunn R. N., Baker G. B., Diksic M., et al. (2006). Modelling sensitization to stimulants in humans: an [11C]raclopride/position emission tomography study in healthy men. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 63, 1386–1395 10.1001/archpsyc.63.12.1386 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks D. J. (2010). Imaging approaches to Parkinson’s disease. J. Nucl. Med. 51, 596–609 10.2967/jnumed.108.059998 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown W. D., Taylor M. D., Roberts A. D., Oakes T. R., Schueller M. J., Holden J. E., et al. (1999). FluoroDOPA PET shows the nondopaminergic as well as dopaminergic destinations of levodopa. Neurology 53, 1212–1218 10.1212/WNL.53.6.1212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckholtz J. W., Treadway M. T., Cowan R. L., Woodward N. D., Li R., Ansari M. S., et al. (2010). Dopaminergic network differences in human impulsivity. Science 329, 532. 10.1126/science.1185778 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chernoloz O., El Mansari M., Blier P. (2012). Long-term administration of the dopamine D3/2 receptor agonist pramipexole increases dopamine and serotonin neurotransmission in the male rat forebrain. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 37, 113–121 10.1503/jpn.110038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark L. A. (2005). Temperament as a unifying basis for personality and psychopathology. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 114, 505–521 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.505 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cloninger C. R. (1987). A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 44, 573–588 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800180093014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dagher A., Robbins T. W. (2009). Personality, addiction, dopamine: insights from Parkinson’s disease. Neuron 61, 502–510 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalley J. W., Everitt B. J., Robbins T. W. (2011). Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron 69, 680–694 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derringer J., Krueger R. F., Dick D. M., Saccone S., Grucza R. A., Agrawal A., et al. (2010). Predicting sensation seeking from dopamine genes: a candidate-system approach. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1282–1290 10.1177/0956797610380699 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doudet D. J., Holden J. E. (2003). Raclopride studies of dopamine release: dependence on presynaptic integrity. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 1193–1199 10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00288-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ducci F., Kaakinen M., Pouta A., Hartikainen A.-L., Veijola J., Isohanni M., et al. (2011). TTC12-ANKK1-DRD2 and CHRNA5-CHRNA3-CHRNB4 influence different pathways leading to smoking behavior from adolescence to mid-adulthood. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 650–660 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eaton N. R., Keyes K. M., Krueger R. F., Balsis S., Skodol A. E., Markon K. E., et al. (2012). An invariant dimensional liability model of gender differences in mental disorder prevalence: evidence from a national sample. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 121, 282–288 10.1037/a0024780 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elsworth J. D., Roth R. H. (2009). “Dopamine,” in Encyclopaedia of Neuroscience, ed. Squire L. R. (Amsterdam: Elsevier; ), 539–547 [Google Scholar]
- Engeln M., Ahmed S., Vouillac C., Tison F., Bezard E., Fernagut P.-O. (2012). Reinforcing properties of pramipexole in normal and parkinsonian rats. Neurobiol. Dis. 49C, 79–86 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ertan F. S., Ertan T., Kiziltan G., Uyguçgil H. (2005). Reliability and validity of the geriatric depression scale in depression in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 76, 1445–1447 10.1136/jnnp.2004.057984 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans A. H., Lawrence A. D., Potts J., Appel S., Lees A. J. (2005). Factors influencing susceptibility to compulsive dopaminergic drug use in Parkinson disease. Neurology 65, 1570–1574 10.1212/01.wnl.0000184487.72289.f0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans A. H., Pavese N., Lawrence A. D., Tai Y. F., Appel S., Doder M., et al. (2006). Compulsive drug use linked to sensitized ventral striatal dopamine transmission. Ann. Neurol. 59, 852–858 10.1002/ana.20822 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flagel S. B., Robinson T. E., Clark J. J., Clinton S. M., Watson S. J., Seeman P., et al. (2010). An animal model of genetic vulnerability to behavioral disinhibition and responsiveness to reward-related cues: implications for addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 388–400 10.1038/npp.2009.142 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flagel S. B., Watson S. J., Akil H., Robinson T. E. (2008). Individual differences in the attribution of incentive salience to a reward-related cue: influence on cocaine sensitization. Behav. Brain Res. 186, 48–56 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.07.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flory J. D., Harvey P. D., Mitropoulou V., New A. S., Silverman J. M., Siever L., et al. (2006). Dispositional impulsivity in normal and abnormal samples. J. Psychiatr. Res. 40, 438–447 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forbes E. E., Brown S. M., Kimak M., Ferrell R. E., Manuck S. B., Hariri A. R. (2009). Genetic variation in components of dopamine neurotransmission impacts ventral striatal reactivity associated with impulsivity. Mol. Psychiatry 14, 60–70 10.1038/sj.mp.4002086 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forbush K. T., Watson D. (2013). The structure of common and uncommon mental disorders. Psychol. Med. 43, 97–108 10.1017/S0033291712001092 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher D. A., O’Sullivan S. S., Evans A. H., Lees A. J., Schrag A. (2007). Pathological gambling in Parkinson’s disease: risk factors and differences from dopamine dysregulation. An analysis of published case series. Mov. Disord. 22, 1757–1763 10.1002/mds.21611 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giladi N., Weitzman N., Schreiber S., Shabtai H., Peretz C. (2007). New onset heightened interest or drive for gambling, shopping, eating or sexual activity in patients with Parkinson’s disease: the role of dopamine agonist treatment and age at motor symptoms onset. J. Psychopharmacol. 21, 501–506 10.1177/0269881106073109 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grace A. A. (2008). Physiology of the normal and dopamine-depleted basal ganglia: insights into levodopa pharmacotherapy. Mov. Disord. 23, S560–S569 10.1002/mds.22020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hack L. M., Kalsi G., Aliev F., Kuo P. H., Prescott C. A., Patterson D. G., et al. (2011). Limited associations of dopamine system genes with alcohol dependence and related traits in the Irish Affected Sib Pair Study of Alcohol Dependence (IASPSAD). Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 35, 376–385 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01353.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinz A., Siessmeier T., Wrase J., Buchholz H. G., Gründer G., Kumakura Y., et al. (2005). Correlation of alcohol craving with striatal dopamine synthesis capacity and D2/3 receptor availability: a combined [18F]DOPA and [18F]DMFP PET study in detoxified alcoholic patients. Am. J. Psychiatry 162, 1515–1520 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1515 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Imperato A., Obinu M. C., Carta G., Mascia M. S., Casu M. A., Gessa G. L. (1996). Reduction of dopamine release and synthesis by repeated amphetamine treatment: role in behavioral sensitization. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 317, 231–237 10.1016/S0014-2999(96)00742-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito H., Kodaka F., Takahashi H., Takano H., Arakawa R., Shimada H., et al. (2011). Relation between presynaptic and postsynaptic dopaminergic functions measured by positron emission tomography: implications of dopaminergic tone. J. Neurosci. 31, 7886–7890 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6588-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joutsa J., Johansson J., Niemelä S., Ollikainen A., Hirvonen M. M., Piepponen P., et al. (2012a). Mesolimbic dopamine release is linked to symptom severity in pathological gambling. Neuroimage 60, 1992–1999 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joutsa J., Martikainen K., Niemelä S., Johansson J., Forsback S., Rinne J. O., et al. (2012b). Increased medial orbitofrontal [18F]fluorodopa uptake in parkinsonian impulse control disorders. Mov. Disord. 27, 778–782 10.1002/mds.24941 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaasinen V., Nurmi E., Bergman J., Eskola O., Solin O., Sonninen P., et al. (2001). Personality traits and brain dopaminergic function in Parkinson’s disease. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 13272–13277 10.1073/pnas.231313198 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karimi M., Tian L., Brown C. A., Flores H. P., Loftin S. K., Videen T. O., et al. (2013). Validation of nigrostriatal PET measures: critical limits. Ann. Neurol. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1002/ana.23798 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler R. C., Hwang I., LaBrie R., Petukhova M., Sampson N. A., Winters K. C., et al. (2008). DSM-IV pathological gambling in the national comorbidity survey replication. Psychol. Med. 38, 1351–1360 10.1017/S0033291708002900 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khan A. A., Jacobson K. C., Gardner C. O., Prescott C. A., Kendler K. S. (2005). Personality and comorbidity of common psychiatric disorders. Br. J. Psychiatry 186, 190–196 10.1192/bjp.186.3.190 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kish S. J., Shannak K., Hornykiewicz O. (1988). Uneven pattern of dopamine loss in the striatum of patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: pathophysiologic and clinical implications. N. Engl. J. Med. 318, 876–880 10.1056/NEJM198804073181402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kristjansson S. D., Agrawal A., Lessov-Schlaggar C. N., Madden P. A., Cooper M. L., Bucholz K. K., et al. (2012). The relationship between rs3779084 in the dopa decarboxylase (DDC) gene and alcohol consumption is mediated by drinking motives in regular smokers. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 36, 162–170 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01596.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger R. F., Markon K. E., Patrick C. J., Benning S. D., Kramer M. D. (2007). Linking antisocial behavior, substance use, and personality: an integrative quantitative model of the adult externalizing spectrum. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 116, 645–666 10.1037/0021-843X.116.4.645 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger R. F., South S. C. (2009). Externalizing disorders: cluster 5 of the proposed meta-structure for DSM-V and ICD-11. Psychol. Med. 39, 2061–2070 10.1017/S0033291709990328 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumakura Y., Cumming P. (2009). PET studies of cerebral levodopa metabolism: a review of clinical findings and modelling approaches. Neuroscientist 15, 635–650 10.1177/1073858409338217 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laakso A., Pohjalainen T., Bergman J., Kajander J., Haaparanta M., Solin O., et al. (2005). The A1 allele of the human D2 dopamine receptor gene is associated with increased activity of striatal l-amino acid decarboxylase in healthy subjects. Pharmacogenetics 15, 387–391 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laakso A., Vilkman H., Bergman J., Haaparanta M., Solin O., Syvälahti E., et al. (2002). Sex differences in striatal presynaptic dopamine synthesis capacity in healthy subjects. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 759–763 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01369-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence A. D., Evans A. H., Lees A. J. (2003). Compulsive use of dopamine replacement therapy in Parkinson’s disease: reward systems gone awry? Lancet Neurol. 2, 595–604 10.1016/S1474-4422(03)00529-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lind P. A., Zhu G., Montgomery G. W., Madden P. A., Heath A. C., Martin N. G., et al. (2012). Genome-wide association study of a quantitative disordered gambling trait. Addict. Biol. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00463.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma J. Z., Beuten J., Payne T. J., Dupont R. T., Elston R. C., Li M. D. (2005). Haplotype analysis indicates an association between the DOPA decarboxylase (DDC) gene and nicotine dependence. Hum. Mol. Genet. 14, 1691–1698 10.1093/hmg/ddi399 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacLaren V. V., Fugelsang J. A., Harrigan K. A., Dixon M. J. (2011). The personality of pathological gamblers: a meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31, 1057–1067 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marinelli M., White F. J. (2000). Enhanced vulnerability to cocaine self-administration is associated with elevated impulse activity of midbrain dopamine neurons. J. Neurosci. 20, 8876–8885 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markon K. E., Krueger R. F., Watson D. (2005). Delineating the structure of normal and abnormal personality: an integrative hierarchical approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88, 139–157 10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.139 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyazaki K., Miyazaki K. W., Doya K. (2011). Activation of dorsal raphe serotonin neurons underlies waiting for delayed rewards. J. Neurosci. 31, 469–479 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3714-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore R. Y., Whone A. L., Brooks D. J. (2008). Extrastriatal monoamine neuron function in Parkinson’s disease: an 18F-dopa PET study. Neurobiol. Dis. 29, 381–390 10.1016/j.nbd.2007.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore R. Y., Whone A. L., McGowan S., Brooks D. J. (2003). Monoamine neuron innervation of the normal human brain: an 18F-DOPA PET study. Brain Res. 982, 137–145 10.1016/S0006-8993(03)02721-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moranta D., Esteban S., García-Sevilla J. A. (2004). Differential effects of acute cannabinoid drug treatment, mediated by CB1 receptors, on the in vivo activity of tyrosine and tryptophan hydroxylase in the rat brain. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch. Pharmacol. 369, 516–524 10.1007/s00210-004-0921-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oleski J., Cox B. J., Clara I., Hills A. (2011). Pathological gambling and the structure of common mental disorders. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 199, 956–960 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182392931 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Sullivan S., Djamshidian A., Ahmed Z., Evans A. H., Lawrence A. D., Holton J. L., et al. (2010). Impulsive-compulsive spectrum behaviours in pathologically confirmed progressive supranuclear palsy. Mov. Disord. 25, 638–642 10.1002/mds.22687 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Sullivan S. S., Wu K., Politis M., Lawrence A. D., Evans A. H., Bose S. K., et al. (2011). Cue-induced striatal dopamine release in Parkinson’s disease associated impulse control disorders. Brain 134, 969–978 10.1093/brain/awr003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patlak C. S., Blasberg R. G. (1985). Graphical evaluation of blood-to-brain transfer constants from multiple-time uptake data. Generalizations. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 5, 584–590 10.1038/jcbfm.1985.87 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pavese N., Simpson B. S., Metta V., Ramlackhansingh A., Chaudhuri K. R., Brooks D. J. (2012). [(18)F]FDOPA uptake in the raphe nuclei complex reflects serotonin transporter availability. A combined [(18F)]FDOPA and [(11)C]DASB PET study in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroimage 59, 1080–1084 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piccini P., Pavese N., Brooks D. J. (2003). Endogenous dopamine release after pharmacological challenge in Parkinson’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 53, 647–653 10.1002/ana.10526 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickering A. D., Gray J. A. (1999). “The neuroscience of personality,” in Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research, 2nd Edn, eds Pervin L. A., John O. P. (New York: The Guilford Press; ), 277–299 [Google Scholar]
- Pine A., Shiner T., Seymour B., Dolan R. J. (2010). Dopamine, time and impulsivity in humans. J. Neurosci. 30, 8888–8896 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6028-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitzer M., Esser G., Schmidt M. H., Laucht M. (2007). Temperament in the developmental course: a longitudinal comparison of New York longitudinal study-derived dimensions with the junior temperament and character inventory. Compr. Psychiatry 48, 572–582 10.1016/j.comppsych.2007.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ray N., Miyasaki J. M., Zurowski M., Ko J. H., Cho S. S., Lang A. E., et al. (2012). Extrastriatal dopaminergic abnormalities of DA homeostasis in Parkinson’s patients with medication-induced pathological gambling: a [11C] FLB-457 and PET study. Neurobiol. Dis. 48, 519–525 10.1016/j.nbd.2012.06.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reinhard J. F., Jr., Bannon M. J., Roth R. H. (1982). Acceleration by stress of dopamine synthesis and metabolism in prefrontal cortex: antagonism by diazepam. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch. Pharmacol. 318, 374–377 10.1007/BF00501182 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson T. E., Berridge K. C. (2008). The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: some current issues. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 363, 3137–3146 10.1098/rstb.2008.0093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowlett J. K., Mattingly B. A., Bardo M. T. (1997). Locomotor activity and dopamine synthesis following 1 and 15 days of withdrawal from repeated apomorphine treatments. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 57, 13–18 10.1016/S0091-3057(96)00397-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salokangas R. K., Vilkman H., Ilonen T., Taiminen T., Bergman J., Haaparanta M., et al. (2000). High levels of dopamine activity in the basal ganglia of cigarette smokers. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 632–634 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.4.632 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saunders B. T., Robinson T. E. (2012). The role of dopamine in the accumbens core in the expression of pavlovian-conditioned responses. Eur. J. Neurosci. 36, 2521–2532 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08217.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schlagenhauf F., Rapp M. A., Huys Q. J., Beck A., Wüstenberg T., Deserno L., et al. (2012). Ventral striatal prediction error signaling is associated with dopamine synthesis capacity and fluid intelligence. Hum. Brain. Mapp. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1002/hbm.22000 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sesack S. R., Aoki C., Pickel V. M. (1994). Ultrastructural localization of D2 receptor-like immunoreactivity in midbrain dopamine neurons and their striatal targets. J. Neurosci. 14, 88–106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sher K. J., Bartholow B. D., Wood M. D. (2000). Personality and substance use disorders: a prospective study. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 68, 818–829 10.1037/0022-006X.68.5.818 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sher K. J., Trull T. J. (1994). Personality and disinhibitory psychopathology: alcoholism and antisocial personality disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 103, 92–102 10.1037/0021-843X.103.1.92 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siessmeier T., Kienast T., Wrase J., Larsen J. L., Braus D. F., Smolka M. N., et al. (2006). Net influx of plasma 6-[18F]fluoro-l-DOPA (FDOPA) to the ventral striatum correlates with prefrontal processing of affective stimuli. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, 305–313 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04903.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slutske W. S., Cho S. B., Piasecki T. M., Martin N. G. (2013). Genetic overlap between personality and risk for disordered gambling: evidence from a national community-based Australian twin study. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 122, 50–55 10.1037/a0029999 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slutske W. S., Moffitt T. E., Poulton R., Caspi A. (2012). Undercontrolled temperament at age 3 predicts disordered gambling at age 32: a longitudinal study of a complete birth cohort. Psychol. Sci. 23, 510–516 10.1177/0956797611429708 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snow B. J., Tooyama I., McGeer E. G., Yamada T., Calne D. B., Takahashi H., et al. (1993). Human positron emission tomographic [18F]fluorodopa studies correlate with dopamine cell counts and levels. Ann. Neurol. 34, 324–330 10.1002/ana.410340304 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spinks T. J., Jones T., Bloomfield P. M., Bailey D. L., Miller M., Hogg D., et al. (2000). Physical characteristics of the ECAT EXACT 3D positron tomograph. Phys. Med. Biol. 45, 2601–2261 10.1088/0031-9155/45/9/313 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stallings M. C., Hewitt J. K., Cloninger C. R., Heath A. C., Eaves L. J. (1996). Genetic and environmental structure of the tridimensional personality questionnaire: three or four temperament dimensions? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 127–140 10.1037/0022-3514.70.1.127 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steeves T. D. L., Miyasaki J., Zurowski M., Lang A. E., Pellecchia G., Van Eimeren T., et al. (2009). Increased striatal dopamine release in Parkinsonian patients with pathological gambling: a [11C] raclopride PET study. Brain 132, 1376–1385 10.1093/brain/awp054 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takeuchi M., Miyaoka H., Tomoda A., Suzuki M., Lu X., Kitamura T. (2011). Validity and reliability of the Japanese version of the temperament and character inventory: a study of university and college students. Compr. Psychiatry 52, 109–117 10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiihonen J., Vilkman H., Rasanen P., Ryynanen O.-P., Hakko H., Bergman J., et al. (1998). Striatal presynaptic dopamine function in type 1 alcoholics measured with positron emission tomography. Mol. Psychiatry 4, 156–161 10.1038/sj.mp.4000365 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tison F., Normand E., Jaber M., Aubert I., Bloch B. (1991). Aromatic l-amino-acid decarboxylase (DOPA decarboxylase) gene expression in dopaminergic and serotonergic cells of the rat brainstem. Neurosci. Lett. 127, 203–206 10.1016/0304-3940(91)90794-T [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomie A., Aguado A. S., Pohorecky L. A., Benjamin D. (2000). Individual differences in Pavlovian autoshaping of lever pressing in rats predict stress-induced corticosterone release and mesolimbic levels of monoamines. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 65, 509–517 10.1016/S0091-3057(99)00241-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Leare K., Goffin K., Bormans G., Casteels C., Mortelmans L., de Hoon J., et al. (2009). Relationship of type 1 cannabinoid receptor availability in the human brain to novelty-seeking temperament. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 66, 196–204 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.530 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voon V., Schoerling A., Wenzel S., Ekanayake V., Reiff J., Trenkwalder C., et al. (2011a). Frequency of impulse control behaviours associated with dopaminergic therapy in restless legs syndrome. BMC Neurol. 11:117. 10.1186/1471-2377-11-117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voon V., Sohr M., Lang A. E., Potenza M. N., Siderowf A. D., Whetteckey J., et al. (2011b). Impulse control disorders in Parkinson’s disease: a multicenter case-control study. Ann. Neurol. 69, 986–996 10.1002/ana.22356 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voon V., Thomsen T., Miyasaki J. M., de Souza M., Shafro A., Fox S. H., et al. (2007). Factors associated with dopaminergic drug-related pathological gambling in Parkinson disease. Arch. Neurol. 64, 212–216 10.1001/archneur.64.8.1089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weintraub D., Papay K., Siderowf A. (2013). Screening for impulse control symptoms in patients with de novo Parkinson disease: a case-control study. Neurology 80, 176–180 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827b915c [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weintraub D., Sohr M., Potenza M. N., Siderowf A. D., Stacy M., Voon V., et al. (2010). Impulse control disorders in Parkinson’s disease: a cross-sectional study of 3090 patients. Arch. Neurol. 67, 589–595 10.1001/archneurol.2010.65 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whone A. L., Bailey D. L., Remy P., Pavese N., Brooks D. J. (2004). A technique for standardized central analysis of 6-18F-Fluoro-l-DOPA PET data from a multicenter study. J. Nucl. Med. 45, 1135–1145 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whone A. L., Moore R. Y., Piccini P. P., Brooks D. J. (2003a). Plasticity of the nigropallidal pathway in Parkinson’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 53, 206–213 10.1002/ana.10427 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whone A. L., Watts R. L., Stoessl A. J., Davis M., Reske S., Nahmias C., et al. (2003b). Slower progression of Parkinson’s disease with ropinirole versus levodopa: the REAL-PET study. Ann. Neurol. 54, 93–101 10.1002/ana.10609 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winstanley C. A., Theobald D. E., Dalley J. W., Robbins T. W. (2005). Interactions between serotonin and dopamine in the control of impulsive choice in rats: therapeutic implications for impulse control disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 669–682 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf M. E., Roth R. H. (1990). Autoreceptor regulation of dopamine synthesis. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 604, 323–343 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1990.tb32016.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yau W.-Y. W., Zubieta J.-K., Weiland B. J., Samudra P. G., Zucker R. A., Heitzeg M. M. (2012). Nucleus accumbens response to incentive stimuli anticipation in children of alcoholics: relationships with precursive behavioral risk and lifetime alcohol use. J. Neurosci. 32, 2544–2551 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1390-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young S. E., Stallings M. C., Corley R. P., Krauter K. S., Hewitt J. K. (2000). Genetic and environmental influences on behavioral disinhibition. Am. J. Med. Genet. B. Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 96, 684–695 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu Y., Panhuysen C., Kranzler H. R., Hesselbrock V., Rounsaville B., Weiss R., et al. (2006). Intronic variants in the dopa decarboxylase (DDC) gene are associated with smoking behaviour in European-Americans and African-Americans. Hum. Mol. Genet. 15, 2192–2199 10.1093/hmg/ddl109 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zald D. H., Cowan R. L., Riccardi P., Baldwin R. M., Ansari M. S., Li R., et al. (2008). Midbrain dopamine receptor availability is inversely associated with novelty-seeking traits in humans. J. Neurosci. 14372–14378 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2423-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang P., Ye Y., Wang X., Gelernter J., Mai J. Z., Li M. D. (2006). DOPA decarboxylase gene is associated with nicotine dependence. Pharmacogenomics 7, 1159–1166 10.2217/14622416.7.7.973 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhu M. Y., Juorio A. V., Paterson I. A., Boulton A. A. (1992). Regulation of aromatic l-amino acid decarboxylase by dopamine receptors in the rat brain. J. Neurochem. 58, 636–641 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1992.tb09765.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]