Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Mar 7.
Published in final edited form as: Ethn Dis. 2012 Winter;22(1):72–78.

Sex and Ethnic Differences in Validity of Self-reported Adult Height, Weight and Body Mass Index

Ming Wen 1, Lori Kowaleski-Jones 1
PMCID: PMC3590903  NIHMSID: NIHMS426899  PMID: 22774312

Abstract

Objectives

Describe self-reported and measured height, weight, and body mass index (BMI) stratified by sex and ethnicity in the United States, explore ethnic variations in the likelihood of under-reporting BMI, and investigate pathways linking race/ethnicity to the underassessment of BMI.

Design

An observational study.

Setting

The entire United States.

Patients or Participants

Data were from the 2007–2008 National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, a nationally representative sample of non-institutionalized civilian Americans.

Main Outcome

Objectively measured and subjectively reported BMI.

Measures

Independent variables include race/ethnicity (non-Hispanic Whites, non-Hispanic Blacks, Hispanics, and others), sex, age groups (age 20–29, 30–49, 50–69, and ≥70), marital status (currently married vs other marital categories), education (less than high school, high school graduate or equivalent, some college, college graduate or above), and poverty income ratio (PIR).

Results

This study confirmed that the use of reported BMI led to underestimates of the population prevalence of overweight and obesity due to the general tendency towards over-reporting height and under-reporting weight. Women were more likely than men to under-report BMI. And Whites were more likely than Blacks and Hispanics to under-report BMI. Other factors positively associated with higher likelihood of under-reporting of BMI included overweight and obese weight status, aged ≥60 years, and college education. Among women, family income was an additional positive covariate.

Conclusions

The results from this study underscore the need for frequently monitoring ethnic differences in validity of reported BMI and highlight the care which needs to be taken in making comparisons across sociodemographic groups based on reported BMI.

Keywords: BMI, Validity, Self-reported, Measurement, Sex, Ethnicity

Introduction

National statistics have shown the prevalence of obesity has been persistently high in the United States.1 Evidence is also abundant indicating large obesity disparities across major sociodemographic groups in the United States, with higher obesity rates found in Hispanics and Blacks compared to Asian and White Americans and in socioeconomically disadvantaged groups vs privileged groups across the life course.2

While little doubt has been cast on these reported trends, the majority of studies on obesity are reliant on self-reported body mass index (BMI), to measure body composition, which is inevitably subject to response bias. It has been frequently reported that adult weight and BMI are underestimated by self-reported measures,3 but nuanced group differences in response bias of height and weight are less documented.4 Accuracy of self-reported weight and height could vary by groups according to different norms and understandings regarding body weight.5 Demographic differences or similarities in the validity of self-reported BMI have been found to have an impact on observed racial/ethnic differences in hypertension6 and acute myocardial infarction,7 biasing our understanding of racial/ethnic disparities in health. However, literature specifically examining whether and how validity of self-reported BMI differs across sociodemographic groups remains sparse.

There are two main sources of reporting errors in height and weight. One is an honest mistake due to lack of recent measurement at home or by health care providers. Disadvantaged groups and older individuals may have a stronger tendency than their respective counterparts for such errors caused by recall bias and lack of information.8,9 The other reason is purposeful under-or over-reporting height and weight that is often related to social desirability. Groups who are faced with stronger social pressure on ideal body images are more likely to exhibit such response bias. In this sense, White women likely have a stronger conscious tendency towards under-reporting BMI due to the desire for a lean body.

Ethnic variation in accuracy of BMI measures based on self-reported weight and height in the United States has not been fully studied.10 Published studies have generated mixed evidence.4,9,1113 Many of these studies used data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) of various waves collected in different years. The NHANES is arguably the best source of investigating validity of self-reported BMI in the United States given that it collects both measured and reported height and weight and that it is a series of nationally representative cross-sectional sample of Americans. An earlier study using data from the Second NHANES of 1976–1980 reported no Black-White differences in reporting bias.12 A study using the Third NHANES of 1988–1994 also found no Black-White differences but much lower sensitivity of self-reported BMI for Mexican American women compared to White women.9 However other studies using the same data found the odds of underestimating BMI was actually greater among non-Hispanic White adults compared to Mexican and African American adults.11,13 And findings from more recent continuous 2001–2006 NHANES data showed that ethnic/racial background was not a significant covariate of the difference between self-reported and measured BMI.4 These empirical discrepancies may be due to differences in survey design (eg, oversampling of certain minority groups) and/or in analytical approach adopted in the analysis (eg, continuous difference between reported and measured values or categorical weight status were used as outcome variables); alternatively, they may reflect real temporal differences across groups in cultural sensitivities about being overweight. Therefore, the trend in ethnic variation in validity of self-reported BMI needs to be investigated periodically.14 This information is not only necessary to accurately estimate ethnic differences in prevalence rates of overweight and obesity but also is useful in risk estimates in etiological studies of health disparities associated with ethnicity and weight status.

Using most recent data from the 2007–2008 NHANES, this study describes self-reported and measured height, weight, and BMI stratified by sex and ethnicity in the United States, explores ethnic variation in the likelihood of underassessment of BMI, and investigates pathways linking race/ethnicity to underassessment of BMI. The extant research has revealed age, marital status, education, and income are relevant sociodemographic factors influencing response bias in BMI.4 Because these factors are all systematically associated with race/ethnicity,15 they are likely to play a certain role in contributing to ethnic variation in validity of self-reported BMI. In addition, we also include objectively measured BMI as a predictor of response bias given evidence that individuals with excess weight tend to under-report weight and BMI significantly more than those with normal weight.12,16

Data and Methods

For these analyses, we used data from the continuous 2007–2008 NHANES, which was conducted by the National Center for Health Statistics, as the most recently released data from a series of cross-sectional, nationally representative, multi-stage probability samples of the non-institutionalized US civilian population beginning in 1960. In 2007–2008, the sample consisted of 8,082 men and women aged ≥20 years; of whom 73.4% (n=5935) were interviewed and 70.6% (n=5707) were both interviewed and examined. Pregnant women (n=57) and participants missing measured weight or height variables (n=95) or missing reported weight or height measures (n=205) or other covariates (n=7) were excluded from the analyses. Missing values in poverty income ratio were filled with predictive values of ordinary least squares regression model on age, sex, marital status, education and household income. This study used data for 2,672 adult men and 2,671 non-pregnant adult women from the continuous NHANES 2007–2008. Both physical measurement and self-reported height and weight information were used in our analyses and differences of self-reported and measured BMI measures were computed. For descriptive purposes, weight status was categorized into underweight (BMI<18), normal weight (BMI≥18 and BMI<25), overweight (BMI≥25 and BMI<30 kg/m2), and obese (BMI≥30). Because we are mainly concerned with the underassessment problem of BMI, a dichotomous variable was constructed to indicate whether the respondent under-reported BMI (self-reported BMI<measured BMI). This variable was treated as our outcome variable in the following sex-stratified logistic regression analysis.

Six sociodemographic factors were explored, including race/ethnicity (non-Hispanic Whites, non-Hispanic Blacks, Hispanics, others), sex, age groups (20–39, 40–59, ≥60), marital status (currently married vs other marital categories), education (less than high school graduate, high school graduate or equivalent, some college, college graduate or above), and poverty income ratio, which is a ratio of annual family income versus federal poverty line for the corresponding family size. The other racial/ethnic category included Asian Americans, Native Hawaiians, Pacific Islanders, those who identified as other race or multiple races. Although the tables include the other race group, it is not discussed in the text because of heterogeneity.

Statistical Analysis

For under-reporting of BMI, a dichotomous outcome, logistic regression analyses with identical configurations were separately performed for men and women. Model 1 is the baseline model with only race/ethnicity included treating Whites as the reference group. Model 2 to Model 6 are nested models subsequently adding marital status, overweight and obese categories, age groups, education, and poverty income ratio to the previous model. The NHANES sampling design was corrected in all the regression models. The statistical software STATA version 11.0 was used in the analyses. Survey commands were employed for both descriptive statistics and analytical models.

Results

Table 1 presents sex-specific descriptive statistics for the total sample as well as for specific racial/ethnic groups on self-reported and measured height, weight, and BMI measures, reporting errors in the three variables (ie, difference in means of measured vs self-reported values), and sociodemographic variables including age, marital status, education and poverty income ratio. Based on both reported and measured BMI across sex, Whites have the lowest average BMI. Among women, Blacks have higher average BMI than Hispanics; and among men, Hispanics’ average BMI is higher than Blacks. Both men (+1.41cm) and women (+.84cm) overestimate their heights, which is the case for all the groups. However, White and Black men exhibit greater magnitude of over-reporting than women, whereas the sex pattern is reversed for Hispanics. As to weight, except for Black men who overestimate their weight, all other sex by ethnic groups underestimate weight ranging from .04kg among White men to 1.74kg among Black women. Because of over-reporting height and under-reporting weight tendencies, reported BMI is consistently lower than measured BMI across the groups. Women’s under-reporting error in BMI is greater than men’s. Blacks are the group that shows the largest sex gap in BMI with women having the largest under-reporting error and men the least (due to their over-reporting weight) across the groups. Group differences in sociodemographic variables are apparent. Non-Hispanic Whites are the oldest, followed by non-Hispanic Blacks, with Hispanics the youngest. Whites are also the most likely to be married, followed by Hispanics and then Blacks. In terms of both education and income, Whites appear most advantaged, followed by Blacks, with Hispanics most disadvantaged.

Table 1.

Sample statistics

Total
Whites
Blacks
Hispanics
Others
Male Female Male Female Male Female Male Female Male Female
Objectively measured height, cm 176.05 162.37 177.44 163.21 176.73 163.42 170.35 157.38 171.35 159.29
Subjectively reported height, cm 177.46 163.21 179.03 163.94 178.09 164.11 171.24 158.94 172.01 160.33
Difference in means between measured and self-reported height, cm −1.41 −.84 −1.59 −.73 −1.36 −.69 −.89 −1.56 −.66 −1.04
Objectively measured weight, kg 88.49 75.29 90.12 74.89 89.59 83.92 84.60 73.05 76.79 65.66
Subjectively reported weight, kg 88.50 73.91 90.08 73.39 90.23 82.18 84.51 72.46 76.39 65.10
Difference in means between measured and self-reported weight, kg −.01 1.38 .04 1.50 −.64 1.74 .09 .59 .40 .56
Objectively measured BMI, kg/m2 28.49 28.53 28.58 28.09 28.65 31.37 29.02 29.51 26.11 25.88
Subjectively reported BMI, kg/m2 28.05 27.74 28.06 27.29 28.42 30.49 28.73 28.72 25.83 25.35
Difference in means between measured and self-reported BMI, kg/m2 .44 .79 .52 .80 .23 .88 .29 .79 .28 .53
Age
 Age group 20–39 36.05% 34.05% 31.65% 30.95% 40.55% 38.52% 53.68% 47.13% 40.89% 37.68%
 Age group 40–59 42.43% 41.19% 43.33% 41.17% 43.48% 42.37% 36.00% 38.17% 44.15% 45.14%
 Age group ≥60 21.52% 24.77% 25.02% 27.87% 15.97% 19.10% 10.32% 14.70% 14.96% 17.18%
Currently married 59.63% 54.12% 62.35% 58.15% 41.43% 28.50% 54.51% 49.86% 69.65% 67.30%
Education
 Less than high school graduate 20.71% 18.61% 14.94% 13.91% 28.09% 27.11% 46.81% 41.07% 18.34% 14.76%
 High school graduate or equivalent 26.42% 24.91% 27.32% 26.25% 27.43% 23.11% 24.13% 19.93% 19.48% 21.44%
 Some college 26.83% 31.33% 28.58% 32.80% 28.87% 29.98% 19.19% 25.42% 20.05% 27.18%
 College graduate or above 26.05% 25.15% 29.16% 27.04% 15.61% 19.81% 9.87% 13.57% 42.12% 36.62%
Poverty income ratioa 3.16 2.98 3.44 3.21 2.68 2.43 2.18 2.08 2.89 3.05
Sample size 2,672 2,671 1,323 1,256 544 576 686 743 119 96
a

Ratio of annual family income versus federal poverty line for the corresponding family size.

Table 2 presents odds ratios from logistic regressions of under-reporting BMI for men. In the baseline model, Blacks and Hispanics are both less likely than Whites to under-report BMI, a pattern consistent with descriptive results presented in Table 1. Marital status is not a significant covariate (Model 2). Overweight and obese weight categories are both significant, positively associated with the odds of under-reporting BMI and renders the Hispanic effect a small increase (+9.2%; Models 2–3). So measured weight is actually a suppressor of the Hispanic effect. There is a small reduction of the race/ethnicity effects when age group is added (−4.2% for Blacks and −5.8% for Hispanics; Models 3–4) and more notable reductions when education is added (−9.3% for Blacks and −23.3% for Hispanics; Models 4–5). The oldest age group and higher education are clearly associated with higher odds of under-reporting BMI. Poverty income ratio is not a significant covariate and does not render any further reduction in the race/ethnicity effects.

Table 2.

Odds ratio of sociodemographic factors on the likelihood of under-reporting BMI for men

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)
Blacks .72c (.58–.89) .73c (.59–.89) .72c (.57–.90) .75b (.59–.94) .82a (.67–1.01) .83a (.68–1.02)
Hispanics .76b (.58–.99) .76b (.58–.99) .69b (.50–.96) .73a (.52–1.01) .90 (.64–1.28) .92 (.65–1.31)
Others .76 (.47–1.22) .76 (.47–1.22) .98 (.57–1.66) 1.02 (.60–1.75) 1.01 (.59–1.73) 1.03 (.60–1.77)
Currently married 1.05 (.85–1.30)
Overweight 2.50c (1.98–3.15) 2.54c (2.00–3.22) 2.48c (1.96–3.14) 2.47c (1.96–3.11)
Obese 1.79c (1.43–2.24) 1.78c (1.44–2.20) 1.82c (1.50–2.19) 1.82c (1.51–2.19)
Age 40–59 .81 (.61–1.06) .81 (.62–1.06) .80a (.61–1.04)
Age ≥60 1.69c (1.25–2.29) 1.78c (1.33–2.39) 1.78c (1.32–2.39)
High school graduates 1.58b (1.07–2.32) 1.55b (1.04–2.29)
Some college 1.82b (1.16–2.87) 1.76b (1.11–2.79)
College graduates or above 2.05c (1.61–2.61) 1.95c (1.54–2.46)
PIRd 1.03 (.96–1.10)

N=2,672; 95% confidence intervals in parentheses; reference group Whites; see text for model descriptions.

a

Significant at 10%.

b

Significant at 5%

c

Significant at 1%.

d

Ratio of annual family income over federal poverty line normalized on family size.

Table 3 presents odds ratios from logistic regressions of under-reporting BMI for women. Although the effect direction remains the same as for men, neither Blacks nor Hispanics is a significant covariate in the baseline model. However, when overweight and obese weight categories are added, which are significant and positive covariates themselves, the effects of Blacks and Hispanics are much strengthened and become statistically significant (+15.7% for Blacks and +12.4% for Hispanics; Models 2–3). There is not much reduction of the race/ethnicity effects when age group is added (−2.7% for Blacks and −5.1% for Hispanics; Models 3–4) and slightly more notable reductions when education is added (−3.9% for Blacks and −7.3% for Hispanics; Models 4–5). As what was found for men, the oldest age group is associated with greater odds of under-reporting BMI. However, the education effect is less clear for women although the direction of the effect is still positive. Meanwhile, poverty income ratio is a significant and positive covariate net of education although it does not offer much explanation for the ethnicity effects (Models 5–6).

Table 3.

Odds ratio of sociodemographic factors on the likelihood of under-reporting BMI for women

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)
Blacks .89 (.70–1.14) .89 (.68–1.18) .75b (.57–.98) .77a (.59–1.02) .80a (.62–1.03) .83 (.63–1.09)
Hispanics .89 (.74–1.06) .89 (.75–1.06) .78b (.64–.94) .82a (.66–1.01) .88 (.73–1.06) .92 (.76–1.12)
Others 1.00 (.57–1.77) 1.00 (.58–1.74) 1.20 (.65–2.20) 1.24 (.67–2.29) 1.26 (.68–2.33) 1.28 (.70–2.34)
Currently married 1.01 (.74–1.38)
Overweight 2.04c (1.61–2.59) 1.99c (1.57–2.53) 2.01c (1.59–2.55) 2.02c (1.61–2.54)
Obese 1.36c (1.10–1.67) 1.39c (1.13–1.70) 1.39c (1.13–1.72) 1.40c (1.13–1.73)
Age 40–59 .96 (.78–1.18) .99 (.81–1.21) .95 (.79–1.14)
Age ≥60 1.44c (1.20–1.72) 1.53c (1.31–1.80) 1.51c (1.29–1.78)
High school graduates 1.16 (.81–1.67) 1.12 (.77–1.61)
Some college 1.48b (1.07–2.05) 1.38a (.96–1.99)
College graduates or above 1.24 (.84–1.83) 1.10 (.71–1.70)
PIRd 1.07a (.99–1.15)

N=2,671; 95% confidence intervals in parentheses; reference group Whites; see text for model descriptions.

a

Significant at 10%.

b

Significant at 5%

c

Significant at 1%.

d

Ratio of annual family income over federal poverty line normalized on family size.

Discussion

As one of the leading preventable causes of death in the United States,17 obesity trends need to be consistently monitored in representative national surveys. Due to financial and logistic constraints, many surveys on body weight are reliant on self-reported height and weight despite evidence on under-assessment of BMI based on self-reported values. Although high correlations between reported and measured BMI have been routinely observed,3,12 many authors have cautioned that using reported BMI in prevalence and etiological studies may incur systematic bias.6 It is also important to regularly assess ethnic differences in BMI reporting errors given ethnic differences in perceptions of weight and body image,6,14 differential access to health care and health information,18 and disparities in a wide range of health outcomes.19

Consistent with previous findings from industrialized societies,4,710 this study confirmed that the use of reported BMI leads to underestimates of the population prevalence of overweight and obesity due to the general tendency towards over-reporting height and under-reporting weight. That said, the magnitude of under-reporting across all sex-ethnic groups is generally small ranging from .23 kg/m2 among Black men to .88 kg/m2 among Black women, all within the 1 BMI unit range. In addition, consistent with the studies using previous NHANES and other data,6,9,12,20,21 high correlations between measured and reported BMI were observed (data not shown). It should be noted that the under-reporting of weight and BMI tends to be smaller in the NHANES than in other studies,12 possibly because participants are aware that the questionnaire survey is followed up by the examination.9 So the under-reporting of BMI based on NHANES data provides a conservative estimate of BMI reporting errors.

Despite the modest degree of discrepancy in BMI reporting in general, sex and ethnic differences in deviations of the reported BMI values from measured BMI values were observed and predictors of accuracy of reported BMI measures were identified, indicating the need for nuanced analyses to refine our knowledge about group differences in validity of adult self-reported BMI. Predictors of likelihood of under-reporting of BMI were overweight and obese weight status, female sex, being White, aged ≥60, and college education. For women, poverty income ratio was an additional predictor positively associated with the tendency towards under-reporting BMI. Indeed, as previously pointed out,13 report biases in BMI may be particularly significant in subgroups such as elderly and heaviest individuals where accuracy is of prime concern.

The finding that Whites are more likely to under-report BMI compared to Blacks and Hispanics is noteworthy. The present study used the most recent NHANES data to revisit the validity issue of reported BMI and provides consistent evidence that Whites have stronger tendency of under-reporting BMI compared to Blacks and Hispanics regardless of sex. This result is consistent with some studies6,11,13 but not others3,12 that have used data from different waves of NHANES. Two reasons are possible for this empirical discrepancy. One regards analytical approach. Earlier NHANES studies3,12 did not oversample minorities making it less feasible or reliable to study these groups. In some studies, discrepancy in BMI was measured as a continuous variable subtracting measured BMI from reported BMI,4,9 whereas others13 and the present study used categorical outcomes which may detect stepwise rather than linear effects of racial/ethnic influences on BMI reporting bias. In the same vein, although reported and measured BMI values appear highly correlated, sensitivity and specificity analyses, which are based on categories of weight status, revealed substantial bias undetectable by linear analyses.9,12 Another possible reason underlying different results reported using NHANES data of various waves is that the widespread obesity epidemic has led to much heightened awareness of the overweight problem, making it a more socially sensitive issue. Whites may feel stronger social pressure towards conforming to norms of the ideal body images, namely lean body composition. Compared to Blacks and Hispanics, Whites have been found to be more accurately aware of their weight problems controlling for their objective weight.14,22,23 This ethnic difference may be due to cultural factors24,25 and/or lack of information as Blacks and Hispanics may be less equipped to either do a home-based scale measure of weight or get objective measures from a medical examiner. If the ethnic difference in the under-reporting bias found in this study is real, then the reported higher prevalence rates of overweight and obesity among Blacks and Hispanics relative to Whites would have been somewhat overestimated. Disparities associated with weight status and the related complications may be less severe than what we thought, especially among women.

Another unique feature of this study was to have explored sociodemographic pathways linking race/ethnicity to BMI reporting bias. We found education played the most salient role in contributing to lower likelihoods of under-reporting BMI among Blacks and Hispanics and income only mattered for women but not for men. It has been proposed in the health stratification literature that education was a more important socioeconomic factor than income linked to lifestyles partly because of its strong impact on health information and perceptions.26 Arguably, the finding that ethnic differences in educational attainment explains, to some extent, the ethnic variation in response bias of BMI lends indirect support to this notion. Why income matters more for women than for men remains elusive and needs further investigation.

This study has some limitations. First, Asians, Pacific Islanders, and Native Americans were excluded from the analyses due to data constraints. This is an important drawback in the health disparity by race/ethnicity literature. Second, there are omitted variables that were not examined in this study. For example, attitudes toward overweight in a person’s social networks may be an important factor of response bias;27 unfortunately, this information is not available. Third, as abovementioned, NHANES tends to produce less report bias compared to reported BMI based on other telephone (eg, Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System) or home surveys (eg, National Health Interview Survey).10 The reported discrepancy estimates should be interpreted with caution. And fourth, the statistical power of this study is relatively limited due to limited sample size of the most recent two-period continuous NHANES survey.

Conclusion

All the eight sex by racial/ethnic groups examined in this study underreported BMI; the amount of the under-reporting was greatest among Black women and least among Black men; and the underreporting bias was more severe among women than among men. For both men and women, personal attributes such as race/ethnicity, age, sex, education and weight status were important factors of report bias in BMI. Among women, family income was an additional positive predictor. Whites’ stronger tendency towards under-reporting BMI was partly but not entirely attributable to their higher levels of education. The results from this study underscore the need for frequently monitoring of ethnic differences in validity of reported BMI and highlight the care which needs to be taken in making comparisons across sociodemographic groups based on reported BMI.

Acknowledgments

Support for this study is provided by an NIH grant awarded to both authors (R01CA140319-01A1). The funding agency had no involvement in the development of this study.

Footnotes

Author Contributions

Study design: Wen, Kowaleski-Jones

Acquisition of data: Wen, Kowaleski-Jones

Data analysis and interpretation: Wen, Kowaleski-Jones

Manuscript draft: Wen, Kowaleski-Jones

Statistical expertise: Wen, Kowaleski-Jones

Acquisition of funding: Wen, Kowaleski-Jones

Administrative: Wen, Kowaleski-Jones

Supervision: Wen, Kowaleski-Jones

References

  • 1.U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (USDHHS) The Surgeon General’s Call to Action to Prevent and Decrease Overweight and Obesity. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office; 2001. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Pan L, Galuska DA, Sherry B, et al. Differences in prevalence of obesity among Black, White, and Hispanic adults - United States, 2006–2008. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2009;58(27):740–744. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kovalchik S. Validity of adult lifetime self-reported body weight. Public Health Nutr. 2009;12(8):1072–1077. doi: 10.1017/S1368980008003728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Stommel M, Schoenborn CA. Accuracy and usefulness of BMI measures based on self-reported weight and height: findings from the NHANES & NHIS 2001–2006. BMC Public Health. 2009;9:421. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-9-421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lee SK. Validity of self-reported weight and height: comparison between immigrant and non-immigrant Mexican Americans in NHANES III. J Immigr Health. 2005;7(2):127–131. doi: 10.1007/s10903-005-2646-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Okosun IS, Bhatt DV, Boltri JM, Ndirangu M. Self-reported and measured height and weight: impact on racial/ethnic differences in hypertension. Ethn Dis. 2008;18(4):415–420. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Oliveira A, Ramos E, Lopes C, Barros H. Self-reporting weight and height: misclassification effect on the risk estimates for acute myocardial infarction. Eur J Pub Health. 2009;19(5):548–553. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckp022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Dahl AK, Hassing LB, Fransson EI, Pedersen NL. Agreement between self-reported and measured height, weight and body mass index in old age-a longitudinal study with 20 years of follow-up. Age Ageing. 2010;39(4):445–451. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afq038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gillum RF, Sempos CT. Ethnic variation in validity of classification of overweight and obesity using self-reported weight and height in American women and men: The Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Nutr J. 2005;4:27. doi: 10.1186/1475-2891-4-27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Yun S, Zhu BP, Black W, Brownson RC. A comparison of national estimates of obesity prevalence from the behavioral risk factor surveillance system and the national health and nutrition examination survey. Int J Obes (Lond) 2006;30(1):164–170. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Craig BM, Adams AK. Accuracy of body mass index categories based on self-reported height and weight among women in the United States. Matern Child Health J. 2008;13:489–496. doi: 10.1007/s10995-008-0384-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Rowland ML. Self-reported weight and height. Am J Clin Nutr. 1990;52:1125–1133. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/52.6.1125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Villanueva EV. The validity of self-reported weight in US adults: a population based cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2001;1:1–11. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-1-11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Gregory CO, Blanck HM, Gillespie C, Maynard LM, Serdula MK. Health perceptions and demographic characteristics associated with underassessment of body weight. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2008;16(5):979–986. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.LaVeist TA. Minority Populations and Health: An Introduction to Health Disparities in the United States. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hill A, Roberts J. Body mass index: a comparison between self-reported and measured height and weight. J Public Health Med. 1998;20(2):206–210. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.pubmed.a024744. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Flegal KM, Carroll MD, Ogden CL, Johnson CL. Prevalence and trends in obesity among US adults, 1999–2000. JAMA. 2002;288:1723–1727. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.14.1723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Francis CK, Turton FE, Diamond LH, et al. Racial and ethnic disparities in healthcare: a position paper of the American College of Physicians. J Natl Med Assoc. 2004;96(9):1178–1184. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Wen M. Racial and ethnic differences in general health status and limiting health conditions among American children: parental reports in the 1999 National Survey of America’s Families. Ethn Health. 2007;12(5):401–422. doi: 10.1080/13557850701300657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Flood V, Webb K, Lazarus R, Pang G. Use of self-report to monitor overweight and obesity in populations: some issues for consideration. Aust N Z J Public Health. 2000;24(1):96–99. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-842x.2000.tb00733.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Niedhammer I, Bugel I, Bonenfant S, Goldberg M, Leclerc A. Validity of self-reported weight and height in the French GAZEL cohort. Int J Obesity Relat Metab Disord. 2000;24(9):1111–1118. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Becker DM, Yanek LR, Koffman DM, Bronner YC. Body image preferences among urban African Americans and Whites from low income communities. Ethn Dis. 1999;9(3):377–386. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Viladrich A, Yeh MC, Bruning N, Weiss R. “Do Real Women Have Curves?” Paradoxical body images among Latinas in New York City. J Immigr Minor Health. 2009;11(1):20–28. doi: 10.1007/s10903-008-9176-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lake AJ, Staiger PK, Glowinski H. Effect of western culture on women’s attitudes to eating and perceptions of body shape. Int J Eat Disord. 2000;27(1):83–89. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(200001)27:1<83::aid-eat9>3.0.co;2-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Olvera N, Suminski R, Power TG. Intergenerational perceptions of body image in Hispanics: role of BMI, gender, and acculturation. Obes Res. 2005;13(11):1970–1979. doi: 10.1038/oby.2005.242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Mirowsky J, Ross CE, Reynolds J. Links between social status and health status. In: Bird CE, Conrad P, Fremont AM, editors. Handbook of Medical Sociology. 5. Upper Saddle River; Prentice Hall: 2000. pp. 47–67. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Christakis NA, Fowler JH. The spread of obesity in a large social network over 32 years. N Engl J Med. 2007;357(4):370–379. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa066082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES