Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2012 Nov 26;22(2):173–184. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2012/12-0022)

Preschool speech error patterns predict articulation and phonological awareness outcomes in children with histories of speech sound disorders

Jonathan L Preston 1, Margaret Hull 2, Mary Louise Edwards 3
PMCID: PMC3586759  NIHMSID: NIHMS416589  PMID: 23184137

Abstract

Purpose

To determine if speech error patterns in preschoolers with speech sound disorders (SSDs) predict articulation and phonological awareness (PA) outcomes almost four years later.

Method

Twenty-five children with histories of preschool SSDs (and normal receptive language) were tested at an average age of 4;6 and followed up at 8;3. The frequency of occurrence of preschool distortion errors, typical substitution and syllable structure errors, and atypical substitution and syllable structure errors were used to predict later speech sound production, PA, and literacy outcomes.

Results

Group averages revealed below-average school-age articulation scores and low-average PA, but age-appropriate reading and spelling. Preschool speech error patterns were related to school-age outcomes. Children for whom more than 10% of their speech sound errors were atypical had lower PA and literacy scores at school-age than children who produced fewer than 10% atypical errors. Preschoolers who produced more distortion errors were likely to have lower school-age articulation scores.

Conclusions

Different preschool speech error patterns predict different school-age clinical outcomes. Many atypical speech sound errors in preschool may be indicative of weak phonological representations, leading to long-term PA weaknesses. Preschool distortions may be resistant to change over time, leading to persisting speech sound production problems.


Preschoolers with speech sound disorders (SSDs) have clinically significant impairments with production of speech sounds of the ambient language. Although many of these speech sound errors resolve after several years (with or sometimes without intervention), age-appropriate speech sound production is not always achieved. For example, in a recent large-scale study, Roulstone, Miller, Wren and Peters (2009) reported that 18% of 8 year olds had unresolved speech sound errors, and Sax (1972) reported that /r, s, z/ were not yet mastered by at least 7% of fifth graders. Additionally, some of these speech sound errors may persist into adulthood, with approximately 1.4% of college freshmen having persisting speech sound errors (Culton, 1986). Moreover, preschoolers with SSDs are at elevated risk of problems with phonological awareness (PA), an important skill for developing reading and spelling (e.g., Bird, Bishop & Freeman, 1995; Nathan, Stackhouse, Goulandris, & Snowling, 2004). However, not all children with SSDs in preschool have persistent speech sound production or PA problems. The present study, therefore, seeks to determine if speech characteristics in preschool are associated with school-age articulation and PA outcomes. Specifically, we aim to determine if preschool speech error types (atypical sound errors, distortion errors), which may reflect different levels of psycholinguistic processing, are indicative of school-age PA and speech sound outcomes.

Psycholinguistic bases of speech sound errors

During speech development, children learn phonological categories (e.g., phonemes and syllable shapes), as well as fine-grained phonetic details associated with those sound categories. The (higher-level) categorical features of the sound patterns of the language make up the phonological representations of the words. When speech is produced, the (lower-level) motoric instantiation of those representations occurs as words are articulated (cf. Pascoe, Stackhouse & Wells, 2006). Many children with SSDs at age 4–5 years produce speech sound errors that include a mix of speech sound error types that may reflect problems in the higher-level phonological representations as well as the lower-level motoric aspects of these productions (Preston & Edwards, 2010).

Phonological representations are thought to be refined during normal phonological development as children acquire more knowledge of the phonemes and sound patterns of the language. As children learn the ambient language, certain predictable patterns of sound errors are observed in their output. These “typical” patterns of developmental speech sound errors have been well characterized in the literature. Typical substitutions and syllable structure errors, often described using phonological process labels (e.g., stopping fricatives, gliding liquids, fronting velars, deleting final consonants, etc.), are observed in most young children with or without SSDs, though these errors are generally observed to occur more frequently in the speech of children with SSDs (e.g., Edwards, 1992; Hodson & Paden, 1981; Ingram, 1976). However, among children with SSDs, unusual or atypical speech sound errors may also occur. These atypical errors are substitutions and syllable structure errors that are not generally found in normal phonological development. Atypical speech sound errors may include, for example, deleting initial consonant singletons, backing of alveolars to velars, glottal replacement of oral consonants and fricatives replacing stops (see Preston, 2008; Preston & Edwards, 2010). It has been postulated that such errors may reflect phonological representations that are particularly weak or poorly defined (Leonard, 1985; Preston & Edwards, 2010; Rvachew, Chiang & Evans, 2007; Rvachew & Grawburg, 2006). Frequent production of atypical speech sound errors might indicate that a child’s developmental path is unusual in how the phonological characteristics of the language are mastered (Leonard, 1985). Moreover, such atypical errors may reflect a phonological representational system that has gone awry, which may indicate the potential for long-term weakness in the foundations of a child’s phonological system.

In contrast, some of children’s speech sound errors may reflect lower-level phonetic problems. Such distortion errors are often considered to have a motoric basis, in that the productions lack articulatory precision (e.g., problems with tongue placement or configuration, as in a dentalized /s, z/ or derhoticized /r/; Dodd, 1995; Dworkin, 1980; Shriberg et al., 2005). These distortion errors therefore likely reflect a motor template for a particular sound that is within the proper phoneme category (i.e., phonologically accurate), but is imprecise in the detailed specifications for the sound (i.e., phonetically inaccurate). Distortions may be observed in both typically developing children and in children with SSDs.

Because problems in higher-level representations and lower-level phonetic realization of speech sounds may have different psycholinguistic bases, these different error types may be useful indicators of aspects of children’s phonological and phonetic development. Errors that reflect weak phonological representations in preschool may be indicative of future difficulty with skills that require well-defined representations, whereas errors that reflect lower-level articulatory precision may be indicative of difficulty refining articulatory targets.

Phonological representations, phonological awareness, and speech sound errors

There is now substantial theoretical and empirical support for the notion that phonological awareness (PA) skills are related to the quality of children’s underlying phonological representations (e.g., Elbro, Borstrom, & Peterson, 1998; Rvachew et al., 2006; Sénéchal et al., 2004). PA is a metalinguistic skill that involves awareness of the sound structure of words. It is a robust predictor of reading decoding and spelling both cross-sectionally and longitudinally (Adams, 1990; Blachman, 2000; Bradley & Bryant, 1983; Catts, Fey, Zhang, & Tomblin, 2001). For example, in a large review of existing literature, the National Early Literacy Panel (2008) described the consistent finding of the important role of PA skills in predicting early literacy acquisition (even when controlling for other relevant variables such as IQ and socioeconomic status); many of the studies reviewed strongly support the notion of a causal link between PA and literacy such that weak PA can cause problems with early decoding and spelling. In preschool children, PA often includes awareness of syllables, rhymes and initial consonants. In young school-age children, PA skills that develop include blending, deleting, and manipulating sounds in words. It is well established that preschoolers with SSDs are at increased risk for PA deficits (Anthony et al., 2011; Bird, Bishop, & Freeman, 1995; Foy & Mann, 2011; Lewis et al., 2011; Lewis & Freebairn, 1992; Peterson, Pennington, Shriberg, & Boada, 2009; Raitano, Pennington, Tunick, Boada, & Shriberg, 2004; Rvachew, Ohberg, Grawburg, & Heyding, 2003). Moreover, for some of these children, long-term deficits in PA, reading, and spelling may be observed at school-age (Clarke-Klein & Hodson, 1995; Lewis & Freebairn, 1992; Nathan et al., 2004; Preston & Edwards, 2007).

Although PA, reading, and spelling deficits are more common in children with co-occurring language impairments, several studies report that even children with SSDs who have typical language skills are at elevated risk for PA and reading problems (Bird et al., 1995; Overby, Trainin, Smit, Bernthal, & Nelson, 2012; Raitano et al., 2004; Rvachew et al., 2003). Moreover, recent functional magnetic resonance imaging results demonstrate that school-age children with residual speech sound errors show an array of cortical and subcortical differences in how they process phonological information in both spoken and written language (Preston, et al., 2012). Thus, problems in producing speech sounds may be associated with weaknesses in processing phonological information in both auditory and written modalities.

Because the PA, reading and spelling outcomes of children with SSDs vary widely, it is of clinical and theoretical interest to determine which children with SSDs are at the greatest risk for persisting PA problems. Recently, Preston and Edwards (2010) found that the preschoolers with SSDs who produced frequent atypical speech sound errors are likely to have weaker PA (when controlling for age and receptive vocabulary) than preschoolers who produced few atypical errors. It was posited that frequent production of atypical phonological errors may reflect poorly specified or weak phonological representations (Preston & Edwards, 2010). Over the course of development, these weaknesses in the phonological foundations of children’s linguistic systems may persist and may be evident in persisting PA problems. If this is the case, it is hypothesized that there may be a long-term association between preschool atypical sound errors and later PA due to weak phonological representations.

Although the empirical link in cross-sectional research between atypical speech sound errors and PA is moderate in terms of effect size, the association has now been replicated several times (Leitao, Hogben, & Fletcher, 1997; Preston & Edwards, 2010; Rvachew, Chiang, & Evans, 2007). However, the long-term association between preschool speech sound errors and school-age PA requires further exploration. Leitao and Fletcher (2004) reported a longitudinal investigation of 14 preschoolers with SSDs who were followed-up at the ages of 12–13. Half of the children were classified as having developmental speech sound errors in preschool (i.e., fewer than 10% of their phonological errors were atypical) and half were classified as having atypical/nondevelopmental errors (i.e., more than 10% of their phonological errors were atypical). Follow-up testing showed that the group who previously exhibited more atypical error patterns performed lower than the group with few atypical errors on several tasks of PA, reading, and spelling. Due to the small sample size, replication is required. The present study extends findings from Preston and Edwards (2010) by studying the long-term association between preschool atypical phonological errors and PA outcomes nearly four years later, and it provides a replication of Leitao and Fletcher’s longitudinal investigation of PA and literacy outcomes using a larger cohort of children with SSDs.

Low-level phonetic production and distortion errors

As children begin to establish phonological categories, they also learn the subtle features of the motor movements involved in the production of acceptable allophones. In preschoolers with SSDs, many of the errors observed are deletions and substitutions of phonemes. However, some errors may include speech sound distortions as well. In contrast, most of the errors exhibited by school-age children with SSDs involve distortions, primarily of rhotics and sibilants (Shriberg, 2009). These school-age errors may be indicative of problems with fine-grained motor specifications for a sound, and these problems may have been established in the preschool years. Although many preschoolers with typically developing speech also produce distortions, most will learn to refine their phonetic productions and achieve phonetically accurate speech. Shriberg et al. (2005) report that most sound errors in preschoolers with SSDs resolve by about age six, with the exception of those involving rhotics and sibilants. Some children with SSDs, however, may lack the ability in their speech systems to “tune in” to subtle features of speech sounds and may therefore fail to refine their productions (Preston et al., 2012; Shriberg, 1994; Shriberg et al., 2005), resulting in distortions that persist.

Shriberg and colleagues have postulated that children with early distortion errors may be likely to have residual speech sound errors later on due to early motor templates for sounds that do not resolve as the child’s speech system matures (Karlsson, Shriberg, Flipsen, & McSweeny, 2002; Shriberg, Flipsen, Karlsson, & McSweeny, 2001). For example, some children may learn a motor plan for a specific sound or sound class that is not appropriate for the target language (e.g., dentalized or lateralized /s, z/, or derhoticized /r/); these distortions may go unresolved and may lead to persisting speech problems. In this case, we would expect that preschool distortion errors might persist into later school-age.

Alternatively, some studies suggest that young children who exhibit more severe SSDs (e.g., more errors) tend to have poorer speech sound production outcomes than children with less severe SSDs (Roulstone, et al., 2009; Shriberg, Gruber, & Kwiatkowski, 1994; Steer & Drexler, 1960). In this case, measures of severity, such as number of sound errors or scores on a standardized test, would be expected to predict later speech sound production outcomes (Bernhardt & Major, 2005). For example, Roulstone et al. (2009) reported that the greater the proportion of sounds in error at age 5, the greater the likelihood of errors at age 8. However, focusing on the specific nature of preschoolers’ production errors might be more informative than considering global measures of speech sound accuracy. Therefore, a longitudinal investigation that includes emphasis on types of preschool speech sound errors and later speech outcomes is needed.

Aims of the Study

The purpose of the present study is to determine if types of preschool speech sound errors can predict school-age outcomes. The first aim is to determine if preschool speech sound errors predict performance on standard clinical measures of PA. We hypothesize that increased production of atypical errors in preschool will be associated with lower school-age PA (and, consequently, lower early literacy skills that depend on PA). The second aim is to determine if preschool speech errors are indicative of school-age speech sound production skills. We hypothesize that children who produce many distortion errors in preschool may be at risk for persisting speech sound errors at school-age.

Method

Participants

Forty-three preschoolers with SSDs, ages 4;0 – 5;9 were recruited through clinical referrals in upstate New York from May 2007–April 2008 (see Preston & Edwards, 2010). Children were primarily from middle socioeconomic homes, and all were speakers of General American English. All children had standard scores less than 89 on the Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2 (Goldman & Fristoe, 2000) and were enrolled in speech therapy. All had normal nonverbal cognition, as reported by the parents and confirmed by scores that were not lower than 1 1/3 SD below the mean on the Pattern Construction subtest of the Differential Ability Scales (Elliott, 1990). To rule out children who had significant language comprehension problems, all children had receptive language skills broadly within normal limits, as defined by scores no lower than 1 1/3 SD below the mean on at least two of the following three instruments: the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-4 (PPVT-4, Dunn & Dunn, 2007), the Sentence Structure subtest of the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Preshool-2 (CELF-P2) and the Concepts and Following Directions subtest of the CELF-P2 (Wiig, Secord, & Semel, 2004). Thus, this cohort included children whose receptive language skills were broadly in the average to above-average range.

Each preschooler participated in a 125-word picture naming task designed to elicit all consonant sounds of English in all word positions (initial, medial, final) at least twice; numerous consonant clusters and multisyllabic words were also elicited (Preston, 2008). Productions were audio recorded and transcribed using narrow phonetic transcription (see Preston & Edwards, 2010 for further details and reliability data). All consonant errors were categorized as distortions, typical sound errors (described by common phonological processes) or atypical sound errors (substitutions, omissions or distortions not commonly observed in typically developing children) based on previous reports in the literature on developmental speech error patterns. The relative occurrence of each of these error types was calculated for each child and quantified as the number of distortions per consonant, the number of typical errors per consonant, and the number of atypical errors per consonant. For example, if the word “spoon”/spun/ was produced as [spuŋ], the dentalized /s/ would be characterized as a distortion, and the backing of the alveolar nasal /n/ to a velar nasal [ŋ] would be considered an atypical substitution because backing is quite uncommon in normal phonological development. Thus, for this word with three target consonants, there would be 1/3 = 0.33 distortions per consonant, 1/3 = 0.33 atypical errors per consonant, and 0/3 = 0 typical errors per consonant. However, if the same word were produced as [pun], there would be no distortions per consonant and no atypical errors per consonant, but 1/3 = 0.33 typical errors per consonant to account for the common pattern of /s/ cluster reduction. The child’s total score in each of these three categories is based on the number of consonants attempted in the 125 word picture naming task. Definitions of error types and additional examples can be found in Preston (2008) and Preston and Edwards (2010).

Approximately three and a half years after the initial assessments, letters were sent to the parents of all of the children who participated in the original study inviting them to participate in a follow-up study (outlined below). Of the original 43 families, 25 replied to the letter and agreed to have their child participate in the summer of 2011 for a follow-up. Descriptive preschool data from these 25 children (18 males, 7 females) are presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics from 25 children with histories of preschool SSD: Preschool measures

Preschool Measure Mean SD Range
Age (yrs;mos) 4;6 5 mos 4;0–5;9
Years of maternal education* 15.8 2.1 12–18
Years of paternal education* 15.4 3.3 9–22
GFTA-2 Standard score 72.5 9.3 50–87
CELF-P2 Concepts & Following Directions Scaled Score 11.0 2.4 7–15
CELF-P2 Sentence Structure Scaled Score 11.2 2.3 6–15
PPVT-4 Standard Score 116.3 12.5 93–145
DAS Pattern Construction T-score 58.1 7.3 43–70
Speech Errors: Distortion errors per consonant 0.052 0.041 0.010–0.156
Speech Errors: Typical errors per consonant 0.437 0.109 .237–0.649
Speech Errors: Atypical errors per consonant 0.065 0.035 0.015–0.145

Notes: GFTA-2 =Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation, 2nd Ed; CELF-P2 = Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Preschool, 2nd Ed; PPVT-4 = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 4th Ed. DAS = Differential Ability Scales. Scaled scores are standardized with mean of 10, SD of 3. Standard scores have mean of 100 and SD of 15. T-scores have a mean of 50 and SD of 10.

*

Years of education reported by parents, with 12= high school, 16= four-year college, etc.

The 25 children who participated in the follow-up were compared to the 18 children who did not participate to determine if the follow-up children differed in any systematic way. There were no significant differences in gender, age, maternal education, paternal education, CELF-P2 subtest scaled scores, nonverbal cognitive scores, GFTA-2 standard scores, number of atypical errors per consonant, number of distortions per consonant, or number of typical error per consonant (all p’s >0.21). The one variable on which the follow-up children differed was on the PPVT-4, with the follow-up children achieving higher average preschool vocabulary scores (median 117) than the children who were not followed up (median 110.5, U =121.5, p = 0.011). Thus, the follow-up group includes children with vocabulary scores that are above the population mean and also above the mean of the other members of the initial cohort.

Follow-up Procedures

Several tasks were selected for the school-age follow-up to assess speech production, PA, and early literacy skills. Norm-referenced tests were selected for comparison to children of the same age. A certified speech-language pathologist (the first author) administered all tests to each participant in a single session lasting 2 to 2 ½ hours conducted in their homes. Families were paid for their participation. Sessions were audio recorded in Praat software (Boersma & Weeninck, 2011) using a Shure WH30 head-mounted microphone (1/2 inch mic-to-mouth distance) and a Hewlett-Packard Elitebook. Recordings were conducted at a sampling rate of 44kHz and were saved as .wav files.

Phonological awareness

PA was assessed using the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP, Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999). The PA Composite score is derived from performance on two subtests: Elision (which requires deletions of sounds from words) and Blending (which requires synthesis of words spoken one sound at a time).

Literacy

Because of the link between PA and the phonological basis of literacy, reading accuracy and spelling were assessed. From the Woodcock-Johnson-III (WJ-3), three subtests were administered. Letter-Word Identification assesses accuracy of reading real words of increasing complexity. Word Attack assesses accuracy of reading nonwords of increasing complexity. The Spelling subtest of the WJ-3 was also administered to assess spelling of real English words of increasing complexity.

In addition, the Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE, Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 1999) was administered to assess speeded reading of real words (Sight Word Efficiency) and nonwords (Phonemic Decoding Efficiency). Children read lists of words or nonwords as quickly as possible, and standard scores are based on the number of items read correctly in 45 seconds. This test has two alternate forms, both of which were administered (with Form A preceding Form B). An average of the standard scores from the two forms was used to obtain a more reliable estimate of performance.

Language

For descriptive purposes, language skills at the school-age follow-up were measured using the Recalling Sentences and Formulated Sentences Subtests of the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-4 (Semel, Wiig, & Secord, 2003), as well as the PPVT-4 (Dunn & Dunn, 2007). These tests were selected for their strong psychometric properties and for their sampling of receptive and expressive vocabulary and morphosyntax. Descriptive statistics, shown in Table 2, indicated that the group means on oral language tasks were average to high average, with individual language performance ranging from mild-moderate delay to superior language skills.

Speech sound production

To measure speech sound production (articulation) accuracy, the GFTA-2 was re-administered. This measure was selected because of its strong psychometric characteristics at school age and to allow for comparison to standardized scores from preschool. Age-based standard scores were derived.

A review of distortion error patterns from preschool data from this cohort indicated that two-thirds (67%) were sibilant distortion errors. In order to determine the extent to which these errors persisted, a picture naming task assessing /s, z/ in word-initial position was administered using images presented in PowerPoint. This task included four repetitions of the following words: zoo, zip, Z, Zack, sip, sack, sank, saw and sick. Additionally, 10 repetitions of the words Sue and see were elicited, for a total of 56 productions of initial /s, z/. Thus, the task was intended to balance depth (repeated attempts at words) with breadth (/s, z/ in different vowel contexts). From the audio recordings of these words, percent of alveolar sibilants correct was scored by a research assistant with a background in clinical phonetics who was trained to exceed a minimum of 80% agreement with the first author on identification of /s, z/ allophones. All distortions and substitutions1 were counted as errors (no omissions occurred). A second listener performed reliability checks on data from 12 participants; inter-rater reliability on scoring accuracy of sibilants was 93% (Cohen’s Kappa = 0.63).

Intervention histories

All of the children who participated in the follow-up study were enrolled in speech-language therapy as preschoolers, but intervention histories varied thereafter. Based on parental report, eighteen children continued to receive speech-language therapy services as kindergarteners (range 1–5 sessions per week, mean 2.6), fourteen received therapy in first grade (range of 1–5 sessions per week, mean of 2.5) and ten received therapy in second grade (range 1–3 sessions per week, mean 2.2). Additionally, seven were diagnosed as having reading difficulty in school, and all seven were reported to have received intervention or tutoring to address reading/spelling.

Data Analysis

Group summary data are presented first using standard scores to understand the group’s overall performance relative to age-related normative data. Correlations between preschool speech errors and school-age outcomes are presented for PA and speech production outcomes. Additionally, subgroup analyses were conducted to support the correlational findings and to attempt to replicate the findings from Leitao and Fletcher (2004). Non-parametric statistics were used due to the sample size.

Results

Group Outcomes

Summary statistics from the speech, PA, and literacy measures at follow-up are provided in Table 2. The group average on the GFTA-2 was approximately 1 SD below the mean for the children’s respective ages (mean of 84). Not surprisingly, this indicates that children with histories of preschool SSDs may have continuing speech sound production problems. Errors observed on the GFTA-2 were on fricatives (/s, z, ʃ, θ, ð/), affricates/ʈ ʃ, d ʒ/), liquids (/r, l/), and consonant clusters (e.g., /tr, sp/). As expected, errors included some common substitutions (e.g., [w] for /r/) and cluster reductions (e.g, [t] for /tr/), but most were distortions (e.g., lateralized or dentalized /s/, derhoticized /r/). Rarely, voicing errors on obstruents were also observed. From the 25 GFTA-2 samples, only one atypical error was observed (an instance of word-initial devoicing of a voiced stop), indicating that atypical errors were very uncommon at this age. Critically, not all children had persisting speech sound production difficulties (GFTA-2 range 46–107), motivating the need to identify preschool factors that were associated with persisting speech sound production problems.

Table 2.

Descriptive statistics from 25 children with histories of preschool SSD: School-age measures

School-age Measure Mean SD Range
Age (yrs; mos) 8;3 7 mos 7;4 – 9;3
Months between preschool session and school-age follow-up 44 3 40–49
GFTA-2 Standard Score 83.8 14.7 46–107
PPVT-4 Standard Score 109.3 13.2 87–143
CELF-4 Recalling Sentences 10.6 3.4 5–19
CELF-4 Formulated Sentences Scaled Score 11.2 2.8 5–16
CTOPP Elision Scaled Score 9.16 2.5 6–16
CTOPP Blending Scaled Score 7.8 2.2 1–11
CTOPP Phonological Awareness Composite 91.0 12.1 64–121
TOWRE Sight Word Efficiency Standard Score 103.3 12.3 85–125
TOWRE Phonemic Decoding Standard Score 96.3 14.1 73–126
WJ-3 Letter-Word Identification Standard Score 101.6 10.3 86–119
WJ-3 Word Attack Standard Score 101.0 8.6 81–119
WJ-3 Spelling Standard Score 98.3 14.4 72–129

Notes: GFTA-2=Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation, 2nd Ed; CELF-4=Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, 4th Ed; PPVT-4=Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 4th Ed. Scaled scores are standardized with mean of 10, SD of 3. Standard scores have mean of 100, SD of 15.

PA composite scores on the CTOPP were approximately two-thirds of a standard deviation below the mean for the children’s ages (group mean of 91), although there was a wide range of performance. This cohort had generally strong language skills (average PPVT-4 standard scores were 109; scaled scores for Recalling Sentences and Formulated Sentences were 10.6 and 11.2, respectively); however PA skills were not commensurate with the average to high-average oral language performance. School-age CTOPP scores was not significantly correlated with school-age GFTA-2 scores (Spearman’s ρ = 0.25, p=0.23).

Reading and spelling of real words was age-appropriate for the group as a whole based on TOWRE Sight Word Efficiency (mean of 103), WJ-3 Letter-Word Identification (mean of 101), and WJ-3 Spelling (mean of 98). Additionally, age-appropriate nonword reading was observed for the group on the WJ-3 Word Attack subtest (mean of 101). The group’s lowest performance was on the TOWRE Phonemic Decoding Efficiency, which requires speeded reading of nonwords; however, the group average was within normal limits (mean of 96).

Predictors of Outcomes

Although the group’s averages are informative, patterns of individual differences are of particular clinical relevance. Because PA and speech sound production accuracy were the outcomes of theoretical interest, analyses focused on predicting these outcomes from preschool speech patterns. Table 3 presents correlations between preschool speech production patterns and school-age outcomes.

Table 3.

Nonparametric correlation between preschool speech measures and school-age outcomes

Preschool Speech Measure School-age Outcome
GFTA-2 Std Score CTOPP PA Composite Std Score

Atypical errors per cons. −.14 −.47*
Typical errors per cons. .33 .09
Distortions per cons. −.42* .01
GFTA-2 Std Score −.08 .29
*

Correlation significant at p<0.05

Notes: GFTA-2: Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2. CTOPP PA Composite: Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing, Phonological Awareness Composite score

Aim 1: Predicting phonological awareness/literacy outcomes

As shown in Table 3, the CTOPP PA Composite score at school-age was associated with the number of atypical errors per consonant in preschool. Greater production of atypical errors in preschool was associated with lower PA scores at school-age (Spearman’s ρ = −0.47, p = 0.008, 1-tailed). Thus, the same association between more atypical errors and lower PA that was observed cross-sectionally in preschool (Preston & Edwards, 2010) was also found to be significant longitudinally. No other preschool speech production variables were significantly associated with school-age PA scores. Moreover, to explore if this association could be explained by socioeconomic status, a partial correlation analysis was run; the relationship between preschool atypical errors and school-age PA remained significant when controlling for maternal education (r =−.38, p = 0.035).

To replicate findings from Leitao and Fletcher (2004), a sub-group analysis was conducted. Participants were divided into two groups based on the proportion of preschool speech sound errors that were atypical according to the following formula:

PercentAtypicalSpeechSoundErrors=[Atypicalerrors/(Atypicalerrors+Typicalerrors)]×100

It should be noted that this formula does not count the raw occurrence of atypical errors per consonant, but instead expresses atypical errors as a ratio to all phonological errors (typical and atypical). Children with greater than 10% Atypical Speech Sound Errors in preschool (n=16) were compared to children with less than 10% Atypical Speech Sound Errors in preschool (n=9). The results, shown in Table 4, essentially replicate the results reported by Leitao and Fletcher (2004), indicating that the group of children with greater than 10% Atypical Speech Sound Errors performed significantly poorer on school-age PA. As shown in Figure 1, six of the seven children who scored 85 or lower on the CTOPP PA Composite at the school-age testing (i.e., to the left of the solid line) had greater than 10% Atypical Speech Sound Errors as preschoolers. Moreover, of the children who had greater than 10% Atypical Speech Sound Errors in preschool (i.e., those above the dotted line), none scored above 100 on the CTOPP PA Composite at the school-age testing. In addition, as seen in Table 4, the group with more than 10% Atypical Speech Sound Errors as preschoolers scored lower on measures of word reading (TOWRE Sight Word Efficiency, WJ-3 Letter-Word Identification), and non-word reading (TOWRE Phonemic Decoding, WJ-3 Word Attack) and spelling (WJ-3 Spelling) at the follow-up testing than the group with fewer Atypical Speech Sound Errors. Effect sizes were medium-to-large. The sub-groups did not differ in school-age GFTA-2 standard scores or vocabulary. Thus, preschool speech sound error patterns were significantly associated with later PA as well as later literacy skills.

Table 4.

Comparison of subgroups: Children with greater than and less than 10% of preschool speech sound errors classified as “atypical”

Task >10% atypical errors (n=16) <10% Atypical errors (n=9) U p* Effect size r

Mean (SD) Median (range) Mean (SD) Median (range)
GFTA-2 82.4 (15.2) 83 (46–107) 86.4 (14.1) 86 (63–104) 60 0.26 0.14
PPVT-4 106.9 (8.2) 108 (92–119) 113.7 (19.1) 120 (87–143) 54 0.16 0.20
CTOPP PA Composite 85.8 (9.1) 88 (64–97) 100.3 (11.3) 103 (82–121) 21.5 0.0015 0.58
TOWRE Sight Words 97.9 (10.9) 96.5 (85.5–120.5) 113.0 (8.1) 113.5 (95–124.5) 23 0.002 0.56
TOWRE Phonemic Decoding 91.5 (14.7) 89 (73–126) 104.8 (7.8) 104.5 (92.5– 117.5) 23 0.002 0.56
WJ-3 Letter-Word ID 98.1 (9.6) 98.5 (85–119) 107.7 (9.1) 109 (90–119) 32 0.012 0.45
WJ-3 Word Attack 98.2 (12.9) 98.5 (81–119) 105.8 (3.2) 106 (99–111) 31 0.010 0.47
WJ-3 Spelling 92.8 (12.9) 90 (72–125) 108.0 (12.0) 106 (90–129) 20.5 0.001 0.58
*

p-values based on Mann-Whitney U, 1-tailed

Figure 1. Relationship between preschool Percent Atypical Speech Sound Errors and school-age Phonological Awareness.

Figure 1

Notes: Solid vertical line represents a standard score of 85 (1 SD below the mean). Dashed horizontal line represents 10% of preschool errors classified as Atypical Speech Sound Errors, based on a 125 item picture naming task. CTOPP = Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing.

Aim 2: Predicting speech sound production outcomes

As can be seen in Table 3, preschool GFTA-2 scores were not significantly correlated with school-age GFTA-2 scores. However, when specific preschool speech sound error types were examined, the number of distortion errors per consonant in the 125 word picture naming task was found to be significantly related to school-age GFTA-2 scores; specifically, children who produced more distortions in preschool had lower GFTA-2 scores almost four years later (Spearman’s ρ = −0.42, p=0.019, 1-tailed).

To further study the relationship between specific preschool distortions and school-age outcomes, the accuracy of the sounds that were most commonly distorted by preschoolers (/s, z/) was examined at follow-up. Fifty-six productions of /s, z/ were elicited from each child (nine words repeated four times, and two words repeated 10 times) on a picture naming task. All errors observed at school-age on this task were perceived as distortions (primarily dentalization, lateralization, and palatalization) or occasionally as substitutions ([θ] for /s/). On average, 80% of sibilants were correct (SD 30%, range 11–100%).

The percent of preschool productions of /s, z/ that were distorted (out of an average of 53 /s, z/ tokens) were calculated from the preschool picture naming task; only tokens that were phonemically correct (i.e., perceived as within the correct /s/ or /z/ category) were considered. Thus, the percent of /s, z/ phonemes distorted in preschool did not include /s, z/ targets that were omitted or replaced by other phonemes (n.b., very similar results were obtained if these tokens were included in the denominator). The correlation between preschool proportion of /s, z/ phonemes distorted and school-age (in)accuracy on these sounds was high and statistically significant (Spearman’s ρ= − 0.77, p < 0.001, 1-tailed, see Fig. 2). The correlation suggests that the more distortions observed on /s, z/ in preschool (when considering phonemically correct productions), the more errors observed on these sounds at the school-age follow-up.

Figure 2. Relationship between preschool distortion errors on /s, z/ and school-age accuracy of /s, z/.

Figure 2

Notes: Solid vertical line represents 75% accuracy on school-age /s, z/ productions, based on 56 attempts at word-initial /s, z/. Preschool percent /s, z/ phonemes distorted is based on tokens that were perceived within the proper /s, z/ phoneme categories and were perceived as either correct or as distorted productions, based on an average of 53 targets on a picture naming task. Dashed horizontal line represents 40% of preschool /s, z/ distorted.

Figure 2 displays the preschool and school-age data on /s, z/. If we consider children who produce fewer than 75% of /s, z/ tokens correctly as having persisting speech production problems on these sounds (i.e., left of the solid vertical line), seven of the 25 children had persisting /s, z/ errors (six male, one female). Five of these seven with persisting /s, z/ errors produced 40% or more of their /s, z/ phonemes as distortions in preschool (i.e., above the dotted horizontal line). All of the 18 children with positive outcomes (i.e., above 75% correct /s, z/ at the school-age follow-up) had 40% or fewer distortions on these sounds in preschool.

Discussion

Twenty-five children with preschool SSDs were followed-up at an average age of 8;3. This is an age at which children rely on their phonological systems to acquire word reading and spelling skills, and it also represents the upper age of speech sound acquisition. It was found that preschool speech sound error patterns could predict school-age PA, literacy, and articulation scores almost four years later.

School-age PA and Literacy Outcomes

An association between atypical speech sound errors and PA skills has been observed cross-sectionally in preschoolers with SSDs (Preston & Edwards, 2010), and the present study suggests that the number of preschool atypical errors per consonant is correlated with school-age PA as well, indicating that individual differences in speech production patterns may have important implications. On average, school-age PA scores were approximately two-thirds of a standard deviation below age-expected norms, in spite of the relative strengths these children had in oral language skills both at preschool and at the school-age follow-up. When one standard deviation below the mean was used as a cut-off for “low” PA on the CTOPP PA Composite, 7 of 25 children (28%) had weaknesses in this domain. Six of these seven children with low PA had greater than 10% Atypical Speech Sound Errors as preschoolers. Moreover, the group of children with greater than 10% Atypical Speech Sound Errors in preschool scored significantly lower on all of the school-age PA, reading and spelling tasks. These results are consistent with the longitudinal findings reported by Leitao and Fletcher (2004), and the associations identified here are in line with the notion that weak phonological representations may underlie both atypical speech errors and poor PA skills (Preston & Edwards, 2010).

These results are contrary to the longitudinal results reported by Rvachew et al. (2007) who found no association between preschool atypical errors and end-of-kindergarten PA. Thus, it is possible that the longitudinal effects may not be robust until beyond kindergarten. It is also possible that seemingly minor methodological differences could have weakened the effects in the study by Rvachew and colleagues. Whereas the present study counted all atypical speech sound errors together, Rvachew et al. analyzed segmental or syllable structure errors separately. Rvachew et al. also used somewhat broader definitions of atypical speech errors and different PA tasks in that younger cohort, which might account for the difference.

Although PA skills were relatively weak with respect to age, reading and spelling scores were age-appropriate for the cohort as a whole. This may be due, in part, to the relative strengths in oral language (particularly vocabulary) and nonverbal cognition (cf. Peterson, et al., 2009). Hence, oral language skills may serve as a protective factor against reading problems for some children with SSDs. It remains to be seen whether these children will (a) continue to compensate for lower PA while maintaining good reading/spelling, (b) develop more slowly in reading/spelling such that these skills begin to approximate their lower PA skills, or (c) show gains in PA skills over time. The longitudinal work that exists suggests that residual weaknesses in PA may continue to be present in some of these children, and that spelling, in particular, may be an area of concern (Bird, et al., 1995; Clarke-Klein & Hodson, 1995; Lewis & Freebairn, 1992; Lewis, Freebairn, & Taylor, 2002). The critical age hypothesis (Bird et al., 1995; Nathan et al., 2004) predicts a relationship between speech sound production and PA at school-age, but there was only a weak and not statistically significant relationship between school-age PA and GFTA-2 scores in the present study. However, preschool speech patterns provided some indication of which children were likely to have later PA and literacy problems. Thus, the specific factors that are associated with literacy risk in children with SSDs might be more observable in preschool (when atypical errors are more likely to be seen) than in school-age (when distortions are the predominant errors). Larger cohorts and more frequent assessments are needed to fully explore individual trajectories of growth in speech sound production and PA.

Monitoring the development of PA skills in children with SSDs is critical, and understanding which children are at greatest risk might drive intervention decision-making. For example, Gillon (2000, 2005) has reported that including PA training in phonological therapy with preschoolers can yield positive outcomes in both speech and early literacy. The present data suggest that children who produce a high proportion of atypical phonological errors (such as 10% or more of their phonological errors) might be particularly good candidates for this type of intervention. From the current dataset, for example, the child with the greatest Percent Atypical Speech Sound Errors in preschool (35%) was diagnosed at the age of 8 years with a reading disability and scored below a standard score of 92 on all of the reading, spelling and PA tasks. This type of child might benefit from direct focus on PA skills from an early age.

School-age Speech Outcomes

School-age articulation scores were, on average, a full standard deviation below the mean for these children’s respective ages based on the GFTA-2. Not surprisingly, this indicates that preschoolers with SSDs are at risk for persisting speech sound production problems at school-age. School-age GFTA-2 scores were not significantly correlated with preschool GFTA-2 scores, suggesting that preschool performance on this instrument was not a robust indicator of later speech sound production outcomes for this cohort. In preschool, GFTA-2 scores were associated with atypical speech sound errors (Preston, 2008); however, only one occurrence of an atypical error was observed at the school-age follow-up, indicating that atypical errors had resolved by this age. Critically, school-age scores on the GFTA-2 were associated with the number of distortions these children produced in preschool.

Distortions occur primarily on later developing sound classes (Shriberg et al., 1994). An analysis of sibilants /s, z/ showed that the greater the occurrence of sibilant distortions in preschool (when /s, z/ targets were not substituted or omitted), the lower the accuracy on these sounds at school-age. These data, therefore, provide support for the hypothesis that early distortion errors may become solidified motor templates that are resistant to change and may lead to persisting speech sound errors in some children (Karlsson et al., 2002; Shriberg et al., 2001). All 18 children who had good outcomes on /s, z/ (greater than 75% accuracy at the school-age follow-up) produced no more than 40% of their /s, z/ as distortions in preschool; however, five of the seven children with poor school-age outcomes had distortions on 40% or more of their preschool /s, z/ tokens. The data provide a preliminary guideline for helping to determine which children are at greatest risk for persisting errors on these sounds. When making clinical decisions about treatment targets for preschoolers with SSDs, clinicians should consider monitoring children’s progress with distortions or directly treating distortion errors (in addition to typical and atypical substitutions or omissions) to prevent the persistence of these errors.

Limitations and Future Directions

One uncontrolled factor in the current study is intervention history. Although all children were enrolled in speech-language therapy as preschoolers, subsequent intervention histories varied. Eighteen children received services in kindergarten, 14 in first grade, and ten in second grade. However, when the parents were asked, “Do you feel your child continues to have speech (articulation) difficulties?” ten parents of children who had been dismissed from services responded “yes.” Of these ten, five children scored below 80 on the GFTA-2. Thus, some children who were dismissed from services had unresolved speech problems. Presumably, the children who were enrolled in services received intervention with varied emphasis on PA and/or specific sound errors, which may contribute to varied outcomes.

The present study did not address children’s reading comprehension outcomes. Although the bulk of reading instruction emphasis in the first few years of school is on word-level reading and spelling, future work might examine whether reading comprehension outcomes differ among subgroups of children with SSDs. Other subgrouping approaches, such as etiological or neurobiological profiles, might also aid in specifying individual trajectories of growth. Finally, future studies of the differential effects of various treatment approaches on speech errors, PA, and literacy outcomes would be of clinical value.

Summary

Different preschool speech error types were associated with different outcome domains (atypical speech sound errors predicted PA, while distortions predicted speech sound production outcomes). From a psycholinguistic perspective, this indicates that speech production and PA may dissociate at some level of processing (cf. Pascoe, Stackhouse, & Wells, 2006). Atypical speech sound errors have been described as reflecting differences at a higher linguistic representational level, and these errors may reflect weaknesses in how phonological information is organized or represented (Dodd, Leahy, & Hambly, 1989; Preston & Edwards, 2010). Although these atypical speech sound errors do not necessarily persist for many years, their occurrence at the ages of 4–5 may be indicative of weaknesses in how children with SSDs process phonological information (Preston & Edwards, 2010), and the frequency of production of these errors appears to indicate risk for long-term PA difficulties. On the other hand, distortions are often described as having a lower-level (motoric) basis (Dodd, 1995; Shriberg, et al., 2005), and frequent distortion errors in preschool may suggest that the child is at risk for long-term speech sound production difficulties. Specifically, it was found that preschool distortions on /s, z/ are associated with long-term errors on these sounds. Thus, early phonetic templates for these later-developing sounds may be resistant to change in children with SSDs (cf. Karlsson et al., 2002; Shriberg et al., 2001). Such knowledge may drive clinical decisions to give attention to distortions, even in preschool, in order to prevent persisting speech problems.

Preschoolers with SSDs may be at risk for persisting speech sound production and PA problems, but these school-age problems may arise via different (psycholinguistic/ neurobiological) paths. Not all children with SSDs are alike in their speech error patterns, and the preschool age is an important age to understand the association between speech sound production and future skills. Preschool speech sound error patterns may be one of several factors speech-language clinicians should evaluate when considering long-term prognosis for later outcomes.

Acknowledgments

Research support was provided by a donation to the LEARN Center at Haskins Laboratories and by NIH grant P01HD001994.

Footnotes

1

For each child who produced substitution errors for /s, z/, these substitutions (which were rare) followed similar patterns to distortions but crossed a phoneme boundary for the listener. For example, children who primarily produced dentalized [s, z] occasionally substituted interdental fricatives [θ, ð]. Although dentalized [s] and [θ] productions were perceived as categorically different for the listener, we do not presume them to be categorically distinct for the speaker, because they often happened on repeated attempts at the same word.

Contributor Information

Jonathan L. Preston, Department of Communication Disorders, Southern Connecticut State University, New Haven, CT & Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, CT

Margaret Hull, Department of Communication Disorders, Southern Connecticut State University, New Haven, CT.

Mary Louise Edwards, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY.

References

  1. Adams MJ. Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  2. Anthony JL, Aghara RG, Dunkelberger MJ, Anthony TI, Williams JM, Zhang Z. What factors place children with speech sound disorders at risk for reading problems? American Journal of Speech Language Pathology. 2011;20(2):146–160. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2011/10-0053). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bernhardt B, Major E. Speech, language and literacy skills 3 years later: A follow-up study of early phonological and metaphonological intervention. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders. 2005;40(1):1–27. doi: 10.1080/13682820410001686004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bird J, Bishop DVM, Freeman NH. Phonological awareness and literacy development in children with expressive phonological impairments. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1995;38:446–462. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3802.446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Blachman BA. Phonological Awareness. In: Kamil ML, Mosenthal PB, Pearson PD, Barr R, editors. Handbook of Reading Research. Vol. 3. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2000. pp. 483–502. [Google Scholar]
  6. Boersma P, Weeninck D. Praat v 5.2.31. 2011 Available from www.praat.org.
  7. Bradley L, Bryant PE. Categorizing sounds and learning to read - a causal connection. Nature. 1983;301:419–421. [Google Scholar]
  8. Catts HW, Fey ME, Zhang X, Tomblin JB. Estimating the risk of future reading difficulties in kindergarten children: A research-based model and its clinical implementation. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools. 2001;32(1):38–50. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2001/004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Clarke-Klein S, Hodson BW. A phonologically based analysis of misspellings by third graders with disordered-phonology histories. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1995;38(4):839–849. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3804.839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Culton GL. Speech disorders among college freshmen: a 13-year survey. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1986;51:3–7. doi: 10.1044/jshd.5101.03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dodd B. The differential diagnosis and treatment of children with speech disorders. San Diego: Singular Publishing Group; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  12. Dodd B, Leahy J, Hambly G. Phonological disorders in children: Underlying cognitive deficits. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1989;7:55–71. [Google Scholar]
  13. Dworkin JP. Characteristics of frontal lispers clustered according to severity. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1980;35:37–44. doi: 10.1044/jshd.4501.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dunn LM, Dunn DM. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. 4. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  15. Edwards ML. In support of phonological processes. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools. 1992;23:233–240. [Google Scholar]
  16. Elbro C, Borstrom I, Peterson DK. Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: The importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items. Reading Research Quarterly. 1998;33(1):36–60. [Google Scholar]
  17. Elliott CD. Differential Ability Scales. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation/Harcourt Brace; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  18. Fletcher SG, Casteel RL, Bradley DP. Tongue-thrust swallow, speech articulation, and age. Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders. 1961;26:201–208. doi: 10.1044/jshd.2603.201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fowler AE. How early phonological development might set the stage for phoneme awareness. In: Brady SA, Shankweiler DP, editors. Phonological Processes in Literacy: A Tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers; 1991. pp. 97–117. [Google Scholar]
  20. Foy J, Mann V. Speech production deficits in early readers: predictors of risk. Reading and Writing. 2011:1–32. doi: 10.1007/s11145-011-9300-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gillon GT. The efficacy of phonological awareness intervention for children with spoken language impairment. Language, Speech, & Hearing Services in Schools. 2000;31(2):126–141. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461.3102.126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gillon GT. Facilitating phoneme awareness development in 3- and 4-year-old children with speech impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 2005;36(4):308–324. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2005/031). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Goldman R, Fristoe M. Goldman Fristoe Test of Articulation. 2. Circle Pines, MN: AGS; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  24. Hodson BW, Paden EP. Phonological processes which characterize unintelligible and intelligible speech in early childhood. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1981;46(4):369–373. [Google Scholar]
  25. Ingram D. Phonological disability in children. New York: Elsevier; 1976. [Google Scholar]
  26. Karlsson HB, Shriberg LD, Flipsen P, Jr, McSweeny JL. Acoustic phenotypes for speech-genetics studies: Toward an acoustic marker for residual /s/ distortions. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics. 2002;16(6):403–424. doi: 10.1080/02699200210128954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Leitao S, Fletcher J. Literacy outcomes for students with speech impairment: Long-term follow-up. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders. 2004;39(2):245–256. doi: 10.1080/13682820310001619478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Leitao S, Hogben J, Fletcher J. Phonological processing skills in speech and language impaired children. European Journal of Disorders of Communication. 1997;32:91–111. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-6984.1997.tb01626.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Leonard LB. Unusual and subtle phonological behavior in the speech of phonologically disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1985;50(1):4–13. doi: 10.1044/jshd.5001.04. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lewis BA, Avrich AA, Freebairn LA, Hansen AJ, Sucheston LE, Kuo I, et al. Literacy outcomes of children with early childhood speech sound disorders: Impact of endophenotypes. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research. 2011;54(6):1628–1643. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0124). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lewis BA, Freebairn L. Residual effects of preschool phonology disorders in grade school, adolescence, and adulthood. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1992;35:819–831. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3504.819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lewis BA, Freebairn LA, Taylor H. Correlates of spelling abilities in children with early speech sound disorders. Reading and Writing. 2002;15(3–4):389–407. [Google Scholar]
  33. Nathan L, Stackhouse J, Goulandris N, Snowling MJ. The development of early literacy skills among children with speech difficulties: A test of the “critical age hypothesis”. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2004;47:377–391. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/031). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. National Early Literacy Panel. Developing Early Literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel. Jessup, MD: National Institute for Literacy; 2008. Retrieved from http://lincs.ed.gov/earlychildhood/NELP/NELP09.html. [Google Scholar]
  35. Overby MS, Trainin G, Smit AB, Bernthal JE, Nelson R. Preliteracy speech sound production skill and later literacy outcomes: A study using the Templin Archive. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 2012;43(1):97–115. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0064). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Pascoe M, Stackhouse J, Wells B. Persisting speech difficulties in children. Chichester, England: Wiley; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  37. Peterson RL, Pennington BF, Shriberg LD, Boada R. What influences literacy outcome in children with speech sound disorder? Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research. 2009;52(5):1175–1188. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Preston JL. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Syracuse University; Syracuse, NY: 2008. Phonological processing and speech production in preschoolers with speech sound disorders. [Google Scholar]
  39. Preston JL, Edwards ML. Phonological processing skills of adolescents with residual speech sound errors. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools. 2007;38:297–308. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2007/032). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Preston JL, Edwards ML. Phonological awareness and speech error types in preschoolers with speech sound disorders. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research. 2010;53:44–60. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/09-0021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Preston JL, Felsenfeld S, Frost SJ, Mencl WE, Fulbright RK, Grigorenko EL, Landi N, Seki A, Pugh KR. Functional brain activation differences in school-age children with speech sound errors: Speech and print processing. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research. 2012;55(4):1068–1082. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2011/11-0056). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Raitano NA, Pennington BF, Tunick RA, Boada R, Shriberg LD. Pre-literacy skills of subgroups of children with speech sound disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45(4):821–835. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00275.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Roulstone S, Miller LL, Wren Y, Peters TJ. The natural history of speech impairment of 8-year-old children in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: Error rates at 2 and 5 years. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2009;11(5):381–391. [Google Scholar]
  44. Rvachew S, Chiang PY, Evans N. Characteristics of speech errors produced by children with and without delayed phonological awareness skills. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools. 2007;38(1):60–71. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2007/006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Rvachew S, Ohberg A, Grawburg M, Heyding J. Phonological awareness and phonemic perception in 4-year-old children with delayed expressive phonology skills. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2003;12(4):463–471. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2003/092). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Sax MR. A longitudinal study of articulation change. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services Schools. 1972;3(1):41–48. [Google Scholar]
  47. Semel E, Wiig EH, Secord WA. Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals. 4. Harcourt Assessment, Inc; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  48. Sénéchal M, Ouellette G, Young L. Testing the concurrent and predictive relations among articulation accuracy, speech perception, and phoneme awareness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2004;89(3):242–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Shriberg LD. Five subtypes of developmental phonological disorders. Clinics in Communication Disorders. 1994;4(1):38–53. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Shriberg LD. Childhood speech sound disorders: From postbehaviorism to the postgenomic era. In: Paul R, Flipsen P, editors. Speech Sound Disorders in Children. San Diego: Plural Publishing; 2009. pp. 1–33. [Google Scholar]
  51. Shriberg LD, Flipsen P, Karlsson HB, McSweeny JL. Acoustic phenotypes for speech-genetics studies: An acoustic marker for residual /ɝ/ distortions. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics. 2001;15(8):631–650. doi: 10.1080/02699200210128954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Shriberg LD, Gruber FA, Kwiatkowski J. Developmental phonological disorders III: Long-term speech-sound normalization. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1994;37:1151–1177. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3705.1151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Shriberg LD, Lewis BA, Tomblin JB, McSweeny JL, Karlsson HB, Scheer AR. Toward diagnostic and phenotype markers for genetically transmitted speech delay. Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research. 2005;48(4):834–852. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/058). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Steer MD, Drexler HG. Predicting later articulation ability from kindergarten tests. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1960;25:391–397. [Google Scholar]
  55. Torgesen JK, Wagner RK, Rashotte CA. Test of Word Reading Efficiency. Austin: Pro-Ed; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  56. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA. Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed, Inc; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  57. Wiig EH, Secord WA, Semel E. Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Preschool. 4. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt/Psych Corp; 2004. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES