Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychology logoLink to Frontiers in Psychology
editorial
. 2013 Mar 1;4:103. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00103

On opposing effects of emotion on contextual or relational memory

Yi-Chieh Chiu 1,*, Florin Dolcos 1, Brian D Gonsalves 1, Neal J Cohen 1
PMCID: PMC3585437  PMID: 23460770

Introduction

An extensive literature has firmly established the role of emotion on memory. For example, behavioral experiments have shown robustly that emotional stimuli are better remembered than neutral stimuli (see Mather and Sutherland, 2011). Mechanisms identified for such enhancements in memory include prioritized initial perceptual processing of emotional stimuli (Nummenmaa et al., 2006; Phelps et al., 2006), and amygdala (AMY)-dependent neurohormonal changes that modulate memory processes supported by the hippocampus (HC) and surrounding medial temporal lobe (MTL) structures (McGaugh, 2000), leading to memory enhancements for emotional material (Dolcos et al., 2012) as well as making such information more resistant to forgetting over time (Dolcos et al., 2005; Ritchey et al., 2008).

A caveat to the above-mentioned phenomena is that they tend to be concerned with the remembering of individual items in isolation, such as a word or an image. But interest in the effects of emotion on memory for real-world events, involving more than solitary items, has led to research on emotion's effects on memory for items in the context of or in relation to other items (contextual or relational information). Puzzlingly, while this recent research has confirmed the enhancing effects of emotion on memory for individual items, outcomes for the effects of emotion on memory for accompanying contextual or relational information have been contradictory, with some studies showing enhanced remembering, while others reporting impaired performances, and some reporting no effect of emotion.

In this article, we highlight two issues that that may help toward resolving the seeming contradictions in the pattern of results in this literature. The first is the need for a more nuanced conceptualization and nomenclature of the types of memory representations being tested. The second is the necessity of considering the differential engagement of HC-dependent, relational memory representations (Cohen and Eichenbaum, 1993; Cohen et al., 1999; Eichenbaum and Cohen, 2001), as opposed to item memory representations, that might be taxed by various experiments.

Inconsistent findings on memory for contextual/relational information

Research on the effects of emotion on memory for contextual or relational information shows decidedly mixed results (Table 1). Some studies find enhanced memory for such information—e.g., better remembering of color information associated with emotional words or scenes (Doerksen and Shimamura, 2001; D'Argembeau and Van der Linden, 2005; MacKay and Ahmetzanov, 2005), screen location memory of negative arousing scenes (Mather and Nesmith, 2008), and temporal order of emotional items within a list (Schmidt et al., 2011). By contrast, other investigations find impaired memory for contextual or relational information—e.g., less detailed memory for scene contexts that form the background for centrally presented emotional items (Kensinger et al., 2007, experiments 1–3), and worse memory for cognitive tasks performed on items (Kensinger and Schacter, 2006b; Cook et al., 2007; but see Kensinger and Schacter, 2006a), for relations of objects superimposed on emotional scenes (Touryan et al., 2007; Rimmele et al., 2011), and for relational bindings between item pairs (Mather and Knight, 2008; Nashiro and Mather, 2011; Pierce and Kensinger, 2011; but see Guillet and Arndt, 2009). Finally, other studies do not find any differences in memory for contextual/relational information for emotional vs. neutral trials (Sharot and Phelps, 2004; Mather et al., 2009).

Table 1.

Sample of relevant studies on emotion's effect on contextual and relational information, organized by outcome and study design.

Enhanced Impaired
  • Source memory
    • • Word-color, word-color frame (Doerksen and Shimamura, 2001)
    • • Word-location (MacKay and Ahmetzanov, 2005)
    • • Word-color, word-location (D'Argembeau and Van der Linden, 2004)
    • • Picture-location
      • ◦ (Mather and Nesmith, 2008)
      • ◦ (Nashiro and Mather, 2011)
    • • Scenes-location, within list order (Schmidt et al., 2011)
    • • Temporal context: predictive preceding neutral item (Knight and Mather, 2009)
      • • Word-task (seen vs. imagined) (Kensinger and Schacter, 2006b)
  • Paired Design
    • • Taboo word-neutral word in sentence, or taboo word-word pairs (Guillet and Arndt, 2009)
  • No effect of emotion

  • Source Memory
    • • Scene-task association (color vs. visual complexity judgment) (Sharot and Yonelinas, 2008)
    • • Paired scene-location (Mather et al., 2009)
  • Paired design
    • • Central neutral word-peripheral emotional/neutral word (Sharot and Phelps, 2004)
  • Source memory
    • • Word, picture-task (animacy vs. commonness) (Kensinger and Schacter, 2006a)
    • • Word-task (seen vs. heard) (Cook et al., 2007)
    • • Scene-color frame (Rimmele et al., 2011)
    • • Scene-locations (set of 4 scenes in 1/8 locations (Mather et al., 2006)
    • • Face-location (Mather and Knight, 2008)
  • Scene context
    • • Emotional item embedded in scene, impaired detailed memory for scene
      • ◦ (Kensinger et al., 2007)
      • ◦ (Christianson et al., 1991)
    • • Neutral peripheral object embedded in emotional scene; binding of scene-object
      • ◦ (Touryan et al., 2007)
      • ◦ (Rimmele et al., 2011)
    • • Temporal context: non-predictive preceding neutral item (Knight and Mather, 2009)
  • Paired designs
    • • Word-word pairs (Pierce and Kensinger, 2011)
    • • Sound-digit, face-hat pairs (Mather and Knight, 2008)
    • • Picture-shape pairs (Nashiro and Mather, 2011)

Resolution of contradictions

Studies of the effects of emotion on memory for contextual or relational information vary widely in the modality and informational structure of the contents tested. We suggest that this variation among studies critically involves different types of relational content and, consequently, differences in the memory representations they test; understanding these differences may help resolve some of the seemingly contradicting findings in the literature.

Differentiation of information content tested

In a subset of designs commonly referred to as “source” memory studies in the literature, contextual/relational information has been operationalized across a wide range of modalities, including visual-perceptual stimulus features (color, location of item), temporal information (item order within a list), or cognitive operations performed (size, animacy judgments). What gives rise to the collective term “source” used to describe the contextual/relational information in these instances, is commonalities in the way such information is structured. That is, “source” information is always far fewer in number compared to the number of trials and can be conceptualized as grouping labels for items. For example, responses in source studies consist of a limited number of alternative choices for each modality (one of two colors or lists, or a specific quadrant on the screen) which can often be specified with a single descriptor (such as “red,” “list 2,” or “animacy”). In addition, for the purpose of making more nuanced distinctions between the types of contextual/relational information tested, it is important to note that source studies have not differentiated between memory for contextual and relational information (to be discussed below). Retrieval queries in these studies have been limited to the recall or recognition of the source information for cued items; when the source is correctly remembered/ attributed, it simultaneously implies accurate memory for the content of the source itself.

In contrast to source memory studies, there are experiments in which there is a one-to-one relationship between the contextual or relational information and trials. In other words, contextual or relational information is trial-unique and as numerous as the trials. Examples include studies that use visual images as background to items, and other studies that present two items in a pair per trial. Importantly, two types of information content can be distinguished and tested to demonstrate memory in these designs. The first is contextual information content, such as studied background scenes and objects shown with emotional items. The second is relational binding information between the contexts and the items, such as which scene was seen with a particular item, or which two items co-occurred during study as a pair.

When the above distinction between studies is made, a pattern does emerge from the literature in terms of the effect of emotion on contextual and relational information. That is, enhancements of memory due to emotion have been from two specific types of source memory studies—those that involve temporal information and visual-perceptual features. For example, emotion enhances the remembering of item order within a list (Schmidt et al., 2011), color source associated with items (Doerksen and Shimamura, 2001; D'Argembeau and Van der Linden, 2005; MacKay and Ahmetzanov, 2005), and location information (Mather and Nesmith, 2008). In contrast, impairments of memory due to emotion tend to involve tests for contextual information as well as for relational binding information between context and items or items pairs. That is, studies consistently demonstrate worse detailed memory for scenes accompanying emotional items (Kensinger et al., 2007, experiments 1–3), and worse recognition memory for the pairing between objects on scenes (Touryan et al., 2007; Rimmele et al., 2011) or item pairs (Mather and Knight, 2008; Pierce and Kensinger, 2011).

Relevant theories from the emotion literature

As demonstrated above, the distinctions among source, context, and relational information afford a means of conceptualizing the research findings. The importance of these distinctions is supported by two influential views in the current emotion literature, with one being relevant to studies showing enhancements in visual-perceptual source memory, the other explaining impairments in contextual information.

First, the object-based framework holds that arousal enhances within-object perceptual bindings intrinsic to the items which then leads to better memory retention of such bindings (Mather, 2007). This framework readily explains emotional enhancements for source memory studies where perceptual features such as the color or location are spatially proximal or overlapping with the emotional items, and thus benefit from enhanced feature-binding through focused attention attracted by the emotional stimuli (Doerksen and Shimamura, 2001; D'Argembeau and Van der Linden, 2005; MacKay and Ahmetzanov, 2005; Mather and Nesmith, 2008). Second, another view emphasizes a trade-off between enhancement of perceptual details for central information and a lack of detailed remembering for peripheral elements (Christianson et al., 1991), with the central vs. peripheral distinction being defined both spatially and conceptually (see Levine and Edelstein, 2009, for review). In this way, the central-peripheral trade-off view explains impaired memory for designs that test contextual information, such as scenes that serve as background for centrally presented emotional items (Kensinger et al., 2007, experiments 1–3) or for objects that are peripheral to emotional scenes (Touryan et al., 2007), since memory for central details is enhanced at the cost of peripheral information.

Memory representations tested

Although each of the presented theories accounts for a subset of extant studies, there are findings for which they do not have direct predictions. For example, temporal source information does not have spatial properties to which the object-based framework or the central-peripheral view can apply. This is also the case for studies with paired stimuli where two items of equivalent attentional, spatial, and conceptual status are shown in any given trial. In order to conceptualize the enhancing/impairing results in a way that generalizes across a range of studies, we propose it is necessary to consider the underlying memory representations likely to result from various experimental designs.

A well-established body of research in the memory domain informs a distinction between item vs. HC-dependent, relational memory representations that support memory for relations among multiple items, and between various items and the larger context involving temporal, spatial, and situational relations (Cohen and Eichenbaum, 1993; Cohen et al., 1999; Konkel and Cohen, 2009). Since there are at least two classes of memory representations involved, it follows that research on the effects of emotion on contextual or relational memory need to consider if and how emotion may affect these representations differently.

From the pattern of results summarized above, we are proposing that there is correspondence between the subset of source studies that shows enhanced memory by emotion to likely rely on item-only memory representations, and other contextual/relational information that shows emotion-driven impairments to involve HC-dependent relational representations. To further clarify, source information enhanced by emotion tends to involve stimuli properties that are perceptual or conceptual in nature, and can thus be “fused” or “unitized” to tax only HC-independent item-memory representations (Cohen et al., 1997; Diana et al., 2008). This is the case with color or location source information that can be associated with items via a visual “snapshot,” and temporal information for multiple items that can be conceptually organized into a single, coherent sequence (as instructed in Schmidt et al., 2011). In contrast, we note that emotion seems to impair information supported by relational memory representations, such as contextual information using complex visual scenes (Kensinger et al., 2007, experiments 1–3) and relational information using item pairs (Mather and Knight, 2008; Pierce and Kensinger, 2011), unless stimulus properties allow encoding of stimuli in a non-relational manner (possibly as internally generated, unitary mental images or concepts for word-word stimuli in Guillet and Arndt, 2009) or that encoding instructions encourage the allocation of attention to visual details of the contextual information (in Kensinger et al., 2007, experiment 4). Lastly, we would further generalize that in instances where temporal, spatial, and situational information cannot be represented in a unitized manner, there would not be an enhancement of such information by emotion.

Conclusion

To our knowledge, the proposal to conceptualize experimental designs in terms of item vs. HC-dependent relational memory representations taxed is a novel one yet to be extensively tested in emotion research. However, the interaction between the MTL and AMY during emotional memory processing, as proposed by the modulation hypothesis inspired by animal research (McGaugh, 2000), is well-established and confirmed by research in humans (reviewed in Dolcos et al., 2012). Also, there is evidence that AMY-MTL interaction during stressful events can impair HC functioning while enhancing item processing supported by the perirhinal cortex (in Mather, 2007), thus providing plausible neural mechanisms for the differential impact of emotion on these memory representations. To conclude, we propose that in addition to extant theories that are motivated by attentional biases in perception caused by emotional stimuli, future research would benefit from differentiating between source, contextual, and relational information, as well as from considering the types of memory representations taxed in various designs, as a way to further our understanding of the effects of emotion on all types of memory phenomena.

References

  1. Christianson S. A., Loftus E. F., Hoffman H., Loftus G. R. (1991). Eye fixations and memory for emotional events. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 17, 693–701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Cohen N. J., Eichenbaum H. (1993). Memory, Amnesia and the Hippocampal System. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press [Google Scholar]
  3. Cohen N. J., Poldrack R. A., Eichenbaum H. (1997). Memory for items and memory for relations in the procedural/declarative memory framework. Memory 5, 131–178 10.1080/741941149 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Cohen N. J., Ryan J., Hunt C., Romine L., Wszalek T., Nash C. (1999). Hippocampal system and declarative (relational) memory: summarizing the data from functional neuroimaging studies. Hippocampus 9, 83–98 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Cook G. I., Hicks J. L., Marsh R. L. (2007). Source monitoring is not always enhanced for valenced material. Mem. Cogn. 35, 222–230 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. D'Argembeau A., Van der Linden M. (2004). Influence of affective meaning on memory for contextual information. Emotion 4, 173–188 10.1037/1528-3542.4.2.173 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. D'Argembeau A., Van der Linden M. (2005). Influence of emotion on memory for temporal information. Emotion 5, 503–507 10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.503 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Diana R. A., Yonelinas A. P., Ranganath C. (2008). The effects of unitization on familiarity-based source memory: testing a behavioral prediction derived from neuroimaging data. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 34, 730–740 10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.730 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Doerksen S., Shimamura A. P. (2001). Source memory enhancement for emotional words. Emotion 1, 5–11 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Dolcos F., Denkova E., Dolcos S. (2012). Neural correlates of emotional memories: a review of evidence from brain imaging studies. Psychologia 55, 80–111 [Google Scholar]
  11. Dolcos F., LaBar K. S., Cabeza R. (2005). Remembering one year later: role of the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system in retrieving emotional memories. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 2626–2631 10.1073/pnas.0409848102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Eichenbaum H., Cohen N. J. (2001). From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection: Memory Systems of the Brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press [Google Scholar]
  13. Guillet R., Arndt J. (2009). Taboo words: the effect of emotion on memory for peripheral information. Mem. Cogn. 37, 866–879 10.3758/MC.37.6.866 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Kensinger E. A., Garoff-Eaton R. J., Schacter D. L. (2007). Effects of emotion on memory specificity: memory trade-offs elicited by negative visually arousing stimuli. J. Mem. Lang. 56, 575–591 [Google Scholar]
  15. Kensinger E. A., Schacter D. L. (2006a). Amygdala activity is associated with the successful encoding of item, but not source, information for positive and negative stimuli. J. Neurosci. 26, 2564–2570 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5241-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kensinger E. A., Schacter D. L. (2006b). Reality monitoring and memory distortion: effects of negative, arousing content. Mem. Cogn. 34, 251–260 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Knight M., Mather M. (2009). Reconciling findings of emotion-induced memory enhancement and impairment of preceding items. Emotion 9, 763–781 10.1037/a0017281 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Konkel A., Cohen N. J. (2009). Relational memory and the hippocampus: representations and methods. Front. Neurosci. 3:166–174 10.3389/neuro.01.023.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Levine L. J., Edelstein R. S. (2009). Emotion and memory narrowing: a review and goal-relevance approach. Cogn. Emotion 23, 833–875 [Google Scholar]
  20. MacKay D. G., Ahmetzanov M. V. (2005). Emotion, memory, and attention in the taboo stroop paradigm an experimental analogue of flashbulb memories. Psychol. Sci. 16, 25–32 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00776.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Mather M. (2007). Emotional arousal and memory binding: an object-based framework. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2, 33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Mather M., Gorlick M., Nesmith K. (2009). The limits of arousal's memory-impairing effects on nearby information. Am. J. Psychol. 3, 349–369 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Mather M., Mitchell K. J., Raye C. L., Novak D. L., Greene E. J., Johnson M. K. (2006). Emotional arousal can impair feature binding in working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 614–625 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.4.614 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Mather M., Knight M. (2008). The emotional harbinger effect: poor context memory for cues that previously predicted something arousing. Emotion 8, 850 10.1037/a0014087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Mather M., Nesmith K. (2008). Arousal-enhanced location memory for pictures. J. Mem. Lang. 58, 449–464 10.1016/j.jml.2007.01.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Mather M., Sutherland M. R. (2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6, 114 10.1177/1745691611400234 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. McGaugh J. L. (2000). Memory–a century of consolidation. Science, 287, 248–251 10.1126/science.287.5451.248 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Nummenmaa L., Hyönä J., Calvo M. G. (2006). Eye movement assessment of selective attentional capture by emotional pictures. Emotion 6, 257–268 10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.257 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Nashiro K., Mather M. (2011). How arousal affects younger and older adults' memory binding. Exp. Aging Res. 37, 108–128 10.1080/0361073X.2011.536746 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Phelps E. A., Ling S., Carrasco M. (2006). Emotion facilitates perception and potentiates the perceptual benefits of attention. Psychol. Sci. 17, 292–299 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01701.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Pierce B. H., Kensinger E. A. (2011). Effects of emotion on associative recognition: valence and retention interval matter. Emotion 11, 139 10.1037/a0021287 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Rimmele U., Davachi L., Petrov R., Dougal S., Phelps E. A. (2011). Emotion enhances the subjective feeling of remembering, despite lower accuracy for contextual details. Emotion 11, 553–562 10.1037/a0024246 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Ritchey M., Dolcos F., Cabeza R. (2008). Role of amygdala connectivity in the persistence of emotional memories over time: an event-related fMRI investigation. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2494–2504 10.1093/cercor/bhm262 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Schmidt K., Patnaik P., Kensinger E. A. (2011). Emotion's influence on memory for spatial and temporal context. Cogn. Emotion 25, 229–243 10.1080/02699931.2010.483123 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Sharot T., Phelps E. A. (2004). How arousal modulates memory: disentangling the effects of attention and retention. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 4, 294–306 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Sharot T., Yonelinas A. P. (2008). Differential time-dependent effects of emotion on recollective experience and memory for contextual information. Cognition 106, 538–547 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Touryan S., Marian D., Shimamura A. (2007). Effect of negative emotional pictures on associative memory for peripheral information. Memory 15, 154–166 10.1080/09658210601151310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Psychology are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES