Abstract
Potentiation at synapses between CA3 and the CA1 pyramidal neurons comprises both transient and sustained phases, commonly referred to as short-term potentiation (STP or transient LTP) and long-term potentiation (LTP), respectively. Here, we utilized four subtype-selective N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor (NMDAR) antagonists to investigate whether the induction of STP and LTP is dependent on the activation of different NMDAR subtypes. We find that the induction of LTP involves the activation of NMDARs containing both the GluN2A and the GluN2B subunits. Surprisingly, however, we find that STP can be separated into two components, the major form of which involves activation of NMDARs containing both GluN2B and GluN2D subunits. These data demonstrate that synaptic potentiation at CA1 synapses is more complex than is commonly thought, an observation that has major implications for understanding the role of NMDARs in cognition.
Key points
N-Methyl-d-aspartate receptor (NMDAR)-dependent potentiation of synaptic transmission is widely accepted as a cellular model of learning and memory.
It is most often studied in the CA1 area of rat hippocampal slices where it comprises a decremental and a sustained phase, which are commonly referred to as short-term potentiation (STP) and long-term potentiation (LTP), respectively.
In this study we show for the first time that STP and LTP are triggered by the activation of different classes of NMDARs and that STP itself comprises two pharmacologically and kinetically distinct components.
We suggest that the mechanistic separation of STP and LTP is likely to have important functional implications in that these two forms of synaptic plasticity can subserve unique physiological functions in a behaving animal.
Introduction
Long-term potentiation (LTP; Bliss & Lomo, 1973) at synapses between CA3 and CA1 pyramidal neurons is dependent on the synaptic activation of N-methyl-d-aspartate receptors (NMDARs) (Collingridge et al. 1983). It is readily induced in hippocampal slices by a brief high-frequency tetanus (Andersen et al. 1977) or by theta patterns of stimulation (Larson et al. 1986). The brief period(s) of high-frequency stimulation relieve the Mg2+ block from the NMDARs leading to an elevation of [Ca2+]i and induction of potentiation (see Bliss & Collingridge, 1993). Two phases of NMDAR-dependent LTP induced by high-frequency stimulation have been described: (1) a transient phase, often referred to as short-term potentiation (STP) or sometimes as transient LTP (t-LTP), that declines to (2) a stable phase, referred to as early LTP (e-LTP), sustained LTP (s-LTP) or, most commonly, simply LTP.
Whether STP and LTP share a common induction mechanism (Gustafsson & Wigstrom, 1990; Hanse & Gustafsson, 1994) or not (Larson et al. 1986; Kauer et al. 1988; Schulz & Fitzgibbons, 1997; Volianskis & Jensen, 2003) remains unresolved. NMDARs are implicated in both processes as high concentrations of the highly selective NMDAR antagonist d-2-amino-5-phosphonopentanoate (AP5; Davies et al. 1981) block both the transient and the sustained phases of LTP (Larson & Lynch, 1988; Anwyl et al. 1989; Malenka, 1991; Schulz & Fitzgibbons, 1997; Volianskis & Jensen, 2003). However, it appears that STP and LTP have a different concentration dependency to AP5. A low concentration of AP5 is sufficient to block LTP (Malenka, 1991; Liu et al. 2004) whereas higher concentrations alone (Malenka, 1991) or in combinations with other NMDAR antagonists (Pananceau & Gustafsson, 1997) are needed to block STP. It has been assumed that this relates to the level of activation of NMDARs, with a strong activation required to enable potentiation to persist into the sustained phase (Gustafsson & Wigstrom, 1990; Hanse & Gustafsson, 1994).
NMDARs are tetra-heteromeric assemblies, most commonly made up of two GluN1 and two GluN2 subunits (GluN2A–D), named according to International Union of Basic and Clinical Pharmacology nomenclature (Collingridge et al. 2009). It has been suggested that different NMDAR subtypes may affect the direction of synaptic plasticity (Hrabetova et al. 2000; Liu et al. 2004; Massey et al. 2004), although no firm rule exists (Berberich et al. 2005, 2007; Weitlauf et al. 2005; Bartlett et al. 2007; Li et al. 2007). In the present study we explored, for the first time, the possibility that different subtypes of NMDARs are involved during induction of different temporal phases of synaptic plasticity by studying potentiation at CA1 synapses in the hippocampus. We find that NMDAR subtype involvement in the induction of STP and LTP is complex. LTP involves the activation of NMDARs that contain GluN2A and GluN2B subunits, expressed most probably as a combination of diheteromeric GluN1/GluN2A and triheteromeric GluN1/GluN2A/GluN2B assemblies. Surprisingly, STP comprises two pharmacologically distinct components. One component of STP, which we term STP1, is induced through activation of NMDARs that contain the GluN2A subunit. STP1 could not be pharmacologically isolated from LTP. Induction of the second component of STP, which we term STP2, is mediated through activation of GluN2B- and GluN2D-containing NMDARs. STP2 can be readily induced following complete pharmacological block of LTP and STP1 and decays more slowly than STP1. These data constitute the first evidence that different NMDAR subtypes mediate the induction of temporally distinct components of synaptic plasticity and that STP comprises two mechanistically distinct processes.
Methods
Slice preparation and electrophysiological recordings
Experiments were performed after institutional approval, according to the UK Scientific Procedures Act, 1986 and European Union guidelines for animal care. Animals were killed by an overdose of isoflurane anaesthesia and death was confirmed by a permanent cessation of the circulation (Schedule 1). As described previously (Volianskis & Jensen, 2003), transverse slices (400 μm) were cut from the septal end of the hippocampus (male Wistar rats, ≍300 g) using a McIllwain tissue chopper. Slices were pre-incubated for at least 2 h at room temperature before the start of the experiments. During the experiments, the slices were perfused with saline (in mm: 124 NaCl, 3.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 2 CaCl2, 2 MgSO4 and 10 glucose, saturated with 95% O2– 5% CO2 at 37 °C) and maintained submerged (32.5°C).
Field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (f-EPSPs) were recorded in the CA1-B area of stratum radiatum using glass electrodes filled with saline solution, amplified (AxoPatch 1D; Axon Instruments, Union City, CA, USA), filtered at 4 kHz (CyberAmp 380; Axon) and digitized (Digidata 1440A; Axon) at 100 kHz. The Schaffer collaterals were stimulated (stimulus duration 100 μs, Master 8; A.M.P.I., Jerusalem, Israel) via a bipolar concentric tungsten electrode (World Precision Instruments (WPI), Sarasota, FL, USA). Stimulation current (A385; WPI) was fixed to three times the threshold for evoking f-EPSPs. The signals were recorded using pCLAMP software (Axon). f-EPSPs were evoked at a baseline frequency of 0.067 Hz. Potentiation was induced by a theta-burst tetanization protocol (four stimuli at 100 Hz, repeated 10 times at a frequency of 5 Hz). Following delivery of the tetanus, stimulation was paused for 3 min. To estimate the maximum amount of NMDAR-dependent potentiation (PMAX) we used an average of the first four f-EPSPs following the 3 min pause in stimulation. In some experiments we paused stimulation for a further 30 min following recording of PMAX. In experiments, which used NMDA antagonists, compounds were bath applied for 30 min before the induction of potentiation and then washed out following recording of PMAX.
Whole-cell voltage clamp recordings were made from the cell bodies of identified (using differential interference contrast optics) pyramidal neurons in the CA1 region. Patch pipettes (3–5 MΩ) were filled with an internal solution containing (in mm) CsMeSO3, 130; NaCl, 8; Mg-ATP, 4; Na-GTP, 0.3; EGTA, 0.5; Hepes 10; QX-314, 5; BAPTA, 10; which had an osmolarity of 280 mOsm and pH of 7.2. Only experiments where there was less than 20% change in Rs (series resistance) were included in analysis. Recordings were made with an Axon 700B amplifier and digitized at a sampling rate of 10 kHz, using a CED 1401 A/D board with Signal Version 2 software. Excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) were evoked by extracellular stimulation of Schaffer collaterals in stratum radiatum using a monopolar stimulating electrode at a rate of either 0.05 or 0.017 Hz. To isolate the NMDAR-mediated component of the EPSC, NBQX (2,3-dihydroxy-6-nitro-7-sulfamoylbenzo(F)quinoxaline, 5 μm) and picrotoxin (50 μm) were included in the bath solution and the cell was voltage clamped at –30 mV. The amplitude of NMDAR-mediated EPSCs was measured offline, from 1 min averages. EPSCs were fitted using a double exponential function.
Analyses of electrophysiological data
The rate of rise of f-EPSPs was measured before (baseline) and after the induction of potentiation and normalized to baseline (defined as 100%). The increase in synaptic transmission was expressed as the amplitude of potentiation by subtracting the control level (100%) from the normalized rising rates of f-EPSPs. This permits the amplitudes of transient and sustained phases of potentiation to be compared directly in terms of percentage increase over baseline. Results are presented by plots of mean estimates of potentiation amplitudes (± standard error of the mean, SEM) plotted over time.
The amplitudes of transient (STP) and sustained (LTP) phases of potentiation and the decay time constants of the transient phases (τ) were determined by a mono-exponential fitting routine (P= LTP + STP e−t/τ, Levenverg–Marquardt method) and by double exponential fits using Prism (GraphPad Software, Inc., La Jolla, CA, USA). Mean estimates of STP and LTP, which were derived from individual experiments, were used to generate the exponential decay functions that are shown overlaid upon the mean data sets to illustrate the goodness of the fits. Statistical comparisons of single and double exponential fits of averaged datasets (see Fig. 9) were done using Prism.
STP is characterized by both its amplitude (percentage increase over baseline) and its duration (τ). We have therefore defined the amount of STP expressed in an individual experiment in terms of the area under its exponential decay (STP = amplitude ×τ) whereas the amount of LTP was defined simply as its amplitude (percentage increase over baseline). Effects of NMDAR antagonists on induction of STP and LTP were quantified in individual experiments in terms of percentage reduction (block) in the amount of STP and in the amplitude of LTP relative to their mean levels in control experiments (i.e. without the application of antagonists). These data were pooled for each concentration of an antagonist (mean ± SEM) and used to construct concentration–response curves describing the antagonist's effects on STP and LTP. The data were fit by single or biphasic sigmoidal concentration–response curves to assess the fitted curves statistically and to interpolate the half maximal inhibitory concentration (IC50) values (Prism).
Analyses of NMDAR antagonists’ effects on recombinant NMDARs expressed in HEK293 cells
The full-length cDNAs encoding rat NMDAR subunits GluN1a and GluN2B, kindly provided by S. Nakanishi (Institute for Immunology, Kyoto University, Japan), were subcloned into the mammalian expression vector pcDNA1/Amp (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA, USA). GluN2A in the RK7 vector was a generous gift from P. Seeburg (University of Heidelberg ZMBH, Heidelberg, Germany). Cell transfection was carried out as described previously (Collett & Collingridge, 2004; Bartlett et al. 2007). Briefly, HEK293 cells were maintained in culture at 37°C, plated on coverslips, then transiently transfected using SuperFect (Qiagen, Valencia, CA, USA) with a molar ratio of cDNA for GluN1a/GluN2 of 1:3, to give optimal expression of NMDARs (Chazot et al. 1994). After transfection, the cells were maintained for 24 h in a glutamine-free medium containing 100 μm d-AP5 to prevent activation of expressed receptors (Anegawa et al. 1995).
As described previously (Bartlett et al. 2007), transfected cells were washed and loaded with the calcium indicator Fluo3-AM (5 μm; Sigma, St Louis, MO, USA) in Hepes-buffered saline (HBS) and subsequently viewed on a confocal microscope (MRC 1024; Bio-Rad, Hercules, CA, USA) equipped with an argon laser. Cells were continuously perfused with HBS containing glycine (10 μm) at a rate of 3 ml min−1. NMDA (with 10 μm glycine) was applied in the presence or absence of the antagonists. An increase in fluorescence intensity, in response to NMDA, identified cells containing functional receptors. During an experiment NMDA (15 μm, 40 s) was applied at 15 min intervals: two responses in control buffer were obtained, followed by two in the presence of the antagonist and a further two in the control buffer. Acquired images were analysed using Scion Image (Scion Corporation, Frederick, MD, USA). For each recording the peak fluorescence (measured as pixel density) of each cell in response to NMDA was normalized to its pre-agonist level because of cell-to-cell variation in size, transfection level and indicator loading. Inhibition was determined by comparing the second application of NMDA in the presence of antagonist with the mean of the second control responses before and after antagonist. Data are presented as mean percentage inhibition (± SEM) versus antagonist concentration. IC50 values were calculated using non-linear regression analysis (Prism).
Chemicals
d-AP5 was acquired from Ascent (Bristol, UK). Ro 25-6981 was obtained from Tocris (Bristol, UK). NVP-AAM077 was a generous gift from Novartis Pharma AG, Switzerland, and UBP145 was synthesized in house as described previously (Morley et al. 2005; Costa et al. 2009). NMDAR antagonists were prepared as stock solutions, stored frozen and added to perfusion saline as indicated in the Results. All other chemicals and salts were from Fisher Scientific (Loughborough, UK) or Sigma (Dorset, UK).
Statistical analyses
Analyses, statistical evaluation and presentation of the data were performed with Platin (custom build software, M.S.J.) and Prism IV (GraphPad Software, Inc.). Student's (two-tailed) t tests were used for either within- (paired) or between- (unpaired) group comparisons. One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Dunnett's Multiple Comparison Test (DT) or Newman–Keuls Multiple Comparison Test (NKMCT) was used in cases of multiple comparisons. An F test was used for statistical evaluation of exponential and sigmoidal fits and for statistical comparison of IC50 values (Prism). Statistical differences were set at P < 0.05.
Results
Differential effects of AP5 on STP and LTP
In control experiments (Fig. 1A), f-EPSPs were potentiated by theta-burst stimulation (TBS). The initial potentiation (PMAX), which was measured at 4 min post-tetanus to avoid contamination from post-tetanic potentiation, was 117 ± 7% (increase over baseline, n= 25). STP declined with a mean time constant of 16.0 ± 1.4 min to a stable LTP of 50 ± 5% (increase over baseline, Fig. 1A). The amplitude of STP, calculated from an exponential fit to the data, was 61 ± 4%, and so was roughly equivalent in magnitude to the LTP that followed. Similar values for STP and LTP were obtained in experiments in which a 30 min pause in stimulation was imposed following TBS (PMAX 111 ± 8%, STP 54 ± 6%, tau 16.1 ± 2.5 min, LTP 48 ± 7%, n= 14, Fig. 1A), confirming the findings of previous studies showing that STP can be stored after a pause in stimulation (Volianskis & Jensen, 2003; Volianskis et al. 2010).
To investigate the NMDAR dependence of STP and LTP, AP5 was applied during baseline and TBS. AP5 (30–100 μm) blocked the induction of both STP and LTP (Fig. 1A). At lower concentrations, however, AP5 had strikingly different effects on STP and LTP (Fig. 1B). Similar differential effects of AP5 were observed in experiments with and without a pause in stimulation and so the data from both types of experiments were pooled (Fig. 1C). Notably, 0.3 μm AP5 reduced STP (P < 0.01, when compared to control, ANOVA, DT) without appreciably affecting LTP whilst 3 μm AP5 essentially blocked LTP (P < 0.001, when compared to control, DT), without further affecting STP to any significant extent (Fig. 1C). Construction of full concentration–response curves revealed that inhibition of LTP by AP5 was fit well with a single sigmoidal function (Fig. 1D, IC50∼ 1 μm) whereas inhibition of STP was best fit with a double sigmoidal function with IC50 values that differed 65-fold (Fig. 1D). The differential sensitivity of the components of potentiation to AP5 is highly suggestive that more than one NMDAR subtype is activated during TBS and is responsible for LTP and the two components of STP.
Effects of antagonists on NMDAR subunits expressed in HEK293 cells
GluN2A, 2B and 2D NMDAR subtypes are expressed in the adult hippocampus (Thompson et al. 2002). To investigate their role in the induction of LTP we selected three additional subtype-selective antagonists: NVP-AAM077 (NVP; Auberson et al. 2002), Ro 25-6981 (Ro; Fischer et al. 1997) and UBP145 (UBP; Costa et al. 2009). The potency of these antagonists was assessed on NMDAR-mediated signals in HEK293 cells expressing recombinant rat GluN2A-, GluN2B- or GluN2D-containing receptors (Fig. 2). NVP showed a greater than 10-fold selectivity for GluN2A over GluN2B, separating the two subunits effectively at a concentration of 100 nm (Fig. 2A). It was also an effective inhibitor of GluN2D. Ro was highly potent at GluN2B but had no effect on either GluN2A or GluN2D up to concentrations of around 10 μm, above which it inhibited GluN2A but not GluN2D (Fig. 2B). Notably, maximal inhibition of GluN2B by Ro depended on the agonist concentration (Fischer et al. 1997) whereas this was not the case for GluN2A. In contrast, UBP was approximately 10-fold more selective for GluN2D over either GluN2A or GluN2B (Fig. 2C). In conclusion, whilst no compound is ideal, the selectivity spectrum of these antagonists is sufficient to address the role of NMDAR subtypes in the induction of STP and LTP, provided that they are used in a comparative and quantitative manner.
Effects of antagonists on NMDAR-mediated EPSCs
Available data suggest that the pharmacology of recombinant NMDARs and native NMDARs is very similar (Buller et al. 1994; Christie et al. 2000; Feng et al. 2004). To establish the extent to which NMDARs containing GluN2A, 2B and 2D subunits contributed to synaptically activated NMDARs in adult hippocampal slices we studied pharmacologically isolated NMDAR-EPSCs (Fig. 3). The high sensitivity of pharmacologically isolated NMDAR-EPSCs to both AP5 and NVP (IC50 of 0.6 and 0.03 μm, respectively) suggests that they are predominately mediated by an NMDAR containing the GluN2A subunit (Fig. 3A and B). Ro, however, had interesting effects. It inhibited NMDAR-EPSCs at concentrations much higher than those that blocked GluN1/GluN2B receptors, but much lower than those that blocked GluN1/GluN2A receptors in HEK293 cells (Fig. 3C). This intermediate sensitivity can most readily be explained by its actions on a GluN1/GluN2A/GluN2B triheteromer (see Hatton & Paoletti, 2005; Gray et al. 2011; Rauner & Kohr, 2011). In support of this, a related GluN2B selective antagonist ifenprodil has been shown to block around 20% of GluN1/GluN2A/GluN2B triheteromeric receptors at a concentration of 30 μm while being around 10 times less potent than Ro (Hatton & Paoletti, 2005). The low sensitivity of the NMDAR-EPSCs to inhibition by UBP demonstrates that GluN2D-containing receptors are unlikely to contribute to synaptic transmission (Fig. 3D). The sensitivity of the NMDAR-EPSC to higher concentrations of UBP correlated closely with its actions on both GluN2A and 2B subunits.
Further support for the suggestion that the NMDARs, which mediate the EPSC, are composed of both GluN2A and 2B but not GluN2D subunits comes from analyses of EPSC decay. The decay of NMDAR-EPSCs recorded during baseline conditions could be described by a double exponential fit (τFast= 56 ± 1.3 ms and τSlow= 316 ± 10 ms), with about 80% of the total EPSC amplitude contributed by the fast component (Fig. 4Aa–Ca). This is considerably faster that the decay of GluN2D-containing receptors (τw > 1 s), but is consistent with a response that involves both GluN2A and GluN2B subunits (Monyer et al. 1994; Vicini et al. 1998). NVP inhibited the fast component to a greater extent than the slow component of the NMDAR-EPSC, whereas Ro had the inverse effect. To visualize this differential sensitivity we scaled the amplitude of the slow component for each antagonist to the baseline response. As shown in Fig. 4A for 0.1 μm NVP, this led to a substantially smaller peak current, reflecting the greater sensitivity of the fast component to this antagonist. In contrast, Ro had a disproportionally smaller effect on the fast component, such that the scaled peak response was slightly larger (Fig. 4B). In contrast, UBP did not differentially affect the two components to any significant extent (Fig. 4C).
To compare the effects of the three antagonists on the the fast and the slow components of the EPSC more precisely, their percentage inhibition was calculated from the two-exponential fits for each concentration of NVP (Fig. 4Ab), Ro (Fig. 4Bb) and UBP (Fig. 4Cb). With respect to Ro, both the fast and the slow components had identical sensitivity, in terms of IC50 values. This strongly suggests that a single population of GluN2B-containing NMDARs (most likely a triheteromeric receptor containing both GluN2A and GluN2B subunits) mediates the majority of the slow component and a significant fraction of the fast component. NVP was slightly more potent on the fast compared to the slow component, suggesting that its actions were mediated via two different receptor populations (most likely GluN2A-containing diheteromers and GluN2A- and GluN2B-containing triheteromers, respectively). UBP did not differentiate between the two components, consistent with a similar sensitivity on both GluN2A- and GluN2B-containing receptors.
In conclusion, these observations support a role of GluN2A- and GluN2B- but not GluN2D-containing NMDARs in the mediation of the EPSC and suggest that the involvement of GluN2B is as part of a triheteromeric assembly.
NVP preferentially inhibits LTP
In experiments in which GluN2A-containing receptors were antagonized by 30 nm NVP, LTP was reduced significantly (P < 0.05, DT) while STP was unaffected. LTP was eliminated but substantial STP remained in experiments with 100 nm NVP (Fig. 5A). This result was strikingly similar to experiments using 3 μm AP5, and again was independent of whether there was a pause in stimulation following TBS (Fig. 5A and B). Concentration–response curves, illustrated in Fig. 5C, show that a single sigmoidal function describes the inhibition of LTP (IC50= 0.02 μm) whereas STP is fitted preferentially with a bi-sigmoidal function (IC50= 0.04 and 0.2 μm). The ability to preserve a substantial amount of STP following complete block of LTP (using 100 nm NVP) enabled us to investigate some of its properties in isolation. We observed that the rate of decay of this residual STP was related to the frequency of stimulation (Fig. 5D), as originally described for STP recorded in the presence of LTP and without a delay in stimulation (Volianskis & Jensen, 2003).
In summary, these results clearly demonstrate a role for GluN2A in the induction of LTP and one component of STP. The lack of complete inhibition of STP by GluN2A selective concentrations of NVP supports the conclusions drawn from the experiments using AP5 that STP comprises two pharmacologically distinct components.
Ro preferentially inhibits STP
Inhibition of GluN2B-containing NMDARs during induction of STP and LTP had dramatically different effects from inhibition of GluN2A-containing NMDARs (Fig. 6). For example, in experiments in which GluN2B-containing receptors were blocked with 1 μm Ro, the induction of STP was substantially reduced (P < 0.001, DT) but LTP was not inhibited significantly when compared to the control (Fig. 6A and B). A significant reduction of LTP was first detected when the concentration of Ro was increased to 5 μm (Fig. 6C), although no further inhibition of STP was observed at this concentration. However, higher concentrations of Ro blocked both LTP and the remaining component of STP. The inhibition of STP by Ro was fit by a double sigmoidal function, with IC50 values that differed around 600-fold (Fig. 6C). Such bi-modal inhibition further supports the notion that more than one NMDAR subtype is involved in the induction of STP. In contrast, a single sigmoidal function was again sufficient to describe the inhibition of LTP, with an IC50 value of 3.8 μm (Fig. 6C). This finding that STP is more sensitive than LTP to inhibition of GluN2B subunits by Ro further demonstrates the differential role of GluN2 subunits in these two phases of potentiation. The sensitivity of LTP to Ro, which parallels inhibition of the NMDAR-EPSC, is consistent with the involvement of triheteromeric NMDARs containing both the GluN2A and the GluN2B subunits in its induction (Hatton & Paoletti, 2005; Gray et al. 2011; Rauner & Kohr, 2011).
The ability of very low concentrations of Ro to antagonize one component of STP strongly suggests that NMDARs containing GluN2B subunits mediate induction of this component. The requirement for high concentrations of Ro to block the second component of STP could be due to its action at triheteromeric receptors containing both the GluN2A and the GluN2B subunits (i.e. similar to that observed with LTP). These findings, when taken together with the results using NVP, suggest that there exist two components of STP, which are induced differentially via the activation of NMDARs that are highly sensitive to antagonism of GluN2A or GluN2B subunits, respectively. In support of this notion, co-application of a GluN2A selective concentration of NVP (100 nm) with 5 μm Ro blocked the induction of STP (Fig. 6D).
UBP also preferentially inhibits STP
The effects of the GluN2D selective antagonist UBP were broadly similar to those of Ro (Fig. 7). For example, the induction of STP was substantially inhibited by 10 μm UBP whereas LTP was unaffected (Fig. 7A and B). LTP was first affected when the concentration of UBP was increased to 30 μm and all phases of potentiation were blocked at 100 μm (Fig. 7C). As with the other antagonists, the inhibition of LTP was best fit with a single sigmoidal function (IC50= 30 μm) and STP with a double sigmoidal function (Fig. 7C).
The ability of the lower concentrations of UBP to inhibit one component of STP (IC50∼ 2 μm) correlates with its ability to inhibit GluN2D subunits (Fig. 2C). The relatively insensitive component of STP was inhibited similarly to LTP (IC50 values for both ∼ 30 μm) at concentrations at which UBP inhibits GluN2A and GluN2B subunits (Fig. 2C). This result is similar to that observed with Ro, and suggests that both Ro and UBP may be inhibiting the same component of STP. To test this directly, we applied a concentration of UBP (30 μm) that eliminated one component of STP and tested the sensitivity of the residual component to Ro. We selected a concentration of Ro (5 μm) that was also maximal for inhibition of the sensitive component to this antagonist. Addition of Ro had no further effect, demonstrating that the UBP- and Ro-sensitive components were identical. The residual component was, however, eliminated by a low concentration of AP5 (0.3 μm). These results provide strong evidence for the existence of two components of STP, one involving activation of GluN2A- and the other both GluN2B- and GluN2D-containing NMDARs.
Correlation between the inhibition of NMDAR-EPSCs and synaptic potentiation
The ability of the four NMDAR antagonists to inhibit NMDAR-EPSCs versus their ability to inhibit LTP and STP is illustrated in Fig. 8A–C. These plots show that antagonism of the NMDAR-EPSC strongly correlates with inhibition of LTP (Fig. 8A). Inhibition of one component of STP, which we now term STP1, has the same rank order potency to the four antagonists as LTP (i.e. NVP > AP5 > Ro > UBP) and also correlates, although less strongly, with the inhibition of the NMDAR-EPSC (Fig. 8B). The second component of STP, termed STP2, has a completely different rank order potency from STP1 and LTP (Ro > NVP > UBP > AP5) and shows no correlation with the NMDAR-EPSC (Fig. 8C). Pharmacologically, therefore, STP1 and STP2 are distinct entities.
In addition to these different sensitivities to the antagonists, inhibition of these two components of STP was also qualitatively different. Inhibition of STP1, using either 0.1 μm NVP or 3 μm AP5, resulted in a large reduction in the amplitude of STP with little effect on its decay time constant (Fig. 8D and E). Conversely, inhibition of STP2 with either Ro (1 μm) or UBP (10 μm) resulted in a smaller effect on the amplitude of STP but dramatically increased its rate of decay (Fig. 8F and G). From this it can be concluded that these two components contribute differently to the expression of STP. For example, at a baseline stimulation rate of 0.067 Hz, the decay of the two components can be described with single exponentials of about 7 and 16 min, respectively. The relative size and kinetics of the three synaptic components (LTP, STP1 and STP2) are compared in Fig. 9.
Discussion
In the present study we have utilized four NMDAR antagonists to investigate the NMDAR subtypes involved in two temporally distinct forms of hippocampal synaptic plasticity, STP and LTP. We have found that inhibition of LTP correlates closely with inhibition of the EPSC and involves activation of both GluN2A and GluN2B receptors, the latter most likely as part of a triheteromeric complex. In addition, we have found that STP comprises two pharmacologically and kinetically distinct components, the predominant one of which involves activation of GluN2B and GluN2D receptors.
Subtype selectivity of NMDAR antagonists
To investigate the role of NMDAR subtypes in LTP we have used four antagonists, the prototypic NMDAR antagonist AP5 and three antagonists showing a degree of selectivity towards the various subtypes. AP5, NVP and UBP are competitive antagonists, binding at the glutamate-binding site with a different potency at the different GluN2 subunits. The mechanism of action of the GluN2B-selective allosteric antagonist Ro is more complex in that it binds at the interface of the heterodimer, which is formed by GluN1 and GluN2B subunits (Karakas et al. 2011). The difference in the mechanisms of action of the antagonists can affect the interpretation of the results of the experiments, especially in the case of Ro, which is sensitive to a number of ambient factors modulating its potency. To be confident in the conclusions of our experiments we first characterized NVP, Ro and UBP in a recombinant receptor assay in HEK293 cells (Fig. 2) describing their selectivity. We then conducted the intracellular and extracellular studies in hippocampal slices and constructed full concentration–response curves for each of the conditions for each of the antagonists. In addition, we designed experiments in which antagonists with different mechanisms of action were co-applied to verify their synergistic effects and our conclusions.
We confirmed that NVP shows approximately 10-fold selectivity for GluN2A- versus GluN2B-containing receptors but found that GluN2D-containing receptors are also fairly sensitive to this antagonist (see also Feng et al. 2004). As expected, Ro was highly selective at GluN2B-containing receptors (Fischer et al. 1997), showing no inhibition of GluN2D-containing receptors. We did, however, confirm that Ro inhibits GluN2A-containing receptors at very high concentrations (Fischer et al. 1997). Furthermore, the maximal inhibition of GluN2B but not GluN2A-containing receptors by Ro was agonist concentration-dependent with greater inhibition found at higher agonist concentrations (Fischer et al. 1997). Given this, and other complexities in the non-competitve action of Ro, caution is needed when interpreting the results of this compound (Fischer et al. 1997; Hatton & Paoletti, 2005; Paoletti & Neyton, 2007). We also characterized in detail UBP145 (Costa et al. 2009) and found that it inhibits GluN2D-containing receptors more potently than GluN2A- or GluN2B-containing receptors, which were inhibited equally by this compound. Clearly, with the exception of Ro, these antagonists are not sufficiently selective for firm conclusions to be drawn when used in isolation. However, by comparing their relative potencies it is possible to make firm conclusions about the roles of NMDAR subtypes in the induction of STP and LTP. For this reason we have constructed full concentration–response curves for each of the antagonists and have based our conclusions on comparative criteria.
Role of NMDAR subtypes in LTP
There is evidence from knockout mice for a role of both GluN2A- and GluN2B-containing NMDARs in LTP (Sakimura et al. 1995; Kiyama et al. 1998; Weitlauf et al. 2005; von Engelhardt et al. 2008). The observation that LTP is highly sensitive to NVP is in agreement with previous observations using this antagonist (Liu et al. 2004; Berberich et al. 2005, 2007; Weitlauf et al. 2005; Bartlett et al. 2007; Li et al. 2007) and it has been suggested that GluN2A-containing receptors are important for induction of LTP (Liu et al. 2004). The possibility that this could be due to its inhibition of GluN2D-containing receptors is extremely unlikely, given that UBP only affected LTP at concentrations that inhibited GluN2A- and GluN2B-containing receptors. A similar conclusion was reached previously based on the relative insensitivity of LTP to a different, less selective, GluN2D antagonist (+/−)-cis-1-(phenanthren-2yl-carbonyl)- piperazine-2,3-dicarboxylic acid (Hrabetova et al. 2000). Pharmacological evidence for an involvement of GluN2B subunit-containing NMDARs in the induction of LTP is also well documented (Hrabetova et al. 2000; Bartlett et al. 2007; Berberich et al. 2007; Li et al. 2007). Thus, based on these various studies, it can be concluded that both GluN2A and GluN2B subunits are involved in the induction of LTP at CA3–CA1 synapses.
In our study, LTP was inhibited by Ro with an IC50 of 3 μm, which is in agreement with our earlier study that used a high concentration of Ro (5 μm) at the CA3–CA1 synapses of juvenile rats (Bartlett et al. 2007), whereas studies that used lower concentrations of this or related compounds have reported less or no inhibition of LTP (Liu et al. 2004; Berberich et al. 2007; Li et al. 2007; Papouin et al. 2012). Therefore, the results can be best explained by the sensitivity of LTP to concentration-dependent inhibition by this class of GluN2B antagonist.
Notably, Ro had no effect on LTP at the concentration range over which it inhibits GluN1/GluN2B diheteromers, suggesting that these species are not involved it its induction. We can discount the possibility that submicromolar concentrations of Ro are unable to antagonize GluN2B-containing receptors in a physiological context, as STP was sensitive to Ro at these concentrations. Although we found that Ro can inhibit GluN2A subunits, this occurred at much higher concentrations than those that blocked LTP (IC50 > 60 μm, see also Fischer et al. 1997). The most likely explanation of our findings is therefore that Ro inhibits LTP via antagonism of GluN1/GluN2A/GluN2B triheteromers, which have been shown to have an intermediate sensitivity to this class of inhibitor (Hatton & Paoletti, 2005; Gray et al. 2011) and are expressed at CA1 synapses of adult rodents (Rauner & Kohr, 2011).
Interestingly, there was a very strong correlation between the ability of all four compounds to antagonize the NMDAR-EPSC and LTP. This is entirely consistent with the widely held view that the NMDARs that mediate the EPSC are also responsible for the induction of LTP. Thus, our results are most readily explained by LTP being induced via post-synaptic NMDARs, which comprise a mixture of GluN1/GluN2A diheteromers and GluN1/GluN2A/GluN2B triheteromers.
Role of NMDAR subtypes in STP
A surprising observation in this study was that STP could be separated pharmacologically into two distinct components. One component, which we refer to as STP1, was antagonized with the same rank order potency as LTP (NVP > AP5 > Ro > UBP). The simplest explanation is that STP1 represents an initial decremental phase of LTP, although we cannot exclude that it is an entirely distinct component of synaptic plasticity, which involves a similar class of NMDAR as LTP for its induction. The second component of STP, STP2, has a completely different sensitivity to NMDAR antagonists with a rank order potency of Ro > NVP > UBP > AP5. STP1 and STP2 also differ in their kinetics, such that STP1 contributes largely to the peak of STP whereas STP2 dominates its decay.
The sensitivity of STP2 to all four antagonists is consistent with the involvement of both GluN2B- and GluN2D- but not GluN2A-containing NMDARs. The high sensitivity of STP2 to Ro, compared to STP1, LTP and NMDAR-EPSCs, suggests that the GluN2B subunit that is involved in its induction is not part of a triheteromeric NMDAR comprising GluN1/GluN2A/GluN2B subunits. The low sensitivity to AP5 also argues against a role of the GluN2A subunit in STP2 but is consistent with a role of the GluN2D subunit (Ikeda et al. 1992; Buller & Monaghan, 1997). In support of this, the high sensitivity of STP2 to both UBP and NVP is fully consistent with a role of GluN2D subunits. The observation that maximally effective concentrations of UBP and Ro are not additive could be explained by either the existence of two distinct GluN2B- and GluN2D-containing subtypes, which both need to be activated for STP2 to be induced, or, more simply, by a single population of GluN1/GluN2B/GluN2D triheteromers. Interestingly, triheteromers of this composition have been described in the CNS (Pina-Crespo & Gibb, 2002; Brickley et al. 2003; Jones & Gibb, 2005; Brothwell et al. 2008). GluN2B/2D-containing receptors that are responsible for the induction of STP2 at the CA3–CA1 synapses might be expressed either extra- (Lozovaya et al. 2004) or pre-synaptically (Thompson et al. 2002; McGuinness et al. 2010), which would explain a lack of correlation between the inhibition of STP2 and the inhibition of the NMDAR-EPSC.
In addition to identifying the NMDAR subtypes involved in STP, the pharmacological experiments showed that the two components of STP could exist independently of each other. Thus, blockade of STP2 enabled STP1 to be recorded together with LTP, whereas blockade of STP1 and LTP enabled STP2 to be recorded in isolation. It has been shown previously that STP, when it is co-induced with LTP, can be ‘stored’ in the absence of stimulation (Volianskis & Jensen, 2003). In this study we extended this observation and show that both STP1 and STP2 can be stored in the absence of stimulation. Both STP1 and STP2 decay in an activity-dependent manner that depends on the number, but not the frequency, of stimuli. However, their decay kinetics differ with estimated tau values of 7 and 16 min, respectively, with a test stimulation frequency of 0.067 Hz. In other words, STP1 and STP2 require approximately 28 and 64 stimuli, respectively, to decay to 1/e of their initial amplitude.
Functional significance of STP and LTP
Although the full physiological significance of STP and LTP remains to be determined, it is interesting to note that STP can be induced independently of LTP both in vitro (Kauer et al. 1988; Anwyl et al. 1989; Malenka, 1991; Colino et al. 1992; Kullmann et al. 1992; Erickson et al. 2010) and in vivo (Buschler et al. 2012). It has also been shown that STP and LTP can be either co-expressed or expressed independently of each other at the level of single synapses (Debanne et al. 1999). Thus, STP can be thought of as an independent process, and a phenomenon that resembles STP has been observed after exploratory learning in rats (Moser et al. 1993, Moser 1994).
There is a huge body of literature that equates NMDAR-LTP with learning and memory (Martin et al. 2000). However, this has usually assumed that NMDAR-LTP is a single process. Speculatively, if LTP indeed relates to long-term memory then STP could be used to store short-term memories. Short-term memory, or working memory, is actually a far more common occurrence with thousands of pieces of information stored for short periods of time but very few for long periods of time in any given day (Albright et al. 2000). If this is the case, then the prediction is that short-term working memory should be particularly sensitive to inhibition by GluN2B and GluN2D preferring antagonists.
The use-dependence of the decay of STP endows it with a particularly interesting functional property (Volianskis & Jensen, 2003). It could, in principle, store information up until the point of recall, assuming that recall involves reactivation of the same synapses that are used to encode the memory (Albright et al. 2000). This would be particularly useful for storing information for variable periods of time until the information is, once recalled, no longer of relevance: for example, remembering where one last placed one's cell phone.
The findings of the present study add to the current knowledge about STP and LTP by showing that NMDAR-dependent potentiation at the CA3–CA1 synapses in the hippocampus is more complex process than generally thought. In terms of induction, we have identified three components of synaptic potentiation that involve the activation of different NMDAR subtypes. A major component of STP involves receptors containing GluN2B and GluN2D subunits whereas LTP involves receptors containing GluN2A and GluN2B subunits.
Acknowledgments
The study was supported by the Danish Medical Research Council (271-05-0712), the MRC (G0601812), the NIH (MH60252) and the Welcome Trust. The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Glossary
- ANOVA
analysis of variance
- AP5
d-2-amino-5-phosphonopentanoate
- DT
Dunnett's Multiple Comparison Test
- EPSCs
excitatory postsynaptic currents
- f-EPSPs
field excitatory postsynaptic potentials
- IC50
half maximal inhibitory concentration
- LTP
long-term potentiation
- NKMCT
Newman–Keuls Multiple Comparison Test
- NMDARs
N-methyl-d-aspartate receptors
- SEM
standard error of the mean
- STP
short-term potentiation
- TBS
theta-burst stimulation
Author contributions
All experiments were performed at the University of Bristol, UK. A.V., N.B., M.S.J. and G.L.C. designed the study. A.V., N.B., V.J.C. and M.W.J. performed the experiments and analysed the data. D.T.M., D.E.J. and G.L.C. supervised chemical, pharmacological and physiological investigations. A.V., N.B. and S.F.M. wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors have read, commented on and approved the final version.
References
- Albright TD, Jessell TM, Kandel ER, Posner MI. Neural science: a century of progress and the mysteries that remain. Neuron. 2000;25(Suppl):S1–55. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80912-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen P, Sundberg SH, Sveen O, Wigstrom H. Specific long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in hippocampal slices. Nature. 1977;266:736–737. doi: 10.1038/266736a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anegawa NJ, Lynch DR, Verdoorn TA, Pritchett DB. Transfection of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors in a nonneuronal cell line leads to cell death. J Neurochem. 1995;64:2004–2012. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1995.64052004.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anwyl R, Mulkeen D, Rowan MJ. The role of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors in the generation of short-term potentiation in the rat hippocampus. Brain Res. 1989;503:148–151. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(89)91716-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Auberson YP, Allgeier H, Bischoff S, Lingenhoehl K, Moretti R, Schmutz M. 5-Phosphonomethylquinoxalinediones as competitive NMDA receptor antagonists with a preference for the human 1A/2A, rather than 1A/2B receptor composition. Bioorg Med Chem Lett. 2002;12:1099–1102. doi: 10.1016/s0960-894x(02)00074-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartlett TE, Bannister NJ, Collett VJ, Dargan SL, Massey PV, Bortolotto ZA, Fitzjohn SM, Bashir ZI, Collingridge GL, Lodge D. Differential roles of NR2A and NR2B-containing NMDA receptors in LTP and LTD in the CA1 region of two-week old rat hippocampus. Neuropharmacology. 2007;52:60–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2006.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berberich S, Jensen V, Hvalby O, Seeburg PH, Kohr G. The role of NMDAR subtypes and charge transfer during hippocampal LTP induction. Neuropharmacology. 2007;52:77–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2006.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berberich S, Punnakkal P, Jensen V, Pawlak V, Seeburg PH, Hvalby O, Kohr G. Lack of NMDA receptor subtype selectivity for hippocampal long-term potentiation. J Neurosci. 2005;25:6907–6910. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1905-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bliss TV, Collingridge GL. A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature. 1993;361:31–39. doi: 10.1038/361031a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bliss TV, Lomo T. Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J Physiol. 1973;232:331–356. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brickley SG, Misra C, Mok MH, Mishina M, Cull-Candy SG. NR2B and NR2D subunits coassemble in cerebellar Golgi cells to form a distinct NMDA receptor subtype restricted to extrasynaptic sites. J Neurosci. 2003;23:4958–4966. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-12-04958.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brothwell SL, Barber JL, Monaghan DT, Jane DE, Gibb AJ, Jones S. NR2B- and NR2D-containing synaptic NMDA receptors in developing rat substantia nigra pars compacta dopaminergic neurones. J Physiol. 2008;586:739–750. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.144618. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buller AL, Larson HC, Schneider BE, Beaton JA, Morrisett RA, Monaghan DT. The molecular basis of NMDA receptor subtypes: native receptor diversity is predicted by subunit composition. J Neurosci. 1994;14:5471–5484. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-09-05471.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buller AL, Monaghan DT. Pharmacological heterogeneity of NMDA receptors: characterization of NR1a/NR2D heteromers expressed in Xenopus oocytes. Eur J Pharmacol. 1997;320:87–94. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(96)00880-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buschler A, Goh JJ, Manahan-Vaughan D. Frequency dependency of NMDA receptor-dependent synaptic plasticity in the hippocampal CA1 region of freely behaving mice. Hippocampus. 2012;22:2238–2248. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chazot PL, Coleman SK, Cik M, Stephenson FA. Molecular characterization of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors expressed in mammalian cells yields evidence for the coexistence of three subunit types within a discrete receptor molecule. J Biol Chem. 1994;269:24403–24409. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christie JM, Jane DE, Monaghan DT. Native N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors containing NR2A and NR2B subunits have pharmacologically distinct competitive antagonist binding sites. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2000;292:1169–1174. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colino A, Huang YY, Malenka RC. Characterization of the integration time for the stabilization of long-term potentiation in area CA1 of the hippocampus. J Neurosci. 1992;12:180–187. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-01-00180.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collett VJ, Collingridge GL. Interactions between NMDA receptors and mGlu5 receptors expressed in HEK293 cells. Br J Pharmacol. 2004;142:991–1001. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjp.0705861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collingridge GL, Kehl SJ, McLennan H. Excitatory amino acids in synaptic transmission in the Schaffer collateral–commissural pathway of the rat hippocampus. J Physiol. 1983;334:33–46. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collingridge GL, Olsen RW, Peters J, Spedding M. A nomenclature for ligand-gated ion channels. Neuropharmacology. 2009;56:2–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.06.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa BM, Feng B, Tsintsadze TS, Morley RM, Irvine MW, Tsintsadze VP, Lozovaya NA, Jane DE, Monaghan DT. NMDA receptor NR2 subunit selectivity of a series of novel piperazine-2,3-dicarboxylate derivatives; preferential blockade of extrasynaptic NMDA receptors in the rat hippocampal CA3–CA1 synapse. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2009;331:618–626. doi: 10.1124/jpet.109.156752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davies J, Francis AA, Jones AW, Watkins JC. 2-Amino-5-phosphonovalerate (2APV), a potent and selective antagonist of amino acid-induced and synaptic excitation. Neurosci Lett. 1981;21:77–81. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(81)90061-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Debanne D, Gahwiler BH, Thompson SM. Heterogeneity of synaptic plasticity at unitary CA3–CA1 and CA3–CA3 connections in rat hippocampal slice cultures. J Neurosci. 1999;19:10664–10671. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-24-10664.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erickson MA, Maramara LA, Lisman J. A single brief burst induces GluR1-dependent associative short-term potentiation: a potential mechanism for short-term memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22:2530–2540. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feng B, Tse HW, Skifter DA, Morley R, Jane DE, Monaghan DT. Structure–activity analysis of a novel NR2C/NR2D-preferring NMDA receptor antagonist: 1-(phenanthrene-2-carbonyl) piperazine-2,3-dicarboxylic acid. Br J Pharmacol. 2004;141:508–516. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjp.0705644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischer G, Mutel V, Trube G, Malherbe P, Kew JN, Mohacsi E, Heitz MP, Kemp JA. Ro 25–6981, a highly potent and selective blocker of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors containing the NR2B subunit. Characterization in vitro. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1997;283:1285–1292. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray JA, Shi Y, Usui H, During MJ, Sakimura K, Nicoll RA. Distinct modes of AMPA receptor suppression at developing synapses by GluN2A and GluN2B: single-cell NMDA receptor subunit deletion in vivo. Neuron. 2011;71:1085–1101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gustafsson B, Wigstrom H. Long-term potentiation in the hippocampal CA1 region: its induction and early temporal development. Prog Brain Res. 1990;83:223–232. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(08)61252-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanse E, Gustafsson B. Onset and stabilization of NMDA receptor-dependent hippocampal long-term potentiation. Neurosci Res. 1994;20:15–25. doi: 10.1016/0168-0102(94)90018-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hatton CJ, Paoletti P. Modulation of triheteromeric NMDA receptors by N-terminal domain ligands. Neuron. 2005;46:261–274. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hrabetova S, Serrano P, Blace N, Tse HW, Skifter DA, Jane DE, Monaghan DT, Sacktor TC. Distinct NMDA receptor subpopulations contribute to long-term potentiation and long-term depression induction. J Neurosci. 2000;20:RC81. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-12-j0002.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ikeda K, Nagasawa M, Mori H, Araki K, Sakimura K, Watanabe M, Inoue Y, Mishina M. Cloning and expression of the ɛ4 subunit of the NMDA receptor channel. FEBS Lett. 1992;313:34–38. doi: 10.1016/0014-5793(92)81178-o. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones S, Gibb AJ. Functional NR2B- and NR2D-containing NMDA receptor channels in rat substantia nigra dopaminergic neurones. J Physiol. 2005;569:209–221. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.095554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karakas E, Simorowski N, Furukawa H. Subunit arrangement and phenylethanolamine binding in GluN1/GluN2B NMDA receptors. Nature. 2011;475:249–253. doi: 10.1038/nature10180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kauer JA, Malenka RC, Nicoll RA. NMDA application potentiates synaptic transmission in the hippocampus. Nature. 1988;334:250–252. doi: 10.1038/334250a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiyama Y, Manabe T, Sakimura K, Kawakami F, Mori H, Mishina M. Increased thresholds for long-term potentiation and contextual learning in mice lacking the NMDA-type glutamate receptor epsilon1 subunit. J Neurosci. 1998;18:6704–6712. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-17-06704.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kullmann DM, Perkel DJ, Manabe T, Nicoll RA. Ca2+ entry via postsynaptic voltage-sensitive Ca2+ channels can transiently potentiate excitatory synaptic transmission in the hippocampus. Neuron. 1992;9:1175–1183. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(92)90075-o. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larson J, Lynch G. Role of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors in the induction of synaptic potentiation by burst stimulation patterned after the hippocampal theta-rhythm. Brain Res. 1988;441:111–118. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(88)91388-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larson J, Wong D, Lynch G. Patterned stimulation at the theta frequency is optimal for the induction of hippocampal long-term potentiation. Brain Res. 1986;368:347–350. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)90579-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li R, Huang FS, Abbas AK, Wigstrom H. Role of NMDA receptor subtypes in different forms of NMDA-dependent synaptic plasticity. BMC Neurosci. 2007;8:55. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-8-55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu L, Wong TP, Pozza MF, Lingenhoehl K, Wang Y, Sheng M, Auberson YP, Wang YT. Role of NMDA receptor subtypes in governing the direction of hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Science. 2004;304:1021–1024. doi: 10.1126/science.1096615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lozovaya NA, Grebenyuk SE, Tsintsadze T, Feng B, Monaghan DT, Krishtal OA. Extrasynaptic NR2B and NR2D subunits of NMDA receptors shape ‘superslow’ afterburst EPSC in rat hippocampus. J Physiol. 2004;558:451–463. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.063792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malenka RC. Postsynaptic factors control the duration of synaptic enhancement in area CA1 of the hippocampus. Neuron. 1991;6:53–60. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(91)90121-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin SJ, Grimwood PD, Morris RG. Synaptic plasticity and memory: an evaluation of the hypothesis. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:649–711. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Massey PV, Johnson BE, Moult PR, Auberson YP, Brown MW, Molnar E, Collingridge GL, Bashir ZI. Differential roles of NR2A and NR2B-containing NMDA receptors in cortical long-term potentiation and long-term depression. J Neurosci. 2004;24:7821–7828. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1697-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGuinness L, Taylor C, Taylor RD, Yau C, Langenhan T, Hart ML, Christian H, Tynan PW, Donnelly P, Emptage NJ. Presynaptic NMDARs in the hippocampus facilitate transmitter release at theta frequency. Neuron. 2010;68:1109–1127. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monyer H, Burnashev N, Laurie DJ, Sakmann B, Seeburg PH. Developmental and regional expression in the rat brain and functional properties of four NMDA receptors. Neuron. 1994;12:529–540. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(94)90210-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morley RM, Tse HW, Feng B, Miller JC, Monaghan DT, Jane DE. Synthesis and pharmacology of N1-substituted piperazine-2,3-dicarboxylic acid derivatives acting as NMDA receptor antagonists. J Med Chem. 2005;48:2627–2637. doi: 10.1021/jm0492498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moser E, Moser MB, Andersen P. Synaptic potentiation in the rat dentate gyrus during exploratory learning. Neuroreport. 1993;5:317–320. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199312000-00035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moser EI, Moser MB, Andersen P. Potentiation of dentate synapses initiated by exploratory learning in rats: dissociation from brain temperature, motor activity, and arousal. Learn Mem. 1994;1:55–73. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pananceau M, Gustafsson B. NMDA receptor dependence of the input specific NMDA receptor-independent LTP in the hippocampal CA1 region. Brain Res. 1997;752:255–260. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(96)01471-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paoletti P, Neyton J. NMDA receptor subunits: function and pharmacology. Curr Opin Pharmacol. 2007;7:39–47. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2006.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papouin T, Ladepeche L, Ruel J, Sacchi S, Labasque M, Hanini M, Groc L, Pollegioni L, Mothet JP, Oliet SH. Synaptic and extrasynaptic NMDA receptors are gated by different endogenous coagonists. Cell. 2012;150:633–646. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.06.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pina-Crespo JC, Gibb AJ. Subtypes of NMDA receptors in new-born rat hippocampal granule cells. J Physiol. 2002;541:41–64. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2001.014001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rauner C, Kohr G. Triheteromeric NR1/NR2A/NR2B receptors constitute the major N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor population in adult hippocampal synapses. J Biol Chem. 2011;286:7558–7566. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M110.182600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakimura K, Kutsuwada T, Ito I, Manabe T, Takayama C, Kushiya E, Yagi T, Aizawa S, Inoue Y, Sugiyama H, et al. Reduced hippocampal LTP and spatial learning in mice lacking NMDA receptor epsilon 1 subunit. Nature. 1995;373:151–155. doi: 10.1038/373151a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schulz PE, Fitzgibbons JC. Differing mechanisms of expression for short- and long-term potentiation. J Neurophysiol. 1997;78:321–334. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.1.321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson CL, Drewery DL, Atkins HD, Stephenson FA, Chazot PL. Immunohistochemical localization of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor subunits in the adult murine hippocampal formation: evidence for a unique role of the NR2D subunit. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 2002;102:55–61. doi: 10.1016/s0169-328x(02)00183-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vicini S, Wang JF, Li JH, Zhu WJ, Wang YH, Luo JH, Wolfe BB, Grayson DR. Functional and pharmacological differences between recombinant N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors. J Neurophysiol. 1998;79:555–566. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.2.555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volianskis A, Jensen MS. Transient and sustained types of long-term potentiation in the CA1 area of the rat hippocampus. J Physiol. 2003;550:459–492. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.044214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volianskis A, Kostner R, Molgaard M, Hass S, Jensen MS. Episodic memory deficits are not related to altered glutamatergic synaptic transmission and plasticity in the CA1 hippocampus of the APPswe/PS1DE9-deleted transgenic mice model of β-amyloidosis. Neurobiol Aging. 2010;31:1173–1187. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Engelhardt J, Doganci B, Jensen V, Hvalby O, Gongrich C, Taylor A, Barkus C, Sanderson DJ, Rawlins JN, Seeburg PH, Bannerman DM, Monyer H. Contribution of hippocampal and extra-hippocampal NR2B-containing NMDA receptors to performance on spatial learning tasks. Neuron. 2008;60:846–860. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weitlauf C, Honse Y, Auberson YP, Mishina M, Lovinger DM, Winder DG. Activation of NR2A-containing NMDA receptors is not obligatory for NMDA receptor-dependent long-term potentiation. J Neurosci. 2005;25:8386–8390. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2388-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]